Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Psychol. 2017 Mar 16;15:168–173. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.017

Social-Learning Parenting Intervention Research in the Era of Translational Neuroscience

Philip A Fisher 1,a,, Elizabeth A Skowron 1,b
PMCID: PMC5559885  NIHMSID: NIHMS856911  PMID: 28813257

Abstract

In the decades since social learning parenting interventions emerged, many evidence-based programs have been implemented at scale in community settings, and much research is now focusing on ways to maintain fidelity and impact during the implementation process. Notably, a considerable amount of theoretical confluence has occurred in parenting interventions from social learning, attachment, and other theoretical perspectives, with parent coaching as an example of this new generation of relational interventions. In addition, research examining the neurobiological effects of early adverse experiences is providing insight into key mediating and moderating mechanisms underlying the effectiveness of social learning parenting interventions, and new strategies for tailoring interventions to the needs of specific populations are being developed, making interventions more efficient, precise, and effective.


Social learning theory, which emerged in the late 1960s, is based on the premise that children acquire behavioral strategies that are modeled by significant others in their environment [1]. Laboratory studies, including the classic “Bobo doll” experiment [2], provided strong support for this assertion. This early work was complemented by evidence from investigations in naturalistic settings by Gerald Patterson and colleagues at the Oregon Social Learning Center beginning in the 1970s. These researchers focused on the emergence of childhood disruptive behavior in the family context [3]. Patterson et al. used longitudinal designs from early childhood through adolescence/adulthood and adapted ethological methodologies to create microsocial coding systems for quantifying behavior as it unfolded in real time in home environments [4]. These investigations provided the foundation for coercion theory [5], which identifies parenting practices—specifically, low rates of positive reinforcement, high rates of harsh and inconsistent limit setting, and poor monitoring of children’s behavior—associated with the development of disruptive and antisocial behavior.

Patterson and colleagues found that in families in which parents exhibited low rates of positive reinforcement and harsh discipline, aversive behavior was observed to be reciprocally reinforcing between parents and children, leading to escalating levels of negative behavior among family members over time. Young children reared in such environments were observed to exhibit low levels of prosocial behavior and high rates of aggression. This led to an increased likelihood of peer rejection and academic difficulties upon school entry. In adolescence, such individuals were at enhanced risk for school drop-out, substance abuse, association with deviant peers, and antisocial/delinquent behavior. In adulthood, numerous problematic (and societally costly) outcomes were observed, including propensity for incarceration, early parenthood, unemployment, and addictions and mental health difficulties.

Coercion theory has had a lasting impact not only because of its precision in providing causal linkages between early family environmental variables and the probability of an antisocial life-course trajectory, but also because the core parenting practices it describes have proved highly malleable in the context of family-based interventions [6]. A “family tree” of the social learning interventions that evolved from coercion theory include, but are not limited to, Parent Management Training-Oregon (PMTO) [7], an individual and group-based parenting program for children and adolescents; Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) [8], which uses in-vivo coaching strategies for parents and young children; The Incredible Years (IY) [9], a parenting- and school-based intervention for preschoolers; and Treatment Foster Care Oregon (TFCO) [10], a family-based intervention for preschoolers through adolescents with severe behavior problems in child welfare and youth justice settings. Each of these interventions is supported by multiple randomized clinical trials.

The evidence base for these social learning interventions has gone beyond simply documenting impact on outcomes of interest. Randomized clinical trials (RCT’s) comprise controlled experiments that allow researchers to test, and in some instances provide validation of, the causal mechanisms stipulated by a theory. The RCT studies of social learning parenting interventions have shown [11] that changes in the key parenting practices targeted by the interventions (parental positive reinforcement, limit setting, monitoring) do indeed predict reductions in disruptive behavior and improved outcomes over the course of child and adolescent development [12]. Specifically, a meta-analysis of 54 intervention trials involving social-learning parenting produced a weighted effect size of .35 for the programs’ impacts on delinquency and antisocial behavior [13].

Current State of the Field

In the past decade empirical research on social learning parenting interventions and related topics has grown substantially. In the ensuing years, many social learning interventions were widely implemented at scales that include cities, states, and entire countries [14]. In some instances these implementations at scale have been paired with rigorous evaluations designed to replicate the original randomized clinical trials. In many instances, the RCTs of large scale implementations have yielded positive evidence of impact [e.g.,15. 16], which is noteworthy in an era in which concerns about replicability of scientific findings have become an important issue. In addition, the focus on scaling these interventions has led to the development of tools to assess implementation completion and fidelity [17, 18].

Recent developments reflect themes in the larger field of parenting intervention research. We have organized our synopsis of the state of the field into two categories: confluence with other theoretical perspectives in the development of novel intervention strategies, and contributions from, and the potential of, translational neuroscience perspectives. These categories not only are highly interrelated, but in many instances are interdependent in terms of ongoing.

Confluence of Social Learning and Attachment Theory and Novel Intervention Strategies

One relatively recent development in the social learning parenting intervention field is its confluence with interventions that have evolved from different theoretical perspectives. Historically, for example, social learning theory and attachment theory were often deemed to be as incompatible as the behaviorist and psychodynamic intellectual traditions from which they emerged [19]. However, in the past decade the compatibility of these different perspectives has been widely discussed. For example, Patterson and Fisher noted that both attachment and coercion theory are grounded in close observation of the moment-by-moment interactions between parents and children [20]. Dozier and colleagues [21] suggested that rather than pit interventions from divergent perspectives against each other to determine which is most effective, it may be useful to view theories as complementary lenses that have more or less relevance, depending on the age and stage of development in question. As such, interventions that employ strategies derived from attachment theory and those from social learning theory may be more impactful than those from a single theoretical perspective. Evidence of the complementarity and convergence of traditionally disparate theoretical perspectives in the parenting intervention field can be derived from several sources. For example, Fisher and Kim [22] found that a social learning intervention for maltreated foster preschoolers had a significant impact on participants’ secure attachment-related behavior toward caregivers. A similar finding was reported by O’Connor et al. [23] in a randomized clinical trial of a social learning intervention for 4- to 6-year-old low-income community children. It may be that interventions that target core parenting behaviors exert a cascading effect on parent-child relationship outcomes [24]. Finally, work by Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer and Van IJzendoorn has documented the impact of a parenting program that combines content derived from attachment theory with social learning–based strategies for behavior management [25]. As such, the field appears to be moving more toward a focus on “relational interventions” [26] rather than focusing on questions about the adequacy/superiority of a single theory.

Parent coaching, including both video coaching and live, in-vivo coaching, is one approach that has emerged during the past decade as particularly emblematic of relational interventions. Video coaching uses footage of parent–child interaction obtained in naturalistic settings (usually the home); parents review the video with a practitioner and discuss what they have observed. In some video coaching programs the video is watched in its entirety, whereas in others the video is edited to show specific interactions. Programs also vary in whether they focus only on the parent responding appropriately to the child or whether they also include missed opportunities or negative parent behavior [27]. Apart from these variations, and regardless of whether the coaching program has roots in attachment or social learning theory (or both/other theories), all approaches emphasize consistent, predictable, and responsive parenting as a key leverage point in promoting healthy development. Not only is the emerging evidence base for video coaching quite strong, this approach also seems to be highly efficient, with programs that occur over relatively short periods of time proving to be the most effective [28].

Contributions from Translational Neuroscience

Social learning parenting interventions were among the first to incorporate neurobiological measures into the assessment protocols of randomized clinical trials. The early studies in this area provided evidence that effects of early adversity on the neuroendocrine stress-regulatory system, specifically the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, could be mitigated via family-based interventions [29]. Notably, attachment-based intervention studies conducted at the same time showed similar effects on HPA axis functioning [30, 31]. Although the specific mechanisms of action underlying these effects remain somewhat unclear, there is some indication that reductions in caregiver stress and the consistency/responsivity of the caregiving environment may contribute to these intervention effects [32].

Recent research in this area has progressed beyond the inclusion of neurobiological measures as outcomes in intervention studies. Figure 1 provides a conceptual model that illustrates how the original social learning intervention research, which stipulated that child outcomes might be improved by targeting malleable parenting practices known to be associated with these outcomes, has been adapted in the context of translational neuroscience. Such research considers (a) neurobiological systems known or hypothesized to be affected by (b) specific classes of adverse early experience, as mediated through (c) potentially modifiable caregiving practices to affect (d) specific child outcomes of importance.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Conceptual model for integrating neurobiological systems into social learning parenting intervention research.

Several directions of work show considerable promise. For example, studies examining how specific classes of stressful early experiences shape brain development are clarifying why individuals exposed to early adversity exhibit altered cognitive and social development. Numerous examples of research in this area, including work with rodents by Sullivan and Baram [33,34] and work with humans by Gee, Tottenham and colleagues [35], document how a responsive and consistent adult caregiver is a buffer against the effects of stress during a period when the immature organism lacks the capacity to mount an adequate biological or behavioral response to stress [36]. In these contexts, in addition to alterations in neuroendocrine functioning, the immune and metabolic systems are negatively affected, with lifelong consequences for inflammatory illnesses, diabetes, heart disease, and obesity [37]. In addition, the limbic threat detection system appears to mature at a faster rate, which may increase the risk for anxiety and affective disorders over the course of development [38].

Additional studies are identifying alterations in the brain and autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity of parents who provide nonresponsive or neglectful care. For example, brain imaging studies [39] show lower activation of neural reward circuitry in response to infant stimuli in the brains of neglectful mothers. Among physically abusive parents, patterns of significant coupling in their autonomic physiology and behavior while parenting suggest that it is more physiologically challenging for these parents to engage with their child in positive ways [40]. Specifically, lower respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) in physically abusive mothers appears to be associated with increased positive parenting in the moment, but subsequent increases in hostile control then immediately follow. Likewise, dynamic changes in maternal parasympathetic tone (RSA) are associated with extent of positive interactive synchrony observed in mother–preschooler interactions. Notably, a meta-analysis of research on ANS activity and parenting revealed an association between basal heart rate activity and risk for maltreatment, although there were no differences in ANS reactivity between maltreating and non-maltreating parents [41]. Together, these findings suggest a potential mediating role for brain responsivity and autonomic functioning in the association between early experiences and parenting behavior; however, the research has produced somewhat equivocal results to date and more investigations are needed in this area.

Translational neuroscience studies can further contribute to future social learning parenting intervention research in key ways. First, knowledge about the effects of adversity on cognitive and social development may help identify putative moderators of intervention effectiveness. For example, those who derive very limited pleasure from interacting with their child may not be as responsive to conventional interventions that encourage parents to be more responsive to their child. Second, to the extent that these adversity effects are modifiable, it may become possible to develop more precisely tailored intervention strategies. For example, noting the low base rates of oxytocin in neglectful mothers, Strathearn and colleagues [42] are investigating the effects of administering intranasal oxytocin in the context of parenting interventions. This this research is ongoing and results are not yet clear; notably, there is at least some evidence that the effects of this sort of intervention are moderated by stressful early experiences, in that the least responsive individuals appear to be those parents most in need of support [43]. Third, individual differences (in terms of both genetic and temperament measures) in sensitivity to environmental influences appear to be key moderators in explaining differential responsiveness to interventions [44, 45]. As such, these variables may prove to be important in guiding research designed to increase the impact of parenting interventions.

Conclusions

For the knowledge base in this area to continue to evolve, new interventions that are being developed should be paired with methodologies for evaluating their effectiveness that are more flexible than those of traditional randomized clinical trials. Adaptive evaluation designs [46], which have existed for decades in medicine and engineering, but are only just beginning to be employed in parenting intervention research, show considerable promise. Although not without limitations, adaptive designs allow for multiple rounds of randomization in order to evaluate how to increase intervention impact for those who are not responding, to forecast risk for early treatment dropout, and generate long-term maintenance of treatment gains. They can then be used to examine how to adapt interventions in terms of dosage or modality of delivery, or how to pair them with other strategies, to be maximally effective.

  • Social learning interventions are among the most successfully scaled parenting interventions.

  • There is increasing integration in the theoretical perspectives underlying parenting interventions, especially in the area of attachment and social learning theory.

  • Research on the effects of early life stress on biobehavioral development helps explain the attention and self-regulation difficulties observed among children who have experienced non-responsive parenting.

  • Social learning parenting interventions that are informed by the neurobiological effects of early stress have the potential to improve outcomes for children from the most disadvantaged backgrounds.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (grant numbers DA035763 to the first author and DA036533 to the second author), National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (grant number HD075716 to the first author), and Harvard Center on the Developing Child (Hemera Frontiers of Innovation initiative, funding granted to the first author).

References

  • 1.Bandura A. Influence of models’ reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative responses. J Personality Soc Psychol. 1965;1:589–595. doi: 10.1037/h0022070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bandura A, Ross D, Ross SA. Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1963;66:3–11. doi: 10.1037/h0048687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Patterson GR, Littman RA, Bricker W. Assertive behavior in children: a step toward a theory of aggression. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1967;32:iii–43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Patterson GR, Reid JB. Social interactional processes within the family: the study of the moment-by-moment family transactions in which human social development is imbedded. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1984;5:237–262. [Google Scholar]
  • 5**.Patterson GR. Coercive Family Process. Vol. 3. Castalia Publishing Company; 1982. This volume describes the seminal longitudinal research by Patterson and colleagues that led to the formulation of coercion theory. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Taylor T, Biglan A. Behavioral family interventions for improving child-rearing: a review of the literature for clinicians and policy makers. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 1998;1:41–60. doi: 10.1023/a:1021848315541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Forgatch MS, Rains LA, Knutson NM. A Course in PMTO: The Basic OSLC Intervention Model. 1, 2, & 3. Oregon Social Learning Center; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Eyberg SM, Funderburk BW, Hembree-Kigin TL, McNeil CB, Querido JG, Hood KK. Parent-child interaction therapy with behavior problem children: one and two year maintenance of treatment effects in the family. Child Fam Behav Ther. 2001;23:1–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gardner F, Leijten F. Incredible Years parenting interventions: Current effectiveness research and future directions. Current Opinion in Psych. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.023. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fisher PA, Chamberlain P. Multidimensional Treatment Foster Care: a program for intensive parenting, family support, and skill building. J Emotion Behav Disord. 2000;8:155–164. [Google Scholar]
  • 11**.Eddy JM, Chamberlain P. Family management and deviant peer association as mediators of the impact of treatment condition on youth antisocial behavior. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:857–863. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.5.857. This study shows that delinquent behavior can be reduced by targeting specific parenting practices; it is one of the strongest pieces of experimental evidence in support of coercion theory. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hakman M, Chaffin M, Funderburk B, Silovsky JF. Change trajectories for parent–child interaction sequences during parent–child interaction therapy for child physical abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 2009;33:461–470. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Piquero AR, Farrington DP, Welsh BC, Tremblay R, Jennings WG. Effects of early family/parent training programs on antisocial behavior and delinquency. J Exp Crim. 2009;5(2):83–120. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Dishion TJ, Forgatch M, Chamberlain P, Pelham WE. The Oregon model of behavior family therapy: from intervention design to promoting large-scale system change. Behav Ther. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.002. online first. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 15.Ogden T, Hagen KA. Treatment effectiveness of Parent Management Training in Norway: a randomized controlled trial of children with conduct problems. J Consul and Clin Psychol. 2008;76(4):607. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.4.607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jones K, Daley D, Hutchings J, Bywater T, Eames C. Efficacy of the Incredible Years Programme as an early intervention for children with conduct problems and ADHD: long-term follow-up. Child Care: Health and Devel. 2008;34(3):380–390. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2008.00817.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17*.Saldana L, Chamberlain P, Wang W, Brown CH. Predicting program start-up using the stages of implementation measure. Admin Policy Ment Health Ment Health Serv Res. 2012;39:419–425. doi: 10.1007/s10488-011-0363-y. This study describes how measurement of implementation using an established assessment tool can facilitate understanding about the success of evidence-based program start-up in community settings. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Forgatch MS, Patterson GR, DeGarmo DS. Evaluating fidelity: predictive validity for a measure of competent adherence to the Oregon model of parent management training. Behav Ther. 2006;36:3–13. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7894(05)80049-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kaufman J, Grasso D. The early intervention foster care program: a glass half full. Child Maltreat. 2006;11: 90–91. doi: 10.1177/1077559505283355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20*.Patterson GR, Fisher PA. Recent developments in our understanding of parenting: bidirectional effects, causal models, and the search for parsimony. Handbook Parent. 2002;5:59–88. This essay describes the evolution of social learning based parenting interventions and contains one of the first discussion of theoretical confluence between attachment and social learning theories in this area. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dozier M, Albus K, Fisher PA, Sepulveda S. Interventions for foster parents: implications for developmental theory. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14:843–860. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402004091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22*.Fisher PA, Kim HK. Intervention effects on foster preschoolers’ attachment-related behaviors from a randomized trial. Prev Sci. 2007;8:161–170. doi: 10.1007/s11121-007-0066-5. This study documents changes in maltreated foster preschoolers’ attachment related behaviors in the context of a social learning parenting intervention. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.O’Connor TG, Matias C, Futh A, Tantam G, Scott S. Social learning theory parenting intervention promotes attachment-based caregiving in young children: randomized clinical trial. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2012;42:358–370. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.723262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Patterson GR, Forgatch MS, DeGarmo DS. Cascading effects following intervention. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22:949–970. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Van Zeijl J, Mesman J, Van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Juffer F, Stolk MN, Koot HM, Alink LR. Attachment-based intervention for enhancing sensitive discipline in mothers of 1-to 3-year-old children at risk for externalizing behavior problems: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74:994–1005. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Toth SL, Gravener-Davis J, Guild DJ, Cicchetti D. Relational interventions for child maltreatment: past, present, & future perspectives. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25:1601–1617. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Balldin S, Fisher PA, Wirtberg I. Video feedback intervention with children: A systematic review. Res Soc Work Prac. doi: 10.1177/1049731516671809. in press. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psych Bull. 2003;129(2):195. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29**.Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M, Gunnar MR, Burraston BO. Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:892–905. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.06.008. This study provided some of the first evidence of neural plasticity following exposure to early adversity in foster children. The study shows that children’s diurnal cortisol levels were less likely to be blunted if they received a therapeutic foster care intervention based in social learning theory. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Dozier M, Peloso E, Lewis E, Laurenceau JP, Levine S. Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:845–859. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Mesman J, Alink LRA, Juffer F. Effects of an attachment-based intervention on daily cortisol moderated by DRD4: A randomized control trial on 1–3-year-olds screened for externalizing behavior. Dev Psychopathol. 2004;20:805–820. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32*.Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M. Intervention effects on foster parent stress: Associations with child cortisol levels. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:1003. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000473. This study provides evidence that one mechanism underlying dysregulated cortisol in maltreated foster children (which can be impacted by an intervention) is caregiver stress managing child problem behaviors. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sullivan R, Perry R, Sloan A, Kleinhaus K, Burtchen N. Infant bonding and attachment to the caregiver: insights from basic and clinical science. Clin Perinatol. 2011;38:643–655. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2011.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34*.Baram TZ, Davis EP, Obenaus A, Sandman CA, Small SL, Solodkin A, Stern H. Fragmentation and unpredictability of early-life experience in mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169:907–915. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091347. The fragmented care model described in this paper is an excellent example of cross-species research (in this case in rodents) can be useful for studying human early adversity. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Gee DG, Gabard-Durnam L, Telzer EH, Humphreys KL, Goff B, Shapiro M, Flannery J, Lumian D, Fareri DS, Caldera C, Tottenham N. Maternal buffering of human amygdala-prefrontal circuitry during childhood but not during adolescence. Psychol Sci. 2014 doi: 10.1177/0956797614550878. online first. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gunnar MR, Hostinar CE, Sanchez MM, Tottenham N, Sullivan RM. Parental buffering of fear and stress neurobiology: Reviewing parallels across rodent, monkey, and human models. Soc Neurosci. 2015;10:474–478. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1070198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.McEwen BS. The brain on stress: toward an integrative approach to brain, body, and behavior. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8:673–675. doi: 10.1177/1745691613506907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38*.Tottenham N, Hare TA, Quinn BT, McCarry TW, Nurse M, Gilhooly T, Millner A, Galvan A, Davidson MC, Eigsti I-M, Thomas KM, et al. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev Sci. 2010;13:46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x. This study is part of a body of research by Tottenham and colleagues showing that early adverse experiences are associated with early maturation of the neural threat detection system. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Strathearn L. Maternal neglect: oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of attachment. J Neuroendocrinol. 2011;23:1054–1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02228.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Skowron EA, Cipriano-Essel E, Gatzke-Kopp LM, Teti DM, Ammerman RT. Early adversity, RSA, and inhibitory control: evidence of children’s neurobiological sensitivity to social context. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56:964–978. doi: 10.1002/dev.21175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Reijman S, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Hiraoka R, Crouch JL, Milner JS, Alink LRA, van IJzendoorn MH. Baseline functioning and stress reactivity in maltreating parents and at-risk adults: Review and meta-analyses of autonomic nervous system studies. Child Maltreatment. 2016;21:327–342. doi: 10.1177/1077559516659937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Strathearn L. Maternal neglect: oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of attachment. J Neuroendocrinol. 2011;23:1054–1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02228.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Sniffing around oxytocin: review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharmacotherapy. Trans Psychiat. 2013;3:e258. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Belsky J, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Genetic differential susceptibility to the effects of parenting. Curr Opinion in Psych. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.021. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Overbeek G. Parenting intervention effects on children’s externalizing behavior: The moderating role of genotype and temperament. Curr Opinion in Psych. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.025. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Brown CH, Ten Have TR, Jo B, Dagne G, Wyman PA, Muthén B, Gibbons RD. Adaptive designs for randomized trials in public health. Ann Rev Public Health. 2013;30:1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.031308.100223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES