Abstract
Emotions can evoke strong reactions that have profound influences, from gross changes in our internal environment to small fluctuations in facial muscles, and reveal our feelings overtly. Muscles contain proprioceptive afferents, informing us about our movements and regulating motor activities. Their firing reflects changes in muscle length, yet their sensitivity can be modified by the fusimotor system, as found in animals. In humans, the sensitivity of muscle afferents is modulated by cognitive processes, such as attention; however, it is unknown if emotional processes can modulate muscle feedback. Presently, we explored whether muscle afferent sensitivity adapts to the emotional situation. We recorded from single muscle afferents in the leg, using microneurography, and moved the ankle joint of participants, while they listened to evocative classical music to induce sad, neutral, or happy emotions, or sat passively (no music). We further monitored their physiological responses using skin conductance, heart rate, and electromyography measures. We found that muscle afferent firing was modified by the emotional context, especially for sad emotions, where the muscle spindle dynamic response increased. We suggest that this allows us to prime movements, where the emotional state prepares the body for consequent behaviour-appropriate reactions.
Introduction
Emotions are responsible for profound changes in the body landscape, by affecting the internal environment, viscera, and the musculoskeletal system. Five basic emotions have been identified: sadness, happiness, fear, anger, and disgust1, where emotional reactions are displayed to the world through our behaviour, actions and reactions. Accordingly, the musculoskeletal system allows us to carry out these activities, interact with others, and explore our surroundings. It also informs us about changes in such activity, our posture, and the speed and quality of movements2, 3. Our behaviour influences social interactions, where even the smallest of facial muscle contractions can signal our intentions and affective thoughts4.
Emotions modulate our readiness to move5, where pleasant experiences prime approach actions and unpleasant experiences prime withdrawal. The temporal kinematics of movement are especially affected by the situational valence6. Emotions, particularly unpleasant ones, can impact movements, for example, producing differences in the maintenance of an isometric contraction7 and of posture8. The continued exposure to such stimuli can magnify the force of sustained voluntary movements7. Manipulating emotional state can also be used to facilitate gait initiation in healthy people9, as well as in patients such as those with Parkinson’s disease10.
The knowledge and control of action is determined, at least partly, by proprioceptive information, which arises from muscle spindles11. This is corroborated by studies on patients lacking myelinated fibres (including those for proprioception), who are unable to maintain a steady joint angle without vision, or adapt their movements to unexpected loads12, 13. These patients show deficits in multi-joint movements and their limb trajectories are severely distorted, even when they have visual input during movements14. The sensitivity of muscle afferents can be modified by the central nervous system through the gamma fusimotor system, as found in animals15, 16. This effect has been sought in humans, yet studies have shown only a small impact of the descending drive on muscle afferent activity17–19, although it has been observed recently, where muscle afferent firing acts as a forward sensory model and predicts the future kinematic activity of the parent muscle20.
Conscious cognitive processes have been shown to influence muscle afferent activity, where attention21, 22 and learning23 can modify their sensitivity, allowing the selection of specific muscle feedback relevant for the behavioural context. However, it is unknown whether instinctual processes, such as emotions, can modulate muscle feedback directly. We hypothesise that emotions can modify the descending motor drive and modulate the firing of muscle afferents, where emotions will prepare the body for making consequent behaviour-appropriate reactions.
Results
We recorded unitary activity from 24 primary (type Ia) muscle afferents, originating in the dorsal flexor ankle muscles, using microneurography24, 25 in 16 resting, healthy participants. The sensitivity of each muscle afferent was assessed during an emotional manipulation paradigm, where the participant closed their eyes and listened to evocative classical music of sad, neutral, or happy valence26–31 (Table 1), or sat passively (no music). A microelectrode was inserted into the common peroneal nerve and the absence of concomitant muscle activity was controlled by recording surface electromyographic activity (EMG). Muscle afferent firing was recorded during sinusoidal plantar flexion/dorsiflexion movements (Fig. 1), imposed at the ankle, while the participant attended to the emotions triggered by each type of music. Electrodermal activity and heart rate were also recorded as emotional markers and participants rated their emotion on a visual analogue scale (VAS), from sad to happy.
Table 1.
Musical piece | Composer | Valence | Number of presentations | Rating (mean ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Nocturnes, Op. 27 No. 1 | Chopin | Sad | 7 | 1.8 ± 0.2 |
Kol Nidrei, Op. 47 | Bruch | Sad | 7 | 2.1 ± 0.5 |
Adagio for Strings | Barber | Sad | 5 | 0.4 ± 0.1 |
Nocturnes, Op. 48 No. 1 | Chopin | Sad | 2 | 0.8 ± 0.5 |
Piano Concerto No. 23, Adagio | Mozart | Sad | 1 | 1.2 |
Symphony No. 5, Adagietto | Malher | Sad | 1 | 2.4 |
Peer Gynt: Solveig’s song, Op. 55 No. 2 | Grieg | Sad | 1 | 3.8 |
The Planets, Venus | Host | Neutral | 10 | 4.9 ± 0.4 |
Pictures at an exposition No. 1 | Mussorgsky | Neutral | 7 | 5.7 ± 0.3 |
Water music suite No. 1 Minuet (mvt 6) | Handel | Neutral | 5 | 4.9 ± 0.2 |
Water music suite No. 2 passepied (mvt 2) | Handel | Neutral | 1 | 5.9 |
Piano Sonata No. 14, Moonlight Sonata | Beethoven | Neutral | 1 | 5.5 |
Radetzky marsch, Op. 228 | Strauss | Happy | 5 | 9.3 ± 0.3 |
Carmen Suite No. 1, Les Toreadors | Bizet | Happy | 5 | 8.4 ± 0.4 |
Piano Concerto No. 23, Allegro assai | Mozart | Happy | 5 | 8.2 ± 0.4 |
Carnival of the Animals No. 14, Finale | Saint-Saëns | Happy | 3 | 9.0 ± 0.7 |
Eine kleine Nachtmusik, K. 525 | Mozart | Happy | 3 | 8.7 ± 0.4 |
Four seasons, Spring, Allegro | Vivaldi | Happy | 2 | 7.9 ± 0.2 |
Carnival of the Animals No. 10, Aviary | Saint-Saëns | Happy | 1 | 8.0 |
We find that muscle afferent activity was modulated by the emotional content. Figure 1 details a full example of responses from a single primary muscle afferent, while listening to the music of different valences. During sad music, the afferent response composed a succession of spike bursts and silences, relating to the lengthening and shortening of the muscle, respectively (Fig. 1a shows a full movement sequence, with all the physiological measures and Fig. 1b left shows three successive movements on an expanded time-scale). The same afferent’s response to movement during neutral music differed considerably (Fig. 1b, middle), where the silent periods between sinusoids disappeared and there was additional firing throughout these parts of the movement cycle. When listening to happy music, silences occurred during muscle shortenings, akin to the sad condition, but these were shorter in duration (Fig. 1b, right). Movement-by-movement rasters (Fig. 1c) show that the afferent’s response was consistent over the movement cycles, for each condition. The mean instantaneous frequency is shown below the rasters, where the minimum and maximum rates were extracted. The difference between these two measures (‘delta’) was used to quantify the afferent’s dynamic firing and we found increases in the sad condition, as compared to the neutral and happy conditions.
For the whole population of afferents (n = 24), the delta significantly differed between the music conditions (ANOVA, F(2,46) = 4.28, p = 0.034, η2 = 0.16). Figure 2a shows that the delta was significantly higher during sad music, as compared to happy music (p = 0.031) and neutral music (p = 0.001), but did not change significantly between happy and neutral music (p = 0.812). Figure 2c shows the distribution of responses in the emotional conditions, as compared to neutral. Here, an increase in delta during the sad condition was found in 21/24 units, where the delta during sad music was significantly increased (t = 4.43, p < 0.001).
We also compared the changes in muscle afferent delta in a sub-population of the same units when no music was listened to, as an auditory control. This sub-set (n = 11) showed the same pattern of responses, where the delta was significantly different between the conditions (ANOVA, F(3,30) = 5.21, p = 0.014, η2 = 0.34). Figure 2b shows that the delta during sad music was significant increased, as compared to the neutral (p = 0.023), happy (p = 0.011), and no music (p = 0.025) conditions. There was no significant difference between the neutral and no music conditions (p = 0.456). The distributions of the responses over the conditions, as compared to neutral, are shown in Fig. 2d. Here, an increase in delta during the sad condition was found in 10/11 units, where the delta during sad music was significantly higher than during neutral music (t = 3.15, p = 0.010).
For the physiological measures of emotion, electrodermal activity increased significantly during sad music, as compared to neutral music (Wilcoxon, W = −166 p = 0.046). In the happy music condition, the electrodermal activity was significantly higher than in all the other conditions (cf. sad W = −193, p = 0.019; neutral W = −324, p < 0.001; no music W = −208, p = 0.004; Fig. 3b). The mean heart rate increased significantly during happy music, as compared to neutral music (W = −139, p = 0.046; Fig. 3c). Heart rate variability was significantly higher during sad music (W = −169, p = 0.008) and happy music (W = −78, p = 0.043; Fig. 3d), as compared to neutral. Heart rate variability was also increased during sad music, as compared to no music (W = −134, p = 0.010). These measures were congruent with the emotion VAS ratings; a significant effect was found between the emotional conditions (Friedman ANOVA = 41.54, p < 0.001), where there were differences between all the comparisons (all p < 0.01; Fig. 3a). This showed that participants felt sad, neutral, and happy emotions when listening to the corresponding music.
Discussion
The present results demonstrate that changes in emotional state lead to the differential encoding of body movement by muscle spindles. We used the auditory modality to provoke emotional changes, as the induced affect is more pervasive compared to other modalities31. The emotional content of the music was sufficient to drive central mechanisms and modulate the descending fusimotor efference to muscles, as seen in the muscle afferent responses, as well as produce physiological changes.
We found that sad emotions caused muscle afferents to respond differently to movement, as compared to the effects from neutral music (in 20/24 units), as seen in an increase in their dynamic firing. The congruence between the sad feelings induced by listening to sad music and those triggered by the experimental situation (e.g. not being able to move), likely amplified the effect32. Sad feelings may be more important for survival33 and prime the body for a context-appropriate behavioural response e.g. withdrawal and avoidance. Unpleasant stimuli generally provoke a more pervasive behavioural and physiological reaction5–7, 31, 34–36, where corticospinal excitability has been found to be higher for unpleasant stimuli5, 37–40, reflecting increased motor preparation. Such studies demonstrate only small modulations in corticospinal excitation by emotions; these changes may not produce an actual movement, yet they will nevertheless influence the execution of future movements39. Presently, we found a small, but consistent increase in the dynamic firing of muscle afferents during sad emotions, which also suggests a change in the readiness to react.
Our findings during happy emotions were more variable, where we found both increases and decreases in the dynamic firing over different participants. The increases in dynamic firing may be akin to the sad condition, where there is priming for action; as generally observed with positive emotions that prompt approach behaviour6. However, we suggest that, for some participants, a situational incongruence may have occurred. The happy music may have evoked a desire to move, but opposingly, the participant was required to remain relaxed for the experiment. Previous work has postulated that pleasant stimuli trigger an urge to move, where corticospinal excitability is decreased due to a need to suppress the response6. Hence, the decreases in the dynamic response of muscle afferents that we found during happy music may reflect a suppression of the urge to move. Thus, the participant’s perception of the situational incongruence may have dictated their overall response, demonstrating the subjective nature of emotional experiences and reactions41.
Muscle proprioceptive messages encode both the position and velocity of movements2, and the present increase in their dynamic firing likely leads to increased proprioceptive acuity during emotional situations, particularly sad ones that may require more urgent attention33. The change in muscle afferent firing that we demonstrate with emotion is not large; however, muscle afferent firing rates are much lower in humans, as compared to animals21. For example, the fusimotor-induced increase in muscle afferent firing rate during isometric contractions in humans (~20 Hz)42 is considerably less than that in awake cats (100–300 Hz)43. At these lower frequencies, the difference in afferent firing will be less pronounced in humans23, as we find. Nevertheless, the muscle afference sent to the brain will be registered and enhance ongoing behaviour, preparing the person for behaviour appropriate to the situation. We are not saying that a subsequent behaviour must necessarily be produced; rather that the emotional response readies the body for action to suit the situation and muscle afferent signals could act as ‘somatic markers’ in driving behavior44.
Muscle afferents are involved in the regulation of motor activities45, where an increase in their dynamic firing may influence movements by directly enhancing alpha motoneuron excitability46. Similarly, a descending command appears to govern increases in sympathetic activity to muscles, rather than direct muscle activity, which is important for the regulation of muscle perfusion during movement47. Regarding the origin of the effect we find, it is improbable that the increase in muscle spindle sensitivity resulted from enhanced muscle sympathetic activity, since it does not influence muscle spindle firing48. The described effect is likely produced by the descending drive from fusimotor neurons, and more specifically, from the dynamic gamma fusimotor drive that increases the tension in intrafusal muscle fibres15, 16, 49, 50.
As well as the dynamic change in muscle afferent firing, we found differences in the physiological measures over the conditions. We expected electrodermal activity to increase in emotional states, as studies have found skin conductance changes when listening to emotional music, and akin to our findings, that happy emotions produce a larger effect34–36. Similarly, our emotional conditions produced changes in the heart rate measures, as found previously31, 34. These measures reflect the effect of feeling the emotions in the music, as neutral music did not show such an impact and the physiological reactions from it were equivalent to the no music condition.
Emotions are vital to human behaviour and for social interactions. They shape our perceptions and actions, particularly through the auditory and visual channels29. We find that emotions modulate the feedback from our muscles, which has direct consequences for our bodily awareness and readiness to react to emotional situations, i.e. perception-action. The effect of emotions on driving preferential action responses may be exploited therapeutically. The addition of external stochastic noise has been shown to reduce physiological tremor and improve motor performance51, as well as postural control52, and the manipulation of internal emotional state may ready the body for action. Music is widely used to enhance emotional impact and emotional priming may be used in physical rehabilitation to enhance feedback in patients with movement disorders10. Likewise, emotional manipulation may be used in patients with affective disorders, such as depression, where listening to sad music may actually regulate mood and behavior53.
Methods
Participants and experimental set-up
The experiments were performed on 16 healthy human volunteers (6 females, mean age 27.7 ± 6.3 years), all of whom gave their written, informed consent. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud-Méditerranée I) and performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Due to the nature of the paradigm (in vivo peripheral nerve single unit recordings during an emotional manipulation), it was important that all our participants were calm and felt relaxed about the procedure. Hence, they participated in a pre-experiment visit to assess their suitability and introduce them to the laboratory environment. For the experiment, participants were seated comfortably in an armchair with their legs positioned in cushioned grooves. This allowed a standardised, relaxed position to be maintained, in the absence of any muscle activity. The knee joints were positioned at an angle of ~120–130°, and their feet rested on supports, giving a neutral position of the ankle, with an angle of ~110° between the leg and the foot. The right foot was laid on a stationary plate and the left foot was placed on a pedal connected to a computer-controlled robot, which was used to impose plantarflexion/dorsiflexion movements.
Before the experiment started, the participant listened to several pieces of classical music deemed to evoke sad, neutral, or happy emotions (up to 5 pieces of each type), and rated each of them on a VAS, ranging from sad (equating to 0, the saddest they could feel) to happy (equating to 10, the happiest they could feel). Although emotions are complex, we chose sad and happy as the emotions to be induced, as these are two of the five basic emotions1, where sad-happy can be represented as a dimension on a continuum. Sad and happy emotions can be readily induced experimentally and have been well-studied, including comparing sad, neutral, and happy emotions through images54–56 and music26, 27, 30, 31, 57. We used the auditory modality to invoked these emotions, as the effect is more pervasive compared to other modalities31. For classical music, sad and happy emotional music has been well-classified26, 27, and produces body physiological changes34–36, evokes neural activity in brain regions relating to emotional processing27, and induces autonomic responses31. Further, even when pleasure can be derived from sadness (e.g. enjoying sad music), the resultant emotion is decoupled from the aesthetic content (liking) of a stimulus53, 58. As our approach introduced constraints on the number of conditions presented (e.g. recording stably from a single muscle afferent), we focused on sad-happy as our emotional dimension, with neutral music and no music as emotional and auditory controls, respectively.
For each participant, the most effective music in triggering each type of emotion, for sad, neutral, and happy pieces, was chosen to be played during physiological recordings. Table 1 gives the name of each piece of music and the number of times the music was used, with the mean emotional ratings from all participants. The musical pieces were subject to signal processing to provide similar levels of acoustic excitation and to avoid strong variations of the sound level. Original music digital recordings in MP3PRO (FHG) format (44100 Hz, 16 bits) were first adjusted to have similar root mean square amplitudes of 80 dB re. 1 bit, using a custom made MATLAB (The Mathworks, Natick, MA) program. An amplitude compression was then performed using Adobe Audition 1.5 (Adobe, San Jose, CA) software. The chosen compression curve flattened the upper 40 dB (all levels from maximum to −40 dB re. maximum) to a constant level. A linear compression was then applied to the lower levels (all levels from −40 dB re. maximum to minimum). These modifications of the music were not particularly noticeable to the participants, but ensured equivalent modulations between the music pieces. The participants rated these music pieces easily for their emotional content (see Fig. 3a and Table 1).
Physiological recordings
The activity of single muscle spindle endings, originating from the tibialis anterior (TA; n = 10) and extensor digitorum longus (EDL; n = 14) muscles, were recorded from the left common peroneal nerve at the popliteal fossa, using the human, in vivo technique of microneurography24, 25. We ensured that the procedure was not painful and that the participant remained calm and relaxed throughout (e.g. we did not use electrical stimulation to find the nerve, we advanced the electrode very slowly and carefully, we frequently asked them how they were, and monitored their physiological responses). Unitary muscle afferent activity was recorded referentially using an insulated tungsten microelectrode (impedance 0.3–1 MΩ, tip diameter ~5 µm, length 30 mm; FHC, Bowdoin, ME). The recordings were monitored continuously using an oscilloscope and a loudspeaker. Neural activity was amplified (×200,000) and band-pass filtered (300–3000 Hz) to ensure an optimal signal-to-noise ratio, and sampled at 20 kHz frequency. Muscle afferents were identified as primary endings on the basis of their irregular spontaneous activity, their high dynamic sensitivity to ramp and hold movements, and their silencing during passive muscle shortenings59.
The absence of concomitant muscle activity was controlled throughout the experiment by recording EMG. Two pairs of surface electrodes were placed over the TA muscle, with an inter-electrode distance of ~4 cm for each pair. The EMGs were recorded with high gain (×10,000), band-pass filtered (30–300 Hz), and sampled at 5 kHz. Further measures of emotional responses were obtained through recording electrodermal activity, using two surface electrodes placed on each side of the left hand (gain: ×500, band-pass: 0.1–100 Hz, sampling frequency: 200 Hz), and heart rate was recorded by means of a passive transducer, strapped around the left index finger. The EMG, electrodermal activity, and heart rate measures were monitored throughout the experiment. These provided feedback to the experimenter that the participant was not stressed, felt trapped, or wanted to move (e.g. no EMG, not a high heart rate, no strong modulations in the electrodermal response).
Experimental paradigm
The participant’s emotional state was changed by listening to classical music through headphones. The music pieces had been previously defined as reliably inducing sad (n = 9 pieces), happy (n = 7), or neutral (n = 5) affective states26–31 (see Table 1). Once activity from a single muscle spindle had been isolated, the participant put on the headphones and was instructed to close their eyes. The participant was told before the recording to listen attentively to the music and disregard the movement imposed at the foot. The music started and, after a delay of ~10 s, a series of 30 sinusoidal plantarflexion/dorsiflexion movements (5° amplitude and 5°/s velocity, over ~1 minute) were imposed at the ankle joint. The movement range was chosen to maintain stability of the microelectrode recording at the level of the knee, but allow sufficient ankle movement21. The maximal angle towards plantar flexion is ~20°60, hence 5° amplitude corresponds to ~25% of the maximal movement possible.
We chose to play 10 s of music before the movements started to allow the participant to ‘feel’ the emotions. Music has been found to be instantly evocative, where the physiological arousal changes quickly from the very beginning of the music35, 61. The initial period allowed sufficient time for continued experience of the emotions during the movement, without inattention or habituation31, 34. Each afferent was recorded during four sequences, where the participant listened to sad, happy, or neutral music, or listened to nothing (‘no music’ condition) through noise-cancelling headphones (Bose; Framingham, MA). These four conditions were presented in a pseudo-random order. After each sequence, the participant was asked to rate the emotion felt for the music on a further VAS, which was used in the analysis.
Data analysis and statistics
The data were stored on a digital tape recorder (DTR 1802, Biologic, Claix, France) and processed off-line using Spike 2 software (CED, Cambridge, UK). The nerve spikes were inspected carefully in an expanded time scale and transformed into an instantaneous frequency curve. The mean instantaneous frequency curve (bin size = 0.005 s), per unit/condition, was obtained by averaging its response to 29 sinusoidal movements, where the first movement was excluded because of a dynamic response from the onset of the movement. The maximum and minimum frequency was extracted from this measure, and the difference (‘delta’; see Fig. 1c) was used as an index to characterise a unit’s dynamic response in each condition. This measure was chosen specifically to reflect the dynamic response of muscle afferents, where similar measures have been used previously62. Note that in the majority of microneurography studies investigating changes in muscle spindle activity use ramp-and-hold movements. In our paradigm, we needed to use a longer-lasting movement stimulus to determine the effects of emotions (which can be induced quickly, but fluctuate over time), hence we were required to deliver continuous movements over time. We measure the changes in firing rate in cycle histograms over the sinusoids, as in previous animal studies dealing with the effect of fusimotor activity63, 64. For six of the units, muscle activity (EMG) was present during some of the movement cycles; in these cases the averaged response was composed of the movements that were EMG-free.
The statistical analysis for the whole population of afferent responses was made using a one-way ANOVA with repeated measures, followed by false-discovery rate adjusted post-hoc tests. A second ANOVA was used to compare the data in a sub-set of units, where the no music condition was also tested. The effect sizes were determined using Eta-squared (η2). The emotional VAS ratings were compared using a non-parametric Friedman’s ANOVA and differences between the music conditions were compared using Dunn’s corrected post-hoc tests. The physiological measures (electrodermal activity, mean and standard deviation of heart rate) were recorded over all the conditions (including the no music condition) and non-parametric Wilcoxon tests were used to compare differences within each measure, due to the data not meeting normality criteria (which was found using Shapiro-Wilk tests). The level of significance was set at p < 0.05.
Data Availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by Centre National de La Recherche Scientifique grants. R.A. is funded by a grant from the FP7-People-COFUND (Marie Curie Actions) of the European Union, under REA grant agreement No. 608743. This publication reflects only the view of the authors and the European Union is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained herein.
Author Contributions
E.R.-C. and J.-M.A. conceived the experiments, E.R.-C., R.A., and J.-M.A. performed the experiments, analysed data, and edited the manuscript. E.R.-C. and R.A. wrote the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Johnson-laird PN, Oatley K. The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field. Cogn. Emot. 1989;3:81–123. doi: 10.1080/02699938908408075. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gandevia SC, Burke D. Does the nervous system depend on kinaesthetic information to control natural limb movements? Behav. Brain Sci. 1992;15:614–632. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0006893X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Proske U, Gandevia SC. The kinaesthetic senses. J. Physiol. 2009;587:4139–46. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.175372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Losch ME, Kim HS. Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986;50:260–268. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.van Loon AM, van den Wildenberg WPM, van Stegeren AH, Ridderinkhof KR, Hajcak G. Emotional stimuli modulate readiness for action: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2010;10:174–181. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.2.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Esteves PO, et al. Motor planning of goal-directed action is tuned by the emotional valence of the stimulus: a kinematic study. Sci. Rep. 2016;6 doi: 10.1038/srep28780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Coombes SA, Cauraugh JH, Janelle CM. Emotion and movement: Activation of defensive circuitry alters the magnitude of a sustained muscle contraction. Neurosci. Lett. 2006;396:192–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.11.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hillman CH, Rosengren KS, Smith DP. Emotion and motivated behavior: Postural adjustments to affective picture viewing. Biol. Psychol. 2004;66:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gélat T, Coudrat L, Le Pellec A. Gait initiation is affected during emotional conflict. Neurosci. Lett. 2011;497:64–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Naugle KM, Hass CJ, Bowers D, Janelle CM. Emotional state affects gait initiation in individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2012;12:207–219. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0071-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Matthews PB. Where does Sherrington’s ‘muscular sense’ originate? Muscles, joints, corollary discharges? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1982;5:189–218. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.05.030182.001201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rothwell JC, et al. Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Brain. 1982;105:515–42. doi: 10.1093/brain/105.3.515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sanes JN, Mauritz KH, Dalakas MC, Evarts EV. Motor control in humans with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. Hum. Neurobiol. 1985;4:101–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sainburg RL, Poizner H, Ghez C. Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. J. Neurophysiol. 1993;70:2136–2147. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.5.2136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hulliger M. The mammalian muscle spindle and its central control. Rev. Physiol. Biochem. Pharmacol. 1984;101:1–110. doi: 10.1007/BFb0027694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Prochazka A, Hulliger M, Zangger P, K. A, Appenteng K. ‘Fusimotor set’: new evidence for alpha-independent control of gamma-motoneurones during movement in the awake cat. Brain Res. 1985;339:136–140. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90632-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vallbo Å, Al-Falahe N. Human muscle spindle response in a motor learning task. J. Physiol. 1990;421:553–568. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp017961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kakuda N, Nagaoka M. Dynamic response of human muscle spindle afferents to stretch during voluntary contraction. J. Physiol. 1998;513:621–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.621bb.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kakuda N, Vallbo AB, Wessberg J. Fusimotor and skeletomotor activities are increased with precision finger movement in man. J. Physiol. 1996;492:921–929. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Dimitriou M, Edin BB. Human muscle spindles act as forward sensory models. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:1763–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hospod V, Aimonetti J, Roll JP, Ribot-Ciscar E. Changes in human muscle spindle sensitivity during a proprioceptive attention task. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:5172–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0572-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ribot-Ciscar E, Hospod V, Roll JP, Aimonetti J. Fusimotor drive may adjust muscle spindle feedback to task requirements in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;101:633–40. doi: 10.1152/jn.91041.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dimitriou M. Enhanced muscle afferent signals during motor learning in humans. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:1062–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Vallbo Å, Hagbarth K. Activity from skin mechanoreceptors recorded percutaneously in awake human subjects. Exp. Neurol. 1968;289:270–289. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(68)90041-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bergenheim, M., Roll, J.-P. & Ribot-Ciscar, E. In Modern Techniques in Neuroscience Research (eds Windhorst, U. & Johansson, H.) 801–819 (Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg, 1999).
- 26.Peretz I, Gagnon L, Bouchard B. Music and emotion: perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain damage. Cognition. 1998;68:111–141. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00043-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mitterschiffthaler MT, Fu CHY, Dalton JA, Andrew CM, Williams SCR. A functional MRI study of happy and sad affective states induced by classical music. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1150–1162. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Baumgartner T, Willi M, Jäncke L. Modulation of corticospinal activity by strong emotions evoked by pictures and classical music: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport. 2007;18:261–265. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328012272e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Baumgartner T, Lutz K, Schmidt CF, Jäncke L. The emotional power of music: How music enhances the feeling of affective pictures. Brain Res. 2006;1075:151–164. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Baumgartner T, Esslen M, Jäncke L. From emotion perception to emotion experience: Emotions evoked by pictures and classical music. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2006;60:34–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Krumhansl CL. An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 1997;51:336–353. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.51.4.336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Spadacenta S, Gallese V, Fragola M, Mirabella G. Modulation of arm reaching movements during processing of arm/hand-related action verbs with and without emotional connotation. PLoS One. 2014;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Borgomaneri S, Gazzola V, Avenanti A. Temporal dynamics of motor cortex excitability during perception of natural emotional scenes. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014;9:1451–1457. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Khalfa S, Roy M, Rainville P, Dalla Bella S, Peretz I. Role of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological differentiation of happy and sad music? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2008;68:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Khalfa S, Isabelle P, Jean-Pierre B, Manon R. Event-related skin conductance responses to musical emotions in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 2002;328:145–149. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00462-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Salimpoor VN, Benovoy M, Longo G, Cooperstock JR, Zatorre RJ. The rewarding aspects of music listening are related to degree of emotional arousal. PLoS One. 2009;4 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nogueira-Campos AA, et al. Corticospinal excitability preceding the grasping of emotion-laden stimuli. PLoS One. 2014;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hajcak G, et al. Emotion facilitates action: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of motor cortex excitability during picture viewing. Psychophysiology. 2007;44:91–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00487.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Coombes SA, et al. Emotion and motor preparation: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of corticospinal motor tract excitability. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;9:380–388. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.4.380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Coelho CM, Lipp OV, Marinovic W, Wallis G, Riek S. Increased corticospinal excitability induced by unpleasant visual stimuli. Neurosci. Lett. 2010;481:135–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Park H-D, Tallon-Baudry C. The neural subjective frame: from bodily signals to perceptual consciousness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2014;369 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wilson LR, Gandevia SC, Burke D. Discharge of human muscle spindle afferents innervating ankle dorsiflexors during target isometric contractions. J. Physiol. 1997;504:221–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1997.221bf.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Prochazka A, Westerman RA, Ziccone SP. Ia afferent activity during a variety of voluntary movements in the cat. J. Physiol. 1977;268:423–448. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 1996;351:1413–20. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Windhorst U. Muscle proprioceptive feedback and spinal networks. Brain Res. Bull. 2007;73:155–202. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gandevia SC, Burke D. Effect of training on voluntary activation of human fusimotor neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1985;54:1422–1429. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.54.6.1422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Boulton D, Taylor CE, Macefield VG, Green S. Contributions of central command and muscle feedback to sympathetic nerve activity in contracting human skeletal muscle. Front. Physiol. 2016;7 doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Macefield, V. G., Sverrisdottir, Y. B. & Wallin, B. G. Resting discharge of human muscle spindles is not modulated by increases in sympathetic drive. J. Physiol.551, 1005–1011 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 49.Matthews PB. The differentiation of two types of fusimotor fibre by their effects on the dynamic response of muscle spindle primary endings. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. Cogn. Med. Sci. 1962;47:324–333. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1962.sp001616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Brown M, Goodwin G, Matthews P. After-effects of fusimotor stimulation on the response of muscle spindle primary afferent endings. J. Physiol. 1969;205:677–694. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Trenado C, et al. Suppression of enhanced physiological tremor via stochastic noise: Initial observations. PLoS One. 2014;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Borel L, Ribot-Ciscar E. Improving postural control by applying mechanical noise to ankle muscle tendons. Exp. Brain Res. 2016;234:2305–2314. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4636-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Sachs ME, Damasio A, Habibi A. The pleasures of sad music: a systematic review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gur RC, et al. Brain activation during facial emotion processing. Neuroimage. 2002;16:651–662. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Esslen M, Pascual-Marqui RD, Hell D, Kochi K, Lehmann D. Brain areas and time course of emotional processing. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1189–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Britton JC, Taylor SF, Sudheimer KD, Liberzon I. Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: Common and differential networks. Neuroimage. 2006;31:906–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Paquette S, Peretz I, Belin P. The ‘Musical Emotional Bursts’: A validated set of musical affect bursts to investigate auditory affective processing. Front. Psychol. 2013;4 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Brattico E, et al. It’s sad but I like it: The neural dissociation between musical emotions and liking in experts and laypersons. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;9:1–21. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Edin BB, Vallbo AB. Classification of human muscle stretch receptor afferents: a Bayesian approach. J. Neurophysiol. 1990;63:1314–1322. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.63.6.1314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Inman, V. T. The joints of the ankle. (Williams & Wilkins, 1976).
- 61.Bigand E, Filipic S, Lalitte P. The time course of emotional responses to music. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2005;1060:429–437. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kakuda N. Response of human muscle spindle afferents to sinusoidal stretching with a wide range of amplitudes. J. Physiol. 2000;527:397–404. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.00397.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Hulliger M, Matthews PB, Noth J. Effects of combining static and dynamic fusimotor stimulation on the response of the muscle spindle primary ending to sinusoidal stretching. J. Physiol. 1977;267:839–856. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Appelberg B, Hulliger M, Johansson H, Sojka P. Fusimotor reflexes in triceps surae elicited by natural stimulation of muscle afferents from the cat ipsilateral hind limb. J. Physiol. 1982;329:211–229. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.