Abstract
Black Americans continue to have higher rates of HIV disease than other races/ethnicities. Conventional individual-level risk behaviors do not fully account for these racial/ethnic disparities. Sexual concurrency may help explain them. Respondent-driven sampling (RDS) was used to enroll 508 sexually active 18 to 30 year old Black men and women in Durham, North Carolina in a cross-sectional survey on HIV-related topics. Consistent condom use was low for all participants, especially with steady partners. Concurrent partnerships in the past six months were relatively common for both men (38%) and women (25%). In general, men involved in concurrent relationships engaged in more risk behaviors than other men (e.g., inconsistent condom use and alcohol and drug use). A majority of concurrent partnerships involved steady partners. HIV prevention programs should address the risks of concurrency and factors that discourage condom use, especially with steady partners with whom condom use is particularly low.
Keywords: Black young adults, HIV risk, concurrency, respondent-driven sampling
Introduction
Black Americans continue to face the greatest burden from HIV/AIDS with higher rates of infection and death than other races/ethnicities (CDC, 2012a, 2012b, 2015). Furthermore, HIV continues to disproportionately impact young adults (ages 20–29) especially young Black adults (CDC, 2014).
In 2013, the rate of new HIV diagnoses for adult/adolescent non-Hispanic Blacks in North Carolina (55.8 per 100,000 population) was 8.2 times greater than that for adult/adolescent whites (6.8 per 100,000 population). Non-Hispanic Black individuals accounted for 968 (64%) of the 1,513 HIV disease cases diagnosed in NC in 2013 (NC Division of Public Health, 2014). Durham, North Carolina is a microcosm of national and state HIV-related disparities. From 2012 to 2014, Blacks (37% of the county population) accounted for 69.7% of all newly diagnosed HIV cases in the county and young adults ages 20–29 (17.1% of the county population) accounted for 42% of newly diagnosed HIV cases (NC HIV/STD Surveillance Unit, 2015). In 2013, Durham County had the 4th highest 3-year average HIV disease rate (25.7 per 100,000 population) among the state’s 100 counties (NC Division of Public Health, 2014).
Individual-level risk behaviors that are associated with HIV transmission include suboptimal condom use, large number of sexual partners, the presence of other STIs, early age at first sexual intercourse and alcohol/drug use before sex (Adimora et al., 2006). These conventional individual-level risk behaviors are not sufficient to fully explain the racial disparity that exists in HIV-infection rates (Adimora & Schoenbach, 2005; Halfors, Iritani, Miller, & Bauer, 2007).
There is evidence suggesting that concurrency, or simultaneous sexual partnerships overlapping in time, may help explain increased HIV/STI rates among Blacks (Adimora, Schoenbach, & Doherty, 2006; Kretzschmar, & Morris, 1996; Morris, Kurth, Mamilton, Moody, & Wakefield, 2009; Neaigus, Jenness, Hagan, Murrill, & Wendel, 2012). At the population level concurrent sexual partnerships increase network connectivity, resulting in sexual bridging of individuals within subgroups at higher risk with those of lower-risk subgroups. Such partnerships also reduce the time between sexual contacts. Both aspects of concurrency may contribute to the spread of HIV/STIs ( Adimora, Schoenbach, & Doherty, 2006; Doherty, Shiboski, Ellen, Adimora & Padian, 2006; Boily, Alary, & Baggaley, 2012). A number of social factors have been proposed as contributing to concurrency in Black communities. Most focus on lower male-to-female ratios and, consequently, on reduced numbers of available sexual/marriage partners for Black women due to high rates of unemployment, incarceration, and premature mortality among Black males (Dolwick Grieb, Davey-Rothwell, and Latkin, 2012)
Previous studies of concurrency and individual-level risk behaviors suggest that Blacks are more likely to use condoms with a “non-main” or “non-steady” partner than with a “main” or “steady” partner (Richards, Risser, Padgett, Rehman, Wolverton & Arafat, 2008; Senn, Carey, Vanable, Coury-Doniger & Urban, 2009). This may indicate that condom use, along with other HIV/STI risk behaviors, is modified by the perception of risk involved in being sexually active with a particular type of partner (Macaluso, Demand, Artz & Hook (2000); Nunn et al., 2011; Richards et al., 2008).
Consideration of concurrency raises the issue of whether HIV prevention programs should specifically address concurrency and if so, whether such programs can reduce its practice. Klichman and Grebler (2010) have argued against special interventions that focus on lowering rates of concurrency. They contend that because the role in HIV epidemics of multiple sex partners, whether concurrent or serial, is more clearly established than that of concurrency, resources should be focused on interventions aimed at reducing multiple partners. However, the work of Morris et al. (2009) supports a special focus on concurrency. They developed a stochastic network model using race-stratified data from U.S. surveys that collected information on both concurrency and assortative mixing (the tendency of people to develop partnerships with others who have a similar risk profile (Doherty et al., 2006)). The model indicated that even a small reduction in rates of concurrency among African Americans could have a profound effect on rates of HIV transmission within that population and significantly reduce Black/White disparities in HIV/AIDS. Also using a stochastic network model, Enns, Brandeau, Igeme & Bendavid (2011) came to similar conclusions about the potential impact of concurrency reduction programs on HIV incidence in sub-Saharan Africa.
There is some evidence that concurrency rates can be reduced via context-driven interventions. The “Zero Grazing” media campaign in Uganda, which addressed concurrency in a direct but culturally appropriate manner, is credited with a dramatic reduction in that nation’s HIV incidence rates (Epstein, 2007). Frye et al. (2013) conducted an HIV prevention intervention for African American men in New York City, where they observed a significant reduction in concurrency from baseline to three-month follow-up. Using a community-based participatory research approach, Andrasik et al. (2012) developed simple messages on concurrency that they then used as the focus of an HIV prevention campaign for the Black community (both African-American and African-born) in Seattle, Washington. A preliminary evaluation of the campaign (Andrasik, Clad, Bove, Tsegaselassie, & Morris, 2015) yielded positive changes in knowledge, attitudes, and behavioral intentions regarding concurrency.
LinCS 2 Durham
Linking Communities and Scientists was a 5-year community-based participatory research project in Durham, North Carolina that sought to link scientists with community members in exploring HIV prevention efforts. Details of the project have been described elsewhere (MacQueen et al., 2015). The project was guided by a Collaborative Council that included grassroots community stakeholders, potential research participants, advocates and policy-makers in allied fields, program managers for HIV and allied service areas, and researchers. As part of this project, a survey was conducted with Black young adults to describe HIV knowledge, attitudes, and risk behaviors. The current analysis expands existing literature by: 1) identifying risk behaviors across four types of sexual partners: “steady”, “casual”, “one-night stand”, and “baby’s parent,” 2) exploring differences in risk behaviors between those in concurrent and non-concurrent partnerships, and 3) ascertaining the combinations of partners involved in concurrent partnerships.
METHODS
We conducted a cross-sectional survey informed by literature review and formative data gathered from community mapping, participant observation, and focus groups. Formative findings aided recruitment design, incentive levels, survey locations, and survey design including local terminology and the framing of questions. Throughout the design and conduct of the research activities, we solicited extensive input and feedback from the Collaborative Council. All study materials were reviewed and approved by institutional review boards at FHI 360 and North Carolina Central University (NCCU).
We sought to recruit a diverse and representative sample of sexually active Black young adults using respondent-driven sampling (RDS) (Heckathorn, 1997). In this chain referral sampling method, primary incentives were used to compensate individuals for time spent completing the survey ($50), and secondary incentives were used to compensate individuals for successfully referring other participants to the survey ($10 each). These incentives reduce bias toward the most cooperative participants. Participants were asked to recruit up to three of their peers using a system of coupons to track recruitment links. Recruitment quotas are set to (1) prevent overrepresentation of referrals from participants with large networks; and (2) encourage long recruitment chains to help reach a diverse group of participants less dependent on the initial recruits. (Heckathorn, 1997, 2002) Respondent-driven sampling has been used successfully in numerous HIV studies in the U.S. and elsewhere to access hard-to-reach, at-risk populations for which generating a probabilistic sampling frame would not be feasible. (Malekinejad et al., 2015; Musyoki et al., 2015; Neaigus, Jenness, Hagan, Murrill & Wendel, 2012; Rapues, Wilson, Packer, Colfax, & Fisher, 2010; Rhodes & McCoy, 2015; Tun et al., 2015; Villanti, German, Sifakis, Flynn, and Holtgrave, 2012)
Eligibility requirements included self-identifying as Black or African American; living in Durham County during the previous six months; being 18–30 years old; and reporting vaginal or anal sex in the previous six months. Women reporting sex with women only were excluded due to low HIV transmission rates in this group.
Enrolled participants were interviewed face-to-face to collect screening data and information about their social networks. Network size estimation is important for assessing homophily (a form of recruitment bias) when using RDS (Heckathorn, 2002). Participants were asked a series of questions on how many Black people they knew who were 18 to 30 years old, living in Durham county, and sexually active in the past six months. Participants then completed an audio-computer assisted survey interview (ACASI).
All data collection activities took place at community-based venues including a historically black university (NCCU), a shopping mall, a housing development, and a non-profit community health center. Enrollment began May 2011 and ended June 2012.
Measures
For this analysis we focused on sexual behaviors associated with risk of HIV acquisition. Sexual activity was explored through a series of questions beginning with the number of sexual partners a person had vaginal or anal sex with in his or her lifetime. Participants were asked how many of their partners were men to determine gender-based categories of sexual partnerships (e.g., men who have sex with men (MSM)). Participants were then asked if they had vaginal or anal sex with each of four types of partners in the previous six months: (1) a steady partner (spouse, boyfriend or girlfriend for whom they had feelings), (2) casual partners (e.g., “friends with benefits” or “booty calls”), (3) one-night stands, and (4) baby’s parent (someone with whom they had a child but were not currently in a committed relationship). Consistency of condom use with each type of partner was assessed as well as alcohol and drug use before and during sex.
Sexual concurrency was measured using the UNAIDS recommended approach, defined as overlapping sexual partnerships in which sexual intercourse with one partner occurs between two acts of intercourse with another partner (Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS, 2009). Participants were asked the first and last month of sex for each of the three most recent sex partners in the past six months. If one partnership ended and another began in the same month those partnerships were not considered concurrent. Given that some of these partnerships may have overlapped, we explored the robustness of our results by also applying a broader definition that considered as concurrent those partnerships ending and beginning in the same month.
Participants were asked about associated risks including incarceration, alcohol and drug use in the previous six months and exchanging sex for money, drugs or other resources (e.g. groceries, paying bills, transportation, and clothing).
In addition to demographic questions, participants were asked if they had ever been tested for HIV other than when donating blood. Participants were also asked eight questions regarding their knowledge of HIV (Vaccari, Poonam & Franchini, 2010; Van de Ven et al., 2002).
Statistical Analyses
We used RDS Analysis Tool (RDSAT 7.1) (Volz et. al, 2012) to estimate the population distribution of major descriptive variables in order to assess the representativeness of the sample, adjusting for biases in the chain referral sampling based on homophily and reported network size (Heckathorn, 2002; Salganik &Heckathorn, 2004).
Based on their gender and lifetime sexual partner experience, we categorized participants into five groups: men who have sex with women, men who have sex with men, men who have sex with men and women, women who have sex with men, women who have sex with men and women. We then reviewed basic demographic criteria (education, income, age) for each group.
We compared those who were or were not in a concurrent partnership within the previous six months by demographic characteristics and HIV behavioral risk factors based on the literature and selected prior to the analysis. Finally, we explored the types of partners involved in participants’ concurrent partnerships by generating separate cross tabulations by gender that identified the frequency of various combinations of partners (steady, casual, one-night stand, or baby’s parent).
Differences were tested using chi-square or Fisher’s exact tests for categorical variables and t-tests for continuous variables. Tests were considered significant at the .05 alpha level for two-sided comparisons.
Results
We screened 568 individuals; of these, 513 were confirmed eligible and included in the study and 508 in the analysis. Because all analyses examined differences by gender, four participants were excluded due to missing or contradictory gender responses and one person identified as transgender was also excluded.
As shown in Table 1, RDS analysis indicated that we recruited a diverse sample of the eligible Black population of Durham. Homophily was low for key selected demographic characteristics (e.g., gender, age, and education) and weighted percentages varied little from observed percentages (MacQueen et al. 2015). Additional association analyses were conducted without RDS adjustments.
Table 1.
Seed (n=12) n (%) |
All Participants (n=508) n (%) |
Weighted analysis Adj. % (95% CI) | Homophily Hx |
2008–2012 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates for Blacks in Durham, NC1 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | For 18–29 year olds | ||||
Male | 6 (50.0) | 235 (46.3) | 44.7 (3.8–52.6) | 0.38 | 8,320 (42.4) |
Female | 6 (50.0) | 273 (53.7) | 55.3 (47.9–63.2) | 0.30 | 11,304 (57.6) |
Age (years) | |||||
18–20 years | 1 ( 8.3) | 154 (30.3) | 28.7 (21.4–35.9) | 0.35 | 18–19 yrs: 4,081 (20.8) |
21–24 years | 8 (66.7) | 205 (40.4) | 37.5 (30.8–45.0) | 0.21 | 20–24 yrs: 8,633 (44.0) |
25–30 years | 3 (25.0) | 149 (29.3) | 33.8 (25.1–43.2) | 0.22 | 25–29: 6,910 (35.2) |
Education Level | For ≥ 25 year olds | ||||
< High school | 1 ( 8.3) | 71 (14.0) | 16.4 (10.2–23.2) | 0.13 | 8,2423 (14.8) |
High school or GED | 3 (25.0) | 187 (36.8) | 38.5 (31.2–47.0) | 0.13 | 12,819 (23.0) |
Some college, assoc. degree, or tech. certif. | 4 (33.3) | 218 (42.9) | 38.7 (31.2–46.4) | 0.26 | 17,568 (31.5) |
≥ Bachelor’s degree | 4 (33.3) | 32 ( 6.3) | 6.4 (2.7–10.2) | 0.12 | 17,079 (30.7) |
Employment | For 20–24 year olds | ||||
Employed | 5 (45.5) | 222 (44.3) | 42.2 (33.0–47.1) | 0.22 | 4,651 (52,1) |
Student | 4 (36.4) | 135 (26.9) | 27.2 (20.3–33.9) | 0.30 | Not in labor force: 1,350 (32.1%) |
Unemployed | 2 (18.2) | 144 (28.7) | 30.6 (26.0–40.6) | 0.19 | 1,406 (15.8) |
U.S. Census Bureau, American FactFinder.
Though diverse, our sample was not fully representative of the Black young adult population of Durham. Most notably, while RDS proved successful at recruiting participants in all but one 2010 Durham County census tract with more than 300 Black residents, that tract had a median household income in the top 25 % for all Black households in the county. We were also unsuccessful recruiting participants in suburban and rural census tracts outside the city limits. To compare our sample to the larger population we used data from the 2008–2012 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates for Blacks in the city of Durham (U.S. Census, American FactFinder, 2015). As noted in the last column of Table 1, the categories for gender, age, education level, and employment status used in that survey are not precisely the same as those used in our study. Nevertheless, the comparison does suggest that our sample is slightly tilted towards males and is somewhat younger, less educated, and more likely to be unemployed than Black young adults in the city of Durham overall.
More women (273) were enrolled in our study than men (235), but the female-to-male sex ratio of 1.16, was lower than the 1.36 sex ratio for 18–29 year old African Americans in Durham. Within our sample of 18–30 year olds we had fewer older participants; 29.3% were 25–30 years old while 35.2% of Durham’s 18–29 year old Blacks were 25–29. In terms of education, about the same proportion of our sample had less than a high school education as the larger population (14.0% vs. 14.8% of Durham residents 25 and older), but the proportion with just high school diplomas or GEDs was considerably higher (36.8% vs. 23.0% ) as was the proportion with some higher education but less than a bachelor’s degree (42.9% vs. 31.5%). The proportion with at least a bachelor’s degree was substantially lower (6.3% vs. 30.7%). No doubt this difference is explained in part by the younger age of our sample (18–30) compared to the comparison group (25 or older), but perhaps not entirely since our sample was also much more likely to be unemployed than Durham Blacks age 20–24 years old (28.7% vs. 15.8%).
In terms of sexual partnering behavior, 44 (16.1%) of the 273 women recruited reported ever having had vaginal or anal sex with a woman. (All these women also reported sex with men because, as noted earlier, women who reported no vaginal or anal sex with a man in the previous six months were excluded from the study.) Among the 235 men recruited, 20 (8.5%, reported ever having had anal sex with a man, with 9 of those (3.8%,) reporting same sex relationships exclusively. An analysis of the 44 women who had sex with both men and women (WSMW) and the 11 men who reported having had sex with both men and women (MSMW) indicated that these groups did not differ on basic demographic criteria (education, income, age) from women who reported only having sex with men (WSM) and men who reported only having sex with women (MSW), respectively. We therefore collapsed WSM and WSMW and also MSW and MSWM to allow for more robust sample sizes for additional analysis. We excluded the 9 men who reported only same sex relationships from further analyses. This decision reflects our concern that, on sex-related variables of interest in this study, men who exclusively partner with men may differ in substantive ways from other men. The small number of these men in our sample prevents exploration of this possibility in a meaningful way.
Sex and Risk Variables
Men reported considerably more sexual partners than women (Table 2). Women were more likely than men to report relationships with a steady partner (“a spouse or boyfriend or girlfriend you have feelings for”) in the past six months while men were more likely to report casual partners and one-night stands.
Table 2.
Male (N=2261) | Female (N=2731) | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Total lifetime partners | <.0001 | ||
| |||
1 | 3 ( 1.3) | 10 ( 3.7) | |
| |||
2–5 | 45 (20.0) | 91 (33.3) | |
| |||
6–7 | 25 (11.1) | 53 (19.4) | |
| |||
8–10 | 30 (13.3) | 41 (15.0) | |
| |||
11–25 | 57 (25.3) | 62 (22.7) | |
| |||
More than 25 | 65 (28.9) | 16 ( 5.9) | |
| |||
Sex partners, by type in last 6 months2 | |||
Spouse, boyfriend, or girlfriend | 200 (88.5) | 258 (94.5) | 0.0150 |
Casual partner, one-night stand, baby’s parent | 179 (79.2) | 157 (57.5) | <0.0001 |
| |||
Total partners in past 6 months | <.0001 | ||
| |||
Mean (SD) | 6.0 (7.3) | 3.1 (4.2) | |
| |||
Median (Range) | 4 (1 to 75) | 2 (1 to 47) | |
| |||
Number of partners in past 6 months - Steady | 0.0304 | ||
| |||
Mean (SD) | 1.8 (1.8) | 1.5 (1.6) | |
| |||
Median (Range) | 1 (0 to 12) | 1 (0 to 22) | |
| |||
Number of partners in past 6 months - Casual | <.0001 | ||
| |||
Mean (SD) | 2.9 (4.7) | 1.1 (2.1) | |
| |||
Median (Range) | 2 (0 to 50) | 1 (0 to 25) | |
| |||
Number of partners in past 6 months - One-night stand | <.0001 | ||
| |||
Mean (SD) | 1.1 (2.3) | 0.3 (1.3) | |
| |||
Median (Range) | 0 (0 to 25) | 0 (0 to 19) | |
| |||
Number of partners in past 6 months - Baby’s parent | 0.7048 | ||
| |||
Mean (SD) | 0.2 (0.5) | 0.2 (0.5) | |
| |||
Median (Range) | 0 (0 to 3) | 0 (0 to 3) | |
| |||
Engaged in concurrent relationship in last 6 months | 0.0019 | ||
| |||
No | 139 (61.8) | 204 (74.7) | |
| |||
Yes | 86 (38.2) | 69 (25.3) | |
| |||
Condom use in last 6 months (with all partners) | <0.0001 | ||
| |||
Consistent | 65 (28.8) | 46 (16.8) | |
| |||
Inconsistent | 142 (62.8) | 162 (59.3) | |
| |||
Never | 19 (8.4) | 65 (23.8) | |
| |||
Condom Use With | |||
| |||
-Steady Partner | 0.0008 | ||
| |||
Always | 55 (27.4) | 51 (19.8) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 116 (57.7) | 131 (50.8) | |
| |||
Never | 30 (14.9) | 76 (29.5) | |
| |||
Total | 201 | 258 | |
| |||
-Casual Partner | 0.0188 | ||
| |||
Always | 97 (56.7) | 63 (45.0) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 64 (37.4) | 57 (40.7) | |
| |||
Never | 10 ( 5.8) | 20 (14.3) | |
| |||
Total | 171 | 140 | |
| |||
-One-Night Stand | 0.0997 | ||
| |||
Always | 75 (75.8) | 33 (61.1) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 20 (20.2) | 15 (27.8) | |
| |||
Never | 4 ( 4.0) | 6 (11.1) | |
| |||
Total | 99 | 54 | |
| |||
-Baby’s Parent | 0.3551 | ||
| |||
Always | 9 (25.7) | 7 (15.2) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 18 (51.4) | 23 (50.0) | |
| |||
Never | 8 (22.9) | 16 (34.8) | |
| |||
Total | 35 | 46 | |
| |||
Ever used alcohol before/during sex in last 6 months | 0.0369 | ||
| |||
No | 56 (24.8) | 91 (33.3) | |
| |||
Yes | 170 (75.2) | 182 (66.7) | |
| |||
Use of Alcohol Before or During Sex With | |||
| |||
-Steady Partner | 0.0357 | ||
| |||
Always | 11 ( 5.5) | 5 ( 1.9) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 138 (68.7) | 166 (64.3) | |
| |||
Never | 52 (25.9) | 87 (33.7) | |
| |||
Total | 201 | 258 | |
| |||
-Casual Partner | 0.1441 | ||
| |||
Always | 17 ( 9.9) | 7 ( 5.0) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 99 (57.9) | 77 (55.0) | |
| |||
Never | 55 (32.2) | 56 (40.0) | |
| |||
Total | 171 | 140 | |
| |||
-One-Night Stand | 0.8331 | ||
| |||
Always | 19 (19.2) | 9 (16.7) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 41 (41.4) | 25 (46.3) | |
| |||
Never | 39 (39.4) | 20 (37.0) | |
| |||
Total | 99 | 54 | |
| |||
-Baby’s Parent | 0.602 | ||
| |||
Always | 1 ( 2.9) | 3 ( 6.4) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 20 (57.1) | 22 (46.8) | |
| |||
Never | 14 (40.0) | 22 (46.8) | |
| |||
Total | 35 | 47 | |
| |||
Ever used drugs before/during sex in last 6 months | 0.0008 | ||
| |||
No | 101 (44.7) | 163 (59.7) | |
| |||
Yes | 125 (55.3) | 110 (40.3) | |
| |||
Use of Drugs Before or During Sex With | |||
| |||
-Steady Partner | 0.0266 | ||
| |||
Always | 12 ( 6.0) | 12 ( 4.7) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 92 (45.8) | 89 (34.5) | |
| |||
Never | 97 (48.3) | 157 (60.9) | |
| |||
Total | 201 | 258 | |
| |||
-Casual Partner | 0.2176 | ||
| |||
Always | 10 ( 5.8) | 9 ( 6.4) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 84 (49.1) | 55 (39.3) | |
| |||
Never | 77 (45.0) | 76 (54.3) | |
| |||
Total | 171 | 140 | |
| |||
-One-Night Stand | 0.916 | ||
| |||
Always | 13 (13.1) | 8 (14.8) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 36 (36.4) | 18 (33.3) | |
| |||
Never | 50 (50.5) | 28 (51.9) | |
| |||
Total | 99 | 54 | |
| |||
-Baby’s Parent | 0.0605 | ||
| |||
Always | 4 (11.4) | 1 ( 2.1) | |
| |||
Sometimes | 17 (48.6) | 17 (36.2) | |
| |||
Never | 14 (40.0) | 29 (61.7) | |
| |||
Total | 35 | 47 | |
| |||
Incarcerated Ever | <.0001 | ||
| |||
No | 136 (60.4) | 224 (82.1) | |
| |||
Yes | 89 (39.6) | 49 (17.9) | |
| |||
Incarcerated in Last 6 Months | 0.0052 | ||
| |||
No | 200 (88.5) | 260 (95.2) | |
| |||
Yes | 26 (11.5) | 13 ( 4.8) | |
| |||
Exchanged sex for drugs, money, or other in last 6 months | 0.5482 | ||
| |||
No | 214 (94.7) | 255 (93.4) | |
| |||
Yes | 12 ( 5.3) | 18 ( 6.6) |
Totals may vary due to missing data.
Had at least one partner of this type in last 6 months.
Consistent condom use was low for all participants, but men were more likely than women to report consistent use with all partners and less likely to report never using condoms. This gender difference held for each type of partner and was significant for steady and casual partners. For both men and women, consistent condom use was highest with one-night stands, lower with casual partners, and lowest with steady partners and mother or father of their baby.
Women were less likely than men to report engaging in concurrent partnerships in the last six months. They were also less likely than men to report use of alcohol or drugs before or during sex. When broken down by partner type, only alcohol and drug use differences with a steady partner were significant.
Concurrency
There were no differences in age, income, HIV knowledge or HIV testing experience between persons who did and did not report concurrency in the past six months (Table 3). Women who reported concurrency had more education than women who did not. More men in concurrent partnerships reported a history of incarceration than other men, but the difference fell short of statistical significance.
Table 3.
Male | Female | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Concurrency | Concurrency | |||
| ||||
Yes (N=86) | No (N=139) | Yes (N=69) | No (N=204) | |
| ||||
Age | ||||
Mean (SD) | 23.3 (3.4) | 22.6 (3.4) | 23.1 (3.5) | 22.8 (3.4) |
Median (Range) | 23 (18 to 30) | 21 (18 to 30) | 22 (18 to 30) | 22 (18 to 30) |
Total | 86 | 139 | 69 | 204 |
P-value | 0.1259 | 0.4957 | ||
Education Level | ||||
| ||||
Less than high school | 10 (11.6) | 19 (13.7) | 2 ( 2.9) | 39 (19.1) |
| ||||
High school or GED | 28 (32.6) | 51 (36.7) | 27 (39.1) | 77 (37.7) |
| ||||
Some college, Technical Certification, or Associates Degree | 42 (48.8) | 60 (43.2) | 35 (50.7) | 76 (37.3) |
| ||||
Bachelors Degree or higher | 6 ( 7.0) | 9 ( 6.5) | 5 ( 7.2) | 12 ( 5.9) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.8429 | 0.0093 | ||
| ||||
Income Level | ||||
| ||||
Less than $10,000 | 51 (59.3) | 87 (67.4) | 49 (73.1) | 133 (67.9) |
| ||||
$10,000 to $14,999 | 14 (16.3) | 14 (10.9) | 7 (10.4) | 32 (16.3) |
| ||||
$15,000 to $24,999 | 9 (10.5) | 15 (11.6) | 9 (13.4) | 15 ( 7.7) |
| ||||
$25,000 to $49,999 | 11 (12.8) | 12 ( 9.3) | 2 ( 3.0) | 15 ( 7.7) |
| ||||
$50,000 or more | 1 ( 1.2) | 1 ( 0.8) | 0 ( 0.0) | 1 ( 0.5) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.6568 | 0.2709 | ||
| ||||
Incarcerated Ever | ||||
No | 45 (52.3) | 90 (65.2) | 56 (81.2) | 168 (82.4) |
Yes | 41 (47.7) | 48 (34.8) | 13 (18.8) | 36 (17.6) |
P-Value | 0.0552 | 0.8233 | ||
Incarcerated in Last 6 Months | ||||
No | 75 (87.2) | 124 (89.2) | 67 (97.1) | 193 (94.6) |
Yes | 11 (12.8) | 15 (10.8) | 2 ( 2.9) | 11 ( 5.4) |
P-Value | 0.6485 | 0.5269 | ||
HIV Knowledge | ||||
| ||||
Mean (SD) | 7.1 (0.9) | 6.8 (1.1) | 7.4 (0.8) | 7.1 (1.1) |
| ||||
Median (Range) | 7 (4 to 8) | 7 (3 to 8) | 8 (4 to 8) | 7 (3 to 8) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.0628 | 0.1237 | ||
| ||||
Ever tested for HIV | ||||
| ||||
Yes | 67 (79.8) | 97 (71.3) | 58 (85.3) | 178 (88.6) |
| ||||
No | 17 (20.2) | 39 (28.7) | 10 (14.7) | 23 (11.4) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.1627 | 0.4783 | ||
| ||||
Frequency of Alcohol Use in Last 6 Months | ||||
| ||||
Never | 9 (10.5) | 24 (17.3) | 4 ( 5.8) | 43 (21.1) |
| ||||
Rarely | 11 (12.8) | 34 (24.5) | 18 (26.1) | 62 (30.4) |
| ||||
Monthly but not weekly | 34 (39.5) | 43 (30.9) | 33 (47.8) | 66 (32.4) |
| ||||
Weekly but not daily | 28 (32.6) | 33 (23.7) | 12 (17.4) | 29 (14.2) |
| ||||
About every day or more | 4 ( 4.7) | 5 ( 3.6) | 2 ( 2.9) | 4 ( 2.0) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.0875 | 0.0236 | ||
| ||||
Ever Gone on Drinking Binge in Last 6 Months | ||||
| ||||
No | 55 (71.4) | 87 (75.7) | 50 (76.9) | 134 (83.2) |
| ||||
Yes | 22 (28.6) | 28 (24.3) | 15 (23.1) | 27 (16.8) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.5134 | 0.2699 | ||
| ||||
Unable to Remember Night Before Drinking in Last 6 Months | ||||
| ||||
Never | 49 (63.6) | 88 (76.5) | 49 (75.4) | 137 (85.1) |
| ||||
Less than monthly | 25 (32.5) | 20 (17.4) | 11 (16.9) | 17 (10.6) |
| ||||
At least once a month | 3 ( 3.9) | 7 ( 6.1) | 5 ( 7.7) | 7 ( 4.3) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.0505 | 0.221 | ||
| ||||
Frequency of Ecstasy/Marijuana/Pills Use in Last 6 Months | ||||
| ||||
Never | 24 (27.9) | 62 (44.6) | 29 (42.0) | 115 (56.4) |
| ||||
Rarely | 11 (12.8) | 28 (20.1) | 16 (23.2) | 26 (12.7) |
| ||||
Monthly but not weekly | 7 ( 8.1) | 8 ( 5.8) | 5 ( 7.2) | 14 ( 6.9) |
| ||||
Weekly but not daily | 20 (23.3) | 18 (12.9) | 7 (10.1) | 25 (12.3) |
| ||||
About every day or more | 24 (27.9) | 23 (16.5) | 12 (17.4) | 24 (11.8) |
| ||||
P-value | 0.0134 | 0.1301 |
Totals may vary due to missing data.
Both men and women in concurrent partnerships reported more alcohol use than their counterparts who did not report concurrency, but the difference was only significant for women. Both men and women in concurrent partnerships were more likely than others to report episodes of binge drinking and of being unable to remember what they had done the night before on a day after drinking, but the differences were not significant. Men in concurrent partnerships were significantly more likely than other men to report use of ecstasy, marijuana, or pills.
There were no significant differences in condom use between women who did and did not report concurrent partnerships (Table 4). Men in concurrent partnerships reported less condom use than other men with all partners except one-night stands; however, these differences were not significant.
Table 4.
Male | Female | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Concurrency | Concurrency | |||
Yes | No | Yes | No | |
Consistent Condom Use With | ||||
-Steady Partner | ||||
No | 59 (78.7) | 87 (69.0) | 51 (81.0) | 156 (80.0) |
Yes | 16 (21.3) | 39 (31.0) | 12 (19.0) | 39 (20.0) |
Total | 75 | 126 | 63 | 195 |
P-value | 0.139 | 0.8689 | ||
-Casual Partner | ||||
No | 36 (45.0) | 38 (42.2) | 30 (50.8) | 47 (58.0) |
Yes | 44 (55.0) | 52 (57.8) | 29 (49.2) | 34 (42.0) |
Total | 80 | 90 | 59 | 81 |
P-value | 0.7154 | 0.3993 | ||
-One-Night Stand | ||||
No | 9 (18.4) | 15 (30.6) | 6 (37.5) | 15 (39.5) |
Yes | 40 (81.6) | 34 (69.4) | 10 (62.5) | 23 (60.5) |
Total | 49 | 49 | 16 | 38 |
P-value | 0.1587 | 0.8919 | ||
-Baby’s Parent | ||||
No | 19 (86.4) | 7 (53.8) | 13 (92.9) | 26 (81.3) |
Yes | 3 (13.6) | 6 (46.2) | 1 ( 7.1) | 6 (18.8) |
Total | 22 | 13 | 14 | 32 |
P-value | 0.0505 | 0.4131 | ||
Any Reported Use of Alcohol Before or During Sex With | ||||
-Steady Partner | ||||
No | 13 (17.3) | 39 (31.0) | 18 (28.6) | 69 (35.4) |
Yes | 62 (82.7) | 87 (69.0) | 45 (71.4) | 126 (64.6) |
Total | 75 | 126 | 63 | 195 |
P-value | 0.033 | 0.32 | ||
-Casual Partner | ||||
No | 18 (22.5) | 36 (40.0) | 26 (44.1) | 30 (37.0) |
Yes | 62 (77.5) | 54 (60.0) | 33 (55.9) | 51 (63.0) |
Total | 80 | 90 | 59 | 81 |
P-value | 0.0144 | 0.4018 | ||
-One-Night Stand | ||||
No | 14 (28.6) | 24 (49.0) | 3 (18.8) | 17 (44.7) |
Yes | 35 (71.4) | 25 (51.0) | 13 (81.3) | 21 (55.3) |
Total | 49 | 49 | 16 | 38 |
P-value | 0.0382 | 0.071 | ||
-Baby’s Parent | ||||
No | 8 (36.4) | 6 (46.2) | 6 (42.9) | 16 (48.5) |
Yes | 14 (63.6) | 7 (53.8) | 8 (57.1) | 17 (51.5) |
Total | 22 | 13 | 14 | 33 |
P-value | 0.5678 | 0.7236 | ||
Any Reported Use of Drugs Before or During Sex With | ||||
-Steady Partner | ||||
No | 28 (37.3) | 69 (54.8) | 36 (57.1) | 121 (62.1) |
Yes | 47 (62.7) | 57 (45.2) | 27 (42.9) | 74 (37.9) |
Total | 75 | 126 | 63 | 195 |
P-value | 0.0168 | 0.4877 | ||
-Casual Partner | ||||
No | 30 (37.5) | 46 (51.1) | 33 (55.9) | 43 (53.1) |
Yes | 50 (62.5) | 44 (48.9) | 26 (44.1) | 38 (46.9) |
Total | 80 | 90 | 59 | 81 |
P-value | 0.0748 | 0.7386 | ||
-One-Night Stand | ||||
No | 21 (42.9) | 28 (57.1) | 7 (43.8) | 21 (55.3) |
Yes | 28 (57.1) | 21 (42.9) | 9 (56.3) | 17 (44.7) |
Total | 49 | 49 | 16 | 38 |
P-value | 0.1573 | 0.4394 | ||
-Baby’s Parent | ||||
No | 10 (45.5) | 4 (30.8) | 7 (50.0) | 22 (66.7) |
Yes | 12 (54.5) | 9 (69.2) | 7 (50.0) | 11 (33.3) |
Total | 22 | 13 | 14 | 33 |
P-value | 0.3915 | 0.2824 |
Men who had concurrent partnerships were more likely than other men to report alcohol use before or during sex with steady partners, casual partners and one-night stands. Men reporting concurrency were more likely to also report use of other drugs besides alcohol (e.g., marijuana and cocaine) before or during sex with steady partners. A difference with casual partners fell short of statistical significance.
Types of Partners Involved in Concurrent Partnerships
Our data allow us to identify the types of sexual partners participants were involved with in concurrent partnerships (Table 5). Each populated cell represents a unique combination of partners with two statistics. The first is the number of participants who reported at least one situation where concurrent partnerships involved that combination of partners. The second statistic is the total number of times that partnerships with this combination of partners were reported. For example, at the intersection of the row “Steady Partner” and the column “Casual Partner”, 33 women reported 38 instances where overlapping partnerships involved a casual partner and a steady partner. As noted in the row below, 20 women reported 29 situations where a partnership with one casual partner overlapped with a partnership with another casual partner. These two partner combinations accounted for 72.8% of all instances of concurrent partnerships reported by women. All concurrencies involving a steady partner (those with another steady partner, a casual partner, a one-night stand, or a baby’s parent) comprised 58.7% of women’s concurrent partnerships.
Table 5.
Steady partner | Casual partner | One-night stand | Baby’s parent | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Female (N=69) | # of Women [# of Concurrencies1] | |||
Steady partner | 6 [6] | 33 [38] | 6 [6] | 4 [4] |
Casual partner | - | 20 [29] | 3 [3] | 3 [3] |
One-night stand | - | - | 0 [0] | 2 [2] |
Baby’s parent | - | - | - | 1 [1] |
Male (N=86) | # of Men [# of Concurrencies1] | |||
Steady partner | 3 [3] | 39 [52] | 9 [10] | 7 [8] |
Casual partner | - | 35 [53] | 7 [9] | 8 [9] |
One-night stand | - | - | 1 [1] | 0 [0] |
Baby’s parent | - | - | - | 0 [0] |
Number of concurrencies equals the number of instances 2 types of sexual partnerships overlapped. Data was collected on a maximum of 3 partners in the previous 6 months.
For men, the combination of two casual partners was about as common as the combination of a steady and casual partner. As with women, these two partner combinations accounted for a substantial majority (72.4%) of reported overlapping partnerships for men. All concurrencies involving a steady partner constituted 50.3% of men’s concurrent partnerships.
Robustness of Findings under a Broader Definition of Concurrency
Defining as concurrent those partnerships where one ended and another began in the same month increased concurrency rates from 38.2% to 46.2% for men and from 25.3% to 30.4% for women. The direction of all associations between concurrency and other risk variables remained the same, though the strength of some associations shifted. One association with concurrency remained significant under both definitions: frequency of alcohol use in the last six months for women. Two differences in risk behavior between men who were and were not in concurrent partnerships became significant: being unable to remember activities the day after drinking and use of condoms with baby’s parent; several others were no longer significant: use of ecstasy/marijuana/pills in the last 6 months; alcohol use before or during sex with steady partners, casual partners and one-night stands; and use of other drugs besides alcohol before or during sex with steady partner. For women, two differences between those who were and were not in concurrent partnerships became significant: binge drinking and use of ecstasy/marijuana/pills in the last 6 months. Beyond increasing the number of partners considered, the broader definition had little effect on our analysis of the types of sexual partners that participants were involved with in concurrent partnerships.
Discussion
In this study we successfully reached a diverse group of Black young adults in Durham, North Carolina. A comparison of demographic data from our survey with those from the U.S. census indicate that our sample was not fully representative of the Black young adult population in Durham, in that our participants were, on average, less educated and more likely to be unemployed than the larger population. Our education and employment data, as well as our inability to recruit from a higher income Black census tract, indicate that we probably recruited a sample whose socio-economic status was lower than that of the overall Black young adult population in Durham. This, in turn, suggests that participants in our study may be at higher risk for HIV disease than other Black young adults in Durham since poverty is strongly associated with HIV among inner-city heterosexuals (CDC, 2011) The high rate of lifetime incarceration for (39.6%) is consistent with this risk profile, for high incarceration rates can create conditions that foster both concurrency and HIV transmission, e.g., fewer available partners for women and interrupted relationships (Harawa and Adimora, 2008).
In our survey men and women alike reported engaging in several behaviors that place them at risk for HIV and other STIs. Gender differences emerged with men having a higher overall risk profile than women, a finding that is similar to previous research (Adimora, Schoenbach, Martinson, Donaldson, Stancil & Fullilove, 2003; Magnus et al., 2009). However, men were more likely than women to report consistent condom use with steady and casual partners.
Men were more likely than women to have engaged in concurrent partnerships in the last six months (37.2% vs. 25.3%), which has been found elsewhere (Adimora et al, 2003; Adimora, Schoenbach & Doherty, 2007; Ketzschmar & Morris, 1996; Magnus et al., 2009). Despite the potential increased risk that concurrency poses, neither men nor women who engaged in concurrent partnerships reported greater use of precautions to reduce that risk. In fact, men involved in concurrent partnerships reported more risk behaviors than other men – less condom use and more alcohol and drug use before and after sex with some types of partners. These findings corroborate previous research which found risk behaviors associated with and more likely to occur in concurrent sexual partnerships (Adimora, Schoenbach & Doherty, 2007;).
Overall, participants reported using condoms with one-night stands more than with casual partners and with casual partners more than with either steady partners or the parent of their child. This suggests that participants were making decisions on condom use based on their perceived risk of sex with different types of partners, corroborating the findings of other studies (Macaluso et al., 2000; Nunn et al., 2011; Richards et al., 2008; Senn et al., 2009).
However, concurrency complicates the picture. Concurrency increases the risk associated with multiple sexual partners, and concurrent partnerships were fairly common in this sample. Furthermore, condom use was inconsistent within concurrent partnerships. About half of all concurrencies involved steady partners, with whom condoms were least likely to be used. The other partners in these concurrent relationships were usually casual partners or one-night stands, with whom condoms were used more often but still inconsistently. Consequently, it seems likely that many persons in concurrent partnerships were bringing the risk of unprotected sex with a casual partner or one-night stand into a steady partnership where unprotected sex is clearly the norm, an issue we did not find highlighted in the literature.
To explore the robustness of our findings related to concurrency, we reanalyzed data under the assumption that partnerships beginning and ending in the same month were concurrent. As expected, the expanded definition increased concurrency rates for both men and women. Although the strength of some associations between concurrency and risk variables did change under the broader definition, the direction of associations remained the same. It’s not surprising that the strength of some associations weakened because this broader definition likely included people who are not engaged in concurrent partnerships, whose risk profile may be different from those who are. It should be noted, however, that partnerships occurring very close in time increase the risk of HIV and other STI transmission even if they are not concurrent.
This study has several limitations. The cross-sectional design of the survey does not allow for causal inferences, but our descriptive analyses do not attempt to explain or predict concurrency. As noted, the sample appears to be somewhat younger, less educated, and more likely to be unemployed than the larger population of Black young adults in Durham. Therefore, survey results may have limited generalizability beyond our participants and other Black young adults like them. Since the questions we used to determine concurrency asked participants about their three most recent partners in the past 6 months, our results may underestimate concurrency regardless of the definition used. Participants who did not engage in concurrency with those partners may have had other partners with whom they had concurrent partnerships earlier in this time period. In addition, our findings may not reveal the full impact of concurrency on our participants because we collected no data on the behaviors of their partners who may or may not have been engaging in their own concurrent partnerships. This is known as indirect concurrency if an individual is not engaging in concurrency but his/her partner is ( Grieb, Davey-Rothwell, & Latkin, 2012) and as reciprocal concurrency if both parties are engaging in concurrency (Neaigus et al., 2012). Finally, the small number of men who reported sex exclusively with men prohibited any meaningful exploration of their sexual behaviors; yet this remains one of the groups at highest risk for HIV (CDC, 2012a, 2012b, 2014).
Despite these limitations, our findings suggest areas for future research and implications for public health practice. First, future research should explore whether certain combinations of partner types within concurrent partnerships are better predictors than others for HIV/STI risk behaviors and HIV/STI transmission. Second, research on concurrency should include measures of indirect and reciprocal concurrency. Finally, it should be noted that although some studies have identified factors that may contribute to concurrency – including social and economic factors (Adimora & Schoenbach, 2005), relationship dynamics (Senn, Scott-Sheldon, Seward, Wright. & Carey, 2011), and attitudes and beliefs about concurrency (Carey, Senn, Seward, and Vanable, 2010), additional research is needed on what motivates individuals to engage in concurrent relationships. This research should include the formulation and testing of a conceptual model of concurrency to explain and predict this behavior.
This study’s findings may improve public health practice for HIV and STI prevention in at least two ways. First, given the low rates of condom use with main partners, practitioners should develop risk reduction strategies that address issues of trust, commitment, stigma and other factors that may diminish condom use within steady partnerships, which are often not monogamous. These strategies could include messages to encourage condom use every time with every partner (Senn et al., 2011).
Second, practitioners should consider programming that places greater emphasis on concurrency and associated risks. This programming would encourage a calculus of risk that includes concurrency, framed in language that reflects the patterning of sexual partnerships within particular populations. While it has been argued that special interventions focusing on reducing sexual concurrency should not be pursued (Klichman and Grebler, 2010), evolving evidence suggests otherwise. As noted previously this includes findings from stochastic network models (Morris et al., 2009; Enns et al., 2011) and interventions in Uganda (Epstein, 2007) and the U.S. (Frye et al., 2013; Andrasik et al., 2012, 2015). We believe our research adds to this growing body of evidence that concurrency is a common occurrence among some Black young adults and that addressing it directly could play an important role in future efforts to reduce rates of HIV transmission in this population.
Acknowledgments
Sources of Support:
Support for this research was received from the National Institute of Nursing Research of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01NR011232. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This research was also supported (in part) by the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Center for AIDS Research (CFAR), an NIH funded program P30 AI50410.
We thank Michelle Laws, Allison Mathews, and Vanessa White for their contributions to the development of the survey. We appreciate Alexandria Horne Anderson’s assistance with developing and administering the survey. Finally we express gratitude to Angie Wheeless for her support with data analysis.
Contributor Information
David H. Jolly, North Carolina Central University, Durham NC.
Monique P. Mueller, FHI 360, Durham NC.
Mario Chen, FHI 360, Durham NC.
Le’Marus Alston, North Carolina Central University, Durham NC.
Marcus Hawley, North Carolina Central University, Durham NC.
Eunice Okumu, FHI 360, Durham NC.
Natalie T. Eley, FHI 360, Durham NC.
Tonya Stancil, Health Education Services.
Kathleen M. MacQueen, FHI 360, Durham NC.
References
- Adimora AA, Schoenbach VJ. Social context, sexual networks and racial disparities in rates of sexually transmitted infections. Journal of Infectious Diseases. 2005;191(Supplement 1):S115–S22. doi: 10.1086/425280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adimora AA, Schoenbach VJ, Doherty IA. HIV and African Americans in the southern United States: sexual networks and social context. Sexual Transmitted Diseases. 2006;33(7):S39–S45. doi: 10.1097/01.olq.0000228298.07826.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adimora AA, Schoenbach VJ, Doherty IA. Concurrent sexual partnerships among men in the United States. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97(12):2230–2237. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.099069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adimora AA, Schoenbach VJ, Martinson FE, Coyne-Beasley T, Doherty I, Stancil TR, et al. Heterosexually transmitted HIV infection among African Americans in North Carolina. Journal Of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes. 2006;41(5):616–23. doi: 10.1097/01.qai.0000191382.62070.a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adimora AA, Schoenbach VJ, Martinson FE, Donaldson KH, Stancil TR, Fullilove RE. Concurrent partnerships among rural African Americans with recently reported heterosexually transmitted HIV infection. Journal Of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes. 2003;34(4):423–29. doi: 10.1097/00126334-200312010-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrasik MP, Chapman CH, Clad R, Murray K, Foster J, Morris M, et al. Developing concurrency messages for the Black community in Seattle, Washington. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2012;24(6):527–548. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2012.24.6.527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrasik MP, Clad R, Bove J, Tsegaselasssie S, Morris M. A preliminary evaluation of a community-based campaign to increase awareness of concurrency and HIV transmission in African American and African-born communities. AIDS and Behavior. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s10461-015-1017-y. Published online 25 February 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boily M, Alary M, Baggaley RF. Neglected issues and hypotheses regarding the impact of sexual concurrency on HIV and sexually transmitted infections. AIDS and Behavior. 2012;16:304–311. doi: 10.1007/s10461-011-9887-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey MP, Senn TE, Seward DX, Vanable PA. Urban African-American men speak out on sexual partner concurrency: Findings from a qualitative study. AIDS and Behavior. 2010;14:38–47. doi: 10.1007/s10461-008-9406-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Characteristics associated with HIV infection among heterosexuals in urban areas with high AIDS prevalence - 24 cities, United States, 2006–2007. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2011;60(31):1045–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV among African Americans. 2012a Retrieved November 10, 2014, from http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/risk/racialethnic/aa/facts/index.html.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV in the United States: At a Glance. 2012b Retrieved November 10, 2014, from http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/statistics/basics/ataglance.html.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV among African American youth. 2014 Retrieved November 10, 2014, from http://www.cdc.gov/nchhstp/newsroom/docs/CDC-Youth-AAs-508.pdf.
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. [Accessed May 31, 015];HIV Surveillance Report, 2013. 2015 25 http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/library/reports/surveillance/ [Google Scholar]
- Dolwick Grieb SM, Davey-Rothwell M, Latkin CA. Concurrent sexual partnerships amoung urban African American high-risk women with main sex partners. AIDS Behavior. 2012;16(2):323–333. doi: 10.1007/s10461-011-9954-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doherty IA, Shiboski S, Ellen JM, Adimora AA, Padian NS. Sexual bridging socially and over time: a simulation model exploring the relative effects of mixing and concurrency on viral sexually transmitted infection transmission. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2006;33(6):368–373. doi: 10.1097/01.olq.0000194586.66409.7a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enns EA, Brandeau ML, Igeme TK, Bendavid E. Assessing effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of concurrency reduction for HIV prevention. International Journal of STDs & AIDS. 2011;22:558–567. doi: 10.1258/ijsa.2011.010322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein H. The invisible cure: African, the West, and the fight against AIDS. New York: Farrar, Strauss, & Giroux; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Frye V, Kirk H, Bonner S, Williams K, Bond KT, Hoover DR, et al. “Straight Talk” for African-American heterosexual men: Results of a single-arm behavioral intervention trial. AIDS Care. 2013;22(5):627–631. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2012.722605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grieb SM, Davey-Rothwell M, Latkin CA. Concurrent sexual partnerships among urban African American high-risk women with main sex partners. AIDS and Behavior. 2012;16(2):323–333. doi: 10.1007/s10461-011-9954-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallfors DD, Iritani BJ, Miller WC, Bauer DJ. Sexual and drug behavior patterns and HIV and STD racial disparities: the need for new directions. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97(1):125–32. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.075747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harawa N, Adimora A. Incarceration, African Americans, and HIV: Advancing a research agenda. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2008;100(1):57–62. doi: 10.1016/s0027-9684(15)31175-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heckathorn DD. Respondent-driven sampling: a new approach to the study of hidden populations. Social Problems. 1997;44(2):174–199. [Google Scholar]
- Heckathorn DD. Respondent-driven sampling II: deriving valid population estimates from chain-referral samples of hidden populations. Social Problems. 2002;49(1):11–34. [Google Scholar]
- Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS. Consultation on concurrent sexual partnerships: recommendations from a meeting of the UNAIDS Reference Group on Estimates, Modelling and Projections; Nairobi, Kenya. 2009; 2009. Retrieved November, 11 2013, from http://www.epidem.org/sites/default/files/reports/Concurrency_meeting_recommendations_Updated_Nov_2009.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Kalichman SC, Grebler T. Reducing Numbers of Sex Partners: Do We Really Need Special Interventions for Sexual Concurrency? AIDS and Behavior. 2010;14:987–990. doi: 10.1007/s10461-010-9737-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenyon C, Colebunders R, Hens N. Determinants of generalized herpes simplex virus-2 epidemics: the role of sexual partner concurrency. International Journal of STDs and AIDS. 2013;24(5):375–382. doi: 10.1177/0956462412472816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kretzschmar M, Morris M. Measures of concurrency in networks and the spread of infectious disease. Mathmatical Biosciences. 1996;33(2):165–195. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(95)00093-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macaluso M, Demand MJ, Artz LM, Hook EW., III Partner type and condom use. AIDS. 2000;4(5):537–546. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200003310-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacQueen KM, Chen M, Jolly D, Mueller MP, Okumu E, Eley NT, Laws M, Isler MR, Kalloo A, Rogers RC. HIV testing experience and risk behavior among sexually active Black young adults: a CBPR-based study using respondent-driven sampling in Durham, North Carolina. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2015;55:433–443. doi: 10.1007/s10464-015-9725-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnus M, Kuo I, Shelley K, Rawls A, Peterson J, Montanez L, et al. Risk factors driving the emergence of a generalized heterosexual HIV epidemic in Washington, District of Columbia networks at risk. AIDS. 2012;23(10):1277–1284. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32832b51da. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malekinejad M, Mohraz M, Razani N, Akbari G, McFarland W, Khairandish P, et al. High HIV Prevalence in a Respondent-Driven Sampling Survey of Injection Drug Users in Tehran, Iran. AIDS and Behavior. 2015;19:440–449. doi: 10.1007/s10461-014-0904-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris M, Kurth AE, Hamilton DT, Moody J, Wakefield S. Concurrent partnerships and HIV prevalence disparities by race: linking science and public health practice. American Journal of Public Health. 2009;99(6):1023–1031. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.147835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musyoki H, Kellogg TA, Geibel S, Muraguri N, Okal J, Tun W, et al. Prevalence of HIV, Sexually Transmitted Infections, and Risk Behaviours Among Female Sex Workers in Nairobi, Kenya: Results of a Respondent Driven Sampling Study. AIDS and Behavior. 2015;19:S46–S58. doi: 10.1007/s10461-014-0919-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neaigus A, Jenness SM, Hagan H, Murrill CS, Wendel T. Reciprocal Sex Partner Concurrency and STDs among Heterosexuals at High-Risk of HIV Infection. Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine. 2012;90(5) doi: 10.1007/s11524-012-9727-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- North Carolina Division of Public Health, Department of Health and Human Services. North Carolina 2013 HIV/STD Surveillance Report. 2014. [Google Scholar]
- North Carolina HIV/STD Surveillance Unit, Division of Public Health, Department of Health and Human Services. 2015 Unpublished data. [Google Scholar]
- Nunn A, Dickman S, Cornwall A, Rosengard C, Kwakwac H, Kimd D, et al. Social, structural and behavioral drivers of concurrent partnerships among African American men in Philadelphia. AIDS Care. 2011;23(11):1392–1399. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2011.565030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapues J, Wilson EC, Packer T, Colfax GN, Raymond HF. Correlates of HIV infection among transfemales, San Francisco, 2010: Results from a respondent-driven sampling study. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103:1485–1492. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards JE, Risser JM, Padgett PM, Rehman HU, Wolverton ML, Arafat RR. Condom use among high-risk heterosexual women with concurrent sexual partnerships, Houston, Texas, USA. International Journal of STDs and AIDS. 2008;19(11):768–771. doi: 10.1258/ijsa.2008.008076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes SD, McCoy TP. Condom use among immigrant Latino sexual minorities: multilevel analysis after respondent-driven sampling. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2015;27(1):27–43. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2015.27.1.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salganik MJ, Heckathorn DD. Sampling and estimation in hidden populations using respondent-driven sampling. Social Methodology. 2004;34(1):193–240. [Google Scholar]
- Senn TE, Carey MP, Vanable PA, Coury-Doniger P, Urban M. Sexual partner concurrency among STI clinic patients with a steady partner: correlates and associations with condom use. Sexually Transmitted Infections. 2009;85(5):343–347. doi: 10.1136/sti.2009.035758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senn TE, Scott-Sheldon LA, Seward DX, Wright EW, Carey MP. Sexual partner concurrency of urban male and female STD clinic patients: A qualitative study. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2011;40:775–784. doi: 10.1007/s10508-010-9688-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tun W, Katzen LL, Abbott SA, Srikrishnan AK, Kelly CA, Sarna A, et al. Using a 2-stage strategy with respondent-driven sampling to recruit a hard-to-reach population for a placebo microbicide gel clinical trial in Nellore, Andhra Pradesh (India) AIDS and Behavior. 2015;19:369–379. doi: 10.1007/s10461-014-0938-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Census Bureau American FactFinder. 2008–2012 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates for Blacks in Durham, NC. Retrieved July 31, 2015 from http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/nav/jsf/pages/searchresults.xhtml?refresh=t.
- Vaccari M, Poonam P, Franchini G. Phase III HIV vaccine trial in Thailand: a step toward a protective vaccine for HIV. Expert Review of Vaccines. 2010;9(9):997–1005. doi: 10.1586/erv.10.104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van de Ven P, Bartholow B, Rawstorne P, Crawford J, Kippax S, Grulich A, et al. Scaling HIV vaccine attitudes among gay men in Sydney, Australia. AIDS Research and Human Retroviruses. 2002;18(18):1333–1337. doi: 10.1089/088922202320935401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villanti A, German D, Sifakis F, Flynn C, Holtgrave D. Smoking, HIV status, and HIV risk behaviors in a respondent-driven sample of HIV injection drug users in Baltimore, Maryland: the BESURE study. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2012;24(2):132–147. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2012.24.2.132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volz E, Wejnert C, Cameron C, Spiller M, Barash V, Degani L, Heckathorn DD. Respondent-driven sampling analysis tool (RDSAT) Version 7.1. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University; 2012. [Google Scholar]