Abstract
Seafood intake in pregnancy has been positively associated with childhood cognitive outcomes which could potentially relate to the high vitamin-D content of oily fish. However, whether higher maternal vitamin D status [serum 25-hydroxy-vitamin D, 25(OH)D] in pregnancy is associated with a reduced risk of offspring suboptimal neurodevelopmental outcomes is unclear. A total of 7065 mother-child pairs were studied from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort who had data for both serum total 25(OH)D concentration in pregnancy and at least one measure of offspring neurodevelopment (pre-school development at 6–42 months; “Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire” scores at 7 years; IQ at 8 years; reading ability at 9 years). After adjustment for confounders, children of vitamin-D deficient mothers (< 50.0 nmol/L) were more likely to have scores in the lowest quartile for gross motor development at 30 months (OR 1.20 95% CI 1.03, 1.40), fine motor development at 30 months (OR 1.23 95% CI 1.05, 1.44), and social development at 42 months (OR 1.20 95% CI 1.01, 1.41) than vitamin-D sufficient mothers (≥ 50.0 nmol/L). No associations were found with neurodevelopmental outcomes, including IQ, measured at older ages. However, our results suggest that deficient maternal vitamin D status in pregnancy may have adverse effects on some measures of motor and social development in children under 4 years. Prevention of vitamin D deficiency may be important for preventing suboptimal development in the first 4 years of life.
Keywords: prenatal vitamin D, 25–hydroxy–vitamin D, motor development, social development, IQ and reading ability, ALSPAC
Introduction
The consumption of fish, or nutrients present in fish, by pregnant women has been linked to neurocognitive development in their children. In observational studies, maternal intake of fish or seafood in pregnancy has been positively associated with cognitive scores in the offspring(1; 2; 3; 4), while children whose mothers had eaten oily fish in early pregnancy had a reduced risk of hyperactivity than those whose mothers did not eat oily fish(3). While these studies tended to interpret these associations as effects of of long-chain omega-3 fatty acids, they might also be explained by the fact that oily fish is the best dietary source of vitamin D. Though the action of sunlight on the skin is the predominant contributor to vitamin D status, dietary vitamin D can play an important role in determining status, as measured by the vitamin D metabolite, 25-hydroxyvitamin D [25(OH)D]1, in serum or plasma(5). Dietary sources of vitamin D (especially oily fish) are particularly important during the winter months when endogenous production of vitamin D status is limited.
It is biologically plausible that vitamin D status in pregnant mothers may affect child neurocognitive development as vitamin D receptors are present in the brain(6) and maternal vitamin D deficiency is known to be associated with abnormal brain development in the young rat(7). In the period from birth to weaning in rats, there appears to be a window during which maternal vitamin D status affects offspring brain development(8) and these developmental changes may not occur if vitamin D is withheld until weaning(9). Furthermore, vitamin D deficiency in late gestation can lead to impaired brain function in adult rats(8). Due to differences between rat and human developmental physiology, the extent to which these findings would apply to humans remains unclear.
Few human studies have assessed the relationship between maternal vitamin D status and neurodevelopmental outcomes. The results of the five published observational studies that exist are inconsistent(10; 11; 12; 13; 14). Indeed, this fact was recently highlighted in the report from Public Health England on Vitamin D and Health from the Scientific Advisory Committee for Nutrition (SACN) (15).
To address this lack of consistent evidence with respect to the association between maternal vitamin D status and cognitive-developmental outcomes in the offspring, we analysed data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort. Our a priori hypothesis was that poorer maternal vitamin D status, as measured by serum 25(OH)D, would be associated with increased probability of suboptimal cognitive or behavioural development scores in childhood of 6 months to 9 years.
Subjects and Methods
Study Design and Participants
Details of ALSPAC methods have been detailed previously (16). In brief, all pregnant women living in the former Avon area in southwest England, who had an expected delivery date between April 1st 1991 and December 31st 1992 were eligible for inclusion. A total of 14,541 women were recruited, and there were 13,617 mother-child pairs with singleton offspring alive at one year. The ALSPAC study website contains details of all the data that are available through a fully searchable data dictionary (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/). Our study sample consisted of mother-child pairs that had both a serum 25(OH)D measure in pregnancy and at least one neurodevelopmental outcome of interest from 6 months to 9 years (Figure 1). A range of outcomes was explored, including motor development, communication and social skills, behaviour, cognition and reading ability.
Outcomes
The ALSPAC pre-school development tests, which were based on questionnaires completed by the mother when the child was between 6 and 42 months of age, provided scores for four domains: fine motor, gross motor, social development, and communication (details published previously(1)). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)(17) was completed by mothers when the child was 81 months of age and was used to assess behavioural development. Intelligence Quotient (IQ) at age 8 years had been assessed in the ALSPAC clinic using the abbreviated form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, as previously described (1). Reading ability (accuracy, comprehension and speed) was assessed at age 9 years by trained psychologists using the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability(18) and by asking children to read real words to derive a reading score. Further details of these outcomes are available in the Supplementary File.
Maternal vitamin D status
Although 25(OH)D has lower biological activity than the active vitamin D hormone, 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D [1,25(OH)2D], serum/plasma 25(OH)D is widely regarded as the most reliable marker of vitamin D status(19). Total maternal serum 25(OH)D concentration (including both vitamin D2 and vitamin D3) in ALSPAC mothers had been measured in a previous study by high-performance liquid chromatography and tandem mass-spectrometry, in accordance with Vitamin D External Quality Assessment Scheme (DEQAS) requirements; full details have been published previously(20), including details of inter-assay coefficients of variation(21).
Statistical analysis
The women with vitamin D measurements were compared to the remaining ALSPAC women. We compared categorical variables with χ2 tests and continuous variables with independent t-tests. We used median (IQR, Inter-quartile Range) to describe maternal vitamin D status. Our main analysis dichotomised women as deficient or sufficient using 25(OH)D concentration ≤ 50.0 nmol/L as the cut-off for vitamin D deficiency, as in previous ALSPAC work(20). We did additional supplementary analyses by dividing women into three categories (< 25.0, 25.0–49.9 and ≥ 50.0 nmol/L) to explore the dose-response relationship.
We used logistic regression to examine the relationship between maternal vitamin D status in pregnancy and odds of suboptimal development with the women in the vitamin-D-sufficient group (> 50.0 nmol/L) as the reference category. We did not input missing confounder or outcome data with replacement values. We defined suboptimal development as scores in the lowest quartile for all subscales of early development, IQ and reading ability, as in previous ALSPAC research (1; 22). For the SDQ, suboptimal behaviour was defined according to published cut-offs (for both the individual scales and overall score) that indicate borderline/abnormal behaviour (17) (see Supplementary File Study Outcomes). Model predictors were assessed for potential multicollinearity. For our final model, variance inflation factor ranged from 1.02 to 2.2 (accordingly tolerance ranged from 0.5-0.99) depending on the variable.
As vitamin D status and childhood cognitive and behavioural development are affected by a range of factors(23; 24), we included potential confounders in our analysis. The confounders chosen were based on previous ALSPAC findings(1; 22) and were from questionnaire and clinic-based data (Table 1). We included ten categorical and two continuous variables. The two continuous variables were maternal age (years), and maternal body mass index (BMI, Kg/m2). As there is a well-established relationship between BMI and 25(OH)D concentration(25), maternal BMI was included in the model, even though it was not statistically associated with 25(OH)D in this dataset (Table 1).
Table 1. Relationship between confounders and maternal Vitamin D status.
Confounder | Maternal vitamin D status | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
< 50.0 nmol/L | ≥ 50.0 nmol/L | ||||||
Mean | SD | n | Mean | SD | n | p value† | |
Age of mother (yrs) | 27.7 | 4.8 | 2443 | 28.6 | 4.7 | 4622 | < 0·0001 |
BMI of mother (Kg/m2) | 23.0 | 4.0 | 2126 | 22.9 | 3.6 | 4095 | 0.43 |
Gestation of vitamin D measure (weeks) | 23.4 | 10.9 | 2771 | 25.7 | 10.3 | 5174 | < 0·0001 |
% | n | % | n | p value ‡ | |||
Breastfeeding | |||||||
Some | 33·0% | 1738 | 67·0% | 3526 | < 0·0001 | ||
None | 38·8% | 553 | 61·2% | 874 | |||
Crowding in the home | |||||||
< one person per room | 33·9% | 2140 | 66·1% | 4170 | < 0·0001 | ||
One or more per room | 43.6% | 176 | 56·4% | 228 | |||
Education of mother | |||||||
Low | 37·5% | 716 | 62·5% | 1195 | < 0·0001 | ||
Medium | 33·4% | 792 | 66·6% | 1577 | |||
High | 31·5% | 755 | 68·5% | 1643 | |||
Ethnicity of mother | |||||||
White | 33·3% | 2171 | 66·7% | 4344 | < 0·0001 | ||
Non–white | 60·6% | 83 | 39·4% | 54 | |||
Gender of child | |||||||
Male | 34·3% | 1266 | 65·7% | 2421 | 0·67 | ||
Female | 34·8% | 1177 | 65·2% | 2201 | |||
Housing status | |||||||
Owned/mortgaged | 32·8% | 1705 | 67·2% | 3487 | < 0·0001 | ||
Other rented | 36·6% | 150 | 63·4% | 260 | |||
Council rented | 41·0% | 491 | 59·0% | 708 | |||
Iodine–to–creatinine ratio in 1st trimester | |||||||
<150 μg/g (deficient) | 33.5% | 186 | 66.5% | 374 | 0.94 | ||
≥150 μg/g (sufficient) | 33.2% | 76 | 66.8% | 151 | |||
Oily fish intake in pregnancy (/week) | |||||||
Never/rarely | 37·7% | 1038 | 62·3% | 1718 | < 0·0001 | ||
Once per fortnight or more | 31·3% | 1191 | 68·7% | 2617 | |||
Parity | |||||||
Zero | 37·0% | 1125 | 63·0% | 1914 | < 0·0001 | ||
One or more | 31·9% | 1179 | 68·1% | 2516 | |||
Season of vitamin D measure | |||||||
Spring | 48.8% | 980 | 51.2% | 1027 | < 0·0001 | ||
Summer | 15.2% | 268 | 84.8% | 1491 | |||
Autumn | 22.4% | 363 | 77.6% | 1257 | |||
Winter | 49.5% | 831 | 50.5% | 847 | |||
Smoking in 1st trimester | |||||||
No tobacco | 31·7% | 1652 | 68·3% | 3567 | < 0·0001 | ||
Smoked tobacco | 42·5% | 689 | 57·5% | 932 | |||
Social class of mother | |||||||
Manual | 36·6% | 383 | 63·4% | 664 | 0·01 | ||
Non–manual | 32·5% | 1447 | 67·5% | 3008 |
p value from independent t-test.
p value for χ2 test.
The ten categorical variables comprised three groups: (i) child factors [gender and breastfeeding (none or some)], (ii) maternal factors [ethnicity (white or non-white), tobacco use in the first trimester (smoker or non-smoker), parity (zero, one or more) and oily fish intake in pregnancy (never/rarely or once a fortnight or more)], and (iii) markers of socio–economic development [maternal education (low = less than O-level or equivalent; medium = O-level, and high = greater than O-level), home ownership (mortgaged/owned, privately rented or housing association/council rented/other), maternal social class based on her occupation (non-manual and manual) and crowding in the home (≤ one person or > one person per room)]. We also included two variables to control for variation in the vitamin D measurement: gestation (week) and season of sample collection [spring (March, April, and May), summer (June, July, and August) autumn (September, October, and November), and winter (December, January and February)]. While it is unlikely that the age of the child at assessment would be confounded by maternal vitamin D status, outcomes were adjusted for child age at the 6-month measurement, owing to the strong association between age and outcomes at this early life stage.
We used three models to adjust the analysis for potential confounders. As 25(OH)D measurements spanned pregnancy, and as gestational week is associated with vitamin D status(26), we do not present unadjusted data; our minimally adjusted model (Model 1) included gestational week of 25(OH)D measurement. Model 2 built on Model 1 by including nine confounders associated with both vitamin D status (Table 1) and cognitive development (parity, tobacco smoking, housing status, crowding, maternal age, BMI, education, ethnic group, and social class) and two child factors (gender and breastfeeding). Model 3 included Model 2 confounders plus two variables (oily fish intake and season of vitamin D measurement) that could affect maternal vitamin D status though including these may represent an over-control.
We used simulations to assess the impact of multiple comparisons. We generated 5000 datasets where 25(OH)D measurements were randomly permutated across valid observations with these data. As a consequence, all analyses maintained the same number of observations and, with all other data unchanged, the correlations between outcomes and confounders were preserved. The analyses were based upon Model 3. The effect of randomisation was to generate a set of results under the null hypothesis to which our set of observed results could be compared. A composite score across the 27 outcomes was based upon the sum of P values. These were modified to one-sided tests to allow results in the same direction to contribute consistently to the score, whether statistically significant or not. P values in the tables are not corrected for multiple comparisons.
Sensitivity analysis
We conducted analyses with two additional confounders (added to Model 3) that might be on the causal pathway: preterm birth (< 37 weeks or ≥ 37 weeks) and birth weight (< 2500 g or ≥ 2500 g). We also explored the effect of including maternal iodine status in the first trimester [sufficient (≥ 150 μg/g) or deficient (< 150 μg/g)] as we have previously shown that this is associated with child cognition in the ALSPAC cohort(22). As just 787 women also had a measure of iodine status in the first trimester, we used a simplified model (total of 13 confounders) to ensure that the model would converge (we dropped ethnicity and crowding in the home as a result of low numbers in the categories of those variables).
As there is ongoing controversy in the published literature with respect to the definition of vitamin D deficiency(27), we conducted sensitivity analyses using a wide range of vitamin D status, namely < 25.0 and < 75.0 nmol/L as cut–offs (Supplementary Tables 3 and 4). Assumptions concerning statistical significance were based on interpretation of confidence intervals, rather than P values, wherever possible, and multiple testing was assessed as described above. Analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (version 21·0; SPSS, Inc., Chicago, USA).
Ethics
The ALSPAC study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Written informed consent was obtained from participants (or from their parent/guardian if under 18 years old).
Role of the funding source
The funding bodies did not have a role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had full access to all the study data used and final responsibility to submit for publication.
Results
Compared with the remainder of the ALSPAC cohort (defined as mother-singleton child pairs from the core sample surviving to one year), the mother-child pairs in this study were more likely to be older, of white ethnicity, with markers of higher socio-economic status [e.g. a higher proportion of breast-feeding mothers, higher educational attainment and social class, and a lower proportion of smokers (Supplementary Table 1)]. However, some of the actual differences were small (e.g. maternal age 28.3 (4.8) vs. 27.7 (4.7) years). The median (IQR) 25(OH)D concentration for all 7065 women with a child that had at least one relevant outcome was 61.3 (42.9 – 84.7) nmol/L, with 4.4% having < 25.0 nmol/L, 34.6% having < 50.0 nmol/L and 65.7% having < 75.0 nmol/L.
The median (IQR) gestational week of vitamin D measurement (available for 7064 women) was 29.6 (12.7, 33.3) weeks, with 26.1% in the first trimester (≤ 13 weeks), 11.8% in the second trimester (14 – 27 weeks) and 62.1% in the third trimester (≥ 28 weeks). The median (IQR) 25(OH)D measurement was 54.9 (40.1 - 72.5) nmol/L in the first trimester, 59.3 (38.6 - 84.2) nmol/L in the second trimester and 65.3 (45.2 - 90.4) nmol/L in the third trimester. Table 1 shows the confounders associated with maternal vitamin D status using the 50 nmol/L cut-off. Women with 25(OH)D concentration ≥ 50.0 nmol/L were more likely to be white, older, and have markers of higher socio-economic status (for example education, home ownership and reduced smoking and crowding).
Results of logistic regression models using the cut-off value for serum 25(OH)D of <50.0 nmol/L to define deficiency are shown in Table 2. In the minimally adjusted analysis (Model 1), the only outcomes associated with vitamin D status were verbal IQ at 8 years and words read per minute at age 9 (Table 2). However, after adjustment for potential confounders, the effect on IQ and reading was attenuated and the only outcomes that remained statistically significant were gross- and fine-motor development at 30 months and social development at 42 months. With further adjustment for oily-fish intake and season (Model 3), the association between maternal vitamin D status and gross-motor development also became significant at 18 months, while remaining associated with gross-motor and fine-motor development at 30 months and social development at 42 months (Table 2). Children born to mothers with 25(OH)D ≤ 50. 0 nmol/L were more likely to have scores in the bottom quartile for these variables.
Table 2. Odds of suboptimal outcomes according to maternal vitamin D status (< 50.0 vs ≥ 50.0 nmol/L), minimally and fully adjusted for potential confounders.
Model 1† | Model 2‡ | Model 3§ | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | OR (95% CI) | p value | n | OR (95% CI) | p value | n | OR (95% CI) | p value | n | ||
ALSPAC pre–school development assessments | Gross Motor Skills | 6 mo ‖ | 0.96 (0.84, 1.09) | 0.49 | 6242 | 1.01 (0.86, 1.18) | 0.92 | 4383 | 0.96 (0.81, 1.13) | 0.59 | 4380 |
18 mo | 0.98 (0.87, 1.10) | 0.74 | 6269 | 1.10 (0.96, 1.27) | 0.18 | 4385 | 1.17 (1.01, 1.36) | 0.04 | 4383 | ||
30 mo | 1.02 (0.91, 1.16) | 0.71 | 5843 | 1.16 (1.00, 1.34) | 0.05 | 4135 | 1.20 (1.03, 1.40) | 0.02 | 4133 | ||
42 mo | 0.99 (0.87, 1.13) | 0.89 | 5695 | 1.04 (0.89, 1.22) | 0.60 | 4073 | 1.09 (0.92, 1.28) | 0.31 | 4070 | ||
Fine Motor Skills | 6 mo ‖ | 0.93 (0.82, 1.05) | 0.24 | 5880 | 1.07 (0.92, 1.25) | 0.39 | 4141 | 1.06 (0.91, 1.25) | 0.47 | 4139 | |
18 mo | 1.07 (0.96, 1.21) | 0.24 | 6268 | 1.03 (0.90, 1.19) | 0.65 | 4383 | 1.09 (0.94, 1.27) | 0.26 | 4381 | ||
30 mo | 1.09 (0.96, 1.23) | 0.18 | 5854 | 1.20 (1.04, 1.40) | 0.02 | 4138 | 1.23 (1.05, 1.44) | 0.01 | 4136 | ||
42 mo | 1.04 (0.92, 1.19) | 0.51 | 5692 | 1.11 (0.95, 1.31) | 0.19 | 4071 | 1.16 (0.98, 1.37) | 0.08 | 4068 | ||
Social Development | 6 mo ‖ | 0.96 (0.84, 1.09) | 0.52 | 6010 | 1.02 (0.87, 1.19) | 0.81 | 4209 | 1.00 (0.85, 1.18) | 0.98 | 4207 | |
18 mo | 1.01 (0.89, 1.15) | 0.86 | 6268 | 1.10 (0.94, 1.28) | 0.22 | 4383 | 1.14 (0.97, 1.34) | 0.11 | 4381 | ||
30 mo | 0.97 (0.86, 1.10) | 0.64 | 5843 | 1.11 (0.95, 1.30) | 0.18 | 4129 | 1.07 (0.91, 1.27) | 0.42 | 4127 | ||
42 mo | 1.04 (0.92, 1.18) | 0.54 | 5689 | 1.19 (1.02, 1.39) | 0.03 | 4069 | 1.20 (1.01, 1.41) | 0.04 | 4066 | ||
Communication | 6 mo ‖ | 0.99 (0.85, 1.15) | 0.90 | 6100 | 0.99 (0.83, 1.20) | 0.95 | 4285 | 0.99 (0.81, 1.20) | 0.90 | 4283 | |
18 mo | 0.99 (0.87, 1.12) | 0.85 | 6279 | 1.11 (0.96, 1.29) | 0.17 | 4390 | 1.12 (0.95, 1.31) | 0.18 | 4388 | ||
Behaviour | Prosocial | 7 yr | 0.92 (0.75, 1.13) | 0.40 | 4791 | 0.97 (0.75, 1.24) | 0.78 | 3513 | 1.00 (0.77, 1.31) | 0.98 | 3511 |
Peer problems | 7 yr | 1.05 (0.88, 1.25) | 0.58 | 4785 | 1.03 (0.83, 1.27) | 0.80 | 3510 | 1.05 (0.83, 1.31) | 0.70 | 3508 | |
Hyperactivity | 7 yr | 1.06 (0.91, 1.24) | 0.47 | 4780 | 1.04 (0.86, 1.26) | 0.68 | 3513 | 1.04 (0.85, 1.26) | 0.74 | 3511 | |
Emotional | 7 yr | 1.17 (0.98, 1.41) | 0.09 | 4785 | 1.14 (0.92, 1.42) | 0.23 | 3511 | 1.20 (0.95, 1.51) | 0.12 | 3509 | |
Conduct | 7 yr | 1.13 (0.99, 1.30) | 0.08 | 4790 | 1.05 (0.88, 1.24) | 0.60 | 3514 | 1.06 (0.89, 1.27) | 0.50 | 3512 | |
Total Score | 7 yr | 1.08 (089, 1.32) | 0.42 | 4777 | 1.13 (0.89, 1.44) | 0.31 | 3510 | 1.24 (0.96, 1.60) | 0.09 | 3508 | |
Cognition | Verbal IQ | 8 yr | 1.19 (1.02, 1.39) | 0.03 | 3997 | 1.08 (0.89, 1.31) | 0.47 | 2952 | 1.00 (0.82, 1.23) | 0.98 | 2950 |
Performance IQ | 8 yr | 1.06 (0.91, 1.24) | 0.43 | 3990 | 0.99 (0.82, 1.20) | 0.92 | 2945 | 1.00 (0.82, 1.23) | 0.98 | 2943 | |
Total IQ | 8 yr | 1.16 (1.00, 1.35) | 0.06 | 3978 | 1.02 (0.84, 1.24) | 0.82 | 2938 | 1.01 (0.82, 1.24) | 0.93 | 2936 | |
Reading ability | Words per min | 9 yr | 1.17 (1.00, 1.36) | 0.05 | 3794 | 1.14 (0.94, 1.39) | 0.18 | 2763 | 1.15 (0.94, 1.42) | 0.17 | 2761 |
Accuracy | 9 yr | 1.16 (0.99, 1.35) | 0.07 | 3802 | 1.04 (0.85, 1.28) | 0.69 | 2767 | 1.03 (0.83, 1.27) | 0.80 | 2765 | |
Comprehension | 9 yr | 1.11 (0.95, 1.30) | 0.18 | 3802 | 1.02 (0.83, 1.25) | 0.87 | 2767 | 1.04 (0.84, 1.29) | 0.73 | 2765 | |
Reading Score | 9 yr | 1.10 (0.95, 1.27) | 0.22 | 4125 | 1.06 (0.88, 1.27) | 0.54 | 3028 | 1.04 (0.86, 1.26) | 0.69 | 3026 |
mo, month; OR, odds ratio; n, number of subjects; yr, years. Suboptimal outcome defined as scores in the bottom quartile for ALSPAC pre–school development assessments, cognition, and reading ability. Published cut-offs(17) were used for behaviour: Prosocial (≤5; 9·8%), Peer problems (≥3; 13·5%), hyperactivity (≥6; 18·7%), emotional symptoms (≥4; 12·2%), conduct problems (≥3; 24·3%), and total score (≥14; 10·5%). Maternal vitamin D status >50.0 nmol/L was the reference group.
Model 1 adjusted for gestational week of vitamin D measurement
Model 2: gestational week of vitamin D measurement plus additional 11 variables: maternal age, maternal BMI, maternal ethnic group, maternal education, maternal social class, parity, tobacco smoking in 1st trimester, home ownership status, crowding index, child gender, breastfeeding
Model 3: additionally adjusted for oily fish and season of vitamin D measurement
age of child at development test included in all models.
For the ALSPAC pre-school development assessments, when the serum 25(OH)D of < 50.0 nmol/L group was divided into < 25.0 and 25.0 – 49.9 nmol/L, there was evidence of a statistically significant trend to decreasing risk of suboptimal development with higher maternal 25(OH)D concentration for gross-motor skills at 18 (P=0.02) and 30 months (P=0.008), fine-motor skills at 30 months (P=0.01) and social development at 42 months (P=0.02), after adjustment for all 12 confounders in Model 3 (Table 3). The effect sizes were larger for odds of suboptimal development in children of mothers in the serum 25(OH)D < 25.0 nmol/L group, than for the serum 25(OH)D of 25.0 – 49.9 nmol/L group (with the ≥ 50.0 nmol/L group as the comparison group) for all outcomes except fine-motor development at 18 months and social development at 30 months.
Table 3. Odds of suboptimal outcomes in offspring according to maternal vitamin D status when the < 50.0 nmol/L group is split into < 25.0 and 25.0 – 49.9 nmol/L and each group is compared to ≥ 50.0 nmol/L (adjusted model 3).
Maternal vitamin D status (nmol/L) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
< 25.0 vs. ≥ 50.0 | 25.0 – 49.9 vs. ≥ 50.0 | Trend | ||||||
OR (95% CI) | n | OR (95% CI) | n | p value | n | |||
ALSPAC pre–school development assessments | Gross Motor Skills | 6 mo† | 1.30 (0.90, 1.88) | 169 | 0.92 (0.77, 1.09) | 1279 | 0.88 | 4380 |
18 mo | 1.40 (1.00, 1.96) | 178 | 1.14 (0.98, 1.33) | 1270 | 0.02 | 4383 | ||
30 mo | 1.52 (1.07, 2.17) | 163 | 1.17 (0.99, 1.37) | 1213 | 0.008 | 4133 | ||
42 mo | 1.24 (0.85, 1.82) | 159 | 1.07 (0.90, 1.27) | 1191 | 0.23 | 4070 | ||
Fine Motor Skills | 6 mo† | 1.24 (0.85, 1.80) | 167 | 1.04 (0.88, 1.24) | 1213 | 0.32 | 4139 | |
18 mo | 1.03 (0.72, 1.47) | 177 | 1.10 (0.94, 1.29) | 1269 | 0.36 | 4381 | ||
30 mo | 1.30 (0.91, 1.88) | 163 | 1.22 (1.04, 1.44) | 1214 | 0.01 | 4136 | ||
42 mo | 1.31 (0.89, 1.92) | 158 | 1.14 (0.96, 1.36) | 1191 | 0.06 | 4068 | ||
Social Development | 6 mo† | 1.02 (0.70, 1.50) | 170 | 1.00 (0.84, 1.19) | 1216 | 0.95 | 4207 | |
18 mo | 1.28 (0.88, 1.85) | 177 | 1.12 (0.95, 1.33) | 1269 | 0.08 | 4381 | ||
30 mo | 0.91 (0.61, 1.36) | 163 | 1.09 (0.92, 1.30) | 1212 | 0.66 | 4127 | ||
42 mo | 1.49 (1.02, 2.18) | 158 | 1.16 (0.98, 1.38) | 1190 | 0.02 | 4066 | ||
Communication | 6 mo† | 1.41 (0.93, 2.14) | 167 | 0.94 (0.77, 1.16) | 1237 | 0.59 | 4283 | |
18 mo | 1.31 (0.92, 1.88) | 179 | 1.09 (0.93, 1.29) | 1272 | 0.11 | 4388 | ||
Behaviour | Prosocial | 7 yr | 1.11 (0.59, 2.09) | 124 | 0.99 (0.75, 1.30) | 1003 | 0.89 | 3511 |
Peer problems | 7 yr | 0.97 (0.56, 1.67) | 124 | 1.05 (0.84, 1.33) | 1002 | 0.80 | 3508 | |
Hyperactivity | 7 yr | 0.63 (0.37, 1.08) | 124 | 1.09 (0.89, 1.33) | 1002 | 0.70 | 3511 | |
Emotional | 7 yr | 0.80 (0.43, 1.49) | 124 | 1.25 (0.99, 1.57) | 1002 | 0.34 | 3509 | |
Conduct | 7 yr | 0.80 (0.50, 1.27) | 124 | 1.10 (0.91, 1.32) | 1003 | 0.88 | 3512 | |
Total Score | 7 yr | 0.68 (0.33, 1.39) | 124 | 1.31 (1.02, 1.70) | 1001 | 0.37 | 3508 | |
Cognition | Verbal IQ | 8 yr | 1.07 (0.67, 1.73) | 103 | 0.99 (0.80, 1.23) | 839 | 0.90 | 2950 |
Performance IQ | 8 yr | 1.40 (0.89, 2.20) | 104 | 0.96 (0.78, 1.18) | 837 | 0.56 | 2943 | |
Total IQ | 8 yr | 1.37 (0.87, 2.17) | 103 | 0.97 (0.78, 1.20) | 834 | 0.54 | 2936 | |
Reading ability | Words per min | 9 yr | 1.11 (0.68, 1.80) | 101 | 1.16 (0.94, 1.43) | 797 | 0.23 | 2761 |
Accuracy | 9 yr | 1.14 (0.70, 1.87) | 101 | 1.02 (0.81, 1.27) | 799 | 0.69 | 2765 | |
Comprehension | 9 yr | 1.01 (0.61, 1.66) | 101 | 1.04 (0.84, 1.30) | 799 | 0.78 | 2765 | |
Reading Score | 9 yr | 0.91 (0.57, 1.45) | 108 | 1.06 (0.87, 1.29) | 872 | 0.88 | 3026 |
mo, month; OR, odds ratio; n, number of subjects; yr, years. Suboptimal outcome defined as scores in the bottom quartile for ALSPAC pre–school development assessments, cognition, and reading ability. Published cut-offs(17) were used for behaviour: Prosocial (≤5; 9·8%), Peer problems (≥3; 13·5%), hyperactivity (≥6; 18·7%), emotional symptoms (≥4; 12·2%), conduct problems (≥3; 24·3%), and total score (≥14; 10·5%). Maternal vitamin D status ≥ 50.0 nmol/L was the reference group.
age of child at development test included in all models.
The interaction between gestational week of 25(OH)D measurement and the vitamin D variable (i.e. deficient vs. sufficient status) was significant for only two of 27 outcomes: fine-motor skills at 30 months and performance IQ (Table 4). However, when the analysis was restricted to the ALSPAC pre-school development assessments and was split into early (≤22 weeks) and late gestation (> 22 weeks), the results suggested that the effect of deficient vs. sufficient vitamin D status on the majority of tests was greater in the second half of gestation. The effect sizes were generally larger in the second half of gestation and results were significant (Table 4) for gross motor development at 18 months (Odds Ratio (OR) 0.97, 95% CI 0.76, 1.23 vs. OR 1.31, 95% 1.08, 1.58) and 30 months (OR 1.07, 95% CI 0.84,1.38 vs OR 1.28, 95% CI 1.05,1.57), fine motor development at 30 months (0.99, 95%CI 0.76,1.29 vs OR. 1.37, 95% CI 1.12,1.67) and social development at 42 months (OR 1.07, 95% CI 0.82,1.41 vs. OR 1.28, 95% CI 1.03,1.58). There were no significant associations in either half of gestation for other neurodevelopmental outcomes, including the SDQ, IQ or reading ability (Table 4).
Table 4. Odds of suboptimal outcomes in offspring by maternal vitamin D status (< 50.0 vs ≥ 50.0 nmol/L) according to whether maternal vitamin D was measured in the first or second half of gestation (Adjusted Model 3).
First half of gestation (≤ 22 weeks) | Second half of gestation (> 22 weeks) | P value for interaction* | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR (95% CI) | P value | n | OR (95% CI) | P value | n | ||||
ALSPAC pre–school development assessments | Gross Motor Skills | 6 mo† | 0.92 (0. 70, 1.22) | 0.56 | 1500 | 0.98 (0.79, 1.21) | 0.84 | 2880 | 0.21 |
18 mo | 0.97 (0.76, 1.23) | 0.78 | 1522 | 1.31 (1.08, 1.58) | 0.005 | 2861 | 0.13 | ||
30 mo | 1.07 (0.84, 1.38) | 0.58 | 1435 | 1.28 (1.05, 1.57) | 0.02 | 2698 | 0.79 | ||
42 mo | 1.03 (0.79, 1.34) | 0.85 | 1422 | 1.10 (0.89, 1.36) | 0.37 | 2648 | 0.72 | ||
Fine Motor Skills | 6 mo† | 1.09 (0.83, 1.44) | 0.52 | 1436 | 1.03 (0.83, 1.27) | 0.80 | 2703 | 0.25 | |
18 mo | 1.05 (0.82, 1.36) | 0.69 | 1522 | 1.10 (0.90, 1.33) | 0.35 | 2859 | 0.46 | ||
30 mo | 0.99 (0.76, 1.29) | 0.95 | 1436 | 1.37 (1.12, 1.67) | 0.002 | 2700 | 0.05 | ||
42 mo | 1.03 (0.78, 1.37) | 0.83 | 1420 | 1.24 (1.00, 1.53) | 0.05 | 2648 | 0.37 | ||
Social Development | 6 mo† | 0.88 (0.66, 1.16) | 0.37 | 1453 | 1.11 (0.90, 1.38) | 0.32 | 2754 | 0.90 | |
18 mo | 1.23 (0.95, 1.60) | 0.12 | 1522 | 1.07 (0.87, 1.32) | 0.51 | 2859 | 0.11 | ||
30 mo | 0.96 (0.74, 1.26) | 0.79 | 1431 | 1.13 (0.91, 1.40) | 0.28 | 2696 | 0.36 | ||
42 mo | 1.07 (0.82, 1.41) | 0.62 | 1420 | 1.28 (1.03, 1.58) | 0.02 | 2646 | 0.26 | ||
Communication | 6 mo† | 0.90 (0.65, 1.23) | 0.50 | 1468 | 1.04 (0.81, 1.34) | 0.75 | 2815 | 0.37 | |
18 mo | 1.27 (0.98, 1.65) | 0.07 | 1524 | 1.04 (0.85, 1.28) | 0.71 | 2864 | 0.17 | ||
Behaviour | Prosocial‡ | 7 yr | 0.75 (0.48, 1.17) | 0.21 | 1216 | 1.15 (0.83, 1.61) | 0.40 | 2301 | 0.10 |
Peer problems | 7 yr | 1.14 (0.78, 1.66) | 0.49 | 1210 | 0.97 (0.73, 1.30) | 0.86 | 2298 | 0.55 | |
Hyperactivity | 7 yr | 0.95 (0.68, 1.33) | 0.75 | 1213 | 1.10 (0.86, 1.41) | 0.46 | 2298 | 0.31 | |
Emotional‡ | 7 yr | 1.25 (0.87, 1.80) | 0.23 | 1214 | 1.17 (0.87, 1.58) | 0.29 | 2301 | 0.71 | |
Conduct | 7 yr | 1.13 (0.84, 1.52) | 0.42 | 1212 | 1.04 (0.82, 1.31) | 0.74 | 2300 | 0.76 | |
Total Score‡ | 7 yr | 1.20 (0.79, 1.82) | 0.40 | 1214 | 1.24 (0.90, 1.71) | 0.18 | 2300 | 0.79 | |
Cognition | Verbal IQ | 8 yr | 1.09 (0.77, 1.55) | 0.64 | 1025 | 0.93 (0.72, 1.21) | 0.60 | 1925 | 0.20 |
Performance IQ | 8 yr | 1.15 (0.83, 1.59) | 0.42 | 1017 | 0.89 (0.68, 1.16) | 0.38 | 1926 | 0.03 | |
Total IQ | 8 yr | 1.18 (0.84, 1.66) | 0.33 | 1015 | 0.90 (0.69, 1.17) | 0.43 | 1921 | 0.13 | |
Reading ability | Words per min | 9 yr | 1.41 (1.00, 1.97) | 0.05 | 936 | 1.00 (0.77, 1.31) | 0.98 | 1825 | 0.20 |
Accuracy | 9 yr | 1.31 (0.92, 1.87) | 0.13 | 938 | 0.87 (0.66, 1.14) | 0.32 | 1827 | 0.06 | |
Comprehension | 9 yr | 1.09 (0.77, 1.55) | 0.62 | 938 | 0.98 (0.75, 1.29) | 0.89 | 1827 | 0.31 | |
Reading Score | 9 yr | 1.30 (0.94, 1.78) | 0.11 | 1060 | 0.91 (0.71, 1.16) | 0.44 | 1966 | 0.20 |
mo, month; OR, odds ratio; n, number of subjects; yr, years. Suboptimal outcome defined as scores in the bottom quartile for ALSPAC pre–school development assessments, cognition, and reading ability. Published cut-offs(17) were used for behaviour: Prosocial (≤5; 9·8%), Peer problems (≥3; 13·5%), hyperactivity (≥6; 18·7%), emotional symptoms (≥4; 12·2%), conduct problems (≥3; 24·3%), and total score (≥14; 10·5%). Maternal vitamin D status ≥ 50.0 nmol/L was the reference group and Model 3 was used (without gestational week of vitamin D assessment as this was used to split analyses).
interaction between vitamin D (deficient/sufficient) and gestational week of sample (continuous variable)
age of child at development test included in all models
ethnicity removed as model would not converge.
Multiple comparisons
While only 4 results in Table 2 were nominally significant at the 5% level, it was noted that 25 of the 27 results in Model 3 showed a detrimental effect for low vitamin D status. Such a result would be highly significant (p<0.0001) if the outcomes were independent. In practice, outcomes were correlated with an average r = 0.12 (range –0.03 to 0.69). The impact of these correlations was assessed using simulations. The scores from the 5000 simulated datasets had a mean (SD) of 13.52 (2.78). This compared to an expected mean (SD) of 13.5 (1.5) if all the outcomes had been independent. The observed results had a score of 6.93 suggesting an empirical two-tail P value of 0.016. Sequential analyses by removing those outcomes with the strongest association from the simulated scores suggested that three outcomes (gross and fine motor development at 30 months and social development at 42 months) had robust associations with the other 24 outcomes having associations consistent with chance (p=0.051).
We also explored defining the score based upon the logit transformation, ln(p/(1-p)). Using this definition, the score more closely approximated to a normal distribution. However this did not change the conclusions.
Sensitivity analysis
When we added the variables, preterm birth and birth weight, to Model 3, the results were fundamentally unchanged (Supplementary Table 2), though the effect of maternal vitamin D status on gross motor development at 18 months and social development at 42 months was no longer statistically significant.
The addition of suboptimal iodine-to-creatinine ratio in the first trimester to Model 3 resulted in considerable sample attrition given the low number of women with iodine measurements (n=787) (Supplementary Table 2). Though the effect sizes were larger than previously, the associations between maternal vitamin D and gross motor development at 18 and 30 months and social development at 42 months were no longer significant, though they remained significant for fine motor development at 18 (OR 1.50, 95% CI 1.02, 2.23) and 30 months (OR 1.61, 95% CI 1.06, 2.46).
We explored whether dichotomising women according to different 25(OH)D cut-offs (25.0 or 75.0 nmol/L) changed the results (Supplementary Tables 3 and 4), bearing in mind the lower relative statistical power that results when the cut-off leads to unequal numbers in each group (the 50.0 nmol/L cut-off was close to the median 25(OH)D concentration of 54.9 nmol/L). When using the 25.0 nmol/L cut-off, the only outcome associated with vitamin D deficiency in the fully adjusted model was gross motor development at 30 months (OR 1.43 95% CI 1.01-2.02); results approached statistical significance for other outcomes (e.g. social development at 42 months, OR 1.40 95% CI 0.97-2.02; Supplementary Table 3). Using a cut-off of 75.0 nmol/L to define deficiency resulted in null associations with the ALSPAC pre-school development assessments, behaviour and cognitive tests, but was associated with higher odds of sub-optimal reading accuracy at 9 years (OR 1.26 95% CI 1.01, 1.57); however, this may be a chance finding as reading accuracy was not associated with vitamin D in any other analyses (Tables 2, 3 and 4 and Supplementary Tables 2 and 3).
Discussion
After adjustment for potential confounders, children born to vitamin-D deficient mothers (serum 25(OH)D of <50.0 nmol/L) were more likely to have sub-optimal gross-motor skills at 30 months, sub-optimal fine-motor skills at 30 months and sub-optimal social development scores at 42 months than were children born to sufficient mothers (≥50.0 nmol/L). Although the effect sizes were relatively small, we consider that the findings were biologically meaningful. Interestingly, no associations were found between maternal vitamin D status and other outcomes (IQ, reading ability).
These results suggest that the vitamin D content of seafood might explain some of the beneficial effects of maternal seafood consumption seen previously in ALSPAC, at least for fine-motor skills at 30 months and social skills at 42 months(1). The classification of maternal seafood consumption by Hibbeln et al.(1) included white fish and shellfish which are not good sources of dietary vitamin D, therefore, we would not expect vitamin D intake to account totally for their findings. Furthermore, our results cannot explain previous associations found in ALSPAC between maternal seafood consumption and IQ(1) or between maternal iodine status and IQ and reading ability(22).
Our findings on fine- and gross-motor skills support previous non-ALSPAC-based research that found a positive association between maternal vitamin D status and infant psychomotor development(11). Although we did not specifically measure scholastic achievement, the lack of an association between maternal vitamin D status and either reading ability or IQ in our study reinforces the findings of a previous study that found no relationship between maternal 25(OH)D status and offspring scholastic achievement(10). While a US study found a relationship between maternal vitamin D status and offspring IQ, the effect estimates were very small and there was very little indication of an association between maternal blood 25(OH)D and cognitive development, achievement, or behaviour between 8 months and 7 years of age(12).
Our findings suggest that some specific aspects of early neurocognitive development may be suboptimal if maternal prenatal vitamin D is deficient (i.e. serum 25(OH)D of < 50.0 nmol/L) in pregnancy. The biological mechanism underpinning this association in humans is not fully understood, but the ubiquitous presence of the vitamin D receptor (VDR) and the hydroxylase enzymes controlling vitamin D metabolism in a wide variety of areas of the human brain(6), as well as neurological developmental mechanisms previously identified in studies of vitamin D deficiency in pregnant rats may be relevant(7; 9; 28; 29). These include enlarged brain ventricles, thinner neocortex(29), and more mitotic cells in the brain(29), suggesting a less differentiated phenotype(28). The active form of vitamin D [1,25(OH)2D], may also affect the development of the brain by influencing the production of cytokines(30), affecting neurotransmission(31) and synaptic plasticity(31) which is likely to affect learning processes(32) and therefore neurocognitive development. 1,25(OH)2D likely affects dopamine activity in the brain owing to the presence of the vitamin D receptor (VDR) in brain areas responsive to dopamine(33). Ventral midbrain dopaminergic neurones are known to play a key role in the modulation of motor behaviour(34). It is therefore feasible that 1,25(OH)2D may affect motor development via its effects on the dopaminergic system. Other potential mechanisms may relate to an association between maternal 25(OH)D status and fetal growth retardation (e.g. reduced fetal head size) which is associated with later developmental disabilities(35). A recent study in the Generation R cohort in the Netherlands found an association between lower maternal 25(OH)D status at 20 weeks gestation and smaller fetal-head circumference in the third trimester(36), suggesting that poorer maternal 25(OH)D status may predispose children to developmental delay via effects on intra-uterine growth restriction.
When we assessed the impact of gestational age on our results for outcomes that were significantly associated with vitamin D in the main analyses, we found that the effect sizes were generally greater when vitamin D was measured in the second half (> 22 weeks) than in the first half (≤ 22 weeks) of pregnancy. There is a small amount of evidence in rats that re-introduction of vitamin D after birth, but before end of weaning, can rescue normal brain development(28); that time period correspond to the third trimester in humans, suggesting a potential crucial window for vitamin D in brain development. However, all interpretations in our analysis of gestational timing need to be interpreted in light of the fact that we only had one measurement of maternal vitamin D status for each woman and so we cannot draw clear conclusions on the effects of gestational timing of vitamin D deficiency. Furthermore, we cannot be sure that our observed effects are confined to the gestational week that the 25(OH)D measurement was made, as some individuals may have persistent pattern of vitamin D status that extends into later pregnancy or infancy.
When the women were split into three groups [serum 25(OH)D of <25.0, 25.0 – 49.9 and ≥ 50.0 nmol/L], adverse outcomes were present in the offspring of mothers with insufficient status (serum 25(OH)D < 50nmol/L) as well as those with severe deficiency (serum 25(OH)D < 25nmol/L). However, there was a trend to larger effect sizes in the more deficient < 25.0 nmol/L group than in the 25.0 – 49.9 nmol/L group; the relatively small sample size in the < 25.0 nmol/L group explains the wider confidence intervals seen for this cut-off. The outcomes that were significantly associated with vitamin D when women were dichotomised on the basis of a cut-off of 50.0 nmol/L were not significant when the cut-off was increased to 75.0 nmol/L. These findings support a vitamin D status cut-off for optimal child outcomes closer to 50.0 nmol/L than to 75.0 nmol/L.
As the women in the ALSPAC study were recruited over 20 years ago, we compared their vitamin D status to more recent measurements in UK women to assess the current relevance of our findings. As 25(OH)D status does not differ between pregnant and non–pregnant women(15) we looked at nationally representative data in UK women from the recent National Diet and Nutrition Survey (NDNS). In the latest report (sampling 2008/9 – 2011/12), 21.7% of women of 19–64 years had a plasma 25(OH)D concentration below 25 nmol/L(37), a higher percentage than the 4.4% of women in ALSPAC. Other studies(38; 39), including those in pregnancy, suggest that many UK women are vitamin D deficient. Currently, the UK National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) recommends that pregnant women should take a supplement of 10 µg (400 IU) of vitamin D per day(40). However use of vitamin D supplements in pregnancy is low, with a recent survey (2005–2009) finding that only 1.4% of UK pregnant women had taken a vitamin D supplement(41). Our findings give further evidence that public-health campaigns should address the vitamin D status of UK pregnant women, and encourage compliance with the 10 µg/d recommendation(40).
Strengths and Limitations
Although our study has several strengths, including the large sample size, there are also limitations. Firstly each woman had only one measure of maternal vitamin D status in pregnancy which may not have reflected status over the whole of pregnancy. In addition, the range of vitamin D status in the ALSPAC women was limited, with approximately one third (34.6%) having a 25(OH)D concentration less than 50.0 nmol/L and only a small proportion having a 25(OH)D concentration less than 25.0 nmol/L (4.4%). Moreover, ALSPAC only has a relatively small number of women from ethnic-minority backgrounds (just 2% of this study sample), who are known to be at particular risk of having low 25(OH)D concentrations(42), suggesting that the results may differ in populations with a larger number of ethnic-minority individuals. Finally, we were not able to control for the association between infant vitamin D status and neurocognitive function as we had no measures of vitamin D status in infancy. Infant vitamin D status may partly explain some of the association seen in this paper between maternal vitamin D status and infant neurodevelopment.
In conclusion, we found that maternal vitamin D status in pregnancy was associated with a number of adverse neurocognitive developmental variables in early childhood, albeit with a small, but nonetheless important, effect size. There is a need for replication of this work in other settings to confirm these results, but the public-health implications of these findings are nevertheless potentially important. Further study is now urgently required, particularly in population groups that are more severely vitamin D deficient such as dark-skinned ethnic-minority women37 who may show a wider range and greater severity of sub-optimal neurocognitive outcomes.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.
Sources of support
This work was supported by the following funding bodies: The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome Trust (grant number 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol currently provide core support for ALSPAC. The work was also supported by a UK Medical Research Council Population Health Scientist Fellowship (S.C.B., grant number MR/K02132X/1). The UK Medical Research Council provided funds to ALSPAC for completion of the 25(OH)D assays used in this paper (grant number G0701603).
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest
SLN is Research Director of D3Tex Ltd which holds the UK Patent (Gulf Cooperation Council Patent pending) on the use of ultraviolet-B (UVB) transparent material for vitamin D deficiency prevention. All other authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Authors’ Contributions to the Manuscript
ALD, SCB and JG designed the current research project. SCB and ALD conducted the statistical analyses with statistical advice from CDS, MPR and JG. MPR, JG, CDS and SLN revised the paper and made suggestions on the content. ALD and SCB wrote the paper. SCB has primary responsibility for final content.
References
- 1.Hibbeln JR, Davis JM, Steer C, et al. Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood (ALSPAC study): an observational cohort study. Lancet. 2007;369:578–585. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60277-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Daniels JL, Longnecker MP, Rowland AS, et al. Fish intake during pregnancy and early cognitive development of offspring. Epidemiology. 2004;15:394–402. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000129514.46451.ce. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gale CR, Robinson SM, Godfrey KM, et al. Oily fish intake during pregnancy - association with lower hyperactivity but not with higher full-scale IQ in offspring. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2008;49:1061–1068. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01908.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Julvez J, Mendez M, Fernandez-Barres S, et al. Maternal Consumption of Seafood in Pregnancy and Child Neuropsychological Development: A Longitudinal Study Based on a Population With High Consumption Levels. Am J Epidemiol. 2016;183:169–182. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwv195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Brock K, Huang WY, Fraser DR, et al. Low vitamin D status is associated with physical inactivity, obesity and low vitamin D intake in a large US sample of healthy middle-aged men and women. J Steroid Biochem. 2010;121:462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2010.03.091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Eyles DW, Smith S, Kinobe R, et al. Distribution of the vitamin D receptor and 1 alpha-hydroxylase in human brain. J Chem Neuroanat. 2005;29:21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2004.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Eyles DW, Feron F, Cui X, et al. Developmental vitamin D deficiency causes abnormal brain development. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(Suppl 1):S247–257. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.O'Loan J, Eyles DW, Kesby J, et al. Vitamin D deficiency during various stages of pregnancy in the rat; its impact on development and behaviour in adult offspring. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:227–234. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Feron F, Burne TH, Brown J, et al. Developmental Vitamin D3 deficiency alters the adult rat brain. Brain Res Bull. 2005;65:141–148. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Strom M, Halldorsson TI, Hansen S, et al. Vitamin D measured in maternal serum and offspring neurodevelopmental outcomes: a prospective study with long-term follow-up. Ann Nutr Metab. 2014;64:254–261. doi: 10.1159/000365030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Morales E, Guxens M, Llop S, et al. Circulating 25-hydroxyvitamin D3 in pregnancy and infant neuropsychological development. Pediatrics. 2012;130:e913–920. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Keim SA, Bodnar LM, Klebanoff MA. Maternal and cord blood 25(OH)-vitamin D concentrations in relation to child development and behaviour. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2014;28:434–444. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Whitehouse AJ, Holt BJ, Serralha M, et al. Maternal serum vitamin D levels during pregnancy and offspring neurocognitive development. Pediatrics. 2012;129:485–493. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hanieh S, Ha TT, Simpson JA, et al. Maternal vitamin D status and infant outcomes in rural Vietnam: a prospective cohort study. PloS one. 2014;9:e99005. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.SACN Vitamin D and Health. [accessed 29th March 2017];2016 https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/sacn-vitamin-d-and-health-report. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, et al. Cohort Profile: the 'children of the 90s'--the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:111–127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Neale M. Neale Analysis of Reading Ability- Revised. Windsor: NFER-Nelson; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Seamans KM, Cashman KD. Existing and potentially novel functional markers of vitamin D status: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89:1997s–2008s. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.27230D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lawlor DA, Wills AK, Fraser A, et al. Association of maternal vitamin D status during pregnancy with bone-mineral content in offspring: a prospective cohort study. Lancet. 2013;381:2176–2183. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62203-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sullivan S, Wills A, Lawlor D, et al. Prenatal vitamin D status and risk of psychotic experiences at age 18years-a longitudinal birth cohort. Schizophr Res. 2013;148:87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bath SC, Steer CD, Golding J, et al. Effect of inadequate iodine status in UK pregnant women on cognitive outcomes in their children: results from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) Lancet. 2013;382:331–337. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60436-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tolppanen AM, Fraser A, Fraser WD, et al. Risk factors for variation in 25-hydroxyvitamin D(3) and D(2) concentrations and vitamin D deficiency in children. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97:1202–1210. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-2516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schoon I, Jones E, Cheng H, et al. Family hardship, family instability, and cognitive development. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2012;66:716–722. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.121228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Samuel L, Borrell LN. The effect of body mass index on optimal vitamin D status in U.S. adults: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2001-2006. Ann Epidemiol. 2013;23:409–414. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2013.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kuoppala T, Tuimala R, Parviainen M, et al. Serum levels of vitamin D metabolites, calcium, phosphorus, magnesium and alkaline phosphatase in Finnish women throughout pregnancy and in cord serum at delivery. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr. 1986;40:287–293. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Heaney RP. Health is better at serum 25(OH)D above 30 ng/mL. J Steroid Biochem. 2013;136:224–228. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2012.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cui X, Gooch H, Groves NJ, et al. Vitamin D and the brain: key questions for future research. J Steroid Biochem. 2015;148:305–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2014.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Eyles D, Brown J, Mackay-Sim A, et al. Vitamin D3 and brain development. Neuroscience. 2003;118:641–653. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00040-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.van Etten E, Mathieu C. Immunoregulation by 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3: basic concepts. J Steroid Biochem. 2005;97:93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Almeras L, Eyles D, Benech P, et al. Developmental vitamin D deficiency alters brain protein expression in the adult rat: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Proteomics. 2007;7:769–780. doi: 10.1002/pmic.200600392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Becker A, Eyles DW, McGrath JJ, et al. Transient prenatal vitamin D deficiency is associated with subtle alterations in learning and memory functions in adult rats. Behav Brain Res. 2005;161:306–312. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kesby JP, Eyles DW, Burne TH, et al. The effects of vitamin D on brain development and adult brain function. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2011;347:121–127. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2011.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Van den Heuvel DMA, Pasterkamp RJ. Getting connected in the dopamine system. Prog Neurobiol. 2008;85:75–93. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Watemberg N, Silver S, Harel S, et al. Significance of microcephaly among children with developmental disabilities. J Child Neurol. 2002;17:117–122. doi: 10.1177/088307380201700205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Miliku K, Vinkhuyzen A, Blanken LM, et al. Maternal vitamin D concentrations during pregnancy, fetal growth patterns, and risks of adverse birth outcomes. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2016;103:1514–1522. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.123752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bates B, Lennox A, Prentice A, et al. National Diet and Nutrition Survey: Results from Years 1, 2, 3 and 4 (combined) of the Rolling Programme (2008/2009 – 2011/2012) London: Public Health England; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Macdonald HM, Mavroeidi A, Fraser WD, et al. Sunlight and dietary contributions to the seasonal vitamin D status of cohorts of healthy postmenopausal women living at northerly latitudes: a major cause for concern? Osteoporosis Int. 2011;22:2461–2472. doi: 10.1007/s00198-010-1467-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Javaid MK, Crozier SR, Harvey NC, et al. Maternal vitamin D status during pregnancy and childhood bone mass at age 9 years: a longitudinal study. Lancet. 2006;367:36–43. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)67922-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.NICE. Vitamin D: increasing supplement use in at-risk groups [PH56] [accessed 15th November 2015]; http://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ph56. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Oliver EM, Grimshaw KEC, Schoemaker AA, et al. Dietary Habits and Supplement Use in Relation to National Pregnancy Recommendations: Data from the EuroPrevall Birth Cohort. Matern Child Hlth J. 2014;18:2408–2425. doi: 10.1007/s10995-014-1480-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cashman KD, Dowling KG, Skrabakova Z, et al. Vitamin D deficiency in Europe: pandemic? The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2016;103:1033–1044. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.120873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.