Abstract
The antisaccade task is a classic paradigm used to study the voluntary control of eye movements. It requires participants to suppress a reactive eye movement to a visual target and to concurrently initiate a saccade in the opposite direction. Although several models have been proposed to explain error rates and reaction times in this task, no formal model comparison has yet been performed. Here, we describe a Bayesian modeling approach to the antisaccade task that allows us to formally compare different models on the basis of their evidence. First, we provide a formal likelihood function of actions (pro- and antisaccades) and reaction times based on previously published models. Second, we introduce the Stochastic Early Reaction, Inhibition, and late Action model (SERIA), a novel model postulating two different mechanisms that interact in the antisaccade task: an early GO/NO-GO race decision process and a late GO/GO decision process. Third, we apply these models to a data set from an experiment with three mixed blocks of pro- and antisaccade trials. Bayesian model comparison demonstrates that the SERIA model explains the data better than competing models that do not incorporate a late decision process. Moreover, we show that the early decision process postulated by the SERIA model is, to a large extent, insensitive to the cue presented in a single trial. Finally, we use parameter estimates to demonstrate that changes in reaction time and error rate due to the probability of a trial type (pro- or antisaccade) are best explained by faster or slower inhibition and the probability of generating late voluntary prosaccades.
Author summary
One widely replicated finding in schizophrenia research is that patients tend to make more errors than healthy controls in the antisaccade task, a psychometric paradigm in which participants are required to look in the opposite direction of a visual cue. This deficit has been suggested to be an endophenotype of schizophrenia, as first order relatives of patients tend to show similar but milder deficits. Currently, most models applied to experimental findings in this task are limited to fit average reaction times and error rates. Here, we propose a novel statistical model that fits experimental data from the antisaccade task, beyond summary statistics. The model is inspired by the hypothesis that antisaccades are the result of several competing decision processes that interact nonlinearly with each other. In applying this model to a relatively large experimental data set, we show that mean reaction times and error rates do not fully reflect the complexity of the processes that are likely to underlie experimental findings. In the future, our model could help to understand the nature of the deficits observed in schizophrenia by providing a statistical tool to study their biological underpinnings.
Introduction
In the antisaccade task ([1]; for reviews, see [2,3]), participants are required to saccade in the contralateral direction of a visual cue. This behavior is thought to require both the inhibition of a reflexive saccadic response towards the cue and the initiation of a voluntary eye movement in the opposite direction. A failure to inhibit the reflexive response leads to an erroneous saccade towards the cue (i.e., a prosaccade), which is often followed by a corrective eye movement in the opposite direction (i.e., an antisaccade). As a probe of inhibitory capacity, the antisaccade task has been widely used to study psychiatric and neurological diseases [3]. Notably, since the initial report [4], studies have consistently found an increased number of errors in patients with schizophrenia when compared to healthy controls, independent of medication and clinical status [5–8]. Moreover, there is evidence that an increased error rate constitutes an endophenotype of schizophrenia, as antisaccade deficits are also present in non-affected, first-degree relatives of diagnosed individuals (for example [5,7]; but for negative findings see for example [9,10]).
Unfortunately, the exact nature of the antisaccade deficits and their biological origin in schizophrenia remain unclear. One path to improve our understanding of these experimental findings is to develop generative models of their putative computational and/or neurophysiological causes [11]. Generative models that capture the entire distribution of responses can reveal features of the data that are not apparent when only considering summary statistics such as mean error rate (ER) and reaction time (RT) [12–15]. Additionally, this type of model can potentially relate behavioral findings in humans to their biological substrate.
Here, we apply a generative modeling approach to the antisaccade task. First, we introduce a novel model of this paradigm based on previous proposals [16–20]. For this, we formalize the ideas introduced by Noorani and Carpenter [17] and extend them into what we refer to as the Stochastic Early Reaction, Inhibition and late Action (SERIA) model. Second, we apply both models to an experimental data set of three mixed blocks of pro- and antisaccades trials with different trial type probability. More specifically, we compare several models using Bayesian model comparison. Third, we use the parameter estimates from the best model to investigate the effects of our experimental manipulation. We found that there was positive evidence in favor of the SERIA model when compared to our formalization of the model proposed in [17]. Moreover, the parameters estimated through model inversion revealed the complexity of the decision processes underlying the antisaccade task that is not obvious from mean RT and ER.
This paper is organized as follows. First, we formalize the model developed in [17] and introduce the SERIA model. Second, we describe our experimental setup. Third, we present our behavioral findings in terms of summary statistics (mean RT and ER), the comparison between different models, and the parameter estimates. Finally, we review our findings, discuss other recent models, potential future developments, and translational applications.
Materials and methods
Ethics statement
All participants gave written informed consent before the study. All experimental procedures were approved by the local ethics board (Kantonale Ethikkomission Zürich, KEK-ZH-Nr.2014-0246).
Race models for antisaccades
In this section, we derive a formal description of the models evaluated in this paper. We start with a formalized version of the model proposed by Noorani and Carpenter in [17] and proceed to extend it. Their approach resembles the model originally proposed by Camalier and colleagues [21] to explain RT and ER in the double step and search step tasks, in which participants are either asked to saccade to successively presented targets or to saccade to a target after a distractor was shown. Common to all these tasks is that subjects are required to inhibit a prepotent reaction to an initial stimulus and then to generate an action towards a secondary goal. Briefly, Camalier and colleagues [21] extended the original ‘horse-race’ model [16] by including a secondary action in countermanding tasks. In [17], Noorani and Carpenter used a similar model in combination with the LATER model [22] in the context of the antisaccade task by postulating an endogenously generated inhibitory signal. Note that this model, or variants of it, have been used in several experimental paradigms (reviewed in [20]). Here, we limit our discussion to the antisaccade task.
The pro, stop, and antisaccade model (PROSA)
Following [17], we assume that the RT and the type of saccade generated in a given trial are caused by the interaction of three competing processes or units. The first unit up represents a command to perform a prosaccade, the second unit us represents an inhibitory command to stop a prosaccade, and the third unit ua represents a command to perform an antisaccade. The time t required for unit ui to arrive at threshold si is given by:
(1) |
(2) |
where ri represents the slope or increase rate of unit ui, si represents the height of the threshold, and t represents time. We assume that, on each trial, the increase rates are stochastic and independent of each other.
The time and order in which the units reach their thresholds si determines the action and RT in a trial. If the prosaccade unit up reaches threshold before any other unit at time t, a prosaccade is elicited at t. If the antisaccade unit arrives first, an antisaccade is elicited at t. Finally, if the stop unit arrives before the prosaccade unit, an antisaccade is elicited at the time when the antisaccade unit reaches threshold. It is worth mentioning that, although this model is motivated as a race-to-threshold model, actions and RTs depend only on the arrival times of each of the units and ultimately no explicit model of increase rates or thresholds is required. Thus, for the sake of clarity, we refer to this approach as a ‘race’ model, in contrast to ‘race-to-threshold’ models that explicitly describe increase rates and thresholds.
Formally (but in a slight abuse of language), the two random variables of interest, the reaction time T ∈ [0,∞[ and the type of action performed A ∈ {pro,anti}, depend only on three further random variables: the arrival times Up, Us, Ua ∈ [0,∞[ of each of the units. The probability of performing a prosaccade at time t is given by the probability of the prosaccade unit arriving at time t, and the stop and antisaccade unit arriving afterwards:
(3) |
The probability of performing an antisaccade at time t is given by
(4) |
The first term on the right side of Eq 4 corresponds to the unlikely case that the antisaccade unit arrives before the prosaccade and the stop units. The second term describes trials in which the stop unit arrives before the prosaccade unit. It can be decomposed into two terms:
(5) |
(6) |
The term describes the condition in which the prosaccade unit is inhibited by the stop unit allowing for an antisaccade. Note that if the prosaccade unit arrives later than the antisaccade unit, the arrival time of the stop unit is irrelevant. That means that we can simplify Eq 4 to
(7) |
Eqs 3 and 7 constitute the likelihood function of a single trial, and define the joint probability of an action and the corresponding RT. We refer to this likelihood function as the PRO-Stop-Antisaccade (PROSA) model. It shares the central assumptions of [17], namely: (i) the time to reach threshold of each of the units is assumed to depend linearly on the rate r, (ii) it includes a stop unit whose function is to inhibit prosaccades and (iii) there is no lateral inhibition between the different units. Finally, (iv) RTs are assumed to be equal to the arrive-at-threshold times. Note that the RT distributions are different from the arrival time distributions because of the interactions between the units described above. The main difference of this model compared to [17] is that we do not exclude a priori the possibility of the antisaccade unit arriving earlier than the other units. Aside from this, both models are conceptually equivalent.
The Stochastic Early Reaction, Inhibition, and Late Action Model (SERIA)
The PROSA model is characterized by a strict association between units and action types. In other words, the unit up leads unequivocally to a prosaccade, whereas the unit ua always triggers an antisaccade. This implies that if the distribution of the arrival times of the units is unimodal and strictly positive, the PROSA model cannot predict voluntary slow prosaccades with a late peak, or in simple words, the PROSA model cannot account for slow, voluntary prosaccades that have been postulated in the antisaccade task [23]. Similarly, it has been argued that prosaccade RT can be described by the mixture of two distributions (for example [2,22]).To account for this, we introduce the Stochastic Early Reaction, Inhibition and Late Action (SERIA) model.
According to this model, and in analogy to the PROSA model, an early reaction takes place at time t if the early unit ue arrives before the late and inhibitory units, ul and ui, respectively. If the inhibitory or late unit arrives before the early unit, a late response is triggered at the time the late unit reaches threshold. Crucially, both early and late responses can trigger pro- and antisaccades with a certain probability. Thus, in parallel to the race processes which determine RTs, an independent, secondary decision process is responsible for which reaction is generated. Fig 1 shows the structure of the SERIA model.
To formalize the concept of early and late responses, we introduce a new unobservable random variable that represents the type of response R ∈ {early,late}. The distribution of the RTs is analogous to the PROSA-model, such that, for instance, the probability of an early response at time t is given by
(8) |
where Ue, Ui, and Ul represent the arrival times of the early, inhibitory, and late units, respectively. The fundamental assumption of the SERIA model is that a secondary decision process, beyond the race between early, inhibitory, and late units, decides the action generated in a single trial. An initial approach to model this secondary decision process is to assume that the action type (pro- or antisaccade) is conditionally independent of the RT given the response type (early or late). Hence, the distribution of RTs is not a priori coupled to the saccade type anymore; RT distributions for both pro- and antisaccades could in principle be bimodal, consisting of both fast reactive and slow voluntary saccades.
Formally, the conditional independency assumption can be written down as
(9) |
(10) |
(11) |
The term p(A|R) is simply the probability of an action, given a response type. We denote it as
(12) |
(13) |
(14) |
(15) |
Since the type of response R is not observable, it is necessary to marginalize it out in Eq 11 to obtain the likelihood of the SERIA model:
(16) |
The complete likelihood of the model is given by substituting the terms in Eq 16
(17) |
(18) |
It is worth noting here that the PROSA model is a special case of the SERIA model, namely, it corresponds to the assumption that πe = 1 and πl = 0. The SERIA model allows for bimodal distributions, as both early and late responses can be pro- and antisaccades. Importantly, one prediction of the model is that late prosaccades have the same distribution as late antisaccades.
A late race competition model for saccade type
Until now, we have assumed that the competition that leads to late pro- and antisaccades does not depend on time in the sense that late actions are conditionally independent of RT. This assumption can be weakened by postulating a secondary race between late responses; this leads us to a modified version of the SERIA model, that we refer to as the late race SERIA model (SERIAlr). The derivation proceeds similarly to the SERIA model, except that we postulate a fourth unit that generates late prosaccades instead of assuming that the late decision process is time insensitive.
This version of the SERIA model includes an early unit ue that, for simplicity, we assume produces only prosaccades, an inhibitory unit that stops early responses ui, a late unit that triggers antisaccades ua, and a further unit that triggers late prosaccades up. As before, if the early unit reaches threshold before any other unit, a prosaccade is generated with probability
(19) |
If any of the late units arrive first, the respective action is generated with probability:
(20) |
(21) |
Finally, if the inhibitory unit arrives first, either a late pro- or antisaccade is generated with probability
(22) |
(23) |
Implicit in the last two terms is the competition between the late units, which are assumed again to be independent of each other. Formally, this competition is expressed as the probability of, for example, the late antisaccade unit arriving before a late prosaccade p(Ua = t)p(Up > t). A schematic representation of the model is shown in Fig 2. This late race is similar to the Linear Ballistic Accumulation model proposed by [24], although in that model decisions are seen as the result of a race of ballistic accumulation processes with fixed threshold but stochastic base line and increase rate. Here we only assume that the late decision process is a GO-GO race [21].
The likelihood of an action is given by summing over all possible outcomes that lead to that action:
(24) |
(25) |
We have left out some possible simplifications in Eqs 24 and 25 for the sake of clarity.
The conditional probability of a late antisaccade is given by the interaction between the late units, such that
(26) |
is analogous to the probability of a late antisaccade 1−πl in the SERIA model. This observation shows that the main difference between the SERIA and SERIAlr model is that the former postulates that the distribution of late pro- and antisaccades are equal and conditionally independent of the action performed, whereas the latter constrains the probability of a late antisaccade to be a function of the arrival times of the late units.
The expected response time of late pro- and antisaccade actions is given by
(27) |
(28) |
We will refer to these terms as the mean response time of pro- and antisaccade actions, in contrast to the mean arrival times, which are the expected value of any single unit.
Non-decision time
The models above can be further finessed to account for non-decision times δ by transforming the RT t to tδ = t−δ. The delay δ might be caused by, for example, conductance delays from the retina to the cortex. In addition, the antisaccade or late units might include a constant delay δa, which is often referred to as the antisaccade cost [1]. Note that the model is highly sensitive to δ because any RT below it has zero probability. In order to relax this condition and to account for early outliers, we assumed that saccades could be generated before δ at a rate η ∈ [0,1] such that the marginal likelihood of an outlier is
(29) |
For simplicity, we assume that outliers are generated with uniform probability in the interval [0,δ]:
(30) |
Furthermore, we assume that the probability of an early outlier being a prosaccade was approximately 100 times higher than being an antisaccade. Because of the new parameter η, the distribution of saccades with a RT larger than δ needs to be renormalized by the factor 1−η. In the case of the PROSA model, for example, this means that the joint distribution of action and RT is given by the conditional probability
(31) |
(32) |
(33) |
A similar expression holds for the SERIA models. However, in the PROSA model a unit-specific delay is equal to an action-specific delay. By contrast, in the SERIA model both early and late responses can generate pro- and antisaccades. Thus, δa represents a delay in the late actions that affects both late pro- and antisaccades.
Parametric distributions of the increase rate
The models discussed in the previous sections can be defined independently of the distribution of the rate of each of the units. In order to fit experimental data, we considered four parametric distributions with positive support for the rates: gamma [13], inverse gamma, lognormal [25] and the truncated normal distribution (similarly to [22] and [24]). Table 1 and Fig 3 summarize these distributions, their parameters, and the corresponding arrival time densities. We considered five different configurations: 1) all units were assigned inverse gamma distributed rates, 2) all units were assigned gamma distributed rates, 3) the increase rate of the prosaccade and stop units (or early and the inhibitory units) was gamma distributed but the antisaccade (late) unit’s increase rate was inverse gamma distributed, 4) all the units were assigned lognormal distributed rates or 5) all units were assigned truncated normal distributed rates.
Table 1. Parametric density functions of the increase rates.
Name | Parameters | Rate p.d.f. | Arrival time p.d.f. |
---|---|---|---|
Gamma | k,θ | ||
Inv. gamma | k,θ | ||
Log normal | μ,σ2 | ||
T. normal | μ,σ2 |
Z is the normalization constant .
All the parametric distributions considered here can be fully characterized by two parameters which we generically refer to as k and θ. Hence, the PROSA model is characterized by the parameters for each unit kp,ka,ks,θp,θa,θs. The SERIA model can be characterized by analogous parameters ke,kl,ki,θe,θl,θi and the probabilities of early and late prosaccades πe and πl. In the case of the SERIAlr model, the probability of a late prosaccade is replaced by the parameters of a late prosaccade unit kp, θp. In addition to the unit parameters, all models included the non-decision time δ, the antisaccade (or late unit) cost δa, and the marginal rate of early outliers η.
Experimental procedures
In this section, we describe the experimental procedures, statistical methods, and inference scheme used to invert the models above. The data is from the placebo condition of a larger pharmacological study that will be reported elsewhere.
Participants
Fifty-two healthy adult males naïve to the antisaccade task were invited to a screening session through the recruitment system of the Laboratory of Social and Neural Systems Research of the University of Zurich. During screening, and after being debriefed about the experiment, subjects underwent an electrocardiogram, a health survey, a visual acuity test, and a color blindness test. Subjects were excluded if any of the following criteria were met: age below 18 or above 40 years, regular smoking, alcohol consumption the day before the experiment, any possible interaction between current medication and levodopa or benserazide, pulse outside the range 55–100bpm, recreational drug intake in the past 6 months, history of serious mental or neurological illness, or if the medical doctor supervising the experiment deemed the participant not apt. All subjects gave their written informed consent to participate in the study and received monetary compensation.
Procedure
Each subject was invited to two sessions. During both visits, the same experimental protocol was followed. After arrival, placebo or levodopa (Madopar DR 250, 200mg of levopa + 50 mg benserazide) was orally administered in the form of shape- and color-matched capsules. The present study is restricted to data from the session in which subjects received placebo. Participants and experimenters were not informed about the identity of the substance. Immediately afterwards subjects were introduced to the experimental setup and to the task through a written document. This was followed by a short training block (see below).
The experiment started 70 minutes after substance administration. Subjects participated in three blocks of 192 randomly interleaved pro- and antisaccade trials. The percentages of prosaccade trials in the three blocks were 20%, 50%, or 80%. This yielded three prosaccade probability (PP) conditions: PP20, PP50, and PP80. Thus, in the PP20 block, subjects were presented a prosaccade cue in 38 trials, while in all other 154 trials an antisaccade cue was shown. The order of the trials was randomized in each block, but the same order was used in all subjects and sessions. The order of the conditions was counterbalanced across subjects.
Stimulus and apparatus
During the experiment, subjects sat in front of a CRT monitor (Philipps 20B40, distance eye-screen: ≃60cm, refresh rate: 75Hz). The screen subtended a horizontal visual angle of 38 degrees of visual angle (dva). Eye movements were recorded using a remote infrared camera (Eyelink II, SR-Research, Canada). Participants’ head was stabilized with a chin rest. Data were stored at a sampling rate of 500 Hz.
During the task, two red dots (0.25dva) that constituted the saccadic targets were constantly displayed at an eccentricity of ±12dva. Displaying the saccadic target before the execution of an antisaccade has been reported to affect saccadic velocity and accuracy, but not RTs [26], and arguably decreases the need for sensorimotor transformations [27]. At the beginning of each trial, a gray fixation cross (0.6 × 0.6 dva) was displayed at the center of the screen. After a random fixation interval (500 to 1000 ms), the cross disappeared, and the cue instructing either a pro- or an antisaccade trial (see below) was shown centered on either of the red dots. As mentioned above, in each block, subjects were presented with a prosaccade cue in either 20, 50, or 80 percent of the trials. The order of the presentation of the cues was randomized. The cue was a green rectangle (3.48 × 0.8dva) displayed for 500ms in either horizontal (prosaccade) or vertical orientation (antisaccade). Once the cue was removed and after 1000ms, the next trial started.
Subjects were instructed to saccade in the direction of the cue when a horizontal bar was presented (prosaccade trial) and to saccade in the opposite direction when a vertical bar was displayed (antisaccade trial, see Fig 4). See [28,29] for similar task designs.
Prior to the main experiment, participants were trained on the task in a block of 50 prosaccade trials, immediately followed by 50 antisaccade trials. During the training, subjects were automatically informed after each trial whether their response was correct or not (see below), or whether they had failed to produce a saccade within 500ms after cue presentation (CP). No feedback was given during the main experimental blocks.
Data preparation
Data were parsed and preprocessed using the Python programming language (2.7). Saccades were detected using the algorithm provided by the eyetracker manufacturer (SR Research), which uses a velocity and acceleration threshold of 22dva/s and 3800dva/s2 [30]. We only considered saccades with a magnitude larger than 2dva. RT was defined as the time between CP and the first saccade larger than 2dva. A prosaccade trial was considered correct if the end position of the saccade was ipsilateral to the cue and, conversely, an antisaccade trial was considered correct if the end position of the saccade was contralateral to the cue.
Trials were excluded from further analysis if a) data were missing, b) a blink occurred between CP and the main saccade, c) the trial was aborted by the experimenter, d) subjects failed to fixate in the interval between fixation detection and CP, e) if a saccade was detected only later than 800ms after CP, f) if the RT was below 50ms, and in the case of an antisaccade if it was below 110ms. Corrective antisaccades were defined as saccades that a) followed a prosaccade error, b) occurred no later than 900ms after CP, and c) had less than 3dva horizontal error from the red circle contralateral to the cue.
Besides the fitted non-decision time δ we assumed a fixed non-decision time of 50ms for all participants [17]. This was implemented by subtracting 50ms of all saccades before being entered into the model. In order to avoid numerical instabilities, RT were rescaled from millisecond to tenths of a second during all numerical analysis. All results are presented in ms.
Modeling
We aimed to answer three questions with the models considered here. First, we investigated which model (i.e. PROSA, SERIA or SERIAlr) explained the experimental data best, and whether all important qualitative features of the data were captured by this model. We did not have a strong hypothesis regarding the parametric distribution of the data and hence, comparisons of parametric distributions were only of secondary interest in our analysis. Second, we investigated whether reduced models that kept certain parameters fixed across trial types were sufficient to model the data. Third, we investigated how the probability of a trial type in a block affected the parameters of the model.
Model space
Initially, we considered 15 different models as shown in Table 2. Each model was fitted independently for each subject and condition. Since our experimental design included mixed blocks, we allowed for different parameters in pro- and antisaccade trials, i.e., different increase-rate distributions depending on the trial type (TT). Under this hypothesis, the PROSA model had 12 free parameters (6 for each TT), whereas the SERIA model required 4 further parameters (πe and πl in each TT). The late race SERIAlr model included 16 parameters for the units (8 for each TT). We did not investigate the case that early reactions could trigger antisaccades but rather fixed the probability of an early antisaccade 1−πe to 10−3. The rationale behind this was that if early reactions are a priori assumed to never trigger antisaccades, rare but possible early antisaccades might cause large biases when fitting a model.
Table 2. Model families with the respective increase-rate distributions.
PROSA | |||
Model |
Prosaccade/ stop units |
Anti. unit | # Param. full/const. |
Inv. gamma | Inv. gamma | 15/13 | |
Gamma | Gamma | 15/13 | |
Gamma | Inv. gamma | 15/13 | |
Lognorm. | Lognorm. | 15/13 | |
T. norm. | T. norm. | 15/13 | |
SERIA | |||
Early/stop units | Late unit | ||
Inv. gamma | Inv. gamma | 19/13 | |
Gamma | Gamma | 19/13 | |
Gamma | Inv. gamma | 19/13 | |
Lognorm. | Lognorm. | 19/13 | |
T. norm. | T. norm. | 19/13 | |
SERIAlr | |||
Early/stop units | Late pro./anti. units | ||
Inv. gamma | Inv. gamma | 19/15 | |
Gamma | Gamma | 19/15 | |
Gamma | Inv. gamma | 19/15 | |
Lognorm. | Lognorm. | 19/15 | |
T. norm. | T. norm. | 19/15 |
Models with parameters constrained to be equal across trial types are referred through the superscript c.
Regarding the non-decision time δ, antisaccade cost δa, and rate of outliers η, we assumed equal parameters in both TT. Consequently, the full PROSA model had 15 free parameters whereas the full SERIA and SERIAlr models had both 19 free parameters.
In addition to the full models, we evaluated restricted versions of each of them by constraining some parameters to be equal across TT. In the case of the SERIA model, we hypothesized that the parameters of all units were equal, irrespective of TT (i.e., that the rate of the units was not affected by the cue presented in a trial). However, we assumed that the probability that an early or late response was a prosaccade was different in pro- and antisaccade trials. Therefore, in the case of the SERIA model, instead of 12 unit parameters (6 per TT), the restricted model had only 6 parameters for the units’ rates. The parameters πe and πl were allowed to differ in pro- and antisaccade trials. In the case of the restricted SERIAlr model, the units that underlie the late decision process were allowed to vary across TT, yielding a restricted model with 4 parameters for the early and inhibitory units, and 8 for the late decision process, half of them for each trial type. In the case of the PROSA model, similarly to [17], it is possible to assume that the parameters of the prosaccade unit remain constant across TT, and that the parameters of the stop and antisaccade units depend on TT, yielding 10 parameters for the units.
Prior distributions for model parameters
To complete the definition of the models, the prior distribution of the parameters was specified. This distribution reflects beliefs that are independent of the data and provides a form of regularization when inverting a model. In order to avoid any undesired bias regarding the parametric distributions considered here, we reparametrized all but the truncated normal distribution in terms of their mean and variance. We then assumed that the log of the mean and variance of the rate of the units were equally normally distributed (see Table 3). Therefore, the parametric distributions had the same prior in terms of their first two central moments. In the case of the truncated normal distribution, instead of an analytical transformation between its first two moments and its natural parameters μ and σ2, we defined the prior distribution as a density of μ and ln σ2. To ensure that μ was positive with high probability (96%) we assumed that μ ∼ N(0.55,0.09). The variance term was distributed as displayed in Table 3. As a further constraint, we restricted the parameter space to enforce that the first two moments of the distributions of rates and RTs existed. We relaxed this constraint for the late units of the SERIAlr in order to allow for ‘flat’ distributions with possibly infinite mean and variance. This can describe a case in which the increase rate of one of the late units is extremely low.
Table 3. Prior probability density functions.
Parameter | Probability density function | Expected value | Variance |
---|---|---|---|
μr | 0.55 | 0.5 | |
0.1 | 0.01 | ||
δ | 0.5 | 1.25 | |
δa | 0.75 | 1.25 | |
πe | Beta(πe;0.5,0.5) | 0.5 | 0.145 |
πl | Beta(πe;0.5,0.5) | 0.5 | 0.145 |
η | Beta(η;0.5,0.5) | 0.5 | 0.145 |
For the non-decision time δ and the antisaccade cost δa, the prior of their log transform was a normal distribution, consistent across all models. Note that the scale of the parameters δ and δa in Table 3 is tenths of a second. The fraction of early outliers η, and early and late prosaccades πe and πl were assumed to be Beta distributed, with parameters 0.5 and 0.5. Thus, for example, the prior probability of an early outlier is given by
(34) |
This parametrization constitutes the minimally informative prior distribution, as it is the Jeffrey’s prior of η, πe and πl. Table 3 displays the parameters used for the prior distributions.
Bayesian inference
Inference on the model parameters was performed using the Metropolis-Hastings algorithm [31]. To increase the efficiency of our sampling scheme, we iteratively modified the proposal distribution during an initial ‘burn-in’ phase as proposed by [32]. Moreover, we extended this method by drawing from a set of chains at different temperatures and swapping samples across chains. This method, called population MCMC or parallel tempering, increases the statistical efficiency of the Metropolis-Hasting algorithm [33] and has been used in similar contexts before [34]. We simulated 16 chains with a 5-th order temperature schedule [35]. For all but the models including a truncated normal distribution, we drew 4.1 × 104 samples per chain, from which the first 1.6 × 104 samples were discarded as part of the burn-in phase. When a truncated normal distribution was included (models m5, m10, and m15), the total number of samples was increased to 6 × 104, from which 2 × 104 were discarded. The convergence of the algorithm was assessed using the Gelman-Rubin criterion [33,36] such that the statistic of the parameters of the model was aimed to be below 1.1. When a simulation did not satisfy this criterion, it was repeated until 99.5 percent of all simulations satisfied it.
Models were scored using their log marginal likelihood or log model evidence (LME). This is defined as the log probability of the data given a model after marginalizing out all its parameters. When comparing different models, the LME corresponds to the log posterior probability of a model under a uniform prior on model identity. Thus, for a single subject with data y, the posterior probability of model k, given models 1 to n is
(35) |
Importantly, this method takes into account not only the accuracy of the model but also its complexity, such that overparameterized models are penalized [37]. A widely used approximation to the LME is the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) which, although easy to compute, has limitations (for discussion, see [38]). Here, we computed the LME through thermodynamic integration [33,39]. This method provides robust estimates and can be easily computed using samples obtained through population MCMC.
One important observation here is that the LME is sensitive to the prior distribution, and thus can be strongly influenced by it [40]. We addressed this issue in two ways: On one hand and as mentioned above, we defined the prior distribution of the increase rates of all models in terms of the same mean and variance. This implies that the priors were equal up to their first two moments, and hence all models were similarly calibrated. On the other hand, we complemented our quantitative analysis with qualitative posterior checks [33] as shown in the results section.
Besides comparing the evidence of each model, we also performed a hierarchical or random effects analysis described in [38,41]. This method can be understood as a form of soft clustering in which each subject is assigned to a model using the LME as assignment criterion. Here, we report the expected probability of the model ri, which represents the percentage of subjects that is assigned to the cluster representing model i. This hierarchical approach is robust to population heterogeneity and outliers, and complements reporting the group-level LME. Finally, we compared families of models [42] based on the evidence of each model for each subject summed across conditions.
Classical statistics
In addition to a Bayesian analysis of the data, we used classical statistics to investigate the effect of our experimental manipulation on behavioral variables (mean RT and ER) and the parameters of the models. We have suggested previously [11,43,44] that generative models can be used to extract hidden features from experimental data that might not be directly captured by, for example, standard linear methods or purely data driven machine learning techniques. In this sense, classical statistical inference can be boosted by extracting interpretable data features through Bayesian techniques.
Frequentist analyses of RT, ER, and parameter estimates were performed using a mixed effects generalized linear model with independent variables subject (SUBJECT), prosaccade probability (PP) with levels PP20, PP50 and PP80, and when pro- and antisaccade trials were analyzed together, trial type (TT). The factor SUBJECT was always entered as a random effect, whereas PP and TT were treated as categorical fixed effects. In the case of ER, we used the probit function as link function.
Analyses were conducted with the function fitglme.m in MATLAB 9.0. The significance threshold α was set to 0.05.
Implementation
All likelihood functions were implemented in the C programming language using the GSL numerical package (v.1.16). Integrals without an analytical form or well-known approximations were computed through numerical integration using the Gauss-Kronrod-Patterson algorithm [45] implemented in the function gsl_integration_qng. The sampling routine was implemented in MATLAB (v. 8.1) and is available as a module of the open source software package TAPAS (www.translationalneuromodeling.org/tapas).
Results
Behavior
Forty-seven subjects (age: 23.8 ± 2.9) completed all blocks and were included in further analyses. A total of 27072 trials were recorded, from which 569 trials (2%) were excluded (see Table 4).
Table 4. Summary of trials per subject.
Valid | Blink | Missing | Aborted | FE | Late S. | Early S. | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total | 26503 | 188 | 60 | 42 | 249 | 0 | 30 | 27072 |
Mean | 563.9 | 4.0 | 1.3 | 0.9 | 5.3 | 0.0 | 0.6 | 576 |
Std. | 9.9 | 5.1 | 2.5 | 1.5 | 5.0 | 0.0 | 1.3 | - |
Min. | 536 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | - |
Max. | 576 | 22 | 15 | 6 | 19 | 0 | 8 | - |
FE: Fixation errors. Late saccades are saccades elicited after 800ms. Early saccades are prosaccades elicited before 50ms after CP or antisaccades elicited before 110ms after CP.
Both ER and RT showed a strong dependence on PP (Fig 5 and Table 5). Individual data is included in the S1 Dataset and is displayed in S1 Fig. The mean RT of correct pro- and antisaccade trials was analyzed independently with two ANOVA tests with factors SUBJECT and PP. We found that in both pro- (F2,138 = 46.9, p < 10−5) and antisaccade trials (F2,138 = 37.3, p < 10−5) the effect of PP was significant; with higher PP, prosaccade RT decreased, whereas the RT of correct antisaccades increased. On a subject-by-subject basis, we found that between the PP20 and PP80 conditions, 91% of the participants showed increased RT in correct antisaccade trials, while 81% demonstrated the opposite effect (a decrease in RT) in correct prosaccade trials. Similarly, there was a significant effect of PP on ER in both prosaccade (F2,138 = 376.1, p < 10−5) as well as in antisaccade (F2,138 = 347.0, p < 10−5) trials. This effect was present in all but one participant in antisaccade trials and in all subjects in prosaccade trials. Exemplary RT data of one subject in the PP50 condition is displayed in Fig 6.
Table 5. Summary of mean RTs and ERs.
Trial type | Action | Reaction times [ms] | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
PP 20 | PP 50 | PP80 | ||
Pro. | Pro. | 330(72) | 319(67) | 284(59) |
Pro. | Anti. | 326(68) | 329(46) | 336(57) |
Anti. | Anti | 354(60) | 378(57) | 389(61) |
Anti. | Pro | 234(50) | 231(47) | 225(31) |
Error rates [%] | ||||
Pro. | 26(15) | 11(8) | 4(4) | |
Anti. | 23(17) | 35(21) | 51(20) |
Standard deviations are shown in brackets.
Modeling
Model comparison results
Initially, we considered the models outlined in Table 2. The LME over all participants (fixed effects analysis) and the posterior probability of all models and all subjects are presented in Fig 7. Independently of the particular parametric distribution of the units, the SERIAlr models had higher evidence compared to the PROSA and SERIA models. A random effects, family-wise model comparison [42] resulted in an expected frequency of r = 87% for the SERIAlr family, r = 11% for the SERIA family, and r = 2% for the PROSA family. In addition, constraining the parameters to be equal across trial types increased the model evidence irrespective of the parametric distribution assigned to the units (Fig 7). Here, the family-wise model comparison showed that models with constrained parameters had an expected frequency of r = 98%. Over all 30 models, (SERIAlr with constrained parameters, early and inhibitory increase rates gamma distributed, and late units’ rate inverse gamma distributed) showed the highest LME with ΔLME > 200 compared to all other models. Following [40], a difference in LME larger than 3 corresponds to strong evidence.
To verify that the SERIAlr family was not preferred simply because the probability of early prosaccades was fixed, we considered models in the SERIA family with the same property (not displayed). We found that although fixing this value increased the LME of the SERIA family, there was still a difference of ΔLME > 90 when comparing the best model of the SERIAlr family and the best model of the SERIA family with a fix probability of early prosaccades.
Fits of four subjects using the posterior samples of the best PROSA (m1), SERIA (), and SERIAlr () models are depicted in Fig 8. Although model m1 was the best model in the PROSA family, it clearly did not explain the apparent bimodality of the prosaccade RT distributions. Instead, RTs were explained through wider distributions. No obvious difference could be observed between the SERIA and SERIAlr models. We further examined the model fits in Fig 9 and Fig 10 by plotting the weighted fits and cumulative density functions of the reciprocal RT in the probit scale (reciprobit plot [22]) collapsed across subjects for the best model of each family. The histograms of RTs clearly show a large number of late prosaccades whose distribution is similar to the distribution of antisaccade RTs. The most pronounced–but still small—difference between the SERIA and SERIAlr models was visible in prosaccade trials in the PP20 condition (left panel, upper half plane), in which antisaccade errors displayed lower RT than correct late prosaccades.
Corrective antisaccades
The RTs of antisaccades that follow an error prosaccade were not directly modeled. However, we hypothesized that corrective antisaccades are delayed late antisaccade actions, whose distribution is given by the response time distribution of late antisaccades
(36) |
A total of 2989 corrective antisaccades were included in the analysis. The mean (±std) end time of the erroneous prosaccades was 268(±63)ms. The mean RT of corrective antisaccades was 447(±103)ms, and the weighted mean arrival time of the late antisaccade unit was 367ms. Fig 11 displays the histogram of the end time of all prosaccade errors, the RT of all corrective antisaccades and the time shifted (+80ms) predicted response time of late antisaccades. Since we did not have a strong hypothesis regarding the magnitude of the delay of the corrective antisaccades, we selected the time shift to be the difference between the mean corrective antisaccade RT and the mean predicted response time of late antisaccades. Visual inspection strongly suggests that the distribution of corrective antisaccade RTs is well approximated by the distribution of the late responses. The short difference between corrective antisaccades’ RT and the expected response time of the late antisaccade unit (80ms) favors the hypothesis that the plan for a corrective antisaccade is initiated before the incorrect prosaccade had finished.
Effects of prosaccade probability on model parameters
The effect of PP on the parameters of the model was investigated by examining the expected value of the parameters of the best scoring model (). Initially, we considered the question of whether the mean arrival or response time of each of the units changed as a function of PP. For arrival times, this corresponds to
(37) |
where i is an index over the units, j is an index over N samples collected using MCMC, and is the estimated delay. In the case of the late units, we considered only the response time of correct actions. Fig 12 left displays the mean arrival and response times. These were submitted to four separate ANOVA tests, which revealed that PP had a significant effect on all four units: early unit (F2,138 = 9.2, p < 10−3), late antisaccade (F2,138 = 26.6, p < 10−3), late prosaccade (F2,138 = 19.6, p < 10−3), and inhibitory unit (F2,138 = 30.9, p < 10−3). We then explored the differences across conditions through planned post hoc tests on each condition for each of the units (Table 6). The arrival times of the early unit did not change significantly between condition PP20 and PP50, but decreased significantly in the PP80 condition as compared to the PP50 block. The response times of late antisaccades increased significantly between the PP20 and the PP50 conditions but not so between the PP50 and PP80. Late prosaccades followed the opposite pattern, showing only a significant decrease in response time between the PP50 and PP80 conditions. Finally, the inhibitory unit changed significantly across all conditions.
Table 6. Post hoc comparison of the effect of PP.
Early | Inhib. | Late pro. | Late anti. | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean [ms] | t138 | p | Mean [ms] | t138 | p | Mean [ms] | t138 | p | Mean [ms] | t138 | p | |
PP20–PP50 | 4 | 0.6 | 0.50 | -30 | -4.0 | <0.001* | 3 | 0.5 | 0.55 | -24 | -36 | <0.001* |
PP20–PP80 | 26 | 3.9 | <0.001* | -58 | -7.8 | <0.001* | 32 | 5.7 | <0.001* | -32 | -5.4 | <0.001* |
PP50–PP80 | 22 | 3.3 | 0.001* | -28 | -3.8 | <0.001* | 29 | 5.1 | <0.001* | -7 | -1.5 | 0.13 |
Effect of PP on the mean arrival time of the early and inhibitory unit, and for late pro- and antisaccade units in the corresponding trial type.
*, p < 0.05.of a two-tailed t-test.
Finally, we examined how the probability of a late antisaccade p(Ua > Up) (Fig 12, right) depended on PP and TT. The estimated parameters for both pro- and antisaccade trials were analyzed with a model with factors SUBJECT, TT, PP and the interaction between TT and PP. An ANOVA test demonstrated that both PP (F2,276 = 51.2, p < 10−3) and TT (F1,276 = 985.0, p < 10−3) had a significant effect, but there was no evidence for an interaction between the two factors (F2,276 = 1.5, p < 0.23), suggesting that PP affected the probability of a late antisaccade equally in pro- and antisaccade trials.
Subject specific parameters
Finally, we investigated how some of the parameters of the model were related to each other across subjects. Because it has been commonly reported that schizophrenia is related with higher ER, but also with increased antisaccade RT, an interesting question is whether higher late-action response times are correlated with the percentage of late errors and inhibition failures, i.e., early saccades that are not stopped. We found that the response time of late pro (F1,135 = 13.6, p < 0.001) and antisaccades (F1,135 = 7.1, p < 0.01) was negatively correlated with the probability of a late error (Fig 13), but no significant interaction between PP and response time was found (pro: F2,135 = 1.7, p = 0.19; anti: F2,135 = 0.3, p = 0.76). Hence, late responders tended to make fewer late errors, suggesting a speed/accuracy trade-off in addition to the main effect of PP. We further considered the question whether the percentage of inhibition failures was correlated with the expected arrival time of the late antisaccade unit in antisaccade trials (Fig 13 right). Note that the number of inhibition failures is the same in both trial types in a constrained model, but inhibition failures are errors in antisaccade trials and correct early reactions in prosaccade trials. We found that these parameters were not significantly correlated (F2,135 = 1.2, p = 0.26). This was also the case when we considered the expected response time of late prosaccades in prosaccade trials (not displayed; F2,135 = 0.0, p = 0.98).
Fig 14 illustrates the posterior distribution of late errors and inhibition failures of two representative subjects as estimated using MCMC. Clearly, PP induced strong differences in the percentage of inhibition failures and late errors in prosaccade trials in both subjects. The effect of PP is less pronounced in late errors in antisaccade trials. The posterior distributions also illustrate how the SERIAlr model can capture individual differences: For example, the percentage of late prosaccade errors in the PP80 condition and the percentage of inhibition failures across all conditions are clearly different in each subject.
Discussion
In this study, we provided a formal treatment of error rates (ER) and reaction times (RT) in the antisaccade task using probabilistic models. We applied these models to data from an experiment consisting of 3 mixed blocks with different probabilities of pro- and antisaccades trials. Model comparison showed that a novel model that allows for late pro- and antisaccades explains our experimental findings better than a model in which all late responses are assumed to be antisaccades. The parameter estimates of the hidden units of the model showed that changes in the inhibitory unit and the late decision process explained most of the overt changes in behavior caused by our experimental manipulation, i.e., differences in trial type probability. Moreover, we found that all units were sensitive to the PP in a block, although late responses tended to plateau when the corresponding trial type was not highly frequent.
Our main finding is that two decision processes are necessary to properly model the antisaccade task: on one hand, an early race between a prepotent response towards a target and an endogenously generated signal to cancel this action, and, on the other hand, a secondary late race between two units encoding the cue-action mapping. Although the late decision process can be closely approximated by assuming that RT and actions are independent (at least in our experimental design), Bayesian model comparison demonstrated that late decisions are more accurately described by a race between two units representing different actions. The two decision processes are the sources of early errors–fast prosaccades in antisaccade trials- and late errors–late actions incongruent with the cue presented. The late decision process displays a speed/accuracy tradeoff and is biased by the probability of a trial type in a block. Moreover, this decision process predicts the RT distribution of corrective antisaccades that follow early errors. Because the extra latency of these corrective antisaccades (80ms) is relatively short, it is unlikely that corrective antisaccades are due to a restart in the decision process. Rather these are late actions that overwrite early errors.
Influence of trial type probability on reaction times and error rates
Our results show that both RT and ER depend on PP. While this was a highly significant factor in our study, there are mixed findings in previous reports. ER in antisaccade trials was found to be correlated with TT probability in several studies [29,46,47]. However, this effect might depend on the exact implementation of the task [47,48]. Changes in prosaccade ER similar to our study have been reported by [29] and [48]. Studies in which the type of saccade was signaled at fixation prior to the presentation of the peripheral cue do not always show this effect [47]. The results on RTs are less consistent in the literature. Our findings of increased anti- and decreased prosaccade RTs with higher PP are in line with the overall trend in [29], and with studies in which the cue was presented centrally [47]. Often, there is an additional increase in RT in the PP50 condition [29,47], which was visible in our data as a slight increase in RT in the PP50 condition on top of the linear effect of PP. Overall, RTs in our study were relatively slow compared to studies in which the TT cue was separated from the spatial cue [46,47]. However, a study with a similar design and added visual search reported even slower RTs in both pro- and antisaccades [29].
Interpretation of model comparison results
Formal comparison of generative models can offer insight into the mechanisms underlying eye movement behavior [11] and might be relevant in translational neuromodeling applications, such as computational psychiatry [49–53]. Here, we have presented what is, to our knowledge, the first formal statistical comparison of models of the antisaccade task. For this, we formalized the model introduced in [17] and proceeded to develop a novel model that relaxes the one-to-one association of early and late responses with pro- and antisaccades, respectively. All models and estimation techniques presented here are openly available under the GPLv3.0 license as part of the open source package TAPAS (www.translationalneuromodeling.org/tapas).
Bayesian model comparison yielded four conclusions at the family level. First, the SERIA models were clearly favored when compared to the PROSA models. Second, including a late race between actions representing late pro- and antisaccades (SERIAlr) resulted in an increase in model evidence, compared to a model not including a late race (SERIA). Third, models in which the race parameters of the early and inhibitory unit were constrained to be equal across TT had a higher LME than models in which all parameters were free. Hence, the effect of the cue in a single trial was limited to the late action, and did not affect the race between an early and inhibitory process. This constitutes an important external validation, as it means that model comparison does favor a model which respects the temporal order of the experiment: Information about TT is only available after the stimulus was presented and, thus, it is unlikely to have an impact on fast reactive responses. Fourth, early responses were nearly always prosaccades. Crucially, these four conclusions are based on family-wise comparison across all parametric distribution of the increase rate of the units.
A further consequence of our findings is that two independent and qualitatively different decision processes lead to an antisaccade: the race process between early and inhibitory units, and the secondary decision process that generates late responses. A separation of decisions into a ‘where’ and a ‘when’ component has been proposed by [54], but mainly in conceptual terms. However, model comparison showed that these two components (‘where’ and ‘when’) cannot be completely dissociated and that time plays a role in late decisions. Nevertheless, the assumption that action type and arrival time of late responses were independent yielded a good fit to this particular data set, suggesting that it is, in many cases, an acceptable approximation to assume a time-independent late decision process. The most obvious difference between the SERIA and SERIAlr can be observed in prosaccade trials in the PP20 condition (left panel, upper half plane Fig 9), in which late prosaccades are slower than antisaccades. We discuss this point in more detail below.
Parametric distribution of reaction times
The parametric distribution of oculomotor RTs has been discussed in great detail in the literature (e.g., [13,55]). Here, we did not aim at determining the most suitable distribution, but rather opted for a practical approach by evaluating different models with a reduced number of parametric distributions. We then based our conclusions on the model with the highest LME. Nevertheless, one can consider the connection of the models presented here with other families of parametric distributions. In particular, the linear relationship
(38) |
could be seen as formally inconsistent with the observation that RT are likely to be explained by stochastic accumulation processes (see for example [56,57], but [58]). This is a weaker constraint than one would expect, because under low noise conditions, for example, a linear relationship can be a good approximation of neural activity. Even if the relationship is not linear, for any continuous function ϕ with an inverse function ϕ−1, the model can be recasted as [59]:
(39) |
(40) |
In any case, linear accumulation models have been shown to yield similar conclusions to stochastic accumulation models [58].
More generally, it can be shown that if RTs follow a generalized inverse normal distribution (GIN) of the form
(41) |
where λ ≤ 0, and Kλ is a modified Bessel function of the second kind, there exists a continuous diffusion process whose first hit distribution (FHD) follows the GIN [60]. A particular case of this distribution is the Wald distribution for which . It is the FHD of the Brownian diffusion process with drift
(42) |
where Wt denotes a Wiener process, x0 > 0, and the absorbing boundary a is zero. More relevant here, when ψ = 0 the distribution reduces to an inverse gamma distribution, the FHD of the process
(43) |
with x0 > 0 and boundary a = 0 (for a detailed mathematical treatment see [60]). Thus, if the rates of a ballistic, linear processes are assumed to be gamma distributed, the RTs follow a distribution that is formally equivalent to a first hit model with stochastic updates and fixed rates. While the model presented here can be seen as a ballistic accumulation model, this equivalence suggests that it is compatible with a diffusion process with infinitesimal mean change proportional to t−1.
Other antisaccade models
In broad terms, three families of antisaccade models can be distinguished (reviewed in [61]). The first set of models is based on a race process with independent saccadic and stop units. These models build on the seminal work of [16] on the stop-signal paradigm. According to this model, a ‘GO’ signal triggers a stochastic ‘race’ process that generates a response once it reaches threshold. Critically, a stop signal triggers a second process that inhibits the first ‘GO’ response if it is the first to reach threshold. Moreover, the rates of both units are assumed to be independent. This model was further extended for the antisaccade task by [17] (but see [14,21], and the review in [20]), who included a third unit such that an antisaccade is generated when a reflexive prosaccade is inhibited by an endogenously-triggered stop process. Note that the original ‘horse-race’ model has also been modified [62] to account for different competing response actions, similarly as in the antisaccade task. The models proposed here belong to this family.
A second type of model relies on lateral or mutual inhibition of competing pro- and antisaccade units. In this direction, Cutsuridis and colleagues [61,63,64] proposed that lateral inhibition is implemented by inhibitory connections in the intermediate layers of the superior colliculus. Thus, saccades are the result of accumulation processes, but these are not independent of each other. Crucially, no veto-like stop signal is required. Although no formal model-fitting has been proposed for this model, qualitative agreement with data suggests that it might capture behavioral patterns relevant in translational applications [64,65]. Since no probabilistic version of this model is available, it is not yet possible to decide on the grounds of model comparison whether mutually dependent or independent race processes best explain current behavioral findings.
Finally, several models that incorporate detailed physiological mechanisms have been proposed [23,66–68]. These models cannot be easily assigned to one of the above categories, as they often employ both an inhibitory mechanism that stops or withholds the reactive responses as well as competition between actions. In addition, while more realistic models possess a more fine-grained representation of the underlying neurobiology, they rely on a large number of parameters and for this reason, it is difficult to fit them to behavioral data (for discussion, see [11]).
Regarding neurobiologically realistic models, the model proposed by [23] is the most similar to the SERIA model. It posits two different mechanisms that interact in the generation of antisaccades: an action-selection module and a remapping module that controls the cue-action mapping. As a consequence, this model allows for the generation of late errors that follow a similar RT distribution as correct antisaccades. Consistent with this observation, the SERIA model can quantitatively distinguish between inhibition and late cue-action mapping errors (Fig 15, left panel). A less obvious similarity between the SERIA model and [23] is that different cues do not lead directly to different dynamics in the action module, but only in the so-called ‘remapping’ module. Furthermore, the incorporation of a late race is conceptually close to the approach proposed by [23], which includes a winner-take-all competition in what we have referred here as late responses. Similarly, our model comparison results show that different cues (i.e., trial types) do not affect the GO/NO-GO process but only the late cue-action mapping.
Parameter changes across trial types
One of the most salient results presented here is that models in which the parameters of the units were constrained to be equal across trial types had a larger LME than models in which all the parameters were free, suggesting that the early and inhibitory race units were not affected by the cue presented in a single trial. While visual inspection of the predicted likelihood under the posterior parameters showed that most of the prominent characteristics of the data were explained correctly, some more subtle effects were not captured accurately by the SERIA model. This is particularly clear in the PP20 condition, in which the SERIA model displays a large bias in prosaccades trials in the PP20 condition. One possible explanation is that restricting the parameters across trial types made the model too rigid to capture this effect. Fig 16 compares the fitted RT distributions for models m8 (SERIA) and m13 (SERIAlr), in which no constraint on the parameters was imposed. Both models are qualitatively almost identical, although as shown in Fig 7, the LME favored the SERIAlr model. Thereby, the qualitative similarity between both models indicates that, in our experiment, the RT of late decisions is only weakly dependent on time. In conclusion, although removing the constraint on the parameters did improve the fit, the differences are marginal and thus did not justify the additional model complexity. As mentioned above this is consistent with the notion that the information about trial type is only available to a subject once the peripheral stimulus (green bar) has been processed, presumably tens of milliseconds after the stimulus onset. In fact, this example illustrates the protection against overfitting provided by the LME, as this is a case in which simpler models were preferred over more complex models despite of slightly less accurate fits.
Arguably, the constrained SERIA model fails to fully capture the RT of late prosaccade in the PP20 and PP50 conditions because of the assumption that late prosaccades have the same arrival time as late antisaccades. As shown in Fig 15, although the response time of late pro- and antisaccades are strongly correlated, the average ratio of the response times changes across conditions.
The effect of trial type probability
It is far from obvious why TT probability affects RT and ER in the antisaccade task. One possible explanation is that increased probability leads to higher preparedness for either pro- or antisaccades. Such a theory posits an intrinsic trade-off between preparations for one of the two action types that leads to higher RTs and ERs in low probability trials. Thus, a trade-off theory predicts that the arrival times of early and late responses should be negatively correlated. Although this hypothesis can explain our behavioral findings in terms of summary statistics, our model suggests a more complicated picture.
The main explanation of our results is the effect of TT probability on the inhibitory unit and the probability of a late prosaccade. A higher probability of antisaccade trials leads to faster inhibition and to a higher number of late prosaccades. This resulted in higher mean RT in prosaccade trials when PP is low. In the case of antisaccades, although the mean arrival times of the late unit increased in the PP50 condition, the increased arrival time of the inhibitory unit on the PP80 condition skewed the antisaccade distribution towards higher RTs. Nevertheless, the SERIAlr implies the anticorrelation of late pro- and antisaccades in a single trial type, as these are the results of a GO-GO race.
Action inhibition
The biological implementation of action inhibition in the antisaccade and other countermanding tasks has received a lot of attention and is still debated [69–73]. Our work adds evidence to the theory that the antisaccade task requires a process that inhibits prepotent responses and is independent of the initiation of a late action [20]. Recent evidence from electrophysiological recordings in the rat brain ([74] reviewed by [71]) suggests that the hypothesized race between GO and inhibitory responses might be implemented by different pathways in the basal ganglia [68]. In addition to the basal ganglia, microstimulation of the supplementary eye fields tends to facilitate inhibition of saccades in the countermanding task [75].
Corrective antisaccades
Although not a primary goal of our model, we considered the question of predicting corrective antisaccades. This problem has received some attention recently [18,61,65,76], as more sophisticated models of the antisaccade task have been developed. We speculated that corrective antisaccades are generated by the same mechanism as late responses. Thus, their RT distribution should follow a similar distribution. Our results strongly suggest that this is the case (see Fig 11). Moreover, the time delay of the corrective antisaccades indicates that, on average, these actions are not the result of the late unit being restarted at the end time of the erroneous prosaccade, as this would lead to much higher RTs. Rather, the planning of a corrective antisaccade might be started much before the end of the execution of an erroneous prosaccade, in accordance with the parallel planning model of the antisaccade task [46] and the ‘GO–STOP+GO’ model in [21].
Translational applications
Despite the large number of studies of clinical patients using the antisaccade task, an important question remains open: What are the causes of the errors in different neurological and psychiatric conditions? For example [77,78] argued that errors in schizophrenia might be explained, at least partially, by a failure to generate a secondary late action based on several modifications of the antisaccade task. However, it was also proposed that the increased ER in schizophrenia is due to high tonic dopamine levels in the basal ganglia, that lead to decreased inhibition of early responses [68]. More generally, different neurological and psychiatric diseases, or even patients with the same condition, might be characterized by a different source of errors. For example, there is intriguing evidence [79] that patients with different diseases such as attention deficits disorders [80], Parkinson’s disease [81], and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis [82] might be characterized by different ratios of early and late errors. An interesting experimental finding in our study related to this is the considerable amount of erroneous antisaccades in prosaccade trials. An increased number of such errors could be caused by reduced cognitive flexibility leading to impaired shifting between tasks as observed for example in obsessive compulsive disorder [83]. The ability to quantify different types of errors through computational modeling might help to further characterize these diseases.
Summary
Here we have presented a novel model of the antisaccade task. While the basic structure of the model follows the layout of a previous model [17], we have introduced two crucial advancements. First, we postulated that late responses could trigger both pro- and antisaccades, which are selected by an independent decision process. Second, the generative nature of our model allows for Bayesian model inversion, which enables the comparison of different models and families of models on formal grounds. To our knowledge this has not been done for any of the previous models of the antisaccade task, which is of relevance for translational applications that aim at better understanding psychiatric diseases by means of computational modeling.
The application of the model to a large data set yielded several novel results. First, the early and inhibitory race processes triggered by different cues are almost identical. Moreover, different PP had very different effects on the individual units, which was not obvious from the linear analysis of the mean RT and ER. Crucially, our modeling approach allowed us to look at a mechanistic explanation or the effects of PP by examining the individual units. In future work we aim to disentangle the mechanisms of behavioral differences caused by neuromodulatory drugs and psychiatric illnesses using formal Bayesian inference.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We thank Saee Paliwal for her remarks and helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the René and Susanne Braginsky Foundation (KES) and the University of Zurich. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Hallett PE. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Res. 1978;18: 1279–1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5: 218–228. doi: 10.1038/nrn1345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hutton SB, Ettinger U. The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. Psychophysiology. 2006;43: 302–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00403.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fukushima J, Fukushima K, Chiba T, Tanaka S, Yamashita I, Kato M. Disturbances of voluntary control of saccadic eye movements in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry. 1988;23: 670–677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Curtis CE, Calkins ME, Grove WM, Feil KJ, Iacono WG. Saccadic disinhibition in patients with acute and remitted schizophrenia and their first-degree biological relatives. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158: 100–106. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.1.100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Harris MS, Reilly JL, Keshavan MS, Sweeney JA. Longitudinal studies of antisaccades in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2006;36: 485–494. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006756 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Reilly JL, Frankovich K, Hill S, Gershon ES, Keefe RS, Keshavan MS, et al. Elevated antisaccade error rate as an intermediate phenotype for psychosis across diagnostic categories. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40: 1011–1021. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Radant AD, Millard SP, Braff DL, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Freedman R, et al. Robust differences in antisaccade performance exist between COGS schizophrenia cases and controls regardless of recruitment strategies. Schizophr Res. 2015;163: 47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Crawford TJ, Sharma T, Puri BK, Murray RM, Berridge DM, Lewis SW. Saccadic eye movements in families multiply affected with schizophrenia: the Maudsley Family Study. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155: 1703–1710. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.12.1703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Radant AD, Dobie DJ, Calkins ME, Olincy A, Braff DL, Cadenhead KS, et al. Antisaccade performance in schizophrenia patients, their first-degree biological relatives, and community comparison subjects: data from the COGS study. Psychophysiology. 2010;47: 846–856. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01004.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Heinzle J, Aponte EA, Stephan KE. Computational models of eye movements and their application to schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2016;11: 21–29. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.03.008 [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wolfe JM, Palmer EM, Horowitz TS. Reaction time distributions constrain models of visual search. Vision Res. 2010;50: 1304–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Palmer EM, Horowitz TS, Torralba A, Wolfe JM. What are the shapes of response time distributions in visual search? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2011;37: 58–71. doi: 10.1037/a0020747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Noorani I, Carpenter RH. Full reaction time distributions reveal the complexity of neural decision-making. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33: 1948–1951. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07727.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Huys QJ, Maia TV, Frank MJ. Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19: 404–413. doi: 10.1038/nn.4238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984;10: 276–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Noorani I, Carpenter RH. Antisaccades as decisions: LATER model predicts latency distributions and error responses. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37: 330–338. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Noorani I, Carpenter RH. Re-starting a neural race: anti-saccade correction. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;39: 159–164. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Noorani I. LATER models of neural decision behavior in choice tasks. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8: 67 doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Noorani I. Towards a unifying mechanism for cancelling movements. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2017;372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Camalier CR, Gotler A, Murthy A, Thompson KG, Logan GD, Palmeri TJ, et al. Dynamics of saccade target selection: race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque. Vision Res. 2007;47: 2187–2211. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.04.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Carpenter RH, Williams ML. Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements. Nature. 1995;377: 59–62. doi: 10.1038/377059a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lo CC, Wang XJ. Conflict Resolution as Near-Threshold Decision-Making: A Spiking Neural Circuit Model with Two-Stage Competition for Antisaccadic Task. PLoS Comput Biol. 2016;12: e1005081 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brown SD, Heathcote A. The simplest complete model of choice response time: linear ballistic accumulation. Cogn Psychol. 2008;57: 153–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gorea A, Rider D, Yang Q. A unified comparison of stimulus-driven, endogenous mandatory and “free choice” saccades. PLoS ONE. 2014;9: e88990 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Edelman JA, Valenzuela N, Barton JJ. Antisaccade velocity, but not latency, results from a lack of saccade visual guidance. Vision Res. 2006;46: 1411–1421. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zhang M, Barash S. Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades. Nature. 2000;408: 971–975. doi: 10.1038/35050097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sato TR, Schall JD. Effects of stimulus-response compatibility on neural selection in frontal eye field. Neuron. 2003;38: 637–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Chiau HY, Tseng P, Su JH, Tzeng OJ, Hung DL, Muggleton NG, et al. Trial type probability modulates the cost of antisaccades. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106: 515–526. doi: 10.1152/jn.00399.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stampe D. Heuristic filtering and reliable calibration methods for video-based pupil-tracking systems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, \& Computers. Springer-Verlag; 1993;25: 137–142. doi: 10.3758/BF03204486 [Google Scholar]
- 31.Robert C, Casella G. Monte Carlo statistical methods Springer Science \& Business Media; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Shaby B, Wells MT. Exploring an adaptative Metropolis algorithm Durham, NC, USA: Department of statistical science. Duke Universitiy; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gelman A, Carlin JB, Stern HS, Rubin DB. Bayesian Data Analysis. Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Aponte EA, Raman S, Sengupta B, Penny WD, Stephan KE, Heinzle J. mpdcm: A toolbox for massively parallel dynamic causal modeling. J Neurosci Methods. 2016;257: 7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.09.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ben Calderhead, Girolami MA. Estimating Bayes factors via thermodynamic integration and population MCMC. Computational Statistics \& Data Analysis. 2009;53: 4028–4045. doi: 10.1016/j.csda.2009.07.025 [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gelman A, Rubin DB. Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Statistical Science. JSTOR; 1992;: 457–472. [Google Scholar]
- 37.MacKay DJC. Information Theory, Inference, and Learning Algorithms. Cambridge University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Stephan KE, Penny WD, Daunizeau J, Moran RJ, Friston KJ. Bayesian model selection for group studies. Neuroimage. 2009;46: 1004–1017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gelman A, Meng XL. Simulating Normalizing Constants: From Importance Sampling to Bridge Sampling to Path Sampling. Statistical Science. Institute of Mathematical Statistics; 1998;13: 163–185. doi: 10.2307/2676756 [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kass RE, Raftery AE. Bayes factors. Journal of the american statistical association. Taylor \& Francis Group; 1995;90: 773–795. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rigoux L, Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Daunizeau J. Bayesian model selection for group studies—revisited. Neuroimage. 2014;84: 971–985. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Penny WD, Stephan KE, Daunizeau J, Rosa MJ, Friston KJ, Schofield TM, et al. Comparing families of dynamic causal models. PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6: e1000709 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Brodersen KH, Schofield TM, Leff AP, Ong CS, Lomakina EI, Buhmann JM, et al. Generative embedding for model-based classification of fMRI data. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7: e1002079 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Stephan KE, Schlagenhauf F, Huys QJ, Raman S, Aponte EA, Brodersen KH, et al. Computational neuroimaging strategies for single patient predictions. Neuroimage. 2017;145: 180–199. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Patterson TNL. The optimum addition of points to quadrature formulae. Mathematics of Computation. 1968;22: 847–856. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Massen C. Parallel programming of exogenous and endogenous components in the antisaccade task. Q J Exp Psychol A. 2004;57: 475–498. doi: 10.1080/02724980343000341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pierce JE, McDowell JE. Effects of preparation time and trial type probability on performance of anti- and pro-saccades. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2016;164: 188–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Pierce JE, McDowell JE. Modulation of cognitive control levels via manipulation of saccade trial-type probability assessed with event-related BOLD fMRI. J Neurophysiol. 2016;115: 763–772. doi: 10.1152/jn.00776.2015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Montague PR, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ, Dayan P. Computational psychiatry. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed). 2012;16: 72–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wang XJ, Krystal JH. Computational psychiatry. Neuron. 2014;84: 638–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Stephan KE, Mathys C. Computational approaches to psychiatry. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;25: 85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Paulus MP, Huys QJ, Maia TV. A Roadmap for the Development of Applied Computational Psychiatry. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. Elsevier; 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Huys QJ, Maia TV, Paulus MP. Computational Psychiatry: From Mechanistic Insights to the Development of New Treatments. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. Elsevier; 2016;1: 382–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Findlay JM, Walker R. A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22: 661–674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Feng G. Is there a common control mechanism for anti-saccades and reading eye movements? Evidence from distributional analyses. Vision Res. 2012;57: 35–50. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30: 535–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ratcliff R, Smith PL, Brown SD, McKoon G. Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed). 2016;20: 260–281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Donkin C, Brown S, Heathcote A, Wagenmakers EJ. Diffusion versus linear ballistic accumulation: different models but the same conclusions about psychological processes? Psychon Bull Rev. 2011;18: 61–69. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0022-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Moscoso del Prado Martin F. A theory of reaction time distributions. 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Barndorff-Nielsen O, BlÆsild P, Halgreen C. First hitting time models for the generalized inverse Gaussian distribution. Stochastic Processes and their Applications. Elsevier; 1978;7: 49–54. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Cutsuridis V. Behavioural and computational varieties of response inhibition in eye movements. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2017;372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Logan GD, Van Zandt T, Verbruggen F, Wagenmakers EJ. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. Psychol Rev. 2014;121: 66–95. doi: 10.1037/a0035230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Cutsuridis V, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Perantonis S. A neural network model of decision making in an antisaccade task by the superior colliculus. Neural Networks. 2007;20: 690–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2007.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Cutsuridis V, Kumari V, Ettinger U. Antisaccade performance in schizophrenia: a neural model of decision making in the superior colliculus. Front Neurosci. 2014;8: 13 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cutsuridis V. Neural competition via lateral inhibition between decision processes and not a STOP signal accounts for the antisaccade performance in healthy and schizophrenia subjects. Front Neurosci. 2015;9: 5 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Brown JW, Bullock D, Grossberg S. How laminar frontal cortex and basal ganglia circuits interact to control planned and reactive saccades. Neural Netw. 2004;17: 471–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2003.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Heinzle J, Hepp K, Martin KA. A microcircuit model of the frontal eye fields. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 9341–9353. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0974-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Wiecki TV, Frank MJ. A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. Psychol Rev. 2013;120: 329–355. doi: 10.1037/a0031542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Carpenter R, Noorani I. Movement suppression: brain mechanisms for stopping and stillness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2017;372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Boucher L, Palmeri TJ, Logan GD, Schall JD. Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. Psychol Rev. 2007;114: 376–397. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Schmidt R, Berke JD. A Pause-then-Cancel model of stopping: evidence from basal ganglia neurophysiology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2017;372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Bissett PG. The countermanding task revisited: mimicry of race models. J Neurosci. 2013;33: 12150–12151. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2091-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Schall JD, Palmeri TJ, Logan GD. Models of inhibitory control. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2017;372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Schmidt R, Leventhal DK, Mallet N, Chen F, Berke JD. Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathways. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16: 1118–1124. doi: 10.1038/nn.3456 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Stuphorn V, Schall JD. Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9: 925–931. doi: 10.1038/nn1714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Pouget P, Murthy A, Stuphorn V. Cortical control and performance monitoring of interrupting and redirecting movements. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2017;372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Reuter B, Rakusan L, Kathmanna N. Poor antisaccade performance in schizophrenia: an inhibition deficit? Psychiatry Res. 2005;135: 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Reuter B, Jager M, Bottlender R, Kathmann N. Impaired action control in schizophrenia: the role of volitional saccade initiation. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45: 1840–1848. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Coe BC, Munoz DP. Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti-saccade task. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2017;372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Hakvoort Schwerdtfeger RM, Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Stroman PW, Munoz DP. Preparatory neural networks are impaired in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during the antisaccade task. Neuroimage Clin. 2012;2: 63–78. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Cameron IG, Pari G, Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Stroman PW, et al. Impaired executive function signals in motor brain regions in Parkinson's disease. Neuroimage. 2012;60: 1156–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Witiuk K, Fernandez-Ruiz J, McKee R, Alahyane N, Coe BC, Melanson M, et al. Cognitive deterioration and functional compensation in ALS measured with fMRI using an inhibitory task. J Neurosci. 2014;34: 14260–14271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1111-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Remijnse PL, van den Heuvel OA, Nielen MM, Vriend C, Hendriks GJ, Hoogendijk WJ, et al. Cognitive inflexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and major depression is associated with distinct neural correlates. PLoS ONE. 2013;8: e59600 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059600 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.