Abstract
Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus (STN-DBS) can improve the motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease (PD) and negate the problematic side effects of dopamine replacement therapy. Although there is concern that STN-DBS may enhance the development of gambling disorder and other impulse control disorders in this patient group, recent data suggest that STN-DBS may actually reduce iatrogenic impulse control disorders, and alleviate obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Here, we sought to determine whether STN-DBS was beneficial or detrimental to performance of the rat gambling task (rGT), a rodent analogue of the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) used to assess risky decision making clinically. Rats chose between four options associated with different amounts and probabilities of sugar pellet rewards versus timeout punishments. As in the IGT, the optimal approach was to favor options associated with smaller per-trial gains but lower timeout penalties. Once a stable behavioral baseline was established, electrodes were implanted bilaterally into the STN, and the effects of STN-DBS assessed on-task over 10 consecutive sessions using an A-B-A design. STN-DBS did not affect choice in optimal decision makers that correctly favored options associated with smaller per-trial gains but also lower penalties. However, a minority (∼25%) preferred the maladaptive “high-risk, high-reward” options at baseline. STN-DBS significantly and progressively improved choice in these risk-preferring rats. These data support the hypothesis that STN-DBS may be beneficial in ameliorating maladaptive decision making associated with compulsive and addiction disorders.
Keywords: addiction, basal ganglia, decision making, impulsivity, Parkinson’s disease, rat gambling task
Significance Statement
Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus (STN-DBS) may constitute a relatively safe and effective alternative to pharmacotherapy, not just for Parkinson’s disease (PD), but for disorders of addiction and compulsion in which decision making is compromised. However, concern remains over whether this manipulation may itself trigger impulse control deficits or risky decision making, as may be predicted from rodent lesion data. Here, we directly test this hypothesis, and evaluate the effects of STN-DBS in rats performing a rodent gambling paradigm based on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) used clinically. Far from inducing impulsivity or exacerbating risky choice, STN-DBS selectively improved decision making in animals exhibiting a risk-preferring strategy at baseline. These data suggest that STN-DBS may be beneficial in the treatment of psychiatric, rather than solely neurologic, conditions.
Introduction
Given the limited efficacy of dopamine replacement therapies (DRT) such as l-DOPA for Parkinson’s disease (PD), deep brain stimulation (DBS) is becoming more common to relieve the motor symptoms of PD (Limousin et al., 1995; Benabid et al., 1998; Martinez-Ramirez and Okun, 2014). In addition, the realization that D2/3 family agonists can lead to the de novo development of serious impulse control and addiction problems, including gambling disorder (GD), has made neurologists reluctant to prescribe this class of drugs, despite their noted efficacy at restoring motor function (Lawrence et al., 2003; Evans and Lees, 2004; Weintraub et al., 2006; Voon et al., 2011). Both the globus pallidus internal (GPi) and subthalamic nucleus (STN) are common targets for DBS, and can lead to significant improvements in motor function. As DBS involves invasive surgery, it is generally reserved for later-stage PD patients for whom DRT has started to fail, yet there is increasing interest in applying DBS at earlier stages of the disease to maximize therapeutic benefit (Charles et al., 2012; Schuepbach et al., 2013).
STN-DBS can result in a more dramatic reduction in DRT dosing as compared to GPi-DBS (Follett et al., 2010; Odekerken et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2014), therefore this region may be a particularly valid target for those experiencing adverse reactions to DRT. However, the STN influences a wide range of non-motor functions, including impulsivity and decision making (Aron and Poldrack, 2005; Frank, 2006; Ballanger et al., 2009b), and rodent studies have been particularly informative in showing that STN lesions can increase incentive motivation, motor impulsivity, and decrease attention (Baunez et al., 1995; Baunez and Robbins, 1997; Baunez et al., 2002; Winstanley et al., 2005; Uslaner and Robinson, 2006). Furthermore, STN-DBS in PD patients can result in speedier, and arguably less reflective, decision making (Frank, 2006), transiently increase loss-chasing in a gambling simulation (Rogers et al., 2011), and may even precipitate the onset of GD (Witt et al., 2004; Smeding et al., 2007), although support for the latter is somewhat ambivalent (Hälbig et al., 2009; Demetriades et al., 2011; Rodriguez-Oroz et al., 2011). Indeed, a recent meta-analysis comparing DBS of the STN and GPi in PD concluded there were no differences in psychiatric complications resulting from targeting of either area (Wang et al., 2016). The fact that the STN influences cognitive function may, in fact, be of benefit to other patient groups, in that STN-DBS has been suggested as an efficacious treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and substance use disorder (SUD; Mallet et al., 2008; Pelloux and Baunez, 2013). However, comparatively little is known regarding the effects of STN-DBS on relevant higher-order cognitive processes, particularly in the absence of PD.
The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which subjects aim to maximize money or points through selecting from four decks of cards, was originally developed as a laboratory-based test of “real-world” decision making sensitive to frontal cortex damage (Bechara et al., 1994). The optimal strategy is to avoid “high-risk, high-reward” decks that are paired with disproportionately heavy penalties, and instead favor those associated with incremental yet consistent gains over time. Numerous clinical populations, including SUD, GD, and OCD, exhibit impairments on the IGT, and such persistent choice of the risky options may be a cognitive endophenotype for psychiatric vulnerability (Winstanley and Clark, 2016). To address whether STN-DBS may improve or impair such decision making, we therefore evaluated the effects of this manipulation in rats performing the rat gambling task (rGT), a behavioral paradigm based on the IGT. Given that STN-DBS may specifically impact individuals exhibiting maladaptive choice strategies, we deliberately analyzed whether this manipulation differentially affected optimal decision-makers versus risk-preferring rats.
Material and Methods
Subjects
Male Long Evans rats (n = 24; initial weight: 250-275 g; Charles River) were pair-housed with free access to water under a reverse light cycle in a climate-controlled colony. Rats were food restricted to 85% of their free-feeding weight and maintained on 14 g of standard rat chow per day plus any sugar pellets earned on task (∼5 g/d). All experimental practices were in accordance with the Canadian Council on Animal Care, and approved by the Animal Care Committee of the University of British Columbia.
Behavioral apparatus
Preoperative behavioral training took place in 12 standard Med Associates five-hole operant chambers housed in ventilated sound-attenuating cabinets. Each chamber featured a food tray outfitted with both a stimulus light and an infrared beam for detecting nose-poke inputs. Sucrose pellets (45 mg; Bio-Serv) could be delivered to this tray from an external food hopper and a house light was positioned on the chamber wall above. An array of five response apertures was located on the opposite wall, each equipped with stimulus lights and infrared beams for detecting input. Four boxes were equipped with a counterbalanced arm and removable roof insert to enable tethering, and postoperatively only these four boxes were used for behavioral testing. It was possible to deliver bilateral DBS via programmable electrical stimulators to animals within two of these latter boxes at any one time. All of the operant chambers ran according to MedPC (RRID: SCR_014721) programs written by C.A.W. and controlled by an IBM-compatible computer.
Behavioral training
Subjects (n = 24) were trained to perform the rat gambling task (rGT) as described previously (Zeeb et al., 2009). In the rGT, subjects sample between four options associated with different magnitudes and probabilities of sugar pellet rewards versus timeout punishments to maximize reward earned. In each 30-min session, subjects chose between one of four options, designated by illumination of response holes 1, 2, 4, and 5 (Fig. 1, task schematic). Each hole was associated with a distinct probability and magnitude of reward or timeout punishments, and are subsequently identified based on the number of potential pellets earned per choice, one pellet (P1), two pellets (P2), three pellets (P3), and four pellets (P4). As in the IGT, the optimal approach in the rGT was to favor options associated with smaller per-trial gains but lower timeout penalties; consistent choice of the smaller reward options was advantageous due to more frequent rewards, but also less frequent and shorter timeouts, with the two-pellet choice (P2) resulting in the most reward earned per unit time.
Subjects began each trial by nose-poking in the illuminated food tray. This response extinguished the tray light and resulted in a 5-s intertrial interval (ITI), during which all lights in the chamber were extinguished. If subjects withheld responding during the ITI, holes 1, 2, 4, and 5 of the array were illuminated for 10 s. A response in any illuminated hole turned off all stimulus lights and led to either onset of the tray light and delivery of a reward, or the start of a timeout “punishment” period. Rewarded trials led to illumination of the food tray and immediate delivery of the appropriate number of sucrose pellets. If a trial was punished, no reward was delivered and the stimulus light within the chosen hole flashed at 0.5 Hz until the punishing timeout elapsed, at which point the tray light was illuminated. If rats failed to make a nose poke in one of the illuminated holes within 10 s, the trial was counted as an omission. Following completion of any trial type, the tray light was reilluminated and subjects could begin a new trial by nose-poking therein. As in the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5CSRT; a rodent analog of the continuous performance test, designed to measure visuospatial attention, motor impulsivity and motivation; Carli et al., 1983; Robbins, 2002), a response made in one of the five response holes during the ITI was punished by a 5 s timeout period and recorded as a premature response. These premature responses serve as a well-validated index of motor impulsivity (Robbins, 2002). The timeout period was marked by illumination of the house light, after which the food tray light was reilluminated and subjects could commence a new trial. Training and testing continued each week day until a statistically stable baseline was observed across all variables measured (44 sessions).
Electrode implantation
Stimulating electrodes consisted of two platinum wires (EEP-1495 MS303/9-A/SPC Plastics One) sheathed in 9 mm of 22-gauge PEEK tubing, such that 0.5 mm of each electrode was left bare. Animals were anesthetized with 2% isoflurane in oxygen and then secured in a stereotaxic frame. Once anesthetized, animals were given 5 mg/kg ketoprofen subcutaneously. The incisor bar was initially set to -3.3 mm. Great care was taken to ensure the skull was level before implanting the electrodes, and the incisor bar adjusted until there was <0.2 mm difference in height between bregma and lambda. Using standard stereotaxic technique under aseptic conditions, sterile bilateral stimulating electrodes were inserted at an angle of five degrees off vertical through bore holes made in the skull, and then secured with bone screws and dental cement, according to the following stereotaxic coordinates: anteroposterior, -3.8 from bregma; medial/lateral, ±3.1 from the midline; dorsoventral from skull, -8.4. Plastic dust caps were used to protect the external electrode contacts. Animals were given a week to recover in their home cage before behavioral testing resumed. Animals were then reassessed on the rGT until stable baseline performance was observed over a minimum of three consecutive sessions.
STN-DBS during rGT performance
A timeline showing the phases of the behavioral experiment is provided in Figure 2. A within-subjects design was used, and all animals experienced STN-DBS. The start of DBS was synchronized to the start of the rGT test session, and remained on throughout the entire 30-min session. Each rat received DBS repeatedly, throughout ten successive, daily rGT test sessions. Animals were divided at random into two groups. Group 1 (G1) received three “mock DBS” rGT sessions, during which the electrodes were connected to the stimulators via spring-loaded tethers, but stimulation was OFF, to acclimatize animals to being tethered. This group then received STN-DBS throughout 10 consecutive rGT sessions, during which the stimulators were turned ON (130 Hz, 60-μs pulse width). Before the first behavioral test session, the current amplitude was adjusted for each individual rat according to former studies (Rouaud et al., 2010), and for each electrode, to just below that which induced the well-known hyperkinetic motor effects in the contralateral paw (treading, “piano playing”). Group 2 (G2) performed the rGT as per baseline training during this period. In each behavioral run of four animals, two therefore received STN-DBS or mock DBS, and two did not. Once group 1 had completed the tenth STN-DBS session, the conditions were switched such that group 2 then received three sessions of mock DBS, followed by ten STB-DBS sessions, and group 1 simply performed the rGT. After five additional behavioral sessions, group 1 was humanely sacrificed (sac) via live decapitation, the brains removed and flash frozen on dry ice, and stored at -80°C. In case any effects of STN-DBS persisted beyond the stimulation epoch itself, care was taken to ensure all rats completed the same number of poststimulation sessions. As such, group 2 performed an additional thirteen rGT sessions before euthanasia, such that data were collected from 18 poststimulation sessions for all rats.
Histology
The brains were immersion-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde at 4°C for 7 d, and then transferred to 30% sucrose solution. Tissue was sectioned into 30-µm sections on a cryostat set to -20°C. These slices were mounted onto 2% gelatin-coated slides and stained with cresyl violet. The location of electrode tips were then plotted onto standard rat brain sections (Paxinos and Watson, 1998).
Data analyses
Data from each experiment were analyzed using SPSS (version 24.0; SPSS). As per previous reports (Zeeb and Winstanley, 2011, 2013; Ferland and Winstanley, 2017), the following rGT variables were analyzed: score (the sum of advantageous choices minus the sum of disadvantageous choices: [P1 + P2]-[P3 + P4]); percentage choice of each option, percentage premature responses, percentage omissions, trials completed, choice- and reward collection latencies. A statistically stable pattern of behavior was verified by a repeated-measures ANOVA across data from at least three consecutive sessions, with session (three levels) and, where appropriate, choice option (four levels: P1-P4) as within-subjects factors, in which the session effects and session × option interactions were not significant. Animals with a mean positive score at baseline were designated as “optimal decision makers,” whereas rats with negative scores were classified as “risk-preferring” (Ferland and Winstanley, 2017). This between-subjects factor (group) was included in all analyses. To determine if risk-preferring rats differed with respect to weight gain as a result of pair-housing, body weight (grams) data over the course of the experiment were analyzed by within-subjects ANOVA with weight record (38 levels) as a within subjects factor, and group as a between-subjects factor.
The effects of mock DBS were also determined by ANOVA, by comparing data from these three sessions to three stable postoperative baseline sessions. Any effect of STN-DBS (sessions 1-3, sessions 4-6) were analyzed through comparison to mock DBS. The final four DBS sessions were similarly compared to the three mock DBS sessions plus the last postoperative baseline session, to correctly populate the ANOVA fields with four sessions in each stimulation condition. Poststimulation sessions were divided into three day bins, and whether any effects of STN-DBS were still evident once stimulation had ceased was again determined via ANOVA through comparison with the three mock DBS sessions. Results are expressed as mean ± SEM. Differences were considered significant at p < 0.05.
Results
Histology
Data from five rats were excluded from the experiment due to electrode failure (n = 1), loss of head-cap (n = 2), seizures on stimulation (n = 1), or general ill-health (n = 1). Two additional rats were excluded from data analyses due to incorrect placement of the electrode, leaving a final n = 17 (optimal decision makers: n = 13; risk-preferring: n = 4). The location of the electrode tips for these 17 subjects is shown in Figure 3.
Effects of STN-DBS on risky choice
Animals became accustomed to tethering over three mock stimulation sessions before DBS, as indicated by stable patterns of choice across all 4 options (Fig. 4A,B; session: F(2,30) = 2.211, p = 0.127; session × risk preference: F(2,30) = 0.066, p = 0.936), and stable score values across session (Fig. 4C; session: F(2,30) = 0.697, p = 0.506). This mock DBS baseline matched the stable postoperative baseline established following electrode implantation, and is therefore a reliable representation of basal decision-making patterns in these rats (Fig. 4C; score, postop/mock: F(1,15) = 0.024, p= 0.880; postop/mock × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.001, p = 0.976; risk preference: F(1,15) = 29.753, p = 0.0001). Animals designated as risk-preferring did not differ from optimal decision makers in terms of weight gain during pair-housing, and all animals gained weight robustly during the course of the experiment (Fig. 5; weight record: F(37,555) = 153.43, p < 0.0001; weight record × risk preference: F(37,555) = 0.44, p = 0.99; risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.11, p = 0.75).
Comparing responding during the mock stimulation sessions to the first three DBS sessions, no effects of DBS on choice were apparent in either optimal-decision-makers or risk-preferring rats (Fig. 4; choice: stimulation on/off × risk preference × option: F(3,45) = 0.524, p = 0.668; score: stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.622, p = 0.44). However, during DBS sessions 4–6, the risk-preferring group selectively and specifically increased choice of the best option, P2 (Fig. 4B; stimulation on/off × risk preference × option: F(3,45) = 3.775, p = 0.017; P2: stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 7.476, p = 0.015). This change in choice is also reflected in a significant increase in risk-preferring rats’ score (Fig. 4C; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 9.426, p = 0.008; risk-preferring only: stimulation on/off: F(1,3) = 11.216, p = 0.044; optimal decision makers only: stimulation on/off: F(1,12) = 0.707, p = 0.417). This effect persisted through the subsequent DBS sessions (Fig. 4; choice: stimulation on/off × risk preference × option: F(3,45) = 2.901, p = 0.045; score: stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 5.837, p = 0.029), but was no longer evident once stimulation was discontinued: risk-preferring rats once again started to make significantly more risky choices in the three poststimulation sessions as compared to on STN-DBS (stimulation on/post × session × risk preference × choice: F(6,90) = 2.667, p = 0.02), and there was no significant difference in choice patterns when comparing mock stimulation to the first three poststimulation sessions (choice: stimulation off/post × risk preference × choice: F(3,45) = 0.612, p = 0.610; score: stimulation off/post × risk preference: F(1,15) = 1.597, p = 0.226).
Effects of STN-DBS on other variables
Again, all variables measured were stable during the three mock DBS sessions, and this behavioral pattern matched that observed in the postoperative baseline (Fig. 6, Table 1; postop/mock: all F ≤ 1.859, p ≥ 0.193; postop/mock × risk preference: all F ≤ 2.514, p ≥ 0.134). Optimal decision-makers and risk-preferring rats did not differ at baseline with respect to the level of premature responses, latency to collect reward, and omissions made (risk preference: all F ≤ 0.462, p ≥ 0.507). Risk-preferring rats consistently performed fewer trials, as reported previously (Barrus et al., 2015; Ferland and Winstanley, 2017), due to the longer periods of timeout punishment resulting from such a disadvantageous choice strategy (Fig. 6C; risk preference: F(1,15) = 8.331, p = 0.011). Contrary to previous datasets, risk-preferring rats also tended to be slower to make a choice at baseline in the current experiment (Table 1; risk preference: F = 6.047, p = 0.027).
Table 1.
Choice latency (s) | Reward collection latency (s) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Phase | Optimal | Risk preferring | Optimal | Risk preferring |
Post-op BL | 1.25 (0.18) | 1.58 (0.21) | 1.56 (0.10) | 1.25 (0.17) |
Mock DBS | 1.27 (0.16) | 1.73 (0.14) | 1.61 (0.10) | 1.33 (0.25) |
DBS S1-3 | 2.36 (0.22) | 2.33 (0.16) | 2.58 (0.35) | 2.05 (0.29) |
DBS S4-6 | 2.16 (0.20) | 2.81 (0.26) | 2.38 (0.22) | 2.46 (0.41) |
DBS S7-10 | 2.28 (0.30) | 2.92 (0.38) | 2.22 (0.25) | 2.55 (0.42) |
Post DBS S1-3 | 1.26 (0.20) | 1.79 (0.37) | 1.65 (0.10) | 1.45 (0.21) |
Bold type indicates a significant difference (p < 0.05) from the mock DBS sessions. Data are mean (SEM).
Comparing the first three DBS sessions to the basal stimulation off condition, there was no effect on premature responding (Fig. 6A; stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 0.652, p = 0.432; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 1.695, p = 0.213). A significant decrease in premature responses was nonetheless evident in the subsequent three DBS sessions, regardless of rats’ basal decision-making strategy (stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 13.560, p = 0.002; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.338, p = 0.569; risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.462, p = 0.507). This effect was transient, and did not persist across the remaining DBS sessions (stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 1.016, p = 0.329; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.022, p = 0.884; risk preference: F(1,15) = 1.028, p = 0.327), and poststimulation levels of premature responding were comparable to those observed prestimulation (stimulation off/post: F(1,15) = 1.617, p = 0.223; stimulation off/post × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.302, p = 0.591).
STN-DBS significantly increased omissions in all rats, and this was evident within the first three sessions of stimulation (Fig. 6B; stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 12.118, p = 0.003; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.490, p = 0.495) and throughout subsequent sessions (stimulation on/off: all F ≥ 29.519, p = 0.0001; stimulation on/off × risk preference: all F ≤ 2.366, p ≥ 0.145). This effect was also temporary, and resolved back to prestimulation levels within the first three sessions of poststimulation testing (stimulation off/post: F(1,15) = 1.0, p = 0.333; stimulation off/post × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.480, p = 0.499). STN-DBS did not affect trials completed in the first three sessions (Fig. 6C; stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 2.922, p = 0.108; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 2.621, p = 0.126; risk preference: F(1,15) = 7.103, p = 0.018). In the subsequent three DBS sessions, risk-preferring rats tended to increase the number of trials completed, thereby tracking the improvement in optimal choice observed in this subgroup, although they still performed fewer trials than optimal decision makers (stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 4.026, p = 0.063; risk preference: F(1,15) = 6.426, p = 0.023). Analyses of subsequent STN-DBS trials revealed no significant difference from mock stimulation, suggesting any effect on trials completed in risk-preferring rats is transient as well as slight (stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.880, p = 0.363; risk preference: F(1,15) = 7.770, p = 0.014).
STN-DBS also increased choice latency within the first three sessions, an effect that appeared to be more pronounced in optimal decision makers, but was clearly evident in both subgroups of animals (Table 1; stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 17.755, p = 0.001; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 3.117, p = 0.098; risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.101, p = 0.754). This effect persisted throughout the remainder of the stimulation sessions (stimulation on/off: all F ≥ 29.750, p < 0.0001; stimulation on/off × risk preference: all F ≤ 0.232, p ≥ 0.637). Once again, as soon as stimulation ceased, choice latencies returned to prestimulation levels, although there was no longer a significant difference between optimal decision-makers and risk-preferring rats (stimulation off/post: F(1,15) = 2.432, p = 0.242; stimulation off/post × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.284 p = 0.602; risk preference: F(1,15) = 1.701, p = 0.212).
Reward collection latency was not significantly different from mock stimulation during the first three sessions of STN-DBS (Table 1; stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 1.85, p = 0.194; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.288, p = 0.60). Similar to choice latency, the time taken to collect reward significantly increased throughout the next three DBS sessions (stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 11.84, p = 0.004; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.462, p = 0.507), and this effect was likewise evident throughout subsequent stimulation sessions (stimulation on/off: F(1,15) = 22.67, p = 0.0001; stimulation on/off × risk preference: F(1,15) = 2.83, p = 0.114). Again, once stimulation ceased, performance returned to prestimulation levels (stimulation off/post: F(1,15) = 2.23, p = 0.16; stimulation off/post × risk preference: F(1,15) = 0.001 p = 0.99; risk preference: F(1,15) = 2.136, p = 0.016).
Discussion
Here, we used a rodent behavioral paradigm with strong translational validity to assess the effects of STN-DBS on risky decision making. We show, for the first time, that STN-DBS selectively improves decision making in animals classified as risk-preferring at baseline, increasing choice of a less risky and more profitable option in these animals. In contrast, optimal decision makers did not alter their choice pattern in response to this manipulation. Although STN-DBS concomitantly increased the time both optimal decision-makers and risk-preferring rats took to make a choice and collect any reward earned, while also increasing the number of omissions made, it did not alter the total number of trials animals completed per session, or increase animals’ tendency to make a premature response. Although not entirely selective for decision making, the effects of STN-DBS on choice in risk-preferring rats are therefore not easily explained as an artifact of universal changes in motivation or locomotion. Indeed, these data suggest that STN-DBS may be a viable alternative to DRT in Parkinsonian patients at risk for impulse control disorders, and may also be an effective treatment for other neurologic or psychiatric conditions hallmarked by risky decision making, such as addiction disorders.
Our data stand in contrast to a previous report which suggested that STN-DBS increased motor impulsivity in rats performing the rGT, while failing to affect decision making (Aleksandrova et al., 2013). Critically, however, STN-DBS did not take place while animals performed the rGT in this former study; instead, stimulation was delivered for two hours before performance of the rGT in an unspecified location. Due to the experimental design employed, it is difficult to determine if the tethering itself contributed to the elevated impulsivity: the animals were not habituated to tethering before the initiation of DBS, and there was no significant difference in the level of impulsive action observed following two hours of tethering, regardless of whether stimulation was on or off. As such, the claim that STN-DBS increases motor impulsivity in this experiment should be interpreted with caution.
Similar to the data reported here, application of STN-DBS during performance of the 5CSRT likewise did not affect premature responding either in rats with 6-hydroxydopamine lesions to the dorsolateral striatum, a model of early PD that results in the loss of dopamine in this area, or in otherwise-intact rats (Baunez et al., 2007). In this latter study, STN-DBS also resulted in slower latencies to respond and increased omissions, again matching the current dataset. Interestingly, in the operant chambers used in this earlier study, animals were required to push a panel inwards to get access to the food tray, and STN-DBS significantly increased the number of such panel pushes. As such, STN-DBS may increase motivation for food reward (Baunez et al., 2005), similar to STN lesions (Rouaud et al., 2010). Although we did not monitor repeated entries to the food tray in the current study, we found that animals were slower to collect the food reward they earned, suggesting animals were not necessarily more motivated to consume the sucrose pellets used as rewards in the rGT. A general boost in motivation is thus unlikely to explain the decrease in risky choice observed in the current study.
This is the first manipulation, to our knowledge, that has selectively improved the maladaptive decision-making pattern of risk-preferring rats in the rGT. The mean score value for these rats increased by around 50 points due to a highly significant and selective increase in choice of the best option, P2. This change in the score variable caused by STN-DBS therefore does not simply reflect random choice among, or indifference to, the various options. These observations may inform our understanding of the neurobiology underlying baseline differences in risky choice. Due to the relatively low incidence of risk-preferring rats, the majority of studies evaluating the impact of neural or pharmacological manipulations have focused instead on modifying choice of optimal decision makers. For example, risky choice can be increased in these animals by lesions to the basolateral amygdala (BLA; Zeeb and Winstanley, 2011), and also by systemic administration of the dopamine D2/3 antagonist eticlopride (Zeeb et al., 2009; Zeeb et al., 2013), although this latter effect did not replicate in subsequent studies (Paine et al., 2013; Barrus and Winstanley, 2016). Disrupting connectivity between the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and BLA slows learning of the optimal strategy, but silencing the OFC does not affect choice once a stable pattern of decision making has been established (Zeeb and Winstanley, 2011, 2013). Whether aberrant activity in the BLA or OFC or dopamine system contributes to an innate preference for the risky options, or how this could be remedied by STN-DBS, remains unknown.
Indeed, the mechanism through which STN-DBS might precipitate cognitive-behavioral change has yet to be fully clarified. Originally, STN-DBS was thought to normalize aberrant activity in PD patients, locally inhibiting STN function through a depolarization block, thereby restoring motor function in PD through disinhibition of the thalamus (for review, see Gubellini et al., 2009). However, DBS can also excite proximal fibers of passage, resulting in enhanced neuronal firing in terminal regions, as well as increasing the firing of afferent projections (Vitek, 2002; McCracken and Grace, 2007; Gradinaru et al., 2009). The STN is highly interconnected with the ventral pallidum, and thereby receives considerable indirect input from numerous limbic structures, but the STN also receives direct input from the OFC and medial prefrontal cortex (Maurice et al., 1998; Haynes and Haber, 2013). It is theoretically possible, therefore, that STN-DBS may exert beneficial effects on risky choice in the rGT through altering activity in these cortical regions (Ballanger et al., 2009a), a conclusion that would fit with the known contribution of these areas to choice on-task (Paine et al., 2013; Zeeb and Winstanley, 2013; Zeeb et al., 2015).
The beneficial effects of STN-DBS were delayed, in that we did not observe significant improvement in decision making within the first three treatment sessions. Similar effects have been observed with regards to the effects of STN-DBS on attention or motivation, when significant effects could only be obtained after multiple daily sessions (Baunez et al., 2007; Rouaud et al., 2010). Although this time course may suggest that STN-DBS does not reliably induce changes in decision making, the need for repeated stimulation could also be required to trigger neuroplasticity which then results in cognitive change. In support of the latter, previous data suggest that chronic (5 d) of STN-DBS results in dramatic inhibition of glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the striatum, potentially through decreased expression of AMPA and NMDA receptor expression or sensitivity (Gubellini et al., 2006). Such changes may reflect neuroplastic processes, such as long-term depression, that could arise through circuit-level changes in the thalamo-cortico-striatal loop as a result of repeated STN-DBS (Albin et al., 1989). Chronic STN-DBS can also increase expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the striatum (Spieles-Engemann et al., 2011). Whether such neuroplastic events could contribute to new learning in risk-preferring rats, and therefore a shift toward more optimal decision making, remains to be empirically determined. Interestingly, DBS of the fornix increases BDNF levels in the hippocampus (Gondard et al., 2015), an effect which may be linked to the ability of this manipulation to improve memory function in an experimental model of dementia (Hescham et al., 2013).
STN-DBS has been found effective in the treatment of OCD (Mallet et al., 2002; Mallet et al., 2008), a patient group that is likewise impaired on the IGT (Cavedini et al., 2010; Starcke et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2015), and in which OFC dysfunction has long been documented (Modell et al., 1989; Saxena et al., 1999; Evans et al., 2004). Although therapeutic benefit may still be attributed to normalized frontal function (Le Jeune et al., 2010), recent work has documented an electrophysiological signature associated with OCD in the ventromedial STN that is associated with OCD symptom severity, as well as clinical benefit post-DBS (Piallat et al., 2011; Welter et al., 2011; Mulders et al., 2016). Disrupted information processing at the level of the STN, and its normalization, may therefore play a more fundamental role in both the manifestation of OCD and its resolution following STN-DBS (Mulders et al., 2016). Whether a similar aberration in activity likewise characterizes, and even drives, maladaptive risky choice or compulsive gambling behavior remains to be determined. Based on the current data, and observations that STN-DBS may be beneficial in reducing compulsive drug use (Pelloux and Baunez, 2013), such a possibility may be worthy of investigation.
With regards to addiction risk, some data suggest that patients who have a family history of alcohol use, and are potentially more vulnerable to addictions themselves, are more likely to develop impulse control problems following DRT (Voon et al., 2007). Interestingly, risk-preferring rats, i.e. those who benefited from STN-DBS in the current study, are uniquely and negatively affected by concurrent cocaine self-administration: their decision making becomes even more risky, whereas optimal decision makers are unaffected, despite self-administering comparable amounts of cocaine (Ferland and Winstanley, 2017). Risk-preferring animals also show enhanced cue-induced drug-seeking with extended time in withdrawal, a putative behavioral marker of enhanced relapse vulnerability (Grimm et al., 2001). As such, risk-preferring rats may constitute a useful model of addiction vulnerability. In humans, the time course across which risky decision making is evident in SUD tracks subjective craving for drug, such that both resolve at a similar time point following successful treatment (Wang et al., 2013). Conversely, unresolved and persistent risky decision making on the IGT in SUD patients is a strong predictor of treatment failure (Stevens et al., 2013). As such, a manipulation that ameliorates risky choice on the rGT/IGT, such as STN-DBS, could have therapeutic efficacy for treating SUD. Indeed, a growing body of preclinical and clinical data suggest that STN-DBS may be useful in reducing drug use and drug craving (Baunez et al., 2005; Rouaud et al., 2010; Pelloux and Baunez, 2013). Should our observation that STN-DBS improves risky decision making translate to human subjects, this would suggest an additional cognitive mechanism by which this manipulation improves treatment outcomes.
To conclude, DBS is increasingly being considered for psychiatric conditions, rather than purely neurologic diseases, due to the urgent need for effective treatments, and lack of innovation in effective pharmacotherapeutics for mental illness. Given the invasive nature of the procedure, a cautious approach is understandable, and the need for further research into the potential cognitive effects of DBS, and its mechanism of actions, should not be minimized. However, by making use of the considerable degree of experimental control possible when using an animal model, this study strongly suggests that STN-DBS does not impair decision making under uncertainty, despite fears to the contrary, but may actually ameliorate the maladaptive persistence in the selection of risky options seen in a subgroup of individuals. These data add to the growing body of research arguing that STN-DBS may be effective more widely in disorders of compulsion and maladaptive drive.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Karen Szumlinski, University of California at Santa Barbara
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Erin Calipari.
In consultation with two expert reviewers, I have reached the decision of “Major Revision” regarding your article submitted to eNeuro. Both reviewers and I felt that the article presents new and interesting data that would be of high relevance to the readers of our journal, a few concerns were raised by Reviewer #1 that influence the interpretation of the results. As such, both reviewers and I would like to see these concerns addressed in a revision. These concerns are the following:
1) On page 6, line 106, it is mentioned that rats were pair-housed with food restriction. It is highly questionable that a pair of rats in each cage were equally food restricted, as rodents are socially hierarchical animals, with a dominant would get more foods than a subordinate. Such disproportional food restrictions between a pair of rats in each cage may have affected performance of the gambling task. In order to resolve this issue, we suggest that the authors provide data pertaining to the weight of the rats to confirm the efficacy of the food restriction.
2) A protocol for the deep brain stimulation is insufficiently described on page 9. Was stimulation given 10 times to the same rats repeatedly, with a set of stimulation on each trial for 10 trials? How long before starting each session was stimulation given? A set of stimulation consisted of 130 Hz bursts, but how long was the burst? Providing greater detail pertaining to the specific parameters of the stimulation should resolve this issue.
3) Although the authors have argued that deep brain stimulation decreased risky choices in risk preferring rats, what Figure 4B&C actually shows is that only less than a half of sessions that deep brain stimulation decreased risky choices, and increased optimal choices in these rats. In Figure 4C, it can be seen that choice scores for the first 4 sessions with deep brain stimulation (ON1-4) in risk-preferring rats are apparently not much different from pre, OFF, and post sessions. The fact that the rats require at least 4 sessions of stimulation is very interesting to all of us but requires some deeper discussion. Is the DBS not effective all of the time or under certain circumstances or does it simply take repeated stimulation in order to observe an effect (implying that some latent neuroadaptation is being produced by the stimulation)? Either way, it would be good to speculate on what each alternate possibility might mean for the clinical treatment setting.
As these concerns are likely to require less than 2 months to address, I welcome the receipt of your revised manuscript to eNeuro.
References
- Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB (1989) The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci 12:366–375. 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aleksandrova LR, Creed MC, Fletcher PJ, Lobo DS, Hamani C, Nobrega JN (2013) Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus increases premature responding in a rat gambling task. Behav Brain Res 245:76–82. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Poldrack RA (2005) The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 57:1285–1292. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballanger B, Jahanshahi M, Broussolle E, Thobois S (2009a) PET functional imaging of deep brain stimulation in movement disorders and psychiatry. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 29:1743–1754. 10.1038/jcbfm.2009.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballanger B, van Eimeren T, Moro E, Lozano AM, Hamani C, Boulinguez P, Pellecchia G, Houle S, Poon YY, Lang AE, Strafella AP (2009b) Stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus and impulsivity: release your horses. Ann Neurol 66:817–824. 10.1002/ana.21795 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrus MM, Winstanley CA (2016) Dopamine D3 receptors modulate the ability of win-paired cues to increase risky choice in a rat gambling task. J Neurosci 36:785–794. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2225-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrus MM, Hosking JG, Zeeb FD, Tremblay M, Winstanley CA (2015) Disadvantageous decision-making is associated with increased motor impulsivity at the population level on a rodent gambling task. J Psychiatry Neurosci 40:108–117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baunez C, Robbins TW (1997) Bilateral lesions of the subthalamic nucleus induce multiple deficits in an attentional task in rats. Eur J Neurosci 9:2086–2099. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01376.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baunez C, Nieoullon A, Amalric M (1995) In a rat model of parkinsonism, lesions of the subthalamic nucleus reverse increases of reaction time, but induce a dramatic premature responding deficit. J Neurosci 15:6531–6541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baunez C, Amalric M, Robbins TW (2002) Enhanced food-related motivation after bilateral lesions of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci 22:562–568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baunez C, Dias C, Cador M, Amalric M (2005) The subthalamic nucleus exerts opposite control on cocaine and “natural” rewards. Nat Neurosci 8:484–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baunez C, Christakou A, Chudasama Y, Forni C, Robbins TW (2007) Bilateral high-frequency stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus on attentional performance: transient deleterious effects and enhanced motivation in both intact and parkinsonian rats. Eur J Neurosci 25:1187–1194. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05373.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50:7–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benabid AL, Benazzouz A, Hoffmann D, Limousin P, Krack P, Pollak P (1998) Long-term electrical inhibition of deep brain targets in movement disorders. Mov Disord 13 [Suppl 3]:119–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carli M, Robbins TW, Evenden JL, Everitt BJ (1983) Effects of lesions to ascending noradrenergic neurons on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction time task in rats - implications for theories of dorsal noradrenergic bundle function based on selective attention and arousal. Behav Brain Res 9:361–380. 10.1016/0166-4328(83)90138-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavedini P, Zorzi C, Piccinni M, Cavallini MC, Bellodi L (2010) Executive dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive patients and unaffected relatives: searching for a new intermediate phenotype. Biol Psychiatry 67:1178–1184. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charles PD, Dolhun RM, Gill CE, Davis TL, Bliton MJ, Tramontana MG, Salomon RM, Wang L, Hedera P, Phibbs FT, Neimat JS, Konrad PE (2012) Deep brain stimulation in early Parkinson's disease: enrollment experience from a pilot trial. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 18:268–273. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demetriades P, Rickards H, Cavanna AE (2011) Impulse control disorders following deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson's disease: clinical aspects. Parkinsons Dis 2011:658415. 10.4061/2011/658415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans A, Lees A (2004) Dopamine dysregulation syndrome in Parkinson's disease. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17:393–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans DW, Lewis MD, Iobst E (2004) The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in normally developing compulsive-like behaviors and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain Cogn 55:220–234. 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00274-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferland J-MN, Winstanley CA (2017) Risk-preferring rats make worse decisions and show increased incubation of craving after cocaine self-administration. Addict Biol. 22:991–1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Follett KA, Weaver FM, Stern M, Hur K, Harris CL, Luo P, Marks WJ Jr, Rothlind J, Sagher O, Moy C, Pahwa R, Burchiel K, Hogarth P, Lai EC, Duda JE, Holloway K, Samii A, Horn S, Bronstein JM, Stoner G, et al. (2010) Pallidal versus subthalamic deep-brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease. N Engl J Med 362:2077–2091. 10.1056/NEJMoa0907083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank MJ (2006) Hold your horses: a dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making. Neural Netw 19:1120–1136. 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gondard E, Chau HN, Mann A, Tierney TS, Hamani C, Kalia SK, Lozano AM (2015) Rapid modulation of protein expression in the rat hippocampus following deep brain stimulation of the fornix brain. Stimulation 8:1058–1064. 10.1016/j.brs.2015.07.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gradinaru V, Mogri M, Thompson KR, Henderson JM, Deisseroth K (2009) Optical deconstruction of parkinsonian neural circuitry. Science 324:354–359. 10.1126/science.1167093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y (2001) Neuroadaptation. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature 412:141–142. 10.1038/35084134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gubellini P, Eusebio A, Oueslati A, Melon C, Kerkerian-Le Goff L, Salin P (2006) Chronic high-frequency stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus and L-DOPA treatment in experimental parkinsonism: effects on motor behaviour and striatal glutamate transmission. Eur J Neurosci 24:1802–1814. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05047.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gubellini P, Salin P, Kerkerian-Le Goff L, Baunez C (2009) Deep brain stimulation in neurological diseases and experimental models: from molecule to complex behavior. Prog Neurobiol 89:79–123. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hälbig TD, Tse W, Frisina PG, Baker BR, Hollander E, Shapiro H, Tagliati M, Koller WC, Olanow CW (2009) Subthalamic deep brain stimulation and impulse control in Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurol 16:493–497. 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02509.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haynes WI, Haber SN (2013) The organization of prefrontal-subthalamic inputs in primates provides an anatomical substrate for both functional specificity and integration: implications for Basal Ganglia models and deep brain stimulation. J Neurosci 33:4804–4814. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4674-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hescham S, Lim LW, Jahanshahi A, Steinbusch HW, Prickaerts J, Blokland A, Temel Y (2013) Deep brain stimulation of the forniceal area enhances memory functions in experimental dementia: the role of stimulation parameters. Brain Stimul 6:72–77. 10.1016/j.brs.2012.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence A, Evans A, Lees A (2003) Compulsive use of dopamine replacement therapy in Parkinson's disease: reward systems gone awry? Lancet 2:595–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Jeune F, Vérine M, N'Diayee K, Drapiere D, Leraye E, Du Montcele ST, Baupe N, Pelissoloe A, Polosane M, Mallete L, Yelnike J, Devauxe B, Fontainee D, Chereaue I, Bourguignone A, Perone J, Sauleaue P, Raoule S, Garine E, Krebse MO, et al. (2010) Decrease of prefrontal metabolism after subthalamic stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a positron emission tomography study. Biol Psychiatry 68:1016–1022. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Limousin P, Pollak P, Benazzouz A, Hoffman D, Lebas JF, Broussole E, Perret JE, Benabid AL (1995) Effects on Parkinsonian signs and symptoms of bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation. Lancet 345:91–95. 10.1016/S0140-6736(95)90062-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y, Li W, Tan C, Liu X, Wang X, Gui Y, Qin L, Deng F, Hu C, Chen L (2014) Meta-analysis comparing deep brain stimulation of the globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus to treat advanced Parkinson disease. J Neurosurg 121:709–718. 10.3171/2014.4.JNS131711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallet L, Mesnage V, Houeto JL, Pelissolo A, Yelnik J, Behar C, Gargiulo M, Welter ML, Bonnet AM, Pillon B, Cornu P, Dormont D, Pidoux B, Allilaire JF, Agid Y (2002) Compulsions, Parkinson's disease, and stimulation. Lancet 360:1302–1304. 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11339-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallet L, Polosane M, Jaafarie N, Baupe N, Weltere ML, Fontainee D, du Montcele ST, Yelnike J, Chéreaue I, Arbuse C, Raoule S, Aouizeratee B, Damiere P, Chabardèse S, Czerneckie V, Ardouine C, Krebse MO, Bardinete E, Chaynese P, Burbaude P, et al. (2008) Subthalamic nucleus stimulation in severe obsessive-compulsive disorder. N Engl J Med 359:2121–2134. 10.1056/NEJMoa0708514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Ramirez D, Okun MS (2014) Rationale and clinical pearls for primary care doctors referring patients for deep brain stimulation. Gerontology 60:38–48. 10.1159/000354880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurice N, Deniau J-M, Glowinski J, Thierry AM (1998) Relationships between the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia in the rat: physiology of the corticosubthalamic circuits. J Neurosci 18:9539–9546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCracken CB, Grace AA (2007) High-frequency deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens region suppresses neuronal activity and selectively modulates afferent drive in rat orbitofrontal cortex in vivo. J Neurosci 27:12601–12610. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3750-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Modell JG, Mountz JM, Curtis GC, Greden JF (1989) Neurophysiologic dysfunction in basal ganglia/limbic striatal and thalamocortical circuits as a pathogenetic mechanism of obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1:27–36. 10.1176/jnp.1.1.27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulders AEP, Plantinga BR, Schruers K, Duits A, Janssen MLF, Ackermans L, Leentjens AFG, Jahanshahi A, Temel Y (2016) Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in obsessive-compulsive disorder: neuroanatomical and pathophysiological considerations. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 26:1909–1919. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Odekerken VJ, van Laar T, Staal MJ, Mosch A, Hoffmann CF, Nijssen PC, Beute GN, van Vugt JP, Lenders MW, Contarino MF, Mink MS, Bour LJ, van den Munckhof P, Schmand BA, de Haan RJ, Schuurman PR, de Bie RM (2013) Subthalamic nucleus versus globus pallidus bilateral deep brain stimulation for advanced Parkinson's disease (NSTAPS study): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet Neurol 12:37–44. 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70264-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paine TA, Asinof SK, Diehl GW, Frackman A, Leffler J (2013) Medial prefrontal cortex lesions impair decision-making on a rodent gambling task: reversal by D1 receptor antagonist administration. Behav Brain Res 243:247–254. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.01.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C (1998) The rat brain in stereotaxic co-ordinates, Ed 4 Sydney: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pelloux Y, Baunez C (2013) Deep brain stimulation for addiction: why the subthalamic nucleus should be favored. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23:713–720. 10.1016/j.conb.2013.02.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piallat B, Polosan M, Fraix V, Goetz L, David O, Fenoy A, Torres N, Quesada JL, Seigneuret E, Pollak P, Krack P, Bougerol T, Benabid AL, Chabardès S (2011) Subthalamic neuronal firing in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Parkinson disease. Ann Neurol 69:793–802. 10.1002/ana.22222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins TW (2002) The 5-choice serial reaction time task: behavioural pharmacology and functional neurochemistry. Psychopharmacology 163:362–380. 10.1007/s00213-002-1154-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez-Oroz MC, López-Azcárate J, Garcia-Garcia D, Alegre M, Toledo J, Valencia M, Guridi J, Artieda J, Obeso JA (2011) Involvement of the subthalamic nucleus in impulse control disorders associated with Parkinson's disease. Brain 134:36–49. 10.1093/brain/awq301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers RD, Wielenberg B, Wojtecki L, Elben S, Campbell-Meiklejohn D, Schnitzler A (2011) Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus transiently enhances loss-chasing behaviour in patients with Parkinson's disease. Exp Neurol 231:181–189. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rouaud T, Lardeux S, Panayotis N, Paleressompoulle D, Cador M, Baunez C (2010) Reducing the desire for cocaine with subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:1196–1200. 10.1073/pnas.0908189107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxena S, Brody AL, Maidment KM, Dunkin JJ, Colgan M, Alborzian S, Phelps ME, Baxter LR Jr.(1999) Localized orbitofrontal and subcortical metabolic changes and predictors of response to paroxetine treatment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 21:683–693. 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00082-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuepbach WM, Raue J, Knudsene K, Volkmanne J, Kracke P, Timmermanne L, Hälbige TD, Hesekampe H, Navarroe SM, Meiere N, Falke D, Mehdorne M, Paschene S, Maaroufe M, Barbee MT, Finke GR, Kupsche A, Grubere D, Schneidere G-H, Seigneurete E, et al. (2013) Neurostimulation for Parkinson's disease with early motor complications. N Engl J Med 368:610–622. 10.1056/NEJMoa1205158 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smeding HM, Goudriaan AE, Foncke EM, Schuurman PR, Speelman JD, Schmand B (2007) Pathological gambling after bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 78:517–519. 10.1136/jnnp.2006.102061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spieles-Engemann AL, Steece-Collier K, Behbehani MM, Collier TJ, Wohlgenant SL, Kemp CJ, Cole-Strauss A, Levine ND, Gombash SE, Thompson VB, Lipton JW, Sortwell CE (2011) Subthalamic nucleus stimulation increases brain derived neurotrophic factor in the nigrostriatal system and primary motor cortex. J Parkinsons Dis 1:123–136. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starcke K, Tuschen-Caffier B, Markowitsch HJ, Brand M (2010) Dissociation of decisions in ambiguous and risky situations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 175:114–120. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens L, Betanzos-Espinosa P, Crunelle CL, Vergara-Moragues E, Roeyers H, Lozano O, Dom G, Gonzalez-Saiz F, Vanderplasschen W, Verdejo-Garcia A, Pérez-García M (2013) Disadvantageous decision-making as a predictor of drop-out among cocaine-dependent individuals in long-term residential treatment. Front Psychiatry 4:149. 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uslaner JM, Robinson TE (2006) Subthalamic nucleus lesions increase impulsive action and decrease impulsive choice - mediation by enhanced incentive motivation? Eur J Neurosci 24:2345–2354. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05117.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vitek JL (2002) Mechanisms of deep brain stimulation: excitation or inhibition. Mov Disord 17 [Suppl 3]:S69–S72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V, Thomsen T, Miyasaki JM, de Souza M, Shafro A, Fox SH, Duff-Canning S, Lang AE, Zurowski M (2007) Factors associated with dopaminergic drug-related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 64:212–216. 10.1001/archneur.64.2.212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V, Sohr M, Lang AE, Potenza MN, Siderowf AD, Whetteckey J, Weintraub D, Wunderlich GR, Stacy M (2011) Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a multicenter case–control study. Ann Neurol 69:986–996. 10.1002/ana.22356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang G, Shi J, Chen N, Xu L, Li J, Li P, Sun Y, Lu L (2013) Effects of length of abstinence on decision-making and craving in methamphetamine abusers. PLoS One 8:e68791. 10.1371/journal.pone.0068791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang JW, Zhang YQ, Zhang XH, Wang YP, Li JP, Li YJ (2016) Cognitive and psychiatric effects of STN versus GPi deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS One 11:e0156721. 10.1371/journal.pone.0156721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub D, Siderowf AD, Potenza MN, Goveas J, Morales KH, Duda JE, Moberg PJ, Stern MB (2006) Association of dopamine agonist use with impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 63:969–973. 10.1001/archneur.63.7.969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welter ML, Burbaude P, Fernandez-Vidale S, Bardinete E, Costee J, Piallate B, Borge M, Besnarde S, Sauleaue P, Devauxe B, Pidouxe B, Chaynese P, Tézenas du Montcele S, Bastiane A, Langboure N, Teillante A, Haynese W, Yelnike J, Karachie C, Mallete L, et al. (2011) Basal ganglia dysfunction in OCD: subthalamic neuronal activity correlates with symptoms severity and predicts high-frequency stimulation efficacy. Transl Psychiatry 1:e5. 10.1038/tp.2011.5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Clark L (2016) Translational models of gambling-related decision-making. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 28:93-120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstanley CA, Baunez C, Theobald DE, Robbins TW (2005) Lesions to the subthalamic nucleus decrease impulsive choice but impair autoshaping in rats: the importance of the basal ganglia in Pavlovian conditioning and impulse control. Eur J Neurosci 21:3107–3116. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04143.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witt K, Pulkowski U, Herzog J, Lorenz D, Hamel W, Deuschl G, Krack P (2004) Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus improves cognitive flexibility but impairs response inhibition in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 61:697–700. 10.1001/archneur.61.5.697 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, Winstanley CA (2011) Lesions of the basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex differentially affect acquisition and performance of a rodent gambling task. J Neurosci 31:2197–2204. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5597-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, Winstanley CA (2013) Functional disconnection of the orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala impairs acquisition of a rat gambling task and disrupts animals' ability to alter decision-making behavior after reinforcer devaluation. J Neurosci 33:6434–6443. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3971-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, Robbins TW, Winstanley CA (2009) Serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of gambling behavior as assessed using a novel rat gambling task. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:2329–2343. 10.1038/npp.2009.62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, Wong AC, Winstanley CA (2013) Differential effects of environmental enrichment, social-housing, and isolation-rearing on a rat gambling task: dissociations between impulsive action and risky decision-making. Psychopharmacology 225:381–395. 10.1007/s00213-012-2822-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, Baarendse PJ, Vanderschuren LJM, Winstanley CA (2015) Inactivation of the prelimbic or infralimbic cortex impairs decision-making in the rat gambling task. Psychopharmacology 232:4481–4491. 10.1007/s00213-015-4075-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang L, Dong Y, Ji Y, Zhu C, Yu F, Ma H, Chen X, Wang K (2015) Dissociation of decision making under ambiguity and decision making under risk: a neurocognitive endophenotype candidate for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 57:60–68. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]