Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Clin Psychol. 2016 Jul 26;73(4):489–499. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22346

Attentional Control Scale for Children: Factor Structure and Concurrent Validity Among Children and Adolescents Referred for Anxiety Disorders

Raquel Melendez 1, Michele Bechor 1, Yasmin Rey 1, Jeremy W Pettit 1, Wendy K Silverman 2
PMCID: PMC5545119  NIHMSID: NIHMS887770  PMID: 27459398

Abstract

Objective

The present study examined the factor structure and concurrent validity of the Attentional Control Scale for Children (ACS-C; Muris, de Jong, & Engelen, 2004), a youth self-rating scale of attentional control.

Method

A multisource assessment approach was used with 186 children and adolescents referred to an anxiety disorders specialty clinic.

Results

Exploratory factor analysis yielded a 2-factor structure with internally consistent and moderately correlated subscales of Attentional Focusing and Attentional Shifting. Total ACS-C and subscale scores demonstrated significant associations with youth and parent ratings of youth anxiety symptoms, youth self ratings of depressive symptoms, and youth diagnosis of attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder.

Conclusions

These findings support use of the ACS-C as a self-rating scale of attentional control among referred youth. Future research is encouraged to examine retest reliability of the ACS-C and to evaluate whether its internal structure could be enhanced by removing or modifying items that performed poorly.

Keywords: children, adolescent, anxiety, attentional control, factor analysis

Introduction

Attentional processes play a prominent role in information processing models of anxiety and its disorders particularly with regard to development, maintenance, and treatment (Field, Hadwin, & Lester, 2011). One attentional process that is garnering growing interest is attentional control. Attentional control (AC) refers to the ability to voluntarily and strategically focus, sustain, and shift one’s attention (Derryberry & Reed, 2002). High levels of AC enable children and adolescents (henceforth referred to as “youth”) to modulate their emotional experiences by strategically focusing attention on and shifting attention away from stimuli (Puliafico & Kendall, 2006). Low levels of AC hinder youths’ ability to adaptively engage with negatively valenced and threatening stimuli, thereby contributing to the development and maintenance of anxiety and its disorders (Lonigan, Vasey, Phillips, & Hazen, 2004; Muris & Ollendick, 2005; Susa, Pitica, Benga, & Miclea, 2012). Low levels of AC also have been implicated in the development and maintenance of disorders that frequently co-occur with anxiety in youth, including depression and attention–deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Bechor, Melendez, Rey, Pettit, & Silverman, 2015; Nigg, 2006).

AC is commonly assessed in adults using the Attentional Control Scale (ACS; Derryberry & Reed, 2002), a 20-item self-report measure. The ACS comprises 9-item and 11-item subscales that represent two proposed facets of AC: maintaining attention on a stimulus (attentional focusing) and shifting attention from one stimulus to another (attentional shifting; Derryberry & Rothbart, 1988). Support for the two-factor structure of the ACS has been obtained among samples of undergraduate students in the United States (Judah, Grant, Mills, & Lechner, 2014), Iceland (Ólafsson et al., 2011), and Poland (Fajkowska & Derryberry, 2010).

Internal consistency for ACS total score and ACS subscales has been adequate: alpha coefficients for total score range from α = .71 (Gyurak & Ayduk, 2007) to α = .88 (Derryberry & Reed, 2002); alpha coefficients for the Focusing subscale are α = .82 (Judah et al., 2014; Ólafsson et al., 2011); and alpha coefficients for the Shifting subscale range from α = .68 (Ólafsson et al., 2011) to α = .77 (Judah et al., 2014). Convergent validity and predictive validity have been supported via significant associations between the ACS and other self-report and performance-based measures of AC (Fajkowska & Derryberry, 2010; Judah et al., 2014).

Further, differential validity of the ACS subscales has been reported: the Focusing subscale uniquely predicted anxiety symptoms after controlling for depressive symptoms and the Shifting subscale uniquely predicted depressive symptoms after controlling for anxiety symptoms (Ólafsson et al., 2011). These differential validity findings are consistent with research and theory indicating difficulties primarily in shifting, or disengaging, attention from negative stimuli in depression (e.g., Gotlib & Joormann, 2010), and difficulties primarily in focusing attention in anxiety due to vigilant monitoring of the environment for threat cues (Moran & Moser, 2015).

AC has been most commonly assessed in youth using the Attentional Control Scale for Children (ACS-C; Muris, de Jong, & Engelen, 2004), which is a 20 item self-rating scale. The ACS-C is a downscaled adaptation of the adult ACS (Derryberry & Reed, 2002). Research supports the ACS-C’s convergent validity and concurrent validity among nonreferred youth. With regards to convergent validity, significant associations have been found between ACS-C scores and scores on performance-based tests of selective attention, attentional switching, and sustained attention (r = .26 to .35; Muris, Mayer, Lint, & Hofman, 2008). With regards to concurrent validity, significant cross-sectional associations have been reported between ACS-C scores and both self and parent ratings on measures of youth anxiety symptom severity (rs = −.52 to −.39; Muris et al., 2004; Muris et al., 2008; Muris, Meesters, & Rompelberg, 2007), youth depressive symptom severity (rs = −.31 to −.23; Muris et al., 2008; Muris et al., 2007), and youth ADHD symptom severity (rs = −.61 to −.43; Muris et al., 2008; Muris et al., 2007).

We are not aware of any published study on the factor structure of the ACS-C. One study (Verstraeten, Vasey, Claes, & Bijttebier, 2010) evaluated the factor structure of a Dutch-language version of the ACS (adult version) among 280 nonreferred youths (mean [M]age = 12.28 years, standard deviation [SD] = 2.46) sampled from two Belgian schools. Among these 280 youths, support was obtained for a two-factor model with factors representing attentional focusing and attentional shifting (Verstraeten et al., 2010). We also are not aware of any published study that has reported on the psychometric properties or validity of the ACS-C in a clinic-referred sample, including youth referred for anxiety and its disorders.

Given the theorized role of AC in development and maintenance of anxiety (Derryberry & Reed, 2002), depression (Joormann & Quinn, 2014), and ADHD (Nigg, 2006), as well as growing interest in targeting AC in interventions for youth with anxiety (Heeren, de Raedt, Koster, & Philippot, 2013; Wass, Porayska-Pomsta, & Johnson, 2011) and with ADHD (Shalev, Tsal, & Mevorach, 2007), there is a need to establish whether the ACS-C is a psychometrically sound measure of AC for use among referred youth. The present study sought to address this need by examining the factor structure and concurrent validity of the ACS-C among youth referred for anxiety. Exploratory factor analysis was used because this was the first study to evaluate the factor structure of the ACS-C. Concurrent validity was evaluated via associations between youth self ratings on the ACS-C and parent ratings and youth self ratings on a measure of anxiety symptoms.

As in past studies among nonreferred youth (Muris et al., 2008; Muris et al., 2007), concurrent validity also was evaluated via associations between youth self ratings on the ACS and youth self ratings on a measure of depressive symptoms and youth diagnosis of co-occurring ADHD. Given low to modest agreement across informant sources in the youth anxiety literature (Silverman & Ollendick, 2005), a multisource assessment approach was used to evaluate concurrent validity. Consistent findings across informant sources would enhance confidence in the robustness of findings. Based on theory and past empirical research in nonreferred samples, we expected scores on the ACS-C would be significantly and negatively associated with scores on measures of anxiety symptoms and depressive symptoms. We also expected youth who met diagnostic criteria for ADHD would display significantly lower scores on the ACS-C than youth who did not meet criteria for ADHD.

Differential validity of ACS-C subscales in relation to anxiety and depressive symptoms was examined in light of theory and evidence supporting differential validity of the ACS in adult samples (Judah et al., 2014; Ólafsson et al., 2011). We expected attentional focusing would significantly predict anxiety symptoms after controlling for depressive symptoms and attentional shifting would significantly predict depressive symptoms after controlling for anxiety symptoms.

Method

Participants

Participants were 186 youths aged 6 to 17 years (58% boys; Mage = 9.66; SDage = 2.48) who were referred to an anxiety disorders specialty clinic. Approximately 83% of the sample identified as Hispanic/Latino, 12% identified as European American, and 5% identified as other race/ethnicity. Annual household income was reported by parents and was as follows: 11% reported below $21,000, 13% reported between $21,000 and $40,000, 20% reported between $41,000 and $60,000, 16% reported between $61,000 and $80,000, and 40% reported over $81,000. The most common primary diagnoses were generalized anxiety disorder (29.0%), social anxiety disorder (21.0%), separation anxiety disorder (18.3%), and specific phobia (11.8%). Of the sample, 20% met criteria for a diagnosis of ADHD (primary, secondary, or tertiary) and 4% met criteria for a diagnosis of major depressive disorder or dysthymia (primary, secondary, or tertiary).

Measures

Diagnostic measure

Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule-Child and Parent Version-IV (ADIS-C/P-IV; Silverman & Albano, 1996)

The ADIS-C/P contains 0- to 8-point clinician severity rating scales to assess the severity and interference of diagnoses. Before conducting interviews, evaluators received extensive training in administration and scoring protocol and met 100% reliability criterion on five videotaped child–parent assessments. The ADIS-C/P has yielded good to excellent interrater reliability estimates for specific anxiety diagnoses (kappa = .57 to 1.0) and ADHD (kappa = .80), as well as excellent retest reliability estimates over 2 weeks (r = .80 to .92; Lyneham, Abbott, & Rapee, 2007; Silverman, Saavedra, & Pina, 2001; Silverman, Kurtines, Jaccard, & Pina, 2009).

Convergent validity for anxiety diagnoses has been demonstrated via significant associations with youths’ self ratings on anxiety (Silverman et al., 2001; Wood, Piacentini, Bergman, McCracken, & Barrios, 2002). The ADIS-C/P also has previously been used as a primary instrument for diagnosing ADHD (Halldorsdottir et al., 2015). Convergent validity for ADHD diagnosis has been demonstrated via significant associations with parent and teacher ratings of youth externalizing symptoms and attention problems (Anderson & Ollendick, 2012; Jarrett, Wolff, & Ollendick, 2007).

Measures completed by youth

ACS-C (Muris et al., 2004)

The ACS-C is a 20-item youth self-rating scale that assesses abilities to focus and shift attention. Responses are scored on a 4-point Likert scale that ranges from 1 (almost never) to 4 (always). After reverse coding, higher scores indicate better AC. Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .74.

Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale-Child Version (RCMAS-C; Reynolds & Richmond, 1978)

The RCMAS-C is a 37-item youth self-rating scale that assesses anxiety symptoms. Each item is rated either yes or no, scored 1 or 0. A Total Anxiety score is computed by summing ratings on 28 items. A lie subscale comprises the remaining nine items. The RCMAS-C has demonstrated high retest reliability (.98) over a 3-week period (Pela & Reynolds, 1982). Convergent validity has been demonstrated via significant correlations with trait anxiety and fear (Ollendick, 1983). Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .88.

Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1985)

The CDI is a 27-item youth self-rating scale that assesses depressive symptoms. Each item contains a unique set of three response options (e.g., I am sad once in a while, I am sad many times, and I am sad all the time) and youths are instructed to select the option that best describes them during the previous two weeks. Thirteen items are reverse scored and summed with the remaining items to obtain an overall score. Convergent validity has been demonstrated via significant correlations with independent evaluator-rated measures of depressive symptoms and youth self ratings on other measures of depressive symptoms (Brooks & Kutcher, 2001; Klein, Dougherty, & Olino, 2005; Shain, Naylor, & Alessi, 1990). Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .88.

Measure completed by parents

RCMAS-Parent Version (RCMAS-P; Reynolds & Richmond, 1978)

In the RCMAS-P, the wording of RCMAS-C items was changed from I to my child, as was done in past research (e.g., Kendall, 1994; Silverman et al., 1999, 2009). Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was .85.

Procedures

The present study was approved by the institutional review board. Parents provided informed consent and youth provided assent. Graduate students who had been thoroughly trained in the study’s procedures conducted the assessments. Upon arrival at the clinic, youth participants and their parents (usually mothers) were administered the respective versions of ADIS-C/P-IV and the RCMAS-C/P. Youth also completed the ACS-C and CDI. All measures were completed at a pretreatment intake assessment.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS statistical software program (version 20). Missing data were minimal, not exceeding 4.8% of cases for any variable. We assessed missing data bias by computing a dummy variable representing the presence or absence of missing data for each variable. This dummy variable was then correlated with all other variables including demographic variables. No significant correlations were observed, indicating no evidence of bias due to missing data. Missing data were accommodated using maximum likelihood multiple imputation averaged across 10 iterations (Graham, 2009).

The data were examined for evidence of non-normality. Evidence of skew was present on the CDI. Evidence of kurtosis was present on the CDI and RCMAS-C. Because of its ability to accommodate non-normality of the data, principal axis factoring was used to examine the factor structure of the ACS-C (Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum, & Strahan, 1999). The number of factors to be extracted was determined by scree plot and a parallel analysis using an SPSS macro (O’Connor, 2000). The scree plot was evaluated such that the primary bend in the plot was used to determine the number of factors for extraction. Oblique (Direct Oblimin) rotations were used because we expected factors to be intercorrelated. Items with loadings of .32 or greater were considered indicators of a factor (Costello & Osborne, 2005).

Bivariate correlations were used to evaluate associations between scores on the ACS-C and other measured variables. Two-tailed Pearson correlations were used for analyses involving pairs of continuous variables. Point biserial correlations were used for analyses involving dichotomous variables for presence or absence of ADHD. Hierarchical regression models were used to examine differential validity of the ACS-C subscales in relation to measures of anxiety and depressive symptom severity.

Results

Means, standard deviations, and correlations between measured variables are presented in Table 1. Scores on the ACS-C did not significantly vary according to youth age, sex, race, or ethnicity. Scores also did not significantly vary by anxiety diagnosis or depression diagnosis.

Table 1.

Means of, Standard Deviations of, and Correlations Between Measured Variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. ACS-C
2. Focusing .81**
3. Shifting .87** .42**
4. RCMAS-C −.39** −.34** −.32**
5. RCMAS-P −.19** −.16* −.21** .23**
6. CDI −.35** −.29** −.34** .72** .29**
7. ADHD Dx −.24** −.21** −.22* .10 .21** .16*
  M 50.71 23.00 27.54 12.34 13.63 10.40
  SD 8.64 4.69 5.68 6.59 5.84 8.20

Note. N = 186. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; ACS-C = Attentional Control Scale for Children; Focusing = ACS-C Attentional Focusing Subscale; Shifting = ACS-C Attentional Shifting Subscale; RCMAS-C = Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale-Child Version; RCMAS-P = Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale - Parent Version; CDI = Children’s Depression Inventory; ADHD Dx = presence of an ADHD diagnosis.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

Exploratory factor analysis

Evaluation of the scree plot and results of the parallel analysis suggested the extraction of three factors. Therefore, three factors were retained for the first exploratory factor analysis. The three-factor solution accounted for 27.79% of the variance in ACS-C items. Only three items loaded on the third factor and internal consistency of the third factor was inadequate (α = .33). Because of the inadequate internal consistency of the third factor, which is common in subscales with a low number of items (Floyd & Widaman, 1995), a two-factor solution was evaluated.

The two-factor solution accounted for 22.97% of the variance in ACS-C items. Item loadings for the two-factor solution are presented in Table 2. Nine items had loadings of .32 or higher on the first factor, with the majority describing ability to focus attention. The first factor was thus labeled “attentional focusing.” Responses to these nine items were summed to create total scores on an Attentional Focusing subscale (α = 0.77). Six items, all describing ability to shift attention, had loadings of .32 or higher on the second factor. Therefore, the second factor was labeled “attentional shifting.” Responses to these six items were summed to create total scores on an Attentional Shifting subscale (α = 0.64). Items 4, 5, 9, 15, and 16 did not have loadings of 0.32 or higher on either factor.

Table 2.

Factor Loadings of ACS-C Items

Item Focusing Shifting
  1. It’s very hard for me to concentrate on a difficult lesson if there is a lot of noise in the class. .55 .09
  2. If I have to concentrate and solve a difficult math problem, I have trouble focusing my attention. .58 −.11
  3. When I am working hard on something, I still get distracted by things going on around me. .65 −.12
  4. My concentration is good, even when somebody turns the music on. .21 .19
  5. When I concentrate myself, I do not notice what is happening in the room around me. −.16 .27
  6. When I am reading in the classroom, I am easily disturbed by other children talking to each other. .62 .02
  7. When I try to concentrate myself, I find it difficult not to think about other things. .49 −.04
  8. I find it difficult to concentrate myself when I am excited about something. .34 −.05
  9. When I am concentrated, I do not notice that I am hungry or thirsty. −.03 .06
10. When I am doing something, I can easily stop and switch to some other task. −.04 .40
11. When I have to start a new task, it takes me a while to get really .58 .07
12. When the teacher explains something, I find it difficult to understand and write it down at the same time. .54 −.05
13. When it is necessary, I can become interested in a new topic very quickly. −.03 .57
14. It is easy for me to read or write while I am also talking to someone on the telephone. .03 .43
15. I have trouble having two conversations at the same time. .19 .14
16. I find it difficult to come up with new ideas quickly. .30 .12
17. After being interrupted or distracted, I can easily shift my attention back to what I was doing before. .31 .35
18. When I am daydreaming or having distracting thoughts, it is easy for me to switch back to the work I have to do. .31 .41
19. It is easy for me to switch back and forth between two different tasks. .16 .60
20. I find it difficult to let go my own way of thinking about something, and to look at it in a different way. .40 −.09
Initial eigenvalues 3.88 2.14
Extraction sums of squared loadings 3.18 1.42
Percentage of variance 15.90 7.08
Internal consistency .77 .64

Note. ACS-C Attentional Control Scale for Children. Items 1, 2, 3, 6, 7, 8, 11, 12, 15, 16, 18, and 20 are reversed for scoring. Items retained in the exploratory factor analysis are bolded.

Concurrent Validity

Bivariate correlations were used to evaluate concurrent associations between scores on the total ACS-C, the two ACS-C subscales, measures of youth anxiety symptoms, the measure of youth depressive symptoms, and youth diagnosis of ADHD (see Table 1). Based on youth and parent responses to the ADIS, dichotomously scored variables representing presence (1) or absence (0) of youth ADHD diagnosis were created and used in analyses. As hypothesized, total scores on the ACS-C were significantly and negatively correlated with scores on measures of youth self-rated and parent-rated anxiety symptom severity and scores on the measure of youth depressive symptom severity (rs = −.39 to −.19). Scores on the total ACS-C also were significantly associated with a diagnosis of ADHD, such that youth who met diagnostic criteria for ADHD displayed significantly lower levels of AC than youth who did not meet criteria for ADHD (r = −.24).

The correlation between attentional focusing and attentional shifting was significant and positive; the strength of the correlation was moderate (r = 0.42). Scores on both the Attentional Focusing subscale and the Attentional Shifting subscale were significantly and negatively correlated with scores on measures of youth self-rated and parent-rated anxiety symptom severity, scores on the measure of youth depressive symptom severity, and a youth diagnosis of ADHD (rs = −.34 to −.16).

Differential Validity

Three hierarchical regression models were used to examine differential validity of the ACS-C subscales in relation to anxiety and depressive symptom severity. In the first model, depressive symptoms was placed as the criterion variable. Youth ratings and parent ratings of anxiety were entered as predictors on the first step, and the Attentional Shifting and Attentional Focusing subscales were entered on the second step. In the second and third models, youth and parent ratings of anxiety were placed as the criterion variables, respectively. Depression symptoms was entered on the first step and the Attentional Shifting and Attentional Focusing subscales were entered on the second step. Results for all three models are shown in Table 3. Attentional focusing significantly predicted youth self ratings of anxiety severity controlling for depressive symptom severity and attentional shifting. In no other instance did attentional focusing or attentional shifting significantly predict symptoms of anxiety or depression.

Table 3.

Hierarchical Regressions Using Attentional Focusing and Attentional Shifting to Predict Anxiety and Depressive Symptom Severity

B SE B Beta
Dependent variable: CDI
Step 1 (ΔR2 = .54***)
RCMAS-C .82 .07 .66***
RCMAS-P .15 .07 .11*
Step 2 (ΔR2 = .01)
ACS-C Attentional Focusing .00 .10 .00
ACS-C Attentional Shifting −.17 .08 −.12

Dependent variable: RCMAS-C
Step 1 (ΔR2 = .52***)
CDI .54 .04 .67***
Step 2 (ΔR2 = .02*)
ACS-C Attentional Focusing −.21 .08 −.15*
ACS-C Attentional Shifting −.03 .07 −.02

Dependent variable: RCMAS-P
Step 1 (ΔR2 = .08)
CDI .16 .05 .23**
Step 2 (ΔR2 = .02)
ACS-C Attentional Focusing −.08 .10 −.07
ACS-C Attentional Shifting −.10 .08 −.09

Note. N = 186. SE = standard error; CDI = Children’s Depression Inventory; RCMAS-C = Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale - Child Version; RCMAS-P = Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale - Parent Version; ACS-C = Attentional Control Scale for Children.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Discussion

Findings from this exploratory factor analysis of the ACS-C among referred youth provide evidence of two moderately correlated and internally consistent factors: Attentional Focusing and Attentional Shifting. The two-factor structure found in this sample aligns with findings of a two-factor structure on the adult ACS (Judah et al., 2014; Ólafsson et al., 2011). The correlation between the subscales in the present sample (r = .42) was comparable to those among adult samples (rs = .45 to .73; Judah et al., 2014; Ólafsson et al., 2011).

Converging evidence from both youth and adult samples indicates the construct of AC as measured by the ACS, and the ACS-C comprises two related but distinguishable factors: One factor, Attentional Focusing, describes the ability to maintain attention on a stimulus; a second factor, Attentional Shifting, describes the ability to shift attention from one stimulus to another. Although the majority of items loaded on either the Attentional Focusing factor or the Attentional Shifting factor, five items did not load on either factor. These same five items also did not load on either factor of the adult ACS in a sample of nonreferred adults (Judah, et al., 2014). These items do not appear to measure either attentional focusing or attentional shifting in youths or adults. If replicated in other youth samples, removal of these items from the ACS-C may lead to improved internal structure.

Three items originally purported to measure attentional shifting on the adult ACS (Derryberry & Reed, 2002) that was loaded on the Attentional Focusing factor of the ACS-C in this sample (items 11, 12, and 20). Two of these same items (items 12 and 20) also were loaded on an Attentional Focusing factor in studies on the factor structure of the adult ACS (Judah et al., 2014; Ólafsson et al., 2011). Thus, a growing body of evidence indicates items 12 and 20, and possibly item 11, should be considered measures of attentional focusing rather than attentional shifting. Of note, the wording of these items appears to align as closely with focusing as shifting (e.g., When I have to start a new task, it takes me a while to get really involved in it; When the teacher explains something, I find it difficult to understand and write it down at the same time).

Concurrent validity of the ACS-C was supported via significant cross-sectional correlations with youth and parent ratings on anxiety symptoms, youth self ratings on depressive symptoms, and a youth diagnosis of ADHD. Although statistically significant, correlation coefficients demonstrating concurrent validity were in the small to moderate range (Cohen, 1988), consistent with reported correlation coefficients for the adult ACS (Judah et al., 2014; Ólafsson et al., 2011) but somewhat lower than reported correlation coefficients for the ACS-C in nonreferred samples of children (Muris et al., 2004, 2008, 2007).

The small to moderate correlations found in the present study may be due to the complex nature of anxiety, depression, and ADHD. These disorders are influenced by and exert influence on many variables, including AC. The strength of the association with any one given variable may be relatively small and may vary across levels of a third variable (i.e., there may be interactive effects). The small to moderate correlations found in the present study also may be due in part to our strategy of sampling youth who were referred to an anxiety disorder specialty clinic. This sampling strategy may have led to a restricted range of scores on symptom measures, which deflate correlation coefficients.

Similar to findings reported in adult samples (Judah et al., 2014; Ólafsson et al., 2011), the present study found some evidence of differential validity of the ACS-C subscales. Attentional focusing was significantly associated with youth self ratings of anxiety after controlling for depression. This finding is consistent with theory and research indicating that impairments in attentional focusing may be specific to anxiety, not depression, and may correspond to high vigilance for threatening stimuli (Moran & Moser, 2015). Attentional shifting was not significantly associated with depression after controlling for anxiety. Support for differential validity of attentional shifting might be found in a sample with more severe levels of depression than the present sample, which had a low rate (4%) of comorbid depression diagnosis.

Strengths and Limitations

The findings of this study should be interpreted in light of its strengths and limitations and characteristics of the sample. Strengths include the use of a clinic-referred sample of youth, semistructured interviews to establish diagnoses, and a multisource assessment approach for youth anxiety symptoms. Limitations include our inability to examine retest reliability of the ACS-C and evaluate its convergent validity via associations with other self-rating or performance-based measures of AC. Further, although ACS-C scores were not significantly correlated with participant age in this study, it would be of interest to examine the ACS-C from a developmental perspective. For example, when do focusing and shifting emerge as separate facets of AC? And do their respective associations with anxiety symptom severity differ across developmental levels? Given sample size constraints and a preponderance of participants in late childhood to early adolescence, we were not in a position to evaluate the factor structure and concurrent validity of the ACS-C across development.

The present sample comprised predominantly Hispanic/Latino participants. The generalizability of findings to other populations is unknown. Previous research using samples of predominantly Hispanic/Latino youth with anxiety disorders generally indicates high similarity to youth from other ethnic groups with respect to phenomenology and treatment response (e.g., Pina, Silverman, Fuentes, Kurtines, & Weems, 2003; Pina & Silverman, 2004). However, the factorial invariance of youth anxiety rating scales across ethnic groups has received mixed support, with some studies supporting factorial invariance (e.g., Pina, Little, Knight, & Silverman, 2009; Varela & Biggs, 2006) and other studies finding different factor structures in Hispanic/Latino youth compared to youth from other ethnic groups (e.g., Wren et al., 2007). The factorial invariance of the ACS-C across ethnic groups remains an open empirical question.

Conclusion

In summary, the present study provides the first empirical data on the factor structure of the ACS-C and evidence of concurrent validity of the ACS-C among referred youth. These findings support use of the ACS-C as a self-rating scale of attentional focusing and attentional shifting among referred youth. The brevity of the measure makes it easy to administer across a variety of clinical settings. As treatments intervening on AC gain traction, the ACS-C holds potential as a tool for gauging treatment progress and outcomes. To this end, researchers are encouraged to evaluate the sensitivity of the ACS-C to treatments that target AC. Future research is encouraged to examine retest reliability and convergent and discriminant validity of the measure and to evaluate whether the internal structure of the measure could be enhanced by removing items that performed poorly.

Acknowledgments

Work on this project was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grants R34 MH097931, UH2 MH101470, and R01 MH079943 to Jeremy W. Pettit and Wendy K. Silverman. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health

References

  1. Anderson SR, Ollendick TH. Diagnosing oppositional defiant disorder using the anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM-IV: Parent version and the diagnostic interview schedule for children. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2012;34(4):467–475. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bechor M, Melendez R, Rey Y, Pettit JW, Silverman WK. Attentional control partially explains the association between anxiety symptoms and depressive symptoms among clinic referred youth. 2015 Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
  3. Brooks SJ, Kutcher S. Diagnosis and measurement of adolescent depression: A review of commonly utilized instruments. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 2001;11(4):341–376. doi: 10.1089/104454601317261546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2. New York: Routledge; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  5. Costello AB, Osborne JW. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation. 2005;10(7):1–9. [Google Scholar]
  6. Derryberry D, Reed MA. Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111(2):225–236. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Derryberry D, Rothbart MK. Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;55(6):958–966. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.55.6.958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fabrigar LR, Wegener DT, MacCallum RC, Strahan EJ. Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods. 1999;4(3):272–299. [Google Scholar]
  9. Fajkowska M, Derryberry D. Psychometric properties of Attentional Control Scale: The preliminary study on a Polish sample. Polish Psychological Bulletin. 2010;41(1):1–7. [Google Scholar]
  10. Field AP, Hadwin JA, Lester KJ. Information processing biases in child and adolescent anxiety: Evidence and origins. In: Silverman WK, Field AP, editors. Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: Research, assessments, and interventions. 2. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2011. pp. 103–128. [Google Scholar]
  11. Floyd FJ, Widaman KF. Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psychological Assessment. 1995;7:286–299. [Google Scholar]
  12. Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2010;6:285. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Graham JW. Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:549–576. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gyurak A, Ayduk Ö. Defensive physiological reactions to rejection the effect of self-esteem and attentional control on startle responses. Psychological Science. 2007;18(10):886–892. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01996.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Halldorsdottir T, Ollendick TH, Ginsburg G, Sherrill J, Kendall PC, Walkup J, Piacentini J. Treatment outcomes in anxious youth with and without comorbid ADHD in the CAMS. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2015;44(6):985–991. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.952008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Heeren A, de Raedt R, Koster EH, Philippot P. The (neuro) cognitive mechanisms behind attention bias modification in anxiety: Proposals based on theoretical accounts of attentional bias. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7(119):1–6. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jarrett MA, Wolff JC, Ollendick TH. Concurrent validity and informant agreement of the ADHD module of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2007;29(3):159–168. [Google Scholar]
  18. Joormann J, Quinn ME. Cognitive processes and emotion regulation in depression. Depression and Anxiety. 2014;31(4):308–315. doi: 10.1002/da.22264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Judah MR, Grant DM, Mills AC, Lechner WV. Factor structure and validation of the Attentional Control Scale. Cognition and Emotion. 2014;28(3):433–451. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.835254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kendall PC. Treating anxiety disorders in children: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;62(1):100–110. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.62.1.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Klein DN, Dougherty LR, Olino TM. Toward guidelines for evidence-based assessment of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34(3):412–432. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kovacs M. The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1985;21(4):995–998. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lonigan CJ, Vasey MW, Phillips BM, Hazen RA. Temperament, anxiety, and the processing of threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33(1):8–20. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lyneham HJ, Abbott MJ, Rapee RM. Interrater reliability of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and parent version. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46(6):731–736. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3180465a09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Moran TP, Moser JS. The color of anxiety: Neurobehavioral evidence for distraction by perceptually salient stimuli in anxiety. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2015;15(1):169–179. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0314-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Muris P, de Jong PJ, Engelen S. Relationships between neuroticism, attentional control, and anxiety disorders symptoms in non-clinical children. Personality and Individual Differences. 2004;37(4):789–797. [Google Scholar]
  27. Muris P, Mayer B, Lint CV, Hofman S. Attentional control and psychopathological symptoms in children. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;44(7):1495–1505. [Google Scholar]
  28. Muris P, Meesters C, Rompelberg L. Attention control in middle childhood: Relations to psychopathological symptoms and threat perception distortions. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45(5):997–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Muris P, Ollendick TH. The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2005;8(4):271–289. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-8809-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Nigg JT. Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47(3–4):395–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. O’Connor BP. SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2000;32(3):396–402. doi: 10.3758/bf03200807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ólafsson RP, Smári J, Guðmundsdóttir F, Olafsdóttir G, Harðardóttir HL, Einarsson SM. Self reported attentional control with the Attentional Control Scale: Factor structure and relationship with symptoms of anxiety and depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2011;25(6):777–782. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ollendick TH. Reliability and validity of the revised fear survey schedule for children (FSSC-R) Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1983;21(6):685–692. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(83)90087-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Pela OA, Reynolds CR. Cross-cultural application of the Revised-Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale: Normative and reliability data for Nigerian primary school children. Psychological Reports. 1982;51(3f):1135–1138. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1982.51.3f.1135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Pina AA, Little M, Knight GP, Silverman WK. Cross-ethnic measurement equivalence of the RCMAS in Latino and Caucasian youth with anxiety disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2009;91(1):58–61. doi: 10.1080/00223890802484183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pina AA, Silverman WK, Fuentes RM, Kurtines WM, Weems CF. Exposure-based cognitive-behavioral treatment for phobic and anxiety disorders: treatment effects and maintenance for Hispanic/Latino relative to European-American youths. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42(10):1179–1187. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200310000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Puliafico AC, Kendall PC. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious youth: A review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2006;9(3–4):162–180. doi: 10.1007/s10567-006-0009-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Reynolds CR, Richmond BO. What I think and feel: A revised measure of children’s manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1978;6(2):271–280. doi: 10.1007/BF00919131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Shain BN, Naylor M, Alessi N. Comparison of self-rated and clinician-rated measures of depression in adolescents. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1990;147(6):793–795. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.6.793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Shalev L, Tsal Y, Mevorach C. Computerized Progressive Attentional Training (CPAT) program: Effective direct intervention for children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology. 2007;13(4):382–388. doi: 10.1080/09297040600770787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Silverman WK, Albano AM. Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for Children-IV (child and parent versions) San Antonio. TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  42. Silverman WK, Kurtines WM, Ginsburg GS, Weems CF, Lumpkin PW, Carmichael DH. Treating anxiety disorders in children with group cognitive-behavioral therapy: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67(6):995–1003. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.6.995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Silverman WK, Kurtines WM, Jaccard J, Pina AA. Directionality of change in youth anxiety treatment involving parents: An initial examination. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009;77(3):474–485. doi: 10.1037/a0015761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Silverman WK, Ollendick TH. Evidence-based assessment of anxiety and its disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2005;34(3):380–411. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Silverman WK, Saavedra LM, Pina AA. Test-retest reliability of anxiety symptoms and diagnoses with the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and parent versions. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40(8):937–944. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200108000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Susa G, Pitică I, Benga O, Miclea M. The self regulatory effect of attentional control in modulating the relationship between attentional biases toward threat and anxiety symptoms in children. Cognition and Emotion. 2012;26(6):1069–1083. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.638910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Varela RE, Biggs BK. Reliability and validity of the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) across samples of Mexican, Mexican American, and European American children: A preliminary investigation. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International Journal. 2006;19(1):67–80. [Google Scholar]
  48. Verstraeten K, Vasey MW, Claes L, Bijttebier P. The assessment of effortful control in childhood: Questionnaires and the Test of Everyday Attention for Children compared. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;48(1):59–65. [Google Scholar]
  49. Wass S, Porayska-Pomsta K, Johnson MH. Training attentional control in infancy. Current Biology. 2011;21(18):1543–1547. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wood JJ, Piacentini JC, Bergman RL, McCracken J, Barrios V. Concurrent validity of the anxiety disorders section of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: child and parent versions. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2002;31(3):335–342. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3103_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wren FJ, Berg EA, Heiden LA, Kinnamon CJ, Ohlson LA, Bridge JA, Bernal MP. Childhood anxiety in a diverse primary care population: Parent-child reports, ethnicity and SCARED factor structure. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46(3):332–340. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31802f1267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES