Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2017 Aug 9;8:133. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00133

Systematic Review of Cognitive Function in Euthymic Bipolar Disorder and Pre-Surgical Temporal Lobe Epilepsy

Emmanuelle C S Bostock 1,*, Kenneth C Kirkby 2, Michael I Garry 1, Bruce V M Taylor 3
PMCID: PMC5552675  PMID: 28848456

Abstract

Background

Bipolar disorder (BD) and temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) overlap in domains including epidemiology, treatment response, shared neurotransmitter involvement and temporal lobe pathology. Comparison of cognitive function in both disorders may indicate temporal lobe mediated processes relevant to BD. This systematic review examines neuropsychological test profiles in euthymic bipolar disorder type I (BD-I) and pre-surgical TLE and compares experimental designs used.

Methods

A search of PubMed, PsychINFO, and Scopus using Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines was conducted. Inclusion criteria were comparison group or pre- to post-surgical patients; reported neuropsychological tests; participants aged 18–60 years. Fifty six studies met criteria: 27 BD-I; 29 TLE.

Results

Deficits in BD-I compared to healthy controls (HC) were in executive function, attention span and verbal memory. Deficits in TLE compared to HC were in executive function and memory. In the pre- to post-surgical comparisons, verbal memory in left temporal lobe (LTL) and, less consistently, visuospatial memory in right temporal lobe (RTL) epilepsy declined following surgery. BD-I studies used comprehensive test batteries in well-defined euthymic patients compared to matched HC groups. TLE studies used convenience samples pre- to post-surgery, comparing LTL and RTL subgroups, few included comparisons to HC (5 studies). TLE studies typically examined a narrow range of known temporal lobe-mediated neuropsychological functions, particularly verbal and visuospatial memory.

Conclusion

Both disorders exhibit deficits in executive function and verbal memory suggestive of both frontal and temporal lobe involvement. However, deficits in TLE are measured pre- to post-surgery and not controlled at baseline pre-surgery. Further research involving a head-to-head comparison of the two disorders on a broad range of neuropsychological tests is needed to clarify the nature and extent of cognitive deficits and potential overlaps.

Keywords: bipolar disorder, temporal lobe, focal seizures neuropsychology, cognition, epilepsy, systematic review

Introduction

Bipolar disorder type I (BD-I) is typically characterized by episodes of mania and depression with inter-episode euthymia. A number of impairments have been noted in the euthymic phase of the illness including social, occupational functioning, and cognitive deficits (1, 2).

Most studies have examined cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with BD-I compared to healthy controls (HC). Five meta-analyses have reported impairments in cognitive domains of executive functioning (37), verbal memory (3, 57), visuospatial memory (7), and attention (4, 6). One meta-analysis found no impairment of verbal memory and executive function in BD-I compared to HC (8). An individual patient data meta-analysis by Bourne et al. (9) reported impaired verbal memory and attention in euthymic BD-I patients relative to HC (9). The absence of an association between cognitive impairment and medication dose in euthymic BD-I patients suggest the effects of medication do not fully account for the cognitive impairments observed (4). These meta-analyses support the assumption that cognitive impairments exhibited in the euthymic phase are trait markers of the disorder.

An alternate research design is to compare and contrast cognitive deficits in BD-I to a reference condition which shares common features. For example, a meta-analysis comparing BD-I and schizophrenia reported more pronounced cognitive deficits in schizophrenia on measures of verbal fluency, verbal memory, executive function, visuospatial memory, mental speed, IQ, and concept formation (10). Similarly a single study comparing BD-I, obsessive compulsive disorder reported impaired verbal and episodic memory compared to HC (11). The BD-I group had greater impairments in learning word lists and delayed recall. These results suggest the importance of the temporal lobes in both disorders in the consolidation and retrieval of memories.

A single study compared BD-I with complex partial seizure disorder (12). It is noted that the classification of epilepsy syndromes has been subject to a number of iterations. In this review, we use the most commonly reported and widely understood term “temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE)” to incorporate the terms complex partial seizure disorder and focal seizures arising from the temporal lobes. The Jones et al. study reported greater impairment of executive function, attention and delayed verbal recall in the TLE group. These results should be interpreted with caution given the small and unequal sample sizes (BD-I n = 26, TLE n = 9). A case can be made for further exploration of similarities and differences between the neuropsychological test profiles seen in euthymic BD-I and interictal TLE. This is particularly so given the localizing pathology of TLE, which allows inferences to be made regarding the contribution of temporal lobe processes to the range of cognitive deficits reported in BD-I.

There are a number of established similarities between BD-I and TLE. These typically include a chronic course punctuated by episodic manifestations of mania and seizures, respectively. Other similarities include: the proposed involvement of kindling mechanisms (13); changes in neurotransmitters (excitatory amino acids, GABA, dopamine and serotonin), voltage-opened ion channels (sodium, calcium and potassium) and second messenger systems (G-proteins, phosphatidylinositol, protein kinase C, myristoylated alanine-rich C kinase substrate), and treatment response to antiepileptic medications in both disorders (14).

In addition, episodes in both disorders commonly feature sensory, perceptual, cognitive, and affective changes (15) including depression (16). Epidemiological studies have shown that the proportion of BD-I among people with epilepsy is more than twice as high as in the general population (17) and that mania is more common in patients with TLE than in the general population (18). BD-I is also associated with comorbid epilepsy but not parental epilepsy (19). Episodes of mania in BD-I and seizures in TLE share precipitating factors including stress, sleep reduction and antidepressant medications (20).

The temporal lobes have also been the subject of neuroimaging research in both disorders. In BD-I many studies have investigated correlates of the disorder to specific brain regions. Meta-analyses of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have reported that BD-I is associated with right lateral ventricular enlargement (21) and an enlarged left amygdala (22). However, studies of temporal lobe size are inconsistent; with reported increases (23), reductions (24) or no differences (2528) likely reflecting the difficulties of defining and measuring the volume of individual cerebral lobes on MRI.

In TLE, MRI studies have reported structural brain abnormalities in the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex (29), thalamus (30), and fornix (31). On voxel-based morphometry, TLE is associated with gray matter pathology in the hippocampus, cingulum, thalamus, and frontal lobes. White matter reductions ipsilateral to the seizure focus were also found in the temporopolar, entorhinal, and perirhinal areas (32). Typically, TLE originates unilaterally from the medial temporal lobe; they may, however, be propagated from other regions which project to limbic areas (33).

Given these potential diffuse structural abnormalities seen in patients with TLE, it could be expected that neuropsychological deficits may not be limited to tasks involving temporal lobe function. Patients with TLE display deficits in memory, general intelligence, language, executive function, and motor speed relative to HC (34, 35). Deficits in verbal memory, language, and psychomotor speed may be affected by factors such as age of onset of epilepsy, general intelligence, the number and dose of antiepileptic medications used, and seizure frequency (35).

The literature describing clinical features, imaging findings, and neuropsychological test profiles is largely in a separate corpus for BD-I and for TLE with only one small study directly comparing the two (12).

The current structured review brings these two bodies of work together in a comparison of the neurocognition literature findings of the two conditions side-by-side. The aim is to determine whether and to what extent the cognitive impairments seen in euthymic BD-I are mirrored by those attributed to a pathology primarily affecting the temporal lobes, that is TLE.

Method

This systematic review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (36).

Inclusion criteria: (a) controlled comparison, (b) patients diagnosed with BD-I as assessed by a recognized criterion-based diagnostic system, (c) BD-I patients rated as euthymic, defined by their scores on a rating scale (<8 HamD, <8 YMRS); patients with unilateral TLE, from any cause, with diagnosis confirmed in a pre-surgical workup, (d) at least one neuropsychological test, (e) adult participants (18–60 years), (f) in the case of more than one article by the same authors results were not identified as being from the same sample, (f) articles published 1980 or later.

Identification of Studies

A comprehensive search of the electronic databases PubMed, PsychINFO, and Scopus for peer-reviewed articles published in English was conducted in the last week of May 2016. Search terms were grouped as follows: bipolar, manic depress*, baseline, asympt*, remit*, stable (group 1); or epilep*, seizure, presurg*, temporal, focal, partial, complex, interictal (group 2) and WAIS, Wechsler, trail making, continuous performance, stroop, digit span, verbal learning, rey, working memory, benton, card sort*, verbal fluency, RAVLT, CAVLT, tower of London (group 3). These search terms were combined as follows: group 1 AND group 3 for BD-I and group 2 AND group 3 for TLE.

Data Extraction

The abstracts located from the search strategy were entered into EndNoteX7. The PRISMA flowchart (Figure 1) sets out the steps in screening conducted by author EB. After the screening of titles and abstracts the remaining 139 studies were examined in full text, using a purpose built coding sheet to assess whether they met inclusion criteria. This process resulted in the study sample of 56, of which 27 related to BD-I and 29 to TLE.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses flow diagram of included studies.

Results

The search strategy identified an initial 3,307 articles of which 56 met inclusion criteria, 27 related to BD-I and 29 to pre-surgical TLE. Studies of BD-I are summarized in Table 1 and of TLE in Table 2 detailing authors, sample sizes, medication (BD-I only as this was not recorded in the TLE samples), neuropsychological test parameters, and summary results.

Table 1.

Summary of included studies on euthymic bipolar disorder.

Reference n Medications bipolar sample Neuropsychological test parameters Results
Altshuler et al. (37) BD 40 Li 25, AC 12, AD 4, AP 6, BZD 3 WAIS-R block design, vocab; TMT, WCST, Stroop; VFT, CVLT; ROCFT; Star mirror tracing Task, PR BD impaired verbal memory and executive functioning
SZ 20
HC 22

Bas et al. (38) BD 60 Li 39, AP 37, LTG 9, VPA 21 Stroop, TMT A and B; WMS-R visual reproduction; RAVLT BD impaired on RAVLT
HC 41

Bora et al. (39) BD 514 Li 63.2%, VPA 47.6%, AP 47.2%, AD 5.7% Stroop. WCST Deficits exist in subgroups who have severe and global cognitive deficits
BD-II 42
HC 416

Chang et al. (40) BD 23 BD Li 13, AP 15, LTG 7, VPA 9 WAIS-R block design and vocab; CVLT, VFT All NS
BD-II 23
HC 23

Chou et al. (41) BD 23 AP 13, VPA 19 The Color Trails Test, WCST; WMS-III – Word Lists Test, Face Test; Go/No-GO, Test for Attentional Performance BD impaired faces memory and WCST
HC 33

Deckersbach et al. (42) BD 25 Li 11, AC 9, AD 9, AP 3 ROCFT Immediate recall BD impaired, copy and recognition preserved
HC 25

Deckersbach et al. (11) BD 30 Li 13, AC 11, AD 8, AP 4 CVLT CVLT BD impaired learning and delayed free recall
Obsessive compulsive disorder 30
HC 30

Dell’Osso et al. (43) BD 15 BD Mono 2, Poly 12 N-Back NS HC and BD
BD-II13
HC 27

Dittmann et al. (44) BD 65 BD Li 27, AD 10, AP 33, CBZ 4, VPA 28 TMT; HAWIE-R; WAIS-III letter number sequencing; RBANS Psychomotor speed, working and delayed memory, verbal learning, executive functioning BD impaired
BD-II 38
HC 62

Dixon et al. (45) BD manic 15 dep 15, eu 15, HC 30 Eu Li 15, AD 8, AP 10, AC 2 WAIS-R Vocab; Stroop; FAS; Hayling sentence completion test; Cognitive Estimates Test VF total responses, Hayling response initiation latency, error score and using strategy, stroop EuBD impaired

Doganavs¸argil-Baysal et al. (46) BD 60 Li 11, MS + AP 12, 2MS 12, VPA 6 WCST, TMT, Stroop; RAVLT; Cancelation test Significant differences on all measures between BD and HC
HC 20

Doganavs¸argil-Baysal et al. (46) BD 54 Li 10, MS + AP 27, 2MS 11, VPA 5 WCST; RAVLT Both measures BD < HC
HC 18

Doruk et al. (47) Manic 20 Unknown Stroop; Serial Digit Learning Test; RAVLT; Cancelation Test NS HC and EuBD
Dep10
Eu 21

Fistikci et al. (48) BD 25 Li 25 WCST; Montreal Cognitive Assessment NS HC and BD
HC 25

Frangou et al. (49) BD 10 Li 5, AC 3 WCST; Hayling sentence completion WCST BD and offspring made more errors
Un offspring 15
HC 43

Hsiao et al. (50) BD 30 VPA 29 WAIS-III digit symbol; TMT; WMS-III Verbal memory, working memory, psychomotor speed, executive function BD impaired
BD-II 37
HC 22

Martino et al. (51) BD 48 AD 16, AP 28, BZD 23, MS 48 WAIS vocab, digit span; TMT, WCST, IGT; Memory battery of Signoret Verbal memory, attention and executive functions impaired
BD-II 37
HC 34

Muralidharan et al. (52) BD 72 AP 27, Li 34, VPA 38 TMT, Stroop; CANTAB; CVLT; WMS-III LNS P on VPA more working memory deficits than Li or HC
HC 40

Muralidharan et al. (53) BD 68 AP 51, Li 32, MS + AP 50, VPA 32 TMT, Stroop; CANTAB; CVLT; WMS-III letter number sequencing Verbal and visuospatial memory, working memory and executive function BD impaired.
HC 38

Normala et al. (54) BD 40 AP 6, MS 11, MS + AD 1, MS + AP22 WAIS Digit span; TMT; Verbal Fluency Executive and attention functioning BD impaired
HC 40

Pattanayak et al. (55) BD 30 AP 5, LTG 2, Li 21, VPA 11 VIQ; TMT, Stroop; N-Back; Postgraduate Institute Memory Scale Attention, information processing speed, executive function, verbal memory BD impaired
HC 20

Radwan (56) BD 30 Unknown WAIS; WCST; WMS; CPT All BD impaired
HC 30

Sepede et al. (57) BD 24 AD 10, AP 15, BZD 8, Li 3, MS 9 CPT Sustained attention impaired BD and rels
Unaffected rels 33
HC 24

Trivedi et al. (58) BD 15 CBZ 1, Li 8, VPA 6 WCST; SWMT; CPT Executive function BD impaired
HC 15

Trivedi et al. (59) BD 15 Unknown WCST; SWMT; CPT Executive function BD impaired
SZ 15, HC 15

Yates et al. (60) BD dep 34 AD 9, AP 18, BZD 9, MS 29 WAIS-III Verbal measure BD impaired
BDEu 31, HC 34

Zubieta et al. (61) BD 15 AP 3, CBZ 2, Li 7, VPA 12 WAIS-R: digit span; WCST, Stroop; WMS-R; verbal fluency; test of variable attention Verbal learning, executive function, motor coordination and sequential memory BD impaired. NS verbal fluency or attention
HC 15

BD, bipolar; BD-II, bipolar II; HC, healthy controls; SZ, Schizophrenia; Li, lithium; AC, anticonvulsant; AD, antidepressant; AP, antipsychotics; BZD, benzodiazepine; CBZ, carbamazepine; LTG, lamotrigine; MS, mood stabilizer; VPA, valproate; Mono, monotherapy; Poly, polytherapy; AMIPB, Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery; BVRT, Benton Visual Retention Test; VLMT, Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest; DCS-R, Diagnosticum für Cerebralschädigung – II; NAART, North American Adult Reading Test; WRAT, Wide Range Achievement Test; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; CPT, Continuous Performance Test; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

Table 2.

Summary of included studies on pre-surgical temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE).

Reference n Neuropsychological test parameters Results
Baxendale and Thompson (62) Right temporal lobe (RTL) 133 WAIS PIQ, VIQ; AMIPB Verbal memory decline post-surg left temporal lobe (LTL)
LTL 157

Baxendale et al. (63) RTL 146
LTL 177
WAIS PIQ, VIQ; AMIPB; Birt Memory and Information Processing Battery RTL and LTL at equal risk of post-surg decline

Berenbaum et al. (64) LTL 57 WAIS Digit Span; CVLT CVLT decline post-surg

Bjørnaes et al. (65) RTL 50 WAIS Digit Span; Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT); Design Learning and Retention Test; Verbal List Learning and Retention; Tactual Performance Test Improvement at 2-year follow up post-surg
LTL 41

Chelune et al. (66) RTL 19 WAIS-R VIQ, PIQ; WMS-R, RAVLT; COWAT, Halstead-Wepman Aphasia Screening Exam, BNT, Speech Sounds Perception Test; Hooper Visual Organization Test, Seashore Rhythm Test LTL decline post-surg
LTL 23

Chiaravalloti et al. (67) RTL 16 WMS-III Faces Subtest; Graduate Hospital Facial Memory Test RTL < LTL both pre- and post-surg
LTL 10

Chiaravalloti (68) RTL 42 CVLT; Graduate Hospital Facial Memory Test Verbal memory post-surg decline LTL, RTL improved. Visuospatial memory post-surg decline RTL, LTL improvement
LTL 28

Fernandes et al. (69) RTL 23 WAIS-R Block design, Vocabulary; WMS-R; RAVLT Cognitive scores post-surg decline low pre-surg scores. Non-verbal memory post-surg RTL decline, verbal and visuospatial memory LTL decline
LTL 24
healthy controls (HC) 28

Giovagnoli et al. (70) RTL 12 Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices; Attentive Matrices; Verbal Selective Reminding Procedure, Story Recall, Verbal Memory Distractor Test; Visual Selective Reminding Procedure, ROCFT, Visual Memory Distractor Test Verbal memory pre- and post-surgery LTL impaired relative to HC. Visual deficits present in both groups relative to HC
LTL 12
HC 36

Gleissner et al. (71) RTL 63 VLMT; DCR-S Verbal memory LTL decline
LTL 52

Glosser et al. (72) RTL 13 Boston Naming Test; CVLT Benton Facial Recognition, Graduate Hospital Facial Memory Recognition of familiar faces and learning new faces RTL impaired. Names of familiar faces LTL impaired
LTL 8
HC 10

Helmstaedter et al. (73) LTL 47 Verbal Learning and Memory Test; RAVLT Delayed recall and recognition post-surg improvement

Hermann and Wyler (74) RTL 14 COWAT; Token Test Language tests pre-surg LTL deficit
LTL 15

Hermann et al. (75) RTL 31 CVLT Verbal memory post-surg RTL increased
LTL 26

Hermann et al. (76) RTL 26 WAIS-R Digit Span; Multilingual Aphasia Examination Visual Naming Test; CVLT CVLT post-surg decline LTL
LTL 36

Köylü et al. (77) RTL 12 List learning task LTL post-surg decline
LTL 14

Lee et al. (78) LTL 38 RAVLT; ROCFT Memory post-surg decline

Loring et al. (79) RTL 13 Selective Reminding Test, Serial Digit Learning, ROCFT, Form Sequence Learning Complex figure RTL impaired. Verbal memory decline LTL
LTL 16

Malikova et al. (80) RTL 11 WAIS-R; WMS-R; Verbal Fluency Test FSIQ, global and verbal memory, attention, and working memory all improved post-surg
LTL 26

Morino et al. (81) RTL 31 WMS-R; Miyake Verbal Retention Test; BVRT Memory RTL improved post-surg. Verbal memory LTL post-surg improved
LTL 31

Quigg et al. (82) RTL 16 TMT; BNT; CVLT; WMS-R Logical Memory Scale BNT and CVLT LTL decreased post-surgery. Language and verbal memory LTL increased. TMT increased
LTL 14

Seidenberg et al. (83) RTL 30 CVLT Free recall LTL decline post-surgery
LTL 46

Selwa et al. (84) RTL 14 WAIS-R; WMS FSIQ, Logical Memory RTL improved post-surg. Verbal memory decline LTL post-surg
LTL 17
Non-surgical TLE 28

Shamim et al. (85) RTL 14 WAIS-III; WMS-III Verbal memory deficit post-surg LTL
LTL 16

Stretton et al. (86) RTL 17 WAIS-III Digit Span; Gesture Span Task; Motor Sequences Task; Dot-Back Paradigm Working memory pre-surg RTL and LTL worse than HC. WM improved post-surg LTL
LTL 16
HC 15

Tisser et al. (87) RTL 10 WAIS-R; WCST WCST RTL and LTL impaired, improved post-surg
LTL 15
HC 22

Trenerry and Jack (88) RTL 34 WAIS-R; WCST The WCST is not useful for lateralizing seizure onset in TLE
LTL 34

Trenerry et al. (89) RTL 36 WAIS-R; WMS-R; RAVLT; Visual Spatial Learning Test Verbal and visual memory LTL improved post-surg
LTL 44

von Rhein et al. (90) RTL 20 VLMT; DCS-R; BNT; Token Test Verbal Memory impaired post-surgery. Naming decline post-surgery LTL
LTL 32

Tests of executive function: WCST, FAS, TMT-B, Stroop, Hayling Sentence Completion Test, CANTAB: Intra Extra Dimensional Set Shifting, CANTAB: Stockings Problem; Tests of Verbal Memory—CVLT, RAVLT, WAIS Vocab, WMS-R: Logical Memory, WMS-R: Verbal Paired Associates, Token Test, VLMT: Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest; Tests of non-verbal memory- ROCFT, WMS: Visual reproduction, WMS-R: Design Memory, WMS-R Visual Reproduction, WMS-III: Face Test, CANTAB: Spatial recognition memory, CANTAB: Pattern Recognition Memory, CANTAB: Paired Associates Learning; Attention span WAIS: Digit Span, Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery (AMIPB); sustained attention CPT; working memory SWMT, N-Back, WAIS: Letter Number Sequencing, WMS-III: Letter Number Sequencing, CANTAB: Spatial Working Memory.

As demonstrated in Table 1, 25 of 27 studies compared patients with BD-I (n = 1,398) to HC (n = 1,142). The remaining studies compared manic, euthymic, and depressed groups. The most commonly reported impairments in BD-I were in executive function, attention span, and verbal memory. No studies found enhanced neuropsychological function in euthymic BD-I.

As shown in Table 2, in the epilepsy literature, all of the TLE studies compared neuropsychological test performance pre- to post-surgery. Only 5 of the 29 studies compared pre-surgical TLE (n = 150) to HC (n = 111); these studies found significant impairments in TLE compared to HC on tests of memory and executive function.

In 26 of the TLE studies, the samples were divided by laterality of seizure focus with the primary pathology affecting the right temporal lobe (RTL n = 846) or the left temporal lobe (LTL n = 1,068). In pre-surgical TLE, the direct comparison of LTL and RTL groups indicated that the LTL group showed impaired verbal memory and the RTL group, less consistently, impaired visuospatial memory.

The most common impairment observed in TLE related to surgery was in verbal memory. This finding was also associated with laterality, 24 studies reporting decline in verbal memory from pre- to post-surgery in LTL patients, whereas in RTL there was a less consistent decline in visuospatial memory. No significant differences in attention were found for laterality or pre- to post-surgery in TLE.

Table 3 shows the results of individual neuropsychological tests that were reported in more than one study, for BD-I and TLE. Of note, the total number of neuropsychological tests used across all studies differed between BD-I (27) and TLE (11); this was not evenly distributed across cognitive domains. The number of studies where any executive function instrument was administered in TLE was only 4 (2 WCST, 2 COWAT) of 29 compared to 21 of 27 in BD-I. One study compared RTL, LTL and HC and showed that all patients were impaired on the WCST relative to HC (87). The number of studies where any verbal memory instrument was administered was 26 of 29 in TLE compared to 14 of 27 in BD-I. The comparable figures for visuospatial memory instruments were 25 of 29 in TLE and 11 of 27 in BD-I. Similarly, only 6 studies measured attention in TLE compared to 17 studies in BD-I. Thus, while both fields have seen sustained research activity in identifying neuropsychological deficits, the focus of inquiry in TLE has been on verbal and visuospatial memory and in BD-I executive function.

Table 3.

Neuropsychological test findings summary table (for tests used more than once) in studies of bipolar disorder (BD) and temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE).

Measure BD
TLE
Pre- to post- surgical Laterality effects
Use in studies, number of participants BD BD < HC sig Use in studies, number of participants TLE sig
Executive function
WCST 12 studies 11 2 studies 1 ↑1
BD n = 859, healthy controls (HC) n = 676 Right temporal lobe (RTL) n = 44, left temporal lobe (LTL) n = 49, HC n = 22
COWAT(FAS) 5 studies 2 2 studies 2 ↑1 Higher score LTL group pre- assoc. with greater impairment post-surg
RTL n = 33, LTL n = 38
BD n = 133, HC n = 130
TMT-B 9 studies 7
BD n = 513, HC = 339
Stroop 10 studies 7
BD n = 895, HC = 642
Hayling Sentence Completion Test 2 studies 2
BD n = 25, HC n = 73
CANTAB: Intra Extra Dimensional Set Shifting 2 studies 2
BD n = 140, HC n = 78
CANTAB: Stockings Problem 2 studies 2
BD n = 140, HC n = 78

Verbal memory
CVLT 5 studies 3 7 studies 6 ↓6 Post-surg LTL decline 6
BD n = 233, HC n = 153 RTL n = 158, LTL n = 215, HC n = 10 RTL > LTL 1
RAVLT 3 studies 2 5 studies 5 ↓5 Post-surg LTL decline 5
BD n = 141, HC = 79 RTL n = 78, LTL n = 176, HC n = 28
Verbal comprehension: WAIS Vocab 4 studies 0
BD n = 187, HC n = 173
WMS-R: Logical Memory 6 studies 4 ↑3 ↓1 LTL < RTL 1
RTL n = 206, LTL n = 256, HC n = 28
WMS-R: Verbal Paired Associates 4 studies 4 ↑2 ↓1 LTL < RTL 1
RTL n = 119, LTL n = 151, HC n = 28
Token Test 2 studies 1 ↑1 Post-surg LTL improved 1
RTL n = 34, LTL n = 47
VLMT: Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest 2 studies 2 ↓2
RTL n = 83, LTL n = 82

Visuospatial memory
ROCFT 2 studies 0 3 studies 2 ↑1 LTL < RTL 1
BD n = 65, HC n = 47 RTL n = 25, LTL n = 62, HC n = 36
WMS: Visual reproduction 3 studies 0 2 studies 0
BD n = 105, HC n = 78 RTL n = 27, LTL n = 29
CANTAB: Spatial recognition memory 2 studies 2
BD n = 140, HC n = 78
CANTAB: Pattern Recognition Memory 2 studies 2
BD n = 140, HC n = 78
CANTAB: Paired Associates Learning 2 studies 2
BD n = 140, HC n = 78
WMS-R: Design Memory 4 studies 2 ↑1 ↓1 RTL decline 1
RTL n = 78, LTL n = 93, HC n = 28
WMS-R Visual Reproduction 5 studies 2 ↑1 ↓1 RTL decline 1
RTL n = 241, LTL n = 484, HC n = 28
WMS-III: Face Test 1 study 1 ↓1 RTL decline 1
RTL n = 16, LTL n = 10
Graduate Hospital Facial Memory 3 studies 3 ↑1 RTL < LTL 2
RTL n = 71, LTL n = 46, HC n = 10
Benton Visual Retention 2 studies 0
RTL n = 31, LTL n = 72
Diagnosticum für Cerebralschädigung 3 studies 2 ↓1
RTL n = 83, LTL n = 253

Spatial ability
WAIS: Block design 4 studies 0
BD n = 124, HC n = 109

Attention span
WAIS: Digit Span 3 studies 2 4 studies 0
BD n = 103, HC n = 89 RTL n = 93, LTL n = 150, HC n = 15
TMT-A (also processing speed) 10 studies 8
BD n = 513, HC = 339
WAIS: Digit Symbol (also processing speed) 3 studies 3
BD n = 91, HC = 86
CANTAB: Rapid Visual Information 2 studies 2
BD n = 140, HC n = 78
Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery (AMIPB) 2 studies 2 ↑1 ↓1
RTL n = 279, LTL n = 334

Sustained attention
CPT 4 studies 2
BD n = 84, HC n = 84

Working memory
SWMT 2 studies 0
BD n = 30, HC n = 30
N-Back 2 studies 0
BD n = 45, HC n = 47
WAIS: Letter Number Sequencing 3 studies 3
BD n = 126, HC n = 126
WMS-III: Letter Number Sequencing 2 studies 3
BD n = 140, HC n = 78
CANTAB: Spatial Working Memory 2 studies 2
BD n = 140, HC n = 78

refers to an increase, ↓ a decrease from pre- to post-surgery, sig refers to the number of studies with a significant result, laterality comments only significant main effects or interactions are discussed.

Notwithstanding the differences in the intensity and focus of neuropsychological testing in both conditions, consistent results emerging from this study emphasize deficits in verbal memory, which have been reported in the majority of studies that have examined this area in both BD-I and TLE.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review directly comparing the literature on cognitive function in BD-I and TLE. Consistent with the meta-analyses of cognition in euthymic BD-I, our review showed impairments on a wide range of cognitive measures. In the individual studies reviewed, the most commonly reported impairments in BD-I were in executive function, attention span, and verbal memory. The impairments of executive function in patients with BD-I may be suggestive of an underlying dysfunction in the prefrontal cortex (3).

In TLE studies, a decline in pre- to post-surgery in verbal memory was commonly reported in patients with seizures originating from the LTL, and less consistently, in visuospatial memory in patients with RTL epilepsy. Functional MRI studies have revealed that the right hemisphere is associated with spatial memory (91). In our sample, executive function was not widely examined in patients with epilepsy; however, an impairment was found. For executive function, two instruments were employed across four studies, three of which showed a difference between pre- to post-surgery scores and one study found impaired performance in TLE relative to HC participants.

In keeping with our results, meta-analyses of memory function pre- to post-surgery reported that, following a resection of the LTL, a clear decline in verbal memory is observed, an effect that is particularly salient for immediate verbal recall. However, the pattern of impairment following partial resection of the RTL showed a trend for improvement on tasks of non-verbal memory (92). This suggests that memory impairments are state markers affected by seizures and abnormalities in the temporal lobes. Other factors that affect cognitive performance in TLE are the chronicity of the condition, older age, lower intellect, and greater abnormalities shown on imaging (92). Another meta-analysis found that the evidence regarding post-surgery outcome on visuospatial memory following right anterior temporal lobectomy was less clear (93).

In TLE, it is unclear whether frontal lobe impairments shown on executive function measures are a product of temporal lobe involvement or are a side effect of the propagation of epileptic activity from the epileptic zone (94). Other evidence has suggested that the prefrontal cortex, in particular the orbitofrontal cortex, is influenced by ictal discharges from the mesial temporal lobe (95). Some studies have shown that the temporal neocortex is implicated in executive function implying that a frontotemporal network is used for processing information (96).

This review emphasizes that prior research on cognitive impairments in the fields of BD-I and TLE has employed methodologies that reflect different research questions. The BD-I literature predominantly examines cognition as a characteristic of the disorder itself, on a par with symptoms and potentially amenable to therapeutic intervention. The TLE literature is concerned with the effects of ablative surgery that aims to remove seizure foci but may consequently also directly affect healthy brain tracts. It addresses whether cognitive function improves or declines subsequent to surgery and the moderating effects of laterality.

The majority of BD-I studies compare euthymic patients with HC, whereas in the epilepsy studies, patients act as their own controls in relation to surgical intervention and laterality. In addition, given that the BD-I studies are interested in trait markers in the euthymic phase, they routinely report the quantitative differences between patients and HC, rendering the results suitable for incorporation in meta-analytic studies. The TLE literature has focused on statistically significant differences pre- to post-surgery and has not reported information in a form suitable for meta-analysis of pre-surgical cognitive functioning. Therefore, we have employed a systematic review methodology to examine and compare the profile of cognitive deficits in the two conditions. As indicated in the Section “Results,” the focus of inquiry of the neuropsychological tests employed in the BD-I studies is predominantly on frontal lobe functions and in TLE on verbal and visuospatial memory.

There are significant differences in the experimental designs examining cognition in the two patient groups. In BD-I studies, cognition is tested broadly with a wide range of measures, with a HC comparison group and statistical control for medication use. This allows for observed deficits to be interpreted as trait markers of BD-I. This is further supported by familial studies that show similar patterns of impairments among family members and patients (89). By contrast, studies assessing cognitive performance in pre-surgical patients with epilepsy do so generally without HC. Only five studies included HC (n = 111), consequently the findings have limited depth compared to those in the BD-I literature (n = 1,142). The pre-surgical neuropsychological workup consists mostly of tests of memory function, which is not surprising given that the surgery involves removing parts of or the whole temporal lobe.

In general, the studies on BD-I report the types of medications taken by patients at the time of testing (as shown in Table 1), which may have impacted the results. By contrast, in the epilepsy samples, it was typically not reported whether patients were receiving medication at the time of testing. A study that examined the effects of atypical antipsychotics on cognition in euthymic BD-I patients found that untreated patients showed better performance than those taking medication (93). Many patients with BD-I are treated with anticonvulsants that may worsen or enhance cognition (97). Of the total sample of 884 patients with BD-I included in the systematic review, 299 were taking lithium at the time of testing. A review of the effects of lithium on cognition found that impairments on tasks of psychomotor speed and verbal memory were present, whereas no effect was found on visuospatial ability or attention (97). Thus, cognitive performance may be impaired in various ways by different medications. A recent randomized crossover study examined the effects of methylene blue on cognition and mood-related symptoms in euthymic BD-I and BD-II. Neither low (15 mg) control doses nor high active doses (195 mg) had a significant effect on cognition (98). In rats, methylene blue prevents methylmalonate-induced seizures and oxidative damage in the striatum (99) providing interesting leads for future research into the overlaps between BD and epilepsy.

While this paper has provided an overview of the literature, it is subject to a number of limitations. One such factor is the differences of experimental designs in BD-I and TLE, which meant that a head-to-head meta-analytic comparison was not feasible. As discussed previously, the effect of medication was not uniformly controlled in BD-I and was either not reported or not systematically recorded in the epilepsy samples. In order to determine whether cognitive deficits are related to the illness and not undesirable side effect of medication, examination of otherwise stable drug-free patients would be of interest. The period of time between episodes (mania, depression, or seizures), time of testing, hospitalizations, and the presence of psychotic features were not considered in this review. In BD-I patients, the presence of sub-clinical symptoms is common, even in those patients who are rated as euthymic at the time of testing and may have impacted performance overall (3).

Although there is wide variation in the diagnostic criteria of euthymia, our study aimed to control for this by using the HAMD and YMRS as cutoffs; however, longitudinal measurements would have been advantageous to characterize proximity to manic or depressive episodes. Residual mood symptoms are also an important consideration in epilepsy where depression is the most common psychiatric comorbidity (18). In community-based samples, the rates of depression in epilepsy range from 20 to 30% and in hospital samples 20–55% (100, 101). It has been established that depression can cause cognitive impairments, particularly in the domains of attention, psychomotor activity, and memory all of which were relevant to this review (102).

We suggest a strong case may be made for a study comparing neuropsychological tests to assess deficits in BD-I, TLE, and matched HC. In future research, a comprehensive test battery employing tests of attention, executive function, memory, and psychomotor speed, coupled with imaging techniques, should be employed in both disorders relative to HC. This would provide valuable information on the effects of both BD-I and TLE on temporal and other cerebral areas as well as the effects of medication on neuropsychological test parameters. This would also be of value in identifying putative temporal lobe involvement in BD-I.

Author Contributions

EB, KK, MG, and BT contributed to the design of the project, the analysis and discussion of the results and write up of the paper with KK, MG, and BT contributing their specialist perspective. EB and KK assessed the suitability of papers for inclusion in the manuscript and contributed to the PRISMA review.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Funding. Author EB’s research is supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award and the Goddard Sapin-Jaloustre Trust.

References

  • 1.Clark L, Goodwin GM. State-and trait-related deficits in sustained attention in bipolar disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2004) 254(2):61–8. 10.1007/s00406-004-0460-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Malhi GS, Ivanovski B, Szekeres V, Olley A. Bipolar disorder: it’s all in your mind? The neuropsychological profile of a biological disorder. Can J Psychiatry (2004) 49(12):813–9. 10.1177/070674370404901204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Robinson LJ, Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Goswami U, Young AH, Ferrier IN, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord (2006) 93(1–3):105–15. 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Torres I, Boudreau V, Yatham L. Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl (2007) 116(434):17–26. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01055.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, Van Os J. Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol Med (2008) 38(06):771–85. 10.1017/S0033291707001675 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord (2009) 113(1):1–20. 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kurtz MM, Gerraty RT. A meta-analytic investigation of neurocognitive deficits in bipolar illness: profile and effects of clinical state. Neuropsychology (2009) 23(5):551. 10.1037/a0016277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mann-Wrobel MC, Carreno JT, Dickinson D. Meta-analysis of neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: an update and investigation of moderator variables. Bipolar Disord (2011) 13(4):334–42. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00935.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bourne C, Aydemir Ö, Balanzá-Martínez V, Bora E, Brissos S, Cavanagh JTO, et al. Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: an individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2013) 128(3):149–62. 10.1111/acps.12133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Krabbendam L, Arts B, van Os J, Aleman A. Cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a quantitative review. Schizophr Res (2005) 80(2–3):137–49. 10.1016/j.schres.2005.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Deckersbach T, Savage CR, Reilly-Harrington N, Clark L, Sachs G, Rauch SL. Episodic memory impairment in bipolar disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: the role of memory strategies. Bipolar Disord (2004) 6:233–44. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00118.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Jones BP, Duncan CC, Mirsky AF, Post RM, Theodore WH. Neuropsychological profiles in bipolar affective disorder and complex partial seizure disorder. Neuropsychology (1994) 8:55–64. 10.1037/0894-4105.8.1.55 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mazza M, Di Nicola M, Della Marca G, Janiri L, Bria P, Mazza S. Bipolar disorder and epilepsy: a bidirectional relation? Neurobiological underpinnings, current hypotheses, and future research directions. Neuroscientist (2007) 13(4):392–404. 10.1177/10738584070130041101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mula M, Marotta AE, Monaco F. Epilepsy and bipolar disorders. Expert Rev Neurother (2010) 10(1):13–23. 10.1586/ern.09.139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Silberman EK, Post R, Nurnberger J, Theodore W, Boulenger J. Transient sensory, cognitive and affective phenomena in affective illness. A comparison with complex partial epilepsy. Br J Psychiatry (1985) 146(1):81–9. 10.1192/bjp.146.1.81 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kanner AM, Palac S. Depression in epilepsy: a common but often unrecognized comorbid malady. Epilepsy Behav (2000) 1(1):37–51. 10.1006/ebeh.2000.0030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bakken IJ, Revdal E, Nesvåg R, Brenner E, Knudsen GP, Surén P, et al. Substance use disorders and psychotic disorders in epilepsy: a population-based registry study. Epilepsy Res (2014) 108(8):1435–43. 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2014.06.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lyketsos CG, Stoline AM, Longstreet P, Ranen NG, Lesser R, Fisher R, et al. Mania in temporal lobe epilepsy. Cogn Behav Neurol (1993) 6(1):19–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sucksdorff D, Brown AS, Chudal R, Jokiranta-Olkoniemi E, Leivonen S, Suominen A, et al. Parental and comorbid epilepsy in persons with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord (2015) 188:107–11. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bostock ECS, Kirkby KC, Garry MI, Taylor BVM. Comparison of precipitating factors for mania and partial seizures: indicative of shared pathophysiology? J Affect Disord (2015) 183:57–67. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.04.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McDonald C, Zanelli J, Rabe-Hesketh S, Ellison-Wright I, Sham P, Kalidindi S, et al. Meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometry studies in bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 56(6):411–7. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Brambilla P, Harenski K, Nicoletti M, Sassi RB, Mallinger AG, Frank E, et al. MRI investigation of temporal lobe structures in bipolar patients. J Psychiatr Res (2003) 37(4):287–95. 10.1016/S0022-3956(03)00024-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Altshuler LL, Bartzokis G, Grieder T, Curran J, Jimenez T, Leight K, et al. An MRI study of temporal lobe structures in men with bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48(2):147–62. 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00836-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Altshuler LL, Conrad A, Hauser P, Ximing L, Guze BH, Denikoff K, et al. Reduction of temporal lobe volume in bipolar disorder: a preliminary report of magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1991) 48(5):482–3. 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810290094018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Johnstone EC, Owens D, Crow TJ, Frith C, Alexandropolis K, Bydder G, et al. Temporal lobe structure as determined by nuclear magnetic resonance in schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1989) 52(6):736–41. 10.1136/jnnp.52.6.736 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Swayze VW, Andreasen NC, Alliger RJ, Yuh WT, Ehrhardt JC. Subcortical and temporal structures in affective disorder and schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry (1992) 31(3):221–40. 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90046-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Harvey I, Persaud R, Ron MA, Baker G, Murray R. Volumetric MRI measurements in bipolars compared with schizophrenics and healthy controls. Psychol Med (1994) 24(03):689–99. 10.1017/S0033291700027847 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hauser P, Matochik J, Altshuler LL, Denicoff KD, Conrad A, Li X, et al. MRI-based measurements of temporal lobe and ventricular structures in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. J Affect Disord (2000) 60(1):25–32. 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00154-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bernasconi N, Andermann F, Arnold DL, Bernasconi A. Entorhinal cortex MRI assessment in temporal, extratemporal, and idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsia (2003) 44(8):1070–4. 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2003.64802.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Natsume J, Bernasconi N, Andermann F, Bernasconi A. MRI volumetry of the thalamus in temporal, extratemporal, and idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Neurology (2003) 60(8):1296–300. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000058764.34968.C2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kuzniecky R, Bilir E, Gilliam F, Faught E, Martin R, Hugg J. Quantitative MRI in temporal lobe epilepsy evidence for fornix atrophy. Neurology (1999) 53(3):496–496. 10.1212/WNL.53.3.496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bernasconi N, Duchesne S, Janke A, Lerch J, Collins D, Bernasconi A. Whole-brain voxel-based statistical analysis of gray matter and white matter in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage (2004) 23(2):717–23. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Travers RF. Limbic epilepsy. J R Soc Med (1991) 84(8):454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Oyegbile TO, Dow C, Jones J, Bell B, Rutecki P, Sheth R, et al. The nature and course of neuropsychological morbidity in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology (2004) 62:1736–42. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000125186.04867.34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wang W, Liou H, Chen C, Chiu M, Chen T, Cheng T, et al. Neuropsychological performance and seizure-related risk factors in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: a retrospective cross-sectional study. Epilepsy Behav (2011) 22:728–34. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.08.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med (2009) 151(4):264–9. 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Altshuler LL, Ventura J, van Gorp WG, Green MF, Theberge DC, Mintz J. Neurocognitive function in clinically stable men with bipolar I disorder or schizophrenia and normal control subjects. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 56(8):560–9. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bas TO, Poyraz CA, Bas A, Poyraz BC, Tosun M. The impact of cognitive impairment, neurological soft signs and subdepressive symptoms on functional outcome in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord (2015) 174:336–41. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bora E, Hıdıroğlu C, Özerdem A, Kaçar ÖF, Sarısoy G, Arslan FC, et al. Executive dysfunction and cognitive subgroups in a large sample of euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2016) 26(8):1338–47. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chang JS, Choi S, Ha K, Ha TH, Cho HS, Choi JE, et al. Differential pattern of semantic memory organization between bipolar I and II disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2011) 35(4):1053–8. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.02.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chou YH, Wang SJ, Lirng JF, Lin CL, Yang KC, Chen CK, et al. Impaired cognition in bipolar I disorder: the roles of the serotonin transporter and brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J Affect Disord (2012) 143(3):131–7. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Deckersbach T, McMurrich S, Ogutha J, Savage CR, Sachs G, Rauch SL. Characteristics of non-verbal memory impairment in bipolar disorder: the role of encoding strategies. Psychol Med (2004) 34(5):823–32. 10.1017/S0033291703001685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dell’Osso B, Cinnante C, Di Giorgio A, Cremaschi L, Palazzo MC, Cristoffanini M, et al. Altered prefrontal cortex activity during working memory task in bipolar disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in euthymic bipolar I and II patients. J Affect Disord (2015) 184:116–22. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Dittmann S, Henning-Fast K, Gerber S, Seemüller F, Riedel M, Severus WE, et al. Cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II patients. Bipolar Disord (2008) 10(8):877–87. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00640.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Dixon T, Kravariti E, Frith C, Murray RM, McGuire PK. Effect of symptoms on executive function in bipolar illness. Psychol Med (2004) 34(5):811–21. 10.1017/S0033291703001570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Doganavşargil-Baysal GO, Zehra G, Akbaş H, Cinemre B, Metin O, Karaman T. Association of serum homocysteine and methionine levels with cognition and functioning in bipolar disorder. Turk Psikiyatri Derg (2013) 24(1):6–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Doruk A, Yazihan NT, Balikci A, Erdem M, Bolu A, Ates MA. Cognitive functions in bipolar manic, depressed and remission episodes. Klinik Psikofarmakoloji Bulteni (2014) 24(1):59–68. 10.5455/bcp.20130506122642 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Fistikci N, Canturk G, Saatcioglu O, Erten E, Keyvan A, Turan N. Executive functions and thyroid volumes in bipolar patients on lithium treatment. Afr J Psychiatry (South Africa) (2014) 17(6):145. 10.4172/Psychiatry.1000145 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Frangou S, Haldane M, Roddy D, Kumari V. Evidence for deficit in tasks of ventral, but not dorsal, prefrontal executive function as an endophenotypic marker for bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 58(10):838–9. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hsiao Y-L, Wu Y-S, Wu JY-W, Hsu M-H, Chen H-C, Lee S-Y, et al. Neuropsychological functions in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Bipolar Disord (2009) 11(5):547–54. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00723.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA, Torralva T, Manes F. Decision making in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Psychol Med (2011) 41(6):1319–27. 10.1017/S0033291710001832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Muralidharan K, Kozicky JM, Bücker J, Silveira LE, Torres IJ, Yatham LN. Are cognitive deficits similar in remitted early bipolar I disorder patients treated with lithium or valproate? Data from the STOP-EM study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2015) 25(2):223–30. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Muralidharan K, Torres IJ, Silveira LE, Kozicky JM, Bücker J, Fernando N, et al. Impact of depressive episodes on cognitive deficits in early bipolar disorder: data from the systematic treatment optimization programme for early mania (STOP-EM). Br J Psychiatry (2014) 205(1):36–43. 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.135525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Normala I, Abdul Hamid AR, Azlin B, Nik Ruzyanei NJ, Hazli Z, Shah SA. Executive function and attention span in euthymic patients with bipolar 1 disorder. Med J Malaysia (2010) 65(3):192–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Pattanayak RD, Sagar R, Mehta M. Neuropsychological performance in euthymic Indian patients with bipolar disorder type I: correlation between quality of life and global functioning. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2012) 66(7):553–63. 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2012.02400.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Radwan DN. Cognitive impairment in Egyptian euthymic patients with bipolar I disorder compared with controls. Middle East Curr Psychiatry (2013) 20(4):197–204. 10.1097/01.XME.0000433325.69290.c9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Sepede G, De Berardis D, Campanella D, Perrucci MG, Ferretti A, Serroni N, et al. Impaired sustained attention in euthymic bipolar disorder patients and non-affected relatives: an fMRI study. Bipolar Disord (2012) 14(7):764–79. 10.1111/bdi.12007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Trivedi JK, Dhyani M, Sharma S, Sinha PK, Singh AP, Tandon R. Cognitive functions in euthymic state of bipolar disorder: an Indian study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (2008) 13(2):135–47. 10.1080/13546800801897346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Trivedi JK, Goel D, Sharma S, Singh AP, Sinha PK, Tandon R. Cognitive functions in stable schizophrenia and euthymic state of bipolar disorder. Ind J Med Res (2007) 126(5):433–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Yates DB, Dittmann S, Kapczinski F, Trentini CM. Cognitive abilities and clinical variables in bipolar I depressed and euthymic patients and controls. J Psychiatr Res (2011) 45(4):495–504. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Zubieta J-K, Huguelet P, O’Neil RL, Giordani BJ. Cognitive function in euthymic bipolar I disorder. Psychiatry Res (2001) 102(1):9–20. 10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00242-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Baxendale S, Thompson P. Defining meaningful postoperative change in epilepsy surgery patients: measuring the unmeasurable? Epilepsy Behav (2005) 6(2):207–11. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2004.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Baxendale S, Thompson PJ, Sander JW. Neuropsychological outcomes in epilepsy surgery patients with unilateral hippocampal sclerosis and good preoperative memory function. Epilepsia (2013) 54(9):131–4. 10.1111/epi.12319 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Berenbaum SA, Baxter L, Seidenberg M, Hermann B. Role of the hippocampus in sex differences in verbal memory: memory outcome following left anterior temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychology (1997) 11(4):585–91. 10.1037/0894-4105.11.4.585 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Bjørnæs H, Stabell KE, Røste GK, Bakke SJ. Changes in verbal and nonverbal memory following anterior temporal lobe surgery for refractory seizures: effects of sex and laterality. Epilepsy Behav (2005) 6(1):71–84. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2004.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Chelune GJ, Naugle RI, Lüders H, Awad IA. Prediction of cognitive change as a function of preoperative ability status among temporal lobectomy patients seen at 6-month follow-up. Neurology (1991) 41(3):399–404. 10.1212/WNL.41.3.399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Chiaravalloti ND, Tulsky DS, Glosser G. Validation of the WMS-III facial memory subtest with the graduate hospital facial memory test in a sample of right and left anterior temporal lobectomy patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2004) 26(4):484–97. 10.1080/13803390490496623 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Chiaravalloti ND, Glosser G. Material-specific memory changes following anterior temporal lobectomy as predicted by the intracarotid amobarbital test. Epilepsia (2001) 42(7):902–11. 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2001.02500.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Fernandes DA, Yasuda CL, Lopes TM, Enrico G, Alessio A, Tedeschi H, et al. Long-term postoperative atrophy of contralateral hippocampus and cognitive function in unilateral refractory MTLE with unilateral hippocampal sclerosis. Epilepsy Behav (2014) 36:108–14. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.04.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Giovagnoli AR, Casazza M, Ciceri E, Avanzini G, Broggi G. Preserved memory in temporal lobe epilepsy patients after surgery for low-grade tumour. A pilot study. Neurol Sci (2007) 28(5):251–8. 10.1007/s10072-007-0831-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Gleissner U, Helmstaedter C, Schramm J, Elger CE. Memory outcome after selective amygdalohippocampectomy in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: one-year follow-up. Epilepsia (2004) 45(8):960–2. 10.1111/j.0013-9580.2004.42203.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Glosser G, Salvucci AE, Chiaravalloti ND. Naming and recognizing famous faces in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology (2003) 61(1):81–6. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000073621.18013.E1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Helmstaedter C, Grunwald T, Lehnertz K, Gleissner U, Elger CE. Differential involvement of left temporolateral and temporomesial structures in verbal declarative learning and memory: evidence from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Cogn (1997) 35(1):110–31. 10.1006/brcg.1997.0930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Hermann BP, Wyler AR. Effects of anterior temporal lobectomy on language function: a controlled study. Ann Neurol (1988) 23(6):585–8. 10.1002/ana.410230610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Hermann BP, Wyler AR, Bush AJ, Tabatabai FR. Differential effects of left and right anterior temporal lobectomy on verbal learning and memory performance. Epilepsia (1992) 33(2):289–97. 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1992.tb02318.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Hermann BP, Wyler AR, Somes G, Dohan FC, Jr, Berry AD, III, Clement L. Declarative memory following anterior temporal lobectomy in humans. Behav Neurosci (1994) 108(1):3–10. 10.1037/0735-7044.108.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Köylü B, Walser G, Ischebeck A, Ortler M, Benke T. Functional imaging of semantic memory predicts postoperative episodic memory functions in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Res (2008) 1223:73–81. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Lee T, Mackenzie RA, Walker AJ, Matheson JM, Sachdev P. Effects of left temporal lobectomy and amygdalohippocampectomy on memory. J Clin Neurosci (1997) 4(3):314–9. 10.1016/S0967-5868(97)90098-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Loring DW, Lee GP, Martin RC, Meador KJ. Material-specific learning in patients with partial complex seizures of temporal lobe origin: convergent validation of memory constructs. J Epilepsy (1988) 1(2):53–9. 10.1016/s0896-6974(88)80057-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Malikova H, Kramska L, Vojtech Z, Lukavsky J, Liscak R. Stereotactic radiofrequency amygdalohippocampectomy: two years of good neuropsychological outcomes. Epilepsy Res (2013) 106(3):423–32. 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2013.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Morino M, Ichinose T, Uda T, Kondo K, Ohfuji S, Ohata K. Memory outcome following transsylvian selective amygdalohippocampectomy in 62 patients with hippocampal sclerosis. J Neurosurg (2009) 110(6):1164–9. 10.3171/2008.9.jns08247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Quigg M, Broshek DK, Barbaro NM, Ward MM, Laxer KD, Yan G, et al. Neuropsychological outcomes after gamma knife radiosurgery for mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: a prospective multicenter study. Epilepsia (2011) 52(5):909–16. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2011.02987.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Seidenberg M, Hermann BP, Dohan FC, Jr, Wyler AR, Perrine A, Schoenfeld J. Hippocampal sclerosis and verbal encoding ability following anterior temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia (1996) 34(7):699–708. 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00146-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Selwa LM, Berent S, Giordani B, Henry TR, Buchtel HA, Ross DA. Serial cognitive testing in temporal lobe epilepsy: longitudinal changes with medical and surgical therapies. Epilepsia (1994) 35(4):743–9. 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1994.tb02505.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Shamim S, Wiggs E, Heiss J, Sato S, Liew C, Solomon J, et al. Temporal lobectomy: resection volume, neuropsychological effects, and seizure outcome. Epilepsy Behav (2009) 16(2):311–4. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.07.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Stretton J, Sidhu MK, Winston GP, Bartlett P, McEvoy AW, Symms MR, et al. Working memory network plasticity after anterior temporal lobe resection: a longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain (2014) 137(5):1439–53. 10.1093/brain/awu061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Tisser L, Palmini A, Paglioli E, Portuguez M, Azambuja N, da Costa JC, et al. Pre and post-operative Wisconsin card sorting test performance in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy due to hippocampal sclerosis. Dement Neuropsychol (2007) 1(2):173–80. 10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Trenerry MR, Jack CR. Wisconsin card sorting test performance before and after temporal lobectomy. J Epilepsy (1994) 7(4):313–7. 10.1016/0896-6974(94)90062-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Trenerry MR, Jack CR, Jr, Ivnik RJ, Sharbrough FW, Cascino GD, Hirschorn KA, et al. MRI hippocampal volumes and memory function before and after temporal lobectomy. Neurology (1993) 43(9):1800–5. 10.1212/WNL.43.9.1800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.von Rhein B, Nelles M, Urbach H, Von Lehe M, Schramm J, Helmstaedter C. Neuropsychological outcome after selective amygdalohippocampectomy: subtemporal versus transsylvian approach. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2012) 83(9):887–93. 10.1136/jnnp-2011-302025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.d’Esposito M, Aguirre G, Zarahn E, Ballard D, Shin R, Lease J. Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res (1998) 7(1):1–13. 10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00004-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Lee T, Yip JT, Jones-Gotman M. Memory deficits after resection from left or right anterior temporal lobe in humans: a meta-analytic review. Epilepsia (2002) 43(3):283–91. 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2002.09901.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Vaz SAM. Nonverbal memory functioning following right anterior temporal lobectomy: a meta-analytic review. Seizure (2004) 13(7):446–52. 10.1016/j.seizure.2003.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Devinsky O. The myth of silent cortex and the morbidity of epileptogenic tissue: implications for temporal lobectomy. Epilepsy Behav (2005) 7(3):383–9. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Lieb JP, Dasheiff RM, Engel J. Role of the frontal lobes in the propagation of mesial temporal lobe seizures. Epilepsia (1991) 32(6):822–37. 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1991.tb05539.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Rusnáková Š, Daniel P, Chládek J, Jurák P, Rektor I. The executive functions in frontal and temporal lobes: a flanker task intracerebral recording study. J Clin Neurophysiol (2011) 28(1):30–5. 10.1097/WNP.0b013e31820512d4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Pachet AK, Wisniewski AM. The effects of lithium on cognition: an updated review. Psychopharmacology (2003) 170(3):225–34. 10.1007/s00213-003-1592-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Alda M, McKinnon M, Blagdon R, Garnham J, MacLellan S, O’donovan C, et al. Methylene blue treatment for residual symptoms of bipolar disorder: randomised crossover study. Br J Psychiatry (2016) 210(1):54–60. 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.173930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Furian AF, Fighera MR, Oliveira MS, de Oliveira Ferreira AP, Fiorenza NG, de Carvalho Myskiw J, et al. Methylene blue prevents methylmalonate-induced seizures and oxidative damage in rat striatum. Neurochem Int (2007) 50(1):164–71. 10.1016/j.neuint.2006.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Blum D, Reed M, Metz A. Prevalence of major affective disorders and manic/hypomanic symptoms in persons with epilepsy: a community survey. Neurology (2002) 58(7):175. [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Kanner AM, Balabanov A. Depression and epilepsy how closely related are they? Neurology (2002) 58(8 Suppl 5):S27–39. 10.1212/WNL.58.8_suppl_5.S27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Gallassi R, Morreale A, Pagni P. The relationship between depression and cognition. Arch Gerontol Geriatr (2001) 33:163–71. 10.1016/S0167-4943(01)00136-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES