Skip to main content
European Journal of Ageing logoLink to European Journal of Ageing
. 2005 May 14;2(2):149–158. doi: 10.1007/s10433-005-0032-7

Self-rated health among older adults: a cross-national comparison

Carola Bardage 1,, Saskia M F Pluijm 2, Nancy L Pedersen 1, Dorly J H Deeg 2, Marja Jylhä 3, Marianna Noale 4, Tzvia Blumstein 5, Ángel Otero 6
PMCID: PMC5547684  PMID: 28794727

Abstract

Self-rated health (SRH) may have different implications in various social and cultural settings. However, few studies are available concerning SRH among older persons across countries. The aim of this study was to analyse whether there are cross-national differences in the association between status characteristics, several diseases common among older persons, activities of daily living (ADL), and SRH. The study base was the Comparison of Longitudinal European Studies on Aging (CLESA), which includes data from six population-based studies on aging conducted in Finland, Israel, Italy, The Netherlands, Spain and Sweden. The study population comprised 5,629 persons, with participants from all countries except Italy. Logistic regression analyses were used to assess the relationship between status characteristics, health conditions, ADL and SRH. To examine whether the association among status characteristics, health conditions, ADL and outcome differed across the CLESA countries, interaction terms defined as “variable*country” were considered separately for each variable. Regression analyses revealed that sex, education, lifetime occupation, heart disease and respiratory disease were differently distributed across countries. Among homogeneous factors, marital status (OR=1.21), hypertension (OR=1.41), stroke (OR=1.67), diabetes (OR=2.15), cancer (OR=1.47), musculoskeletal diseases (OR=2.44), and ADL (OR=2.72) turned out to be significantly associated with fair or poor SRH. The results indicate that there are differences in self-ratings of health across countries. These differences cannot be explained entirely by status characteristics, self-reported diseases or functional ability. However, an important finding was that in all countries most of the indicators of medical and functional health were homogeneously associated with SRH.

Keywords: CLESA project, Self-rated health, Cross-national comparison, Ageing

Introduction

Self-rated health (SRH) is well established as a strong and independent predictor of mortality (Idler and Benyamini 1997). SRH measurements serve as useful tools for identifying individuals and groups at risk for poor health, and for monitoring health changes in populations. At present, SRH is one of the most widely used indicators of health in survey research and is one of the health indicators recommended both by the WHO and the European Union Commission for health monitoring (de Bruin et al. 1996). However, information on the comparability of SRH across countries, particularly among older persons, is scarce.

SRH presumably presents a summary statement of how the individual perceives various health conditions. SRH represents the individual’s perception of multiple facets of health, including medical diagnoses, health conditions, disease symptoms, impairments, functional losses and disabilities, and psychosocial problems/symptoms (Verbrugge and Jette 1994). Self-ratings of health are influenced as well by emotional status, psychological distress (Rakoski et al. 1993), personality factors, and biographic characteristics (Stoller 1984). Age, race and culture, socio-economic status and gender appear to have been viewed as fundamental social categories, which form the context within which self-assessments of health are constructed and appear in almost every study investigating subjective health (Idler 1992). Yet, as a comprehensive measure of health, SRH was shown to be a significant predictor of health outcomes including measures of mortality and morbidity (Benyamini et al. 2000) in a number of countries with homogeneous populations as well as in countries with multi-ethnic population groups (Idler and Benyamini 1997). The question of whether the relationship between various definitions of objective health (specific chronic conditions and ADL) and personal resources (education, marital status, gender and occupation), and SRH is similar across countries in Europe is important for our understanding of the conceptual structure of SRH and its prediction of future events such as morbidity, use of health services, and death.

Previous studies have shown that there is diversity in SRH, health status, social life, and living conditions between different European countries (Kind et al. 1998; Bobak et al. 2000). The impact of status characteristics on indicators of health, among them SRH, and health care use has been reported throughout the literature (Marmot et al. 1991; Grundy and Sloggett 2003). Health surveys repeatedly show that men die earlier than women, but women tend to have higher rates of morbidity (Verbrugge 1989; MacIntyre et al. 1996). Gender differences in health status have been reported to be inconsistent across age and health measures (Matthews et al. 1999; McDonough and Walters 2001). After adjusting for health indicators, marital status has also been shown to affect SRH, i.e. single persons report poorer health than do married persons (Fylkesnes and Førde 1991; Davies 1995). Several studies have shown that socio-economic status is associated with inequalities in health, i.e. SRH, morbidity and health-related behaviours (Townsend and Davidson 1992; Ferrie et al. 2002). Most of the previous studies have found a positive relationship between high education, status employment and SRH (Idler 1993; Johnson and Wolinsky 1993), although the converse has also been reported (Fylkenes and Førde 1992). These findings emphasize the importance of considering the impact of status characteristics when comparing differences in SRH across countries.

In several studies, cultural differences have been found in the mean level of SRH (Cockerham et al. 1983). After adjusting for illness indicators, Hispanics were more likely to report fair or poor SRH than were non-Hispanics (Shetterly et al. 1996). After adjusting for age, coronary heart disease, parental lifespan, and socio-economic and marital status, men in Rotterdam, The Netherlands perceived their SRH better than did men in Kaunas, Lithuania (Appels et al. 1996). In a study using samples from the European Longitudinal Study on Aging, women and men in Florence, Italy were three and four times, respectively, more likely to report good SRH than were men in Tampere, Finland. Several health-related variables were adjusted for in the analyses (Jylhä et al. 1998). In a study using data from the 1992 World Value Survey, the average level of self-perceived health was worse in former communist countries than in Western Europe, for both men and women, when taken diversities in economic and social conditions into account (Carlson 1998). SRH may have different implications in various social and cultural settings (Nilsson and Orth-Gomér 2000; Zimmer et al. 2000). However, few studies are available concerning SRH among older persons (aged 65 years and older) across countries (Su and Ferraro 1997; Jylhä et al. 1998). This study includes older populations in four different European countries and Israel. The European countries represent the cultural and geographic distribution in Europe from the north (Finland and Sweden) via the west (The Netherlands) to the Mediterranean region (Spain). Israel in itself represents a variety of European and Asian cultures. This study offers a rare opportunity to examine whether the relationship between several factors and poor SRH holds across different countries with either homogeneous cultural populations or countries with multicultural populations.

The aim of this study was to analyse whether there are cross-national differences in the association between status characteristics, several diseases common among older persons, activities of daily living (ADL), and SRH.

Methods

Study population and design

The study used data from the Comparison of Longitudinal European Studies on Aging (CLESA), which includes data from six population-based studies on aging conducted in Finland, Israel, Italy, The Netherlands, Spain and Sweden. The total study population is comprised of 11,557 respondents, aged 65–89 years. A detailed description of the methodology of the CLESA project, as well as each study included, is presented elsewhere (Pedersen et al. 1991; Jylhä et al. 1992; Maggi et al. 1994; Béland and Zunzunegui 1995a, 1995b; Beekman et al. 1997; Walter-Ginzburg et al. 2001; Minicuci et al. 2003). Because there was no measure of SRH in the Italian survey, data from this country were not included in the present analyses. Among the other countries, the response rate on the SRH question was 95%, giving a study population of 5,629 persons. The mean age was 76 years. Women comprised 52% of the sample.

Dependent variable

Most countries had in common one global question in which the respondent was asked to make a general statement of his/her health (Table 1). Because the number of categories on this question varied from three to five among the countries, SRH was reclassified into three levels, “excellent, very good or good”, “fair”, and “poor or very poor” for the CLESA project. For the purpose of analyses, these were further classified into two levels, “good” and “fair and poor”, by collapsing the first two and the last two categories respectively (Table 1). Manor et al. (2000) justified the categorization of SRH from a categorical variable into a dichotomous variable.

Table 1.

Classification scheme of self-rated health categories by country

Country Question regarding self-rated health Original coding CLESA coding Categories used in the analyses
Finland How would you evaluate your present health: is it ...? 1=Very good 1, 2=Excellent, very good and good 1=Good
2=Fairly good 3=Fair 2=Fair and poor
3=Average 4, 5=Poor and very poor
4=Fairly bad
5=Bad
Sweden How would you rate your general health status? 1=Good 1=Excellent, very good and good 1=Good
2=Reasonable 2=Fair 2=Fair and poor
3=Bad 3=Poor and very poor
The Netherlands How in general is your health? 1=Excellent 1, 2=Excellent, very good and good 1=Good
2=Good 3=Fair 2=Fair and poor
3=Fair 4, 5=Poor and very poor
4=Sometimes good/bad
5=Poor
Spain How would you describe your state of health at present? 1=Very good 1, 2=Excellent, very good and good 1=Good
2=Good 3=Fair 2=Fair and poor
3=Fair 4, 5=Poor and very poor
4=Poor
5=Very poor
Israel How would you rate your health now? 1=Excellent 1, 2=Excellent, very good and good 1=Good
2=Good 3=Fair 2=Fair and poor
3=Fair poor 4=Poor and very poor
4=Poor

Independent variables

The status characteristics included were age, sex, marital status, education and lifetime occupation. Marital status was classified as “married” (i.e. married/cohabiting) and “unmarried” (i.e. never married/divorced/widowed). Education was coded into three levels: less than elementary school, elementary school, and more than elementary school (i.e. secondary school and college/university). Lifetime occupation was classified into three categories: non-manual workers, manual workers (i.e. non-skilled manual workers, skilled manual workers, and farmers), and housewives.

Diseases common among older persons and included in the CLESA studies were heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, stroke, cancer, respiratory diseases and musculoskeletal diseases. Respondents with these diseases were classified based upon self-reports.

In all six countries, ADL was assessed by means of a structured face-to-face interview. Because the number, type and response format of the ADL items differed in each study, a procedure was devised to harmonise the data. A four-item scale was constructed out of the ADL items which four of the six countries had in common (bathing, dressing, transferring and toileting). Cronbach’s alpha (CA) of the four-item ADL measure varied from 0.81 in Spain to 0.92 in Finland, and was similar to the CA of scales including five or six items, indicating very good reliability in each country. Finally, the four-item scale was dichotomised into “non-disabled”, i.e. no help was needed, and “disabled” if need of help was indicated in any of the items. A detailed description of the harmonisation process of the ADL items can be found elsewhere (Pluijm et al. 2005).

Statistical analyses

Descriptive analyses by country were obtained by using the SAS system for Windows, release 8.02 (SAS Institute 1999–2001). Differences between proportions were tested by the chi-square test. PROC LOGISTICS in the SAS statistics program was elected for the multivariate approach.

Logistic regression analyses were conducted as follows. In the first set of analyses, the association between status characteristics, health conditions, ADL and SRH were analysed separately for each country (with good SRH as the reference). These analyses were adjusted for age and sex. All variables in the logistic regression analyses, except age, were dichotomised. Age was included as a continuous variable in the analyses. In the second set of analyses, the data from all five countries were pooled together and all variables were included in the model. Country was treated as a set of dummy variables with The Netherlands as the reference country. The Netherlands was chosen based on the following criteria: first, it is the country with the lowest prevalence of fair or poor SRH; second, it has the greatest sample size. To take into account the different design of the studies and to generalize the study sample to the reference population, information on the setting-specific population distribution, stratified by sex and age classes, and on the sampling techniques of the six original studies was collected and a set of country-specific weights were calculated and applied to the raw data (Noale et al. 2005).

To examine whether the association between the independent variables and the outcome measure differed across countries, interaction terms defined as “variable*country” were included in the multivariate models. If an interaction term was significant (p<0.05), the country-specific odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the specific variable were calculated following the method of Figueiras et al. (1998). If an interaction term was not significant, the pooled OR and 95% CI were calculated (Kleinbaum 1994). All explanatory variables were entered as dummy variables.

Results

Descriptive analyses

Characteristics of the study population, in relation to SRH status, are shown in Table 2. The oldest and the youngest age groups are missing in The Netherlands and Israel, due to the original study design, and are therefore not included in the analyses. The older persons in Israel rated fair or poor health to the greatest extent, followed by the older persons in the similar age groups in Spain, Finland, Sweden and The Netherlands. Those in the oldest age groups (75 years and older) rated fair or poor health to a greater extent than those of younger ages in most of the countries. In all countries, except in Finland, a greater proportion of women than of men had fair or poor SRH. Married persons had significantly better health than did unmarried persons in The Netherlands, Spain and Israel. There was more poor health among persons with lower education. In the Swedish sample, there were no respondents with less than elementary school education. In all countries, except in Finland, there was more fair or poor health among housewives. Manual workers rated significantly fair and poor health to a greater extent than did non-manual workers in Finland, Sweden and Israel. A greater proportion of respondents with heart disease, diabetes, respiratory diseases and musculoskeletal diseases reported fair or poor health. In all countries, except in The Netherlands, those with hypertension reported fair or poor health to a greater extent than did non-hypertensives. Stroke was related to fair or poor health in all countries but the relationships were statistically significant only in Israel and The Netherlands. In all countries, those with cancer rated fair or poor health to a greater extent than those without the disease. The result was statistically significant only in The Netherlands. There was poorer health among disabled persons than among those with no limitation.

Table 2.

Percent of respondents reporting fair/poor self-rated health by age, sex, marital status, education level, lifetime occupation, heart disease, hypertension, stroke, diabetes, cancer, respiratory diseases, musculoskeletal diseases, and ADL (n number of respondents)

Finland Sweden The Netherlands Spain Israel
n Percentage with fair/poor self-rated health n Percentage with fair/poor self-rated health n Percentage with fair/poor self-rated health n Percentage with fair/poor self-rated health n Percentage with fair/poor self-rated health
Total 566 63.4 734 49.7 2,085 40.0 1,105 68.4 1,139 76.4
Age 566 734 2,085 1,105 1,139
65–74 333 59.5* 479 47.0 962 36.3** 545 66.6 - -
75–84a 182 70.9 230 54.3 1,123 43.2 433 71.8 801 75.4
85–89 51 62.7 25 60.0 - - 127 64.6 338 78.7
Sex 566 734 2,085 1,105 1,139
Women 312 62.5 465 52.3* 1,074 45.1*** 546 76.6*** 523 84.1***
Mena 254 64.6 269 45.3 1,011 34.6 559 60.5 616 69.8
Marital status 566 727 2,085 1,105 1,139
Unmarried 291 68.0 314 50.6 903 43.4** 428 72.9* 606 81.3***
Marrieda 275 58.5 413 49.6 1,182 37.4 677 65.6 528 70.4
Education level 564 681 2,079 1,100 1,096
<Elementary 57 71.9*** 0 - 244 44.7** 871 71.7* 469 82.1***
Elementary 416 67.8*** 633 50.9** 791 45.6*** 205 56.1 151 73.5
>Elementarya 91 38.5 48 31.2 1,044 34.6 24 50.0 476 71.4
Life occupation 566 496 1,895 1,094 1,118
Housewives 41 48.8 160 59.4*** 235 44.3* 270 78.1*** 258 84.5***
Manual 296 71.6*** 249 49.4** 1,038 40.3 742 66.0 473 76.7*
Non-manuala 229 55.5 87 31.0 622 35.7 82 59.8 387 69.8
Heart disease 566 618 2,085 1,101 1,139
Yes 202 77.2*** 165 73.9*** 476 62.8*** 258 82.2*** 433 84.1***
Noa 364 55.8 453 37.7 1,609 33.2 843 64.3 706 71.1
Hypertension 566 631 2,029 1,096 1,139
Yes 125 73.6 170 62.3** 184 38.6 498 75.3*** 371 84.1***
Noa 441 60.4 461 41.2 1,845 39.9 598 62.9 768 72.7
Stroke 466 622 2,085 1,100 1,139
Yes 26 84.6 18 77.8 154 58.4*** 68 75.0 48 91.7*
Noa 440 63.2 604 45.9 1,931 38.5 1,032 67.8 1,091 75.7
Diabetes 466 631 2,085 1,098 1,139
Yes 53 81.1* 67 67.2* 205 64.4*** 213 80.3*** 207 90.8***
Noa 413 62.2 564 44.9 1,880 37.4 885 65.6 932 73.2
Cancer 466 579 2,084 1,095
Yes 26 73.1 22 50.0 225 57.8*** 82 70.7 68 80.9
Noa 440 63.9 557 46.5 1,859 37.8 1,013 68.2 1,071 76.1
Respiratory diseases 537 684 2,085 1,096 1,139
Yes 55 94.5*** 120 67.5** 284 66.5*** 361 59.8*** 227 91.2***
Noa 482 62.0 564 44.7 1,801 35.8 735 63.1 912 72.7
Musculoskeletal diseases 542 704 2,085 1,098 1,139
Yes 240 73.7*** 391 67.8*** 793 53.1*** 766 76.5*** 465 87.1***
Noa 302 58.3 313 26.8 1,292 32.0 332 50.0 674 69.0
ADL 566 723 2,006 1,105 1,135
Disabled 49 87.9** 84 81.1*** 121 62.3*** 265 86.6*** 223 91.8***
Non-disablea 517 61.1 639 44.9 1,885 38.3 840 62.6 912 72.3

aReference group. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001; the p-value represents the difference in SRH for each potential risk factor within each country

Regression analyses

Table 3 shows the odds ratios (ORs) for each of the status characteristics and health factors in relation to fair or poor SRH, within each country, when adjusting for age and sex. Age was significantly related to fair or poor SRH in The Netherlands and in Spain, but not in the other countries. Women had fair or poor SRH in all countries except in Finland. Married persons in Finland and Israel rated better health than did unmarried. In Finland and The Netherlands, those with elementary school or less had fair or poor health compared to those with higher education. Swedish housewives had more fair or poor health. In Finland, Sweden and The Netherlands, being a manual worker was related to fair or poor SRH. Further, in all countries, heart disease, diabetes, respiratory diseases and musculoskeletal diseases were related to fair or poor SRH. Those with hypertension in Sweden, Spain and Israel had fair or poor SRH. In Finland, The Netherlands, Spain and Israel, stroke was associated with fair or poor SRH. Only in The Netherlands did those with cancer have poor or fair SRH. ADL was significantly related to fair or poor SRH in all countries.

Table 3.

Associations between status characteristics, health status, and fair or poor self-rated health. Analyses are adjusted for age and sex

Finland Sweden The Netherlands Spain Israel
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Age 1.03 1.00–1.06 1.00 0.97–1.04 1.03 1.01–1.05*** 1.02 1.00–1.05* 1.01 0.97–1.05
Sex
Woman 0.85 0.55–1.33 1.65 1.07–2.55* 1.64 1.35–1.99*** 2.18 1.70–2.80*** 2.39 1.76–3.24***
Mena
Marital status
Unmarried 1.62 1.09–2.42* 0.90 0.57–1.41 1.07 0.87–1.32 1.04 0.77–1.41 1.45 1.03–2.05*
Marrieda
Education level
<Elementary 5.87 2.61–13.20*** - - 1.38 1.01–1.88* 1.43 0.57–3.58 1.30 0.94–1.80
Elementary 4.34 2.52–7.49*** 1.54 0.72–3.27 1.48 1.20–1.81*** 0.88 0.34–2.26 1.23 0.71–2.12
>Elementarya
Life occupation
Housewives 0.82 0.44–1.55 3.65 1.73–7.70*** 1.17 0.86–1.60 1.53 0.87–2.68 0.71 0.41–1.24
Manual 2.30 1.55–3.41*** 1.93 1.05–3.52* 1.30 1.06–1.61* 1.32 0.82–2.13 1.23 0.86–1.74
Non-manuala
Heart disease
Yes 2.68 1.78–4.02*** 5.09 2.93–8.86*** 3.81 2.99–4.84*** 2.88 2.02–4.12*** 2.44 1.71–3.48***
Noa
Hypertension
Yes 1.41 0.90–2.20 2.15 1.32–3.48** 0.83 0.60–1.15 1.90 1.46–2.47*** 2.24 1.59–3.18***
Noa
Stroke
Yes 3.01 1.02–8.91* 3.68 0.82–16.56 2.25 1.50–3.39*** 2.20 1.14–4.23* 3.89 1.11–13.74*
Noa
Diabetes
Yes 2.42 1.24–4.72** 2.24 1.02–4.95* 2.69 1.93–3.73*** 2.02 1.42–2.86*** 6.28 3.33–11.83***
Noa
Cancer
Yes 1.29 0.58–2.87 1.20 0.42–3.42 1.93 1.43–2.59*** 1.29 0.79–2.08 0.91 0.43–1.91
Noa
Respiratory diseases
Yes 9.64 3.13–29.78*** 2.30 1.25–4.25** 3.65 2.73–4.89*** 2.72 2.02–3.66*** 4.21 2.45–7.23***
Noa
Musculoskeletal diseases
Yes 2.37 1.61–3.48*** 5.06 3.18–8.04*** 2.36 1.94–2.87*** 3.01 2.27–4.00*** 2.69 1.88–3.84***
Noa
ADL
Disabled 5.20 2.00–13.48*** 4.64 2.02–10.66*** 2.46 1.57–3.87*** 4.68 2.93–7.46*** 3.80 2.16–6.66***
Non-disableda

aReference group. Statistically significant differences between reference group and comparing group(s): *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001

The next step in the analyses used multivariable models, including all potential variables (Table 4). The elderly population in Israel rated the poorest health compared to the elderly population in The Netherlands, followed by the elderly populations in Finland and Spain. In Sweden, the proportion of persons with fair or poor SRH was not significantly different from the elderly population in The Netherlands. When the heterogeneity of each potential variable across countries was tested, sex, education level, life occupation, heart disease and respiratory diseases turned out to be non-homogeneous factors of fair or poor SRH. Thus, the country-specific odds ratios were calculated for each of these factors. In all countries, except in Finland, women had poorer health than did men. Those with less than elementary school education had poorer health than those with more than elementary school in Finland and Israel. Those with only elementary school education in Finland and The Netherlands rated more fair or poor health than those with higher education. Swedish housewives and Finnish manual workers had more fair or poor health than did non-manual workers. Heart disease was related to fair or poor SRH in all countries, but the strengths of the associations differed among the countries. Respiratory diseases were related to fair or poor health in all countries except in Sweden. Age, marital status, hypertension, stroke, diabetes, cancer, musculoskeletal diseases and ADL turned out to be homogeneous factors of fair or poor SRH. Therefore, the odds ratios in the pooled sample were calculated for these variables. Table 4 shows that age was not independently associated with fair or poor SRH, but all other factors were significantly associated with SRH. It deserves mentioning that supplementary analyses using only respondents in the common age group 75–84 years yielded very similar results. Thus, to gain most possible power in the analyses, data from all age groups were used and presented in the tables.

Table 4.

Multivariate analyses for the association between country, status characteristics, health status, and fair or poor self-rated healtha

Interaction term country*factor OR (95% CI)
Country Country-specific estimates
Finland 2.08 (1.64–2.64)***
Sweden 0.96 (0.73–1.27)
Spain 1.50 (1.20–1.88)***
Israel 4.86 (3.88–6.10)***
(vs. The Netherlands)
Age (per 1-year increase) Homogeneous Pooled estimate 1.00 (0.99–1.01)
Sex Non-homogeneous Country-specific estimates
Woman (vs. man) Finland 0.67 (0.42–1.08)
Sweden 1.70 (1.04–2.78)*
The Netherlands 1.35 (1.08–1.70)**
Spain 1.49 (1.10–2.02)**
Israel 1.55 (1.09–2.22)*
Marital status Homogeneous Pooled estimates
Unmarried (vs. married) 1.21 (1.04–1.40)*
Education level Non-homogeneous Country-specific estimates
<Elementary (vs. >elementary) Finland 3.83 (1.59–9.25)**
Sweden -
The Netherlands 1.22 (0.87–1.72)
Spain 1.18 (0.43–3.24)
Israel 1.47 (1.03–2.08)*
Elementary (vs. >elementary) Country-specific estimates
Finland 3.31 (1.83–5.99)***
Sweden 1.26 (0.56–2.83)
The Netherlands 1.28 (1.02–1.60)*
Spain 0.85 (0.30–2.39)
Israel 1.33 (0.74–2.37)
Life occupation Non-homogeneous Country-specific estimates
Housewives (vs. non-manual) Finland 0.58 (0.29–1.14)
Sweden 2.62 (1.27–5.42)**
The Netherlands 1.05 (0.75–1.48)
Spain 1.53 (0.86–2.75)
Israel 1.02 (0.64–1.62)
Manual (vs. non-manual) Finland 1.81 (1.17–2.78)**
Sweden 1.43 (0.74–2.76)
The Netherlands 1.05 (0.75–1.48)
Spain 1.53 (0.86–2.75)
Israel 1.28 (0.87–1.89)
Heart disease Non-homogeneous Country-specific estimates
Yes (vs. no) Finland 2.62 (1.71–4.02)***
Sweden 3.51 (1.97–6.25)***
The Netherlands 3.82 (2.97–4.91)***
Spain 2.09 (1.43–3.07)***
Israel 1.79 (1.23–2.61)**
Hypertension Homogeneous Pooled estimates
Yes (vs. no) 1.41 (1.20–1.67)***
Stroke Homogeneous Pooled estimates
Yes (vs. no) 1.67 (1.18–2.36)**
Diabetes Homogeneous Pooled estimate
Yes (vs. no) 2.15 (1.73–2.67)***
Cancer Homogeneous Pooled estimate
Yes (vs. no) 1.47 (1.15–1.89)**
Respiratory diseases Non-homogeneous Country-specific estimates
Yes (vs. no) Finland 9.12 (2.87–28.92)***
Sweden 1.89 (0.97–3.68)
The Netherlands 3.55 (2.60–4.84)***
Spain 2.08 (1.51–2.84)***
Israel 2.80 (1.60–4.90)***
Musculoskeletal diseases Homogeneous Pooled estimate
Yes (vs. no) 2.44 (2.12–2.81)***
ADL Homogeneous Pooled estimate
Yes (vs. non-disabled) 2.72 (2.09–3.55)***

aStatistically significant differences between reference group and comparing group(s): *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001

Discussion

This study used data from the CLESA study, which is designed to describe the health and functional status of older populations in a variety of different cultures. Such a study presents a singular opportunity to assess the underlying cross-national differences in SRH as well as differences in the aging process within countries. The benefit of this study is that it includes older populations in four different European countries and Israel. Thus, it offers a rare opportunity to examine whether the relationship between several determinants and poor SRH holds across different countries with either homogeneous cultural populations or multicultural populations. The question of whether the relationship between various medical conditions, functional abilities, status characteristics, and SRH is similar across countries in Europe is important for our understanding of the conceptual structure of SRH and its prediction of future events such as morbidity, use of health services, and death.

The results found here indicated that there are differences in SRH between countries. The elderly population in Israel rated fair or poor health to the greatest extent, followed by the elderly populations in Finland, Spain, Sweden, and The Netherlands. These cross-national differences cannot be explained entirely be status characteristics, self-reported diseases or functional ability. Another important finding was that in all countries most of the indicators of medical and functional health were homogeneously associated with SRH.

Previous studies have found diverging results regarding the relationship between age and SRH, when differences in medical health and functional ability were taken into account (Liang et al. 1991; Johnson and Wolinsky 1993). The diverging results indicate that the relationship between age and SRH may not be linear. A curvilinear relationship between age and SRH has been suggested, indicating that those in the youngest and the oldest age groups rate the highest SRH, whereas the “middle-aged”, i.e. people in the age group 45–75, have the worst SRH, after adjusting for medical conditions and functional ability (Moum 1992; Idler 1993). In studies of people 60 years and older, some studies have shown higher SRH among the oldest whereas other studies showed no differences between the ages (Johnson et al. 1991; Johnson and Wolinsky 1993). In our study, the associations between age and SRH found within some countries did not remain when all the other variables were added to the models, which is in line with the findings in the latter studies.

When it comes to gender and SRH, gender differences are inconsistent across age and health measures (Su and Ferraro 1997; Matthews et al. 1999). A review of work on SRH and gender concluded that about 75% of the studies showed no or weak relationship between gender and SRH, when education, race, employment and social network were adjusted for. In several age groups, women rate their health as poorer than men do, but this does not seem to apply to people of 65 and older (Bjørner et al. 1996). These results imply that if there is an effect of gender on SRH, it is small, which is in accordance with the findings here. Although the associations were weak, women had poorer SRH than did men in Sweden, The Netherlands, Spain and Israel.

Marital status was a homogeneous factor weakly associated with fair or poor SRH. Education level and lifetime occupation turned out to be non-homogeneous factors of fair or poor SRH. Those with less than elementary school education had poorer health than those with more than elementary school in Finland and Israel. Those with only elementary school education in Finland and The Netherlands rated more fair or poor health than those with higher education. Occupation was not independently associated with fair or poor SRH in the multivariate analyses, with a few exceptions. Prior studies have found a positive impact of higher education level and status employment on SRH. The associations, however, were weak when controlling for illness indicators and other socio-economic variables (Johnson et al. 1991; Moum 1992; Fylkenes and Førde 1992), which is consistent with the results found here.

Heart disease was related to fair or poor SRH in all countries, but the strength of the associations differed among the countries. Respiratory diseases were associated with poorer health in all countries except in Sweden. Hypertension, stroke, diabetes, cancer and musculoskeletal diseases turned out to be homogeneous factors of fair or poor SRH. Most of the findings here are in agreement with previous studies, which have shown a strong association between medical health and functional ability (Jylhä et al. 1986; Johnson and Wolinsky 1993; Ferraro et al. 1997). In line with other studies adjusting for diseases common among older persons (Kriegsman et al. 1996; Nybo et al. 2001), ADL-disability was associated with fair or poor SRH in all countries. It would have been of interest to evaluate whether the severity of the diseases differed across the countries, but this was not possible. Despite this, our results suggest that SRH is a useful global measure which correlates with several indicators of medical and functional health in these European countries.

A number of explanations may account for the differences in SRH across countries found here. One explanation may be found in the extent to which the health variables cover the range and severity of different health conditions in different countries. Another explanation may be differences in cultural factors (such as ways of life), in history, and in social structures across countries. There may also be differences in how people from different cultures evaluate their health, i.e. how they take different health issues into their global evaluations (Idler and Benyamini 1997; Jylhä et al. 1998). Important issues have been raised about cross-cultural differences which would be inaccessible to most respondents. Angel and Guarnaccia (1989) conclude from their study that “for Mexican Americans and Puerto Ricans the adjectives associated with normal health differ from those used by non-Hispanics”. A similar case of cross-cultural differences in a single study occurs in the work of Appels et al. (1996) in which the normative category for Lithuanians is “poor” whereas it is “good” for the Dutch. Another explanation for the differences in SRH across countries may be that slightly different measurements of SRH were used, both in terms of the wording of the question and in terms of response alternatives. Although the wording of the question differed, a global question in which the respondent was asked to make a general statement of his/her health was used in all countries. It was agreed by the investigators from each country that all items refer to an overall present assessment of health. Furthermore, the measures of SRH was harmonised to ensure optimal comparability. As the CLESA project makes use of already existing studies, this harmonisation effort is limited to post-harmonisation. Therefore, caution is still needed in interpreting the findings in terms of genuine cross-national differences. Furthermore, international comparisons of rates of specific categories of SRH are problematic because of cultural and linguistics differences; emphasis may be directed towards comparing the determinants of SRH.

The differences in health-ratings among the countries may also be explained by other factors and intermediate variables not included in this study. Although status characteristics and morbidity were adjusted for in the analyses, there may be social and economic inequalities (such as personal and household income, retirement age and funding, housing situation, members in the household, voluntary associations, and family and friends, which in turn may lead to health inequalities) between the countries not captured in this study (van Doorslaer et al. 1997; Carlson 1998; Kopp 2000; Kunst et al. 2005) which may explain the findings. Moreover, it is noteworthy that, despite harmonisation and common categories, there are still a few points which need to be kept in mind with the variables concerning status characteristics. In several of these variables, the distributions vary greatly between countries and these differences reflect both cultural and, in some cases, sample-related differences (Minicuci et al. 2003). There is also a wide range of other factors in the individual’s life situation which affects how one rates one’s own health. These factors may be different between individuals, between groups in a society, and between countries. Among them, psychosocial problems/symptoms, emotional status, psychological distress, personality and lifestyle would have been of interest to further evaluate. However, such analyses were not possible to do in an effective way using data from the CLESA study.

A limitation with this study is that the method used for selection of the CLESA samples and sampling strategies differed across countries. Some of the samples are geographically defined whereas others are national samples. Therefore, the various country samples cannot be considered to be representative for the total elderly population from which the sample was drawn. This should be borne in mind when interpreting the results.

Conclusions

There are differences in SRH between countries. These differences cannot be entirely explained by cross-national differences in status characteristics, health condition or functional ability. In this study, we did not find strong associations between status characteristics and SRH in the countries. However, within each country, SRH was associated with several indicators of medical and functional health.

Acknowledgements

The CLESA Working Group: Finland: M. Jylhä (Principal Investigator), S. Nikula, V. Autio. Israel: J. Gindin (Principal Investigator; First Principal Investigator: the late Modan Baruch of the Chaim Sheba Medical Center), T. Blumstein, A. Chetrit, A. Walter-Ginzburg, K. Shamay. Italy: S. Maggi (Project Leader), N. Minicuci, M. Noale, C. Marzari. The Netherlands: D.J.H. Deeg (Principal Investigator), S.M.F. Pluijm. Spain: A. Otero (Principal Investigator), M.J. Garcia de Yebenes, A. Rodríguez-Laso, M.V. Zunzunegui. Sweden: N.L. Pedersen (Principal Investigator), C. Bardage. The CLESA Project is supported by EU QoL2000-00664. LASA is supported by The Netherlands Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sports and the Vrije Universiteit. ILSA was supported by the CNR (National Research Council) and the Italian Department of Health—National Institutes of Health. Aging in Leganés is supported by the Spanish Health Research Fund (FIS), the Madrid Regional Research Fund and private foundations (La Caixa, BBVA). CALAS was supported by the US National Institute on Aging grants R01-5885-03 and R01-5885-06. The TamELSA is supported by the Academy of Finland, the Juho Vainio Foundation, the Yrjö Jahnsson Foundation, and the Medical Research Fund of Tampere University Hospital. SATSA is supported by the US National Institute on Aging (AG 04563, 10175) and the Swedish Social Research Council.

References

  1. Angel Social Sci Med. 1989;28:1229. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(89)90341-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Appels Social Sci Med. 1996;42:681. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(95)00195-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beekman J Affect Disord. 1997;46:219. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00145-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. B Rev Gerontol. 1995;5:215. [Google Scholar]
  5. B Rev Gerontol. 1995;5:207. [Google Scholar]
  6. Benyamini J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Social Sci. 2000;55:107. doi: 10.1093/geronb/55.2.p107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bjørner JB, Søndergaard Kristensen T, Orth-Gomér K, Tibblin G, Sullivan M, Westerholm P (eds) (1996) Self-rated health; a useful concept in research, prevention and clinical medicine. The Swedish Council for Planning and Coordination of Research, Ord & Form AB, Uppsala
  8. Bobak Social Sci Med. 2000;51:1343. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00096-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carlson Social Sci Med. 1998;46:1355. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(97)10093-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cockerham J Gerontol. 1983;38:349. doi: 10.1093/geronj/38.3.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Davies Women Health. 1995;23:13. doi: 10.1300/J013v23n02_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. de Bruin A, Picavet HSJ, Nossikov A (1996) Health interview surveys. Towards international harmonization of methods and instruments. WHO, Geneva, Regional Publications European Ser no 58 [PubMed]
  13. Ferraro J Health Social Behav. 1997;38:38. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ferrie J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002;56:922. doi: 10.1136/jech.56.12.922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Figueiras Stat Med. 1998;17:2099. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19980930)17:18&#x0003c;2099::AID-SIM905&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fylkesnes Social Sci Med. 1991;32:141. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90053-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fylkenes Social Sci Med. 1992;35:271. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(92)90023-J. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Grundy Social Sci Med. 2003;56:935. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00093-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Idler Int Rev Health Psychol. 1992;1:33. [Google Scholar]
  20. Idler J Gerontol Social Sci. 1993;48:289. [Google Scholar]
  21. Idler J Health Social Behav. 1997;38:21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Johnson J Health Social Behav. 1993;34:105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Johnson Int Q Community Health Educ. 1991;11:29. doi: 10.2190/Y2BT-XQH1-P0XC-1N91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jylh J Gerontol. 1986;41:710. doi: 10.1093/geronj/41.6.710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jylh Scand J Social Med Suppl. 1992;47:1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jylh J Gerontol Social Sci. 1998;53:144. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kind BMJ. 1998;52:736. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7133.736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kleinbaum Logistic. 1994;regression:a. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kopp MS, Skrabski Á, Szedmák S (2000) Self-rated health and social transitions. In: Nilsson P, Orth-Gomér K (eds) Self-rated health in a European perspective. The Swedish Council for Planning and Coordination of Research, Ord & Form AB, Uppsala, pp 85–102
  30. Kriegsman J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49:1407. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(96)00274-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kunst Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34:295. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Liang Med Care. 1991;29:1161. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199112000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. MacIntyre Social Sci Med. 1996;42:617. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(95)00335-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Maggi Aging Clin Exp Res. 1994;6:464. [Google Scholar]
  35. Manor Int J Epidemiol. 2000;29:149. doi: 10.1093/ije/29.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Marmot Lancet. 1991;337:1387. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93068-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Matthews Social Sci Med. 1999;48:49. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00288-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McDonough Social Sci Med. 2001;52:547. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00159-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Minicuci Aging Clin Exp Res. 2003;15:187. doi: 10.1007/BF03324499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Moum Social Sci Med. 1992;35:935. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(92)90108-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nilsson P, Orth-Gomér K (eds) (2000) Self-rated health in a European perspective. The Swedish Council for Planning and Coordination of Research, Ord & Form AB, Uppsala
  42. Noale Exp Gerontol. 2005;40:89. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2004.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nybo J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:601. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49121.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pedersen Acta Genet Med Gemellol. 1991;40:7. doi: 10.1017/s0001566000006681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pluijm S, Bardage C, Nikula S et al. (2005) Harmonising a measure of ADL across six countries. J Clin Epidemiol (in press) [DOI] [PubMed]
  46. Rakoski Res Aging. 1993;15:92. [Google Scholar]
  47. SAS Institute (1999–2001) SAS release 8.02. SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC
  48. Shetterly Am J Public Health. 1996;86:1798. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.12.1798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Stoller J Health Social Behav. 1984;25:260. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Su J Gerontol Social Sci. 1997;52B:27. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52b.1.s27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Townsend P, Davidson N (1992) Inequalities in health. The Black Report. Penguin, London
  52. Walter-Ginzburg J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:1. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49001.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. van J Health Econ. 1997;16:93. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6296(96)00532-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Verbrugge J Health Social Behav. 1989;30:282. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Verbrugge Social Sci Med. 1994;38:1. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90294-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Zimmer J Health Social Behav. 2000;41:465. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from European Journal of Ageing are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES