Abstract
Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a syndromic diagnosis. It is characterized by progressive impairment of higher (cortical) visual function with imaging evidence of degeneration affecting the occipital, parietal, and posterior temporal lobes bilaterally. Most cases will prove to have Alzheimer pathology. The aim of this review is to summarize the development of the concept of this disorder since it was first introduced. A critical discussion of the evolving diagnostic criteria is presented and the differential diagnosis with regard to the underlying pathology is reviewed. Emphasis is given to the visual dysfunction that defines the disorder, and the classical deficits, such as simultanagnosia and visual agnosia, as well as the more recently recognized visual field defects, are reviewed, along with the evidence on their neural correlates. The latest developments on the imaging of PCA are summarized, with special attention to its role on the differential diagnosis with related conditions.
Keywords: posterior cortical atrophy, Alzheimer’s disease (AD), Balint’s syndrome, visual agnosia, visual fields, hemianopia, magnetic resonance imaging imaging
Introduction
In 1988, Benson et al. described an intriguing progressive condition characterized by a complex visual disorder occurring in the absence of ocular dysfunction (1). Their title introduced the syndromic diagnosis “posterior cortical atrophy” (PCA) which has proven apt and has survived to the present. The most common deficits in their cohort were components of the Bálint (simultanagnosia, optic ataxia, and ocular apraxia) and Gerstmann (agraphia, acalculia, finger agnosia, and right-left disorientation) syndromes (2, 3); additional features were alexia, visual agnosia, and transcortical sensory aphasia. Eventually non-visual functions, such as language and memory, were affected but until a very late stage these deficits were relatively mild and the visual disorder remained the main source of impairment throughout the course of the disease. Insight was preserved until late. Neuroimaging of these patients indicated disproportionate volume loss in the posterior cortical regions, particularly in the occipital and posterior parietal lobes (1). This clinicoradiological syndrome was soon found to be associated with AD (4, 5), but, as opposed to the typical (amnestic) form of the disease, there was an anterior–posterior gradient in PCA, with the greater severity of change occurring in the occipital, parietal and posterior temporal lobes.
With an increasing number of cases coming to autopsy other pathological entities—such as corticobasal degeneration (CBD), Lewy body disease (LBD), and prion diseases (6, 7)—were occasionally described as the underlying cause of PCA. More recently, PCA was reported in an individual with a mutation associated with frontotemporal dementia (8). AD, however, remains the single dominant cause, accounting for 62–100% of the cases in the largest cohorts (6, 7, 9). Thus, PCA has been recognized as one of the atypical variants of AD (10) and indeed it is occasionally referred to as the “visual variant” of AD but this implies a more certain pathological diagnosis than is usually the case. From an integrative perspective, it may also be understood within a continuum of phenotypic variation of AD, since considerable clinical overlap occurs between PCA and other AD variants, especially its amnestic and language presentations (11, 12), more so at later stages. With time all PCA patients will progress to dementia of the AD type. However, it is as a visual disorder that PCA gains singularity, for the visual deficits are usually extremely disabling, even when the patient may still be considered cognitively preserved. This characteristic implies that PCA patients differ from patients with more cognitive presentations of AD in several aspects, including diagnosis and management.
Epidemiology
The prevalence of PCA is unknown. In clinical cohorts published by centers for cognitive disorders, it has been found to represent about 5% of the total AD cases (13, 14). No estimate is available from ophthalmic services. Age at diagnosis is mostly within the late 50s and early 60s (15–17) but PCA can affect individuals from the 40s (18) to the 80s (7). Females and males are equally represented in several studies (7, 17), but some have observed a female predominance (18–20).
Patients with PCA and their families will usually describe a time-consuming search for the diagnosis, including appointments with several specialists—usually optometrists and ophthalmologists—before a neurological disorder is suspected (21). It is not rare for patients to be provided with numerous pairs of spectacles or even undergo cataract surgery or other procedures only to learn later that the problem is not in the eyes. Given this experience, it is a common impression amongst specialists dealing with PCA patients that this condition is underdiagnosed.
Clinical Profile
Visual Manifestations
One of the difficulties in diagnosing patients with PCA is that, although they complain about problems with their vision, descriptions of their symptoms are often difficult for the non-specialist to analyze. They may just say they cannot see, describe their vision as blurry, or may refer to difficulties performing specific tasks such as driving or reading. Only a detailed examination may uncover the specific deficit(s) leading to functional impairment. In the following sections the most important visual deficits in PCA are described.
Simultanagnosia
Simultanagnosia refers to the failure to perceive multiple visual locations simultaneously or to shift attention from one object to another, which results in a very restricted effective visual field (22). A patient may miss an object he or she has just seen or report that objects seem to appear or disappear from view. Simultanagnosia has been consistently demonstrated as the most frequent deficit in PCA, occurring in above 90% of the patients in several series (7, 15–17). It is a pervasive deficit that may be associated with some unusual behavior including the reverse-size phenomenon. This describes patients preferring to look at objects at distance, in order to appreciate them globally, or finding it easier to read small than large letters—such as the text rather than the headlines of a newspaper (23). In severe cases of simultanagnosia, perception of even a single, large object may be impaired, and sometimes an individual part of it may be mistaken for a different object [so-called “partonomic” error (24)].
Tasks relying on visual integration are used to test for simultanagnosia. Established tests include interpretation of a complex visual scene (25), such as the Boston cookie-theft picture, and reading fragmented letters. Failure to read the Ishihara pseudoisochromatic plates despite preserved color perception is a conspicuous feature in many patients with simultanagnosia (24, 26). The latter is often the only abnormality seen in the basic visual assessment of a PCA patient and its usefulness to raise the diagnostic suspicion cannot be overemphasized, although it is usually misinterpreted as a color deficit. However, the patients have as much trouble with the first (control) plate, which does not require color vision as do the subsequent plates. In its purest form simultanagnosia is considered due to impaired visual attention, which can be considered both in terms of shifts of attention to regions within the visual field and also shifts of attention related to the scale of the object to be processed. The former will be mirrored in impairment of ocular motor behavior but the latter may not be. However, it has also been argued that the attentional deficit may be object based resulting in failure to identify overlapping figures (objects at the same spatial location), or collocated objects where linking features have been weakened [such identifying correctly a star of David where the two component triangles are the same but not different colors (27)]. It should also be considered that the perception of illusory contours is a very early process in object identification and may occur as early as V2: this is likely related to the synthesis of partially occluded objects (28). Impairment of this early function in the identification of surfaces, which has been reported in simultanagnosia (29) would certainly seriously impair the identification of fragmented images.
Other Elements of the Bálint Syndrome—Optic Ataxia and Ocular Apraxia
Simultanagnosia may occur in isolation or may be associated with optic ataxia and ocular apraxia, constituting the Bálint syndrome. Optic ataxia—lack of eye-hand coordination—refers to impaired reaching to objects when guided by vision with the preserved ability to do so when the object is accessed by means of other sensory modalities, e.g., sound (22), while ocular apraxia is a disorder of fixation, with the patient failing to fixate a specific object within the visual field in the absence of any ocular motor deficit (30). In PCA, the Bálint syndrome is often incomplete. Silmultanagnosia is thought to be an early finding, initially presenting in isolation or associated with ocular apraxia, with optic ataxia developing later in the course of disease (17). Bálint syndrome is classically seen in the context of biparietal damage due to vascular disorders; however, it has been associated with PCA so often that its occurrence in a progressive manner should raise suspicion of the diagnosis.
Visual Agnosia
Visual agnosia is a visuoperceptual disorder. It is defined as the inability to recognize objects presented visually, in the absence of any ocular or semantic deficit that could otherwise account for it (31). It is further divided into apperceptive and associative, according to the defective process being in the perceptual analysis of the object or in attributing a meaning to it, respectively. In PCA, the apperceptive subtype predominates (16, 32), demonstrated by the patient failing to copy a figure or match a figure with a sample (33). A particular form of visual agnosia affects the recognition of faces (prosopagnosia), a deficit that is a source of great social embarrassment. As with global visual agnosia, prosopagnosia in PCA is thought to be perceptual rather than agnosic in nature (17).
Reading Disturbance
Trouble reading is one of the most frequent and disabling deficits for which PCA patients seek help. It can be due to acquired primary alexia (34), but most often reading impairment in PCA results from a combination of deficits including simultanagnosia, ocular apraxia, visual crowding (16, 24) and potentially homonymous visual field defects.
Visual Field Defects
The occurrence of visual field defects in PCA has been a controversial subject, mainly due to the eloquence of the higher order visuospatial deficits that may arguably compromise the interpretation of visual field tests (35). Indeed, early PCA series dismissed visual field defects as exceptional in this condition (15). However, homonymous hemianopia or quadrantanopia was found in almost 50% of the patients in another series (7) and prevalence is even higher in groups of PCA patients who have visual fields performed as part of the workup (36–38), suggesting this deficit may be overlooked if not routinely searched for. Homonymous visual field defects are increasingly recognized as an early sign in PCA (39), and their occurrence may, remarkably, precede the higher order visual disorder (40, 41). Figure 1 shows a typical visual field test result in a patient with PCA. The inferior quadrants are possibly more affected in the visual variant of AD (4, 42); this would imply involvement of the underlying optic radiations as occurs with bilateral occipitoparietal infarction, but this needs confirmation.
Other Visual Deficits
Besides these major, well-characterized deficits, patients with PCA have been reported to complain of a variety of visual problems, including perceived motion of static stimuli, visual crowding, color washout, and prolonged color afterimages (23, 24). In addition, related deficits previously documented in AD patients and attributed to visual cortical pathology, e.g., abnormal contrast sensitivity and loss of color discrimination, particularly affecting short wave length (blue) stimuli (43), may apply to PCA as well. Basic visual skills, such as form detection and discrimination, color perception, and motion coherence were more recently highlighted as possibly contributing to the higher order visual deficits classically reported in PCA (19). A specific dysfunction of the magnocellular (M) pathway in AD, associated with impairment of motion perception and loss of achromatic contrast, has been proposed (44).
Non-Visual Deficits
Non-visual deficits in PCA are mainly represented by disturbances of numeracy and literacy. These deficits may occur as part of a Gerstmann syndrome (agraphia, acalculia, left-right disorientation, and finger agnosia) or may be isolated. Attentional disorders may occur as visual (16, 45) or spatial neglect (46) or a combination of the two. Ideomotor apraxia is not rare, but it is usually mild in early stages, and if prominent should raise the suspicion of an alternative diagnosis, such as CBD. The same can be said if features of asymmetric parkinsonism are found. Poly-mini-myoclonus has been suggested to be an overlooked sign in PCA, present in a majority of patients (17). Visual hallucinations and REM-sleep behavior disorder have been occasionally observed in PCA, and are also thought to indicate an underlying non-AD pathology, namely DLB (17). Depression is common and is thought to be mainly reactional (35).
It should be noted that dressing and constructional apraxias, which are very common in PCA (7, 47), do not constitute apraxia in its proper definition, but rather visuospatial deficits (48).
Neural Correlates of the Visual Deficits in PCA
The higher order visual deficits observed in PCA patients are better understood as reflecting regional disruption of the two visual pathways for processing of object (ventral, “what” stream) and space (dorsal, “where” stream) (49, 50). This understanding has been favored by imaging studies showing correlation of simultanagnosia, the prototype of the visuospatial disorder in PCA, with greater atrophy in the dorsal (occipitoparietal) regions (20), while visuoperceptual deficits, e.g., visual agnosia, are associated with predominant volume loss in the ventral (occipitotemporal) regions (34). Based on this schematic representation of visual processing, a clinical classification of PCA into a dorsal and a ventral subtype has been proposed (16). The first subtype would be represented by patients with predominant Bálint syndrome, apraxia, and neglect, while the second would be characterized by disproportionate visual agnosia, prosopagnosia, and alexia. A third subtype has been suggested to include patients with primary visual failure and impairment in basic visual skills (9, 51), in whom more marked occipital damage would be expected. Indeed, homonymous hemianopia was recently found to be associated with lateralized occipital degeneration in PCA (38). Interestingly, however, even in the presence of visual field deficits, the primary visual cortex remains relatively unaffected compared to higher visual areas.
In clinical practice, most patients present with a confluence of deficits relating to the occipital, parietal, and posterior temporal lobes, and this is mirrored by imaging studies showing considerable overlap of patterns of atrophy when these subgroups of patients are analyzed in combination (20).
Diagnosis
The critical element on the diagnosis of PCA clinico-radiological syndrome is the recognition of a progressive focal posterior cortical dysfunction associated with imaging evidence of damage to posterior cortical regions. As part of the clinical characterization, a formal neuropsychological assessment is necessary to establish the degree of involvement of individual cognitive domains and confirm that the disorder is relatively restricted to occipital and parietal regions.
Diagnostic Criteria
Several clinical criteria for PCA have been published (7, 15, 16, 18), which are highly consistent in their definition of PCA. They all emphasize a higher visual disorder of insidious onset, manifesting with deficits of the dorsal and/or ventral stream, and which occurs with relative preservation of more anterior functions, such as memory and language. Some variability exists though in the delimitation of the syndrome. Remarkably, the Tang-Wai criteria (Box 1) introduce visual field defects as a core feature of PCA, given the same importance as simultanagnosia, constructional dyspraxia, environmental disorientation, and any element of the Gerstmann syndrome; at the same time, early parkinsonism and visual hallucinations, meant to distinguish LBD—a disease that may present with posterior cortical deficits but usually shows concomitant or fast developing involvement of other cortical and subcortical regions—are deemed exclusion criteria. Such phenotypic refinement may understandably impact on the specificity of these criteria.
Box 1. Diagnostic criteria for posterior cortical atrophy Tang-Wai et al. (7).
Core features
Insidious onset and gradual progression
Presentation of visual complaints in the absence of significant primary ocular disease explaining the symptoms
Relative preservation of anterograde memory and insight early in the disorder
Disabling visual impairment throughout the disorder
Absence of stroke or tumor
Absence of early parkinsonism and hallucinations
Any of the following findings:
Simultanagnosia with or without optic ataxia or ocular apraxia
Constructional dyspraxia
Visual field defect
Environmental disorientation
Any of the elements of Gerstmann syndrome
Supportive features
Alexia
Presenile onset
Ideomotor or dressing apraxia
Prosopagnosia
Investigations
Neuropsychological deficits referable to parietal and/or occipital regions
Focal or asymmetric atrophy in parietal and/or occipital regions on structural imaging
Focal or asymmetric hypoperfusion/hypometabolism in parietal and/or occipital regions on functional imaging
Indeed, by excluding patients with specific LBD features, Tang-Wai et al. aimed to rule out LBD as a distinct condition from PCA; however, thereby, PCA caused by LBD or mixed (LBD-AD) pathologies are likely to be excluded as well (6, 7). Another conflicting issue is the inclusion of patients with PCA due CBD. Asymmetric parkinsonism and apraxia are suggested to distinguish PCA-CBD from PCA-AD, but these features are not addressed in a structured manner. In fact, if parkinsonism develops early in the course of disease, patients with PCA-CBD will be excluded. Therefore, while these criteria were not designed for the etiological diagnosis of PCA, they may be more specific for AD. On one hand, this characteristic has been critical for the very recognition of PCA as a variant of AD, on the other hand, a vacuum is left on how to classify those patients with a progressive posterior cortical dysfunction whose clinical features extend beyond the PCA typical phenotype.
A different (lumpers) approach to the diagnosis of PCA has recently been suggested (52). While preserving the core clinical description of the primary PCA syndrome (Box 2), the newly developed classification provides a solution on how to deal with atypical features, including them into a stratified framework to the etiological diagnosis (Figure 2): at the first level, the presence of the PCA clinico-radiological syndrome is established; the second level consists in of deciding whether PCA occurs in isolation (PCA-pure) or whether criteria for another neurodegenerative condition (e.g., visual hallucinations for LBD) are also met (PCA-plus); at the third level, these phenotypical categories are combined with the presence of pathology-specific biomarkers to yield a disease-level PCA description. Ideally, at this level, the patient will be given a diagnosis of PCA-AD, PCA-LBD, PCA-CBD, PCA-prion, and others. If a patient has PCA-pure and positive biomarkers for AD, the definitive diagnosis of PCA-AD may be given in vivo. However, since disease biomarkers are available only for AD and prion, and AD may manifest with such diverse phenotypes, diagnosis of PCA-LBD and PCA-CBD are currently presumptive and dependent on negativity to AD biomarkers. For instance, when patients fulfill criteria for both PCA and CBD and AD biomarkers are negative, the diagnosis of probable PCA-CBD may be appropriate. Alternatively, if a patient with PCA-plus tests positive for AD biomarkers, the diagnosis of AD may be given, although as disease-specific markers are not available for the second clinical suspected condition, the possibility of a dual pathology is still reasonable. These criteria take into account the suggested diagnostic criteria for AD (10) in their comprehensive approach of phenotype and disease-specific biomarkers. Their novelty is to include under the term PCA patients with posterior cortical dysfunction whom would otherwise not be given a unified diagnosis. Only further pathological studies will confirm the accuracy of these criteria. An expected consequence is an increment in the proportion of patients with non-AD pathologies in PCA cohorts. In the clinic the major priority is to recognize the syndrome and manage appropriately whatever the underlying pathology might be. Predictors of whether AD is the underlying pathology or not will not affect management materially until features of the other conditions are clinically apparent.
Box 2. Core features of the posterior cortical atrophy clinicoradiological syndrome Crutch et al. (52).
Clinical features:
Insidious onset
Gradual progression
Prominent early disturbance of visual ± other posterior cognitive functions
Cognitive features:
At least three of the following must be present as early or presenting features ± evidence of their impact on activities of daily living:
Space perception deficit
Simultanagnosia
Object perception deficit
Constructional dyspraxia
Environmental agnosia
Oculomotor apraxia
Dressing apraxia
Optic ataxia
Alexia
Left/right disorientation
Acalculia
Limb apraxia (not limb-kinetic)
Apperceptive prosopagnosia
Agraphia
Homonymous visual field defect
Finger agnosia
All the following must be evident:
Relatively spared anterograde memory function
Relatively spared speech and non-visual language functions
Relatively spared executive functions
Relatively spared behavior and personality
Neuroimaging:
Predominant occipitoparietal or occipitotemporal atrophy/hypometabolism/hypoperfusion on magnetic resonance imaging/18F-labeled fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography/SPECT, single-photon emission computed tomography.
Exclusion criteria:
Evidence or a brain tumor or other mass lesion sufficient to explain the symptoms
Evidence of significant vascular disease including focal stroke sufficient to explain the symptoms
Evidence of afferent visual cause (e.g., optic nerve, chiasm, or tract)
Evidence of other identifiable causes for cognitive impairment (e.g., renal failure)
In the following sections, the imaging studies that support PCA as a clinico-radiological syndrome are reviewed, as well as in vivo pathological biomarkers for AD. In the particular case of LBD, to which no disease-specific biomarker is available, metabolic studies that are associated with the disease are also mentioned.
Imaging Studies
Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)
The syndrome of PCA can manifest without any detectable gray or white matter volume loss at MRI (24), but more commonly patients present with marked atrophy in the occpitoparietal and occipitotemporal regions bilaterally, but often more severe in the right hemisphere (15, 53, 54) (Figure 3). Although the disparate posterior volume loss is used to distinguish PCA from typical AD at imaging, it is not rare for patients with PCA to present coexisting atrophy in the mesial temporal regions, including the hippocampi (54). However, when directly compared, studies have generally shown only subtle differences in levels of atrophy between PCA and AD, with PCA patients showing greater atrophy in the right visual association cortex, while left hippocampal atrophy predominates in amnestic AD (53, 54). Millington et al. (38) used multimodal MR imaging to investigate structural and functional brain changes in a cohort of patients with PCA, all with visual field defects. Compared with healthy controls, cortical activation was reduced in the occipital lobes, with no significant lateralization, while gray matter loss was greater in extrastriate occipital regions, but more marked in the hemisphere contralateral to the visual field deficit (Figure 4). Likewise, reduction in white matter integrity, which was widespread, was lateralized to the hemisphere originating the hemianopia but in the occipital lobes only.
Positron Emission Tomography with Fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG-PET)
Functional imaging is used as evidence of neurodegeneration in PCA, being particularly useful when MRI is considered normal. When examined with FDG-PET, patients with PCA show hypometabolism that is more marked in the occipitoparietal regions, sometimes with involvement of the frontal eye fields, but with relative sparing of the mesial regions of the frontal and temporal lobes (55–57). Directly compared with amnestic AD, PCA patients present more severe occipitoparietal and/or occipital hypometabolism in the right hemisphere (47, 55, 57, 58).
Positron emission tomography with fluorodeoxyglucose has also been studied in the differential diagnosis between PCA and LBD, with variable results (57, 59). A common problem is that, in line with overlapping posterior presentations, occipital hypometabolism may occur in both conditions (7, 60). In a comparative study, patients with PCA showed greater hypometabolism in the right temporooccipital cortex, while LBD was distinguished by hypometabolism predominating in the left occipital cortex (57). In another study (59), however, rather than lateralization, it was the degree of asymmetry and anterior extension that best distinguished them: PCA and LBD were both associated with bilateral occipitoparietal hypometabolism, but a higher degree of asymmetry favored PCA, while extension to orbitofrontal and anterior temporal regions was suggestive of LBD. The relative preservation of the posterior cingulate cortex compared with the precuneus and cuneus—a sign that has shown to distinguish LBD from typical AD (61, 62)—was not specific to LBD when compared with PCA (59).
PET Imaging of Microglia
This novel imaging modality is based on the involvement of microglia activation in the pathogenesis of neurodegenerative diseases. The PET tracer 11C-PBR28 binds to the translocator protein-18 kDa, which is overexpressed in activated microglia; therefore, its regional distribution can be interpreted as an effect of local degeneration (63). In 11C-PBR28-PET, patients with PCA showed greater binding in occipital, posterior parietal, and temporal regions compared to controls and were distinguished from amnestic AD patients by higher binding in the occipital cortex bilaterally (64). Despite these encouraging results, TPSO PET imaging still has some limitations, including the existence of non-binders (65).
Dopamine Transporter Imaging
Albeit not part of PCA routine assessment, this test may be very helpful when the clinical diagnosis of LBD is in the differential, as well as in patients with established PCA who progress with LBD features, e.g., parkinsonism and visual hallucinations. The finding of low dopamine transporter concentration in basal ganglia, measured with PET or single photon emission tomography (SPECT), supports the diagnosis of LBD (60).
Electrophysiology Studies
The study of event-related potentials (ERPs) involves the quantification of eletroencephalographic changes caused by cognitive, motor, or sensory stimulation. The responses are stereotyped and time-locked with stimulus, permitting analysis of the various stages of the neural process. The best recognized finding of ERP in AD patients is an abnormal P3b (also known as P-300), a higher cortical response associated with an update in working memory (66). Potentials associated with earlier visual processing, such as P1 and N1, may also be abnormal (67). The latter is ascribed to dysfunction of the visual association cortex (67), which is commonly involved by AD pathology (68). Interestingly, in non-symptomatic PSEN1 carriers (preclinical AD) tested with a visual recognition memory task, ERP were reduced in frontal and increased in occipital regions, suggesting that posterior cortical decline in AD is preceded by increased reliance on these regions (69). There is no group study of the visually evoked potential (VEP) in PCA, but single-case reports show that they may be normal (70) or delayed (40). This is likely explained by variable disease stages and clinical presentations. Indeed, Mares et al. (71) studied a PCA patient with pure alexia, showing bilaterally absent N170—an ERP component associated with word reading and activation of the visual word form area—and concomitantly abnormal P1 (initially delayed in the left hemisphere only, later bilaterally), indicating that the early visual cortical dysfunction may have contributed to the reading disorder. Since visual ERPs depend on the integrity of the visual system, the finding of abnormal VEPs may favor the diagnosis of PCA in the appropriate clinical context, but there is no evidence to support their use in the differential diagnosis with amnestic AD.
AD Biomarkers
Positron Emission Tomography with an Amyloid-binding Tracer (PET-Amyloid)
Amyloid imaging has a role in the diagnosis of AD, more so in presenile patients—when greater deposition of β-amyloid is not expected—but has no use in distinguishing PCA from amnestic AD, as the same global pattern of deposition is seen (17, 72). This is no surprise, since in pathological studies of PCA, the anterior-posterior gradient refers to the disparate concentration of tau-derived neurofibrillary tangles in posterior regions, while the distribution of β-amyloid is diffuse (7).
Cerebrospinal Fluid (CSF) Analysis with Measurement of AD Pathology Biomarkers
Cerebrospinal fluid examination is not required for the diagnosis of PCA, but this test is usually recommended in presenile dementia to exclude treatable causes. Furthermore, in the current diagnostic approach to AD, CSF biomarkers have a positive predictive value when they show low concentrations of amyloid β, increased total tau, and increased phospho-tau (10). As with the PET-amyloid, the application of these tests to PCA patients did not help to distinguish them from amnestic AD (35), although they have a place in the differential diagnosis of AD and alternative underlying pathologies.
Tau-PET
Several tau-specific tracers for PET, including 18F-AV-1451 (Flortaucipir) were recently made available for clinical assessment of various tauopathies (73). Although their affinity for specific tau deposits is still to be established, the application of tau-PET to the diagnosis of AD is promising. Because 18F-AV-1451 binds to hyperphosphorylated paired helical filament tau and neurofibrillary tangles (73), and tau pathology—but not amyloid—is at disproportionate higher concentration in posterior brain regions in PCA (5, 7), the tau-PET has the additional potential to distinguish PCA from other AD variants. A pattern with localized elevation of 18F-AV-1451 to posterior regions has indeed been shown to strongly correlate with PCA (74, 75), distinguishing it from amnestic AD (76). Moreover, regional tau-binding mirrors regional patterns of both hypometabolism (74, 75) and atrophy (75) across AD major phenotypes, PCA included. For this reason, it is possible that this test be included as an in vivo imaging biomarker of AD in future (75).
Serum Biomarkers of AD
Decrement of peripheral β-amyloid does occur in AD, but later than CSF levels, so that an important effect is observed at the stage of dementia only (77). Plasma levels of Aβ have not been studied in PCA. Markers of neuronal injury, the tau, and neurofilament light (NFL) proteins are elevated in the sera of AD patients, but levels significantly overlap with those of controls and individuals with mild cognitive impairment, although NFL may be more accurate (78). These difficulties have prevented the recognition of a serum signature of AD. Plasma NFL levels further correlate with longitudinal measures of cognition and atrophy in AD and have been suggested as a tool for screening patients at risk of cognitive decline (78); however, NFL is not specific for AD, thus cannot help in the differential diagnosis with other neurodegenerative conditions.
PCA as a Phenotype within the Spectrum of AD Clinical Variability
The predominance of right hemisphere deficits in PCA patients observed in metabolic (47, 55, 57, 58, 79), as well as structural (20, 54, 80) imaging is intriguing. Some of the syndrome’s most characteristic deficits correlate with either bilateral (e.g., simultanagnosia) or dominant hemisphere (e.g., Gerstmann syndrome) dysfunction (18). Besides, a selective vulnerability of the right hemisphere cannot be easily hypothesized on a pathological basis. The explanation for such disparity may instead lie with the syndrome definition. Failure in visual object and space perception, which underlies the concept of PCA as a higher order visual disorder, is indeed associated with bilateral occipital and right parietal damage (74, 81). A left hemisphere dysfunction would manifest with predominant language deficits; accordingly, relatively focal left parietal atrophy/hypometabolism is a topographical marker of logopenic primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA) (82), the “language” variant of AD (10). In line with syndrome continuity, when patients with PCA develop language problems, these are usually dominated by word retrieval deficits, a clinical overlap with lvPPA (12).
In clinic, the differentiation between PCA and amnestic AD is more often a challenge, for the latter is common and can also present with visuospatial deficits. The basis for these common deficits is a shared neuroanatomic substrate that critically involves the parietal lobes. This has been identified as the “default model network” (DMN), after the observation that it activates during non-focused rest, which encompasses structures such as the medial-temporal lobe, precuneus, posterior cingulate, and temporoparietal junction (83), with a central role suggested to the precuneus (84). The DMN is commonly affected across AD variants, however at disproportionate, syndrome specific regional severities (11, 85). In typical AD, pathology starts in the entorhinal cortex, then stereotypically spreads to limbic regions then to isocortex, including parietal association cortex (68, 86). In PCA, the higher pathology burden is found in the primary visual and visual association cortices, with the posterior parietal regions being commonly involved as well (5). Likely reflecting pathology, at metabolic imaging, the highest degree of overlap between AD variants localizes to the dorsal DMN (85). From a dynamic perspective, the parietal lobe may thus be seen as a hub where PCA and AD meet, in diverging directions, within a common network of progression. Accordingly, patients with PCA show decreased functional connectivity in the visual network, in various regions—including the precuneus—within the DMN, as well as more anterior structures (87). In addition, white matter loss is more diffuse in PCA than would be expected from its relatively focal posterior presentation (38, 87), likely anticipating more anterior deficits. A consequence of these converging patterns of degeneration between PCA and typical AD is that the diagnosis of patients presenting with focal parietal deficits may be challenging. For instance, in a patient with a relative isolated visuoconstructive disorder and right parietal hypometabolism, the differential diagnosis between AD and PCA may not be accurate until further memory or visual deficits develop, and/or hypometabolism extends to more temporal or occipital regions, respectively, although the preservation of memory and the lateralized right presentation could increase the odds for PCA.
Genetics
Posterior cortical atrophy is predominantly a sporadic condition. However, the PCA phenotype has rarely been described in association with genetic mutations known to be implicated in familial AD, [PSEN1 (88, 89) and PSEN2 (90)], mutations associated with frontotemporal dementia [MAPT (91) and GRN genes (8)], as well as in the gene of the prion protein (PRNP) (92).
In recent years, there has been an effort to understand the factors driving phenotypic variation in AD. Previous investigations of the commonest genetic risk factor to late onset AD, the allele ε4 of the APOE yielded conflicting results in PCA, with some studies suggesting that variation in this gene confers an increased risk to the visual variant of AD (54, 93), while no association was found by others (13, 94). A recently published consortium study, which included the largest number of PCA patients to date, reported a robust association between variation in/near APOE/TOMM40 and risk for PCA, but with a smaller effect than that for amnestic AD (95).
Variants of TREM2 and PSEN2 that modify the risk for AD have also been reported in PCA (93), but there is no evidence for any particular effect in PCA as compared to amnestic AD.
Treatment
No study is available reporting the effectiveness of acetyl-cholinesterase inhibitors in this condition. Nonetheless, given the strong pathological association with AD, most clinicians dealing with PCA patients find it appropriate to offer them a trial of these drugs. Likewise, memantine is sometimes tried in individual cases.
A considerable part of the management of PCA consists of assisting patients in taking decisions about their occupational and daily lives, considering that, albeit slowly, vision and cognition will continue to deteriorate. Among lifestyle changes stopping driving should be recommended. One of us (GTP) considers the condition to be appropriate grounds for registration as “severely sight impaired” which in the UK is equivalent to being registered as “legally blind” despite normal visual acuity in the early stages.
Conclusion
Posterior cortical atrophy, a clinico-radiological syndrome that in most cases represents a focal form of AD, is unique among the known dementing conditions for causing a highly disabling visual disorder with preserved cognitive status in the early stages. The course of PCA due to AD is stereotyped with virtually all patients later developing memory loss and progressing into full dementia. The recognition of PCA as an atypical variant of AD and the availability of accurate AD biomarkers has made PCA a condition where a diagnosis of definitive AD can be given in vivo. When non-AD pathologies are the cause of PCA, clues for the alternative pathology are often found in clinical features as well as imaging.
Despite the progress in the understanding of the neural basis of PCA in recent years, patients still frequently experience a painful delay in diagnosis, mainly because it is not appreciated that their symptoms are associated with brain dysfunction by optometrists and ophthalmologists who are consulted. The need of increasing awareness among clinicians cannot be overestimated, and this should involve not only neurologists, but general practitioners, optometrists, and ophthalmologists.
For all who see patients with visual symptoms we would emphasize the following. First, in the anamnesis, take note of visual symptoms that have an emphasis on spatial disorientation. Second, in the basic clinical assessment, such features as unexplained difficulty with Ishihara plates, variable homonymous defects on perimetry and a tendency to omit letters on the acuity chart should raise suspicion of the disorder.
Author Contributions
GP conceived project and revised early drafts of the article. HB, RM, and MJ-G revised early drafts of the article. MNMdS generated first draft of article.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Funding. MNMdS was supported by a scholarship from the Brazilian Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa (CNPq). RM held a Medical Research Council Studentship and a Foulkes Fellowship. HB is a Royal Society University Research Fellow.
References
- 1.Benson DF, Davis RJ, Snyder BD. Posterior cortical atrophy. Arch Neurol (1988) 45:789–93. 10.1001/archneur.1988.00520310107024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bálint R. Seelenlä. hmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der des Aufmerksamkeit. Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol (1909) 25:51–181. 10.1159/000210464 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gerstmann J. Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left, agraphia and acalculia. Arch Neurol Psychiatry (1940) 44:398–407. 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1940.02280080158009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Levine DN, Lee JM, Fisher CM. The visual variant of Alzheimer’s disease: a clinicopathologic case study. Neurology (1993) 43:305–13. 10.1212/WNL.43.2.305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hof PR, Vogt BA, Bouras C, Morrison JH. Atypical form of Alzheimer’s disease with prominent posterior cortical atrophy: a review of lesion distribution and circuit disconnection in cortical visual pathways. Vision Res (1997) 37:3609–25. 10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00240-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Renner JA, Burns JM, Hou CE, McKeel DW, Jr, Storandt M, Morris JC. Progressive posterior cortical dysfunction: a clinicopathologic series. Neurology (2004) 63:1175–80. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000140290.80962.BF [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tang-Wai DF, Graff-Radford NR, Boeve BF, Dickson DW, Parisi JE, Crook R, et al. Clinical, genetic, and neuropathologic characteristics of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology (2004) 63:1168–74. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000140289.18472.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Caroppo P, Belin C, Grabli D, Maillet D, De SA, Migliaccio R, et al. Posterior cortical atrophy as an extreme phenotype of GRN mutations. JAMA Neurol (2015) 72:224–8. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.3308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Alladi S, Xuereb J, Bak T, Nestor P, Knibb J, Patterson K, et al. Focal cortical presentations of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain (2007) 130:2636–45. 10.1093/brain/awm213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Hampel H, Molinuevo JL, Blennow K, et al. Advancing research diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease: the IWG-2 criteria. Lancet Neurol (2014) 13:614–29. 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70090-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Warren JD, Fletcher PD, Golden HL. The paradox of syndromic diversity in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol (2012) 8:451–64. 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Crutch SJ, Lehmann M, Warren JD, Rohrer JD. The language profile of posterior cortical atrophy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2013) 84:460–6. 10.1136/jnnp-2012-303309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Snowden JS, Stopford CL, Julien CL, Thompson JC, Davidson Y, Gibbons L, et al. Cognitive phenotypes in Alzheimer’s disease and genetic risk. Cortex (2007) 43:835–45. 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70683-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Koedam EL, Lauffer V, van der Vlies AE, van der Flier WM, Scheltens P, Pijnenburg YA. Early-versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease: more than age alone. J Alzheimers Dis (2010) 19:1401–8. 10.3233/JAD-2010-1337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mendez MF, Ghajarania M, Perryman KM. Posterior cortical atrophy: clinical characteristics and differences compared to Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2002) 14:33–40. 10.1159/000058331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McMonagle P, Deering F, Berliner Y, Kertesz A. The cognitive profile of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology (2006) 66:331–8. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000196477.78548.db [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Singh TD, Josephs KA, Machulda MM, Drubach DA, Apostolova LG, Lowe VJ, et al. Clinical, FDG and amyloid PET imaging in posterior cortical atrophy. J Neurol (2015) 262:1483–92. 10.1007/s00415-015-7732-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kas A, de Souza LC, Samri D, Bartolomeo P, Lacomblez L, Kalafat M, et al. Neural correlates of cognitive impairment in posterior cortical atrophy. Brain (2011) 134:1464–78. 10.1093/brain/awr055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lehmann M, Barnes J, Ridgway GR, Wattam-Bell J, Warrington EK, Fox NC, et al. Basic visual function and cortical thickness patterns in posterior cortical atrophy. Cereb Cortex (2011) 21:2122–32. 10.1093/cercor/bhq287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lehmann M, Crutch SJ, Ridgway GR, Ridha BH, Barnes J, Warrington EK, et al. Cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in posterior cortical atrophy and typical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging (2011) 32:1466–76. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ruis C, van den Berg E, van Zandvoort MJ, Boshuisen K, Frijns CJ. Ophthalmic impairment or higher-order visual deficit? Posterior cortical atrophy: a case report. Appl Neuropsychol Adult (2012) 19:153–7. 10.1080/09084282.2012.670165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McIntosh RD, Schenk T. Two visual streams for perception and action: current trends. Neuropsychologia (2009) 47:1391–6. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Crutch SJ, Lehmann M, Gorgoraptis N, Kaski D, Ryan N, Husain M, et al. Abnormal visual phenomena in posterior cortical atrophy. Neurocase (2011) 17:160–77. 10.1080/13554794.2010.504729 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Beh SC, Muthusamy B, Calabresi P, Hart J, Zee D, Patel V, et al. Hiding in plain sight: a closer look at posterior cortical atrophy. Pract Neurol (2015) 15:5–13. 10.1136/practneurol-2014-000883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cooper SA, O’Sullivan M. Here, there and everywhere: higher visual function and the dorsal visual stream. Pract Neurol (2016) 16:176–83. 10.1136/practneurol-2015-001168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Brazis PW, Graff-Radford NR, Newman NJ, Lee AG. Ishihara color plates as a test for simultanagnosia. Am J Ophthalmol (1998) 126:850–1. 10.1016/S0002-9394(98)00187-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Luria AR. Disorders of “simultaneous perception” in a case of bilateral occipito-parietal brain injury. Brain (1959) 82:437–49. 10.1093/brain/82.3.437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.von der Heydt R, Peterhans E, Baumgartner G. Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses. Science (1984) 224(4654):1260–2. 10.1126/science.6539501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Barton J, Malcolm G, Hefter R. Spatial processing in Balint syndrome and prosopagnosia: a study of three patients. J Neuroophthalmol (2007) 27:268–74. 10.1097/WNO.0b013e31815b9b85 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Battaglia-Mayer A, Caminiti R. Optic ataxia as a result of the breakdown of the global tuning fields of parietal neurones. Brain (2002) 125:225–37. 10.1093/brain/awf034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cooper SA. Higher visual function: hats, wives and disconnections. Pract Neurol (2012) 12:349–57. 10.1136/practneurol-2011-000153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mendez MF. Posterior cortical atrophy: a visual variant of Alzheimer’s disease. In: Cronin-Golomb A, Hof PR. editors. Vision in Alzheimer Disease. (Vol. 34), Basel: Karger; (2004). p. 112–25. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Giovagnoli AR, Aresi A, Reati F, Riva A, Gobbo C, Bizzi A. The neuropsychological and neuroradiological correlates of slowly progressive visual agnosia. Neurol Sci (2009) 30:123–31. 10.1007/s10072-009-0019-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Migliaccio R, Agosta F, Scola E, Magnani G, Cappa SF, Pagani E, et al. Ventral and dorsal visual streams in posterior cortical atrophy: a DT MRI study. Neurobiol Aging (2012) 33:2572–84. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Crutch SJ, Lehmann M, Schott JM, Rabinovici GD, Rossor MN, Fox NC. Posterior cortical atrophy. Lancet Neurol (2012) 11:170–8. 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70289-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pelak VS, Smyth SF, Boyer PJ, Filley CM. Computerized visual field defects in posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology (2011) 77:2119–22. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823e9f2a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Formaglio M, Krolak-Salmon P, Tilikete C, Bernard M, Croisile B, Vighetto A. [Homonymous hemianopia and posterior cortical atrophy]. Rev Neurol (Paris) (2009) 165:256–62. 10.1016/j.neurol.2008.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Millington RS, James-Galton M, Maia Da Silva MN, Plant GT, Bridge H. Lateralized occipital degeneration in posterior cortical atrophy predicts visual field deficits. Neuroimage Clin (2017) 14:242–9. 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Chan LT, Lynch W, De MM, Horton JC, Miller BL, Rabinovici GD. Prodromal posterior cortical atrophy: clinical, neuropsychological, and radiological correlation. Neurocase (2015) 21:44–55. 10.1080/13554794.2013.860176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wan SL, Bukowska DM, Ford S, Chen FK. Posterior cortical atrophy presenting with superior arcuate field defect. Case Rep Ophthalmol Med (2015) 2015:796381. 10.1155/2015/796381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Cordano C, Alessandria G, Leonardi A, Cellerino M, Nourbakhsh B, Nobili F, et al. Homonymous hemianopia as the first sign of posterior cortical atrophy. J Neurol Sci (2017) 373:38–40. 10.1016/j.jns.2016.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Trick GL, Trick LR, Morris P, Wolf M. Visual field loss in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Neurology (1995) 45:68–74. 10.1212/WNL.45.1.68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cronin-Golomb A, Rizzo JF, Corkin S, Growdon JH. Visual function in Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1991) 640:28–35. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1991.tb00186.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sartucci F, Borghetti D, Bocci T, Murri L, Orsini P, Porciatti V, et al. Dysfunction of the magnocellular stream in Alzheimer’s disease evaluated by pattern electroretinograms and visual evoked potentials. Brain Res Bull (2010) 82:169–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Andrade K, Samri D, Sarazin M, de Souza LC, Cohen L, Thiebaut de SM, et al. Visual neglect in posterior cortical atrophy. BMC Neurol (2010) 10:68. 10.1186/1471-2377-10-68 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Zilli EM, Heilman KM. Allocentric spatial neglect with posterior cortical atrophy. Neurocase (2015) 21:190–7. 10.1080/13554794.2013.878731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Peng G, Wang J, Feng Z, Liu P, Zhang Y, He F, et al. Clinical and neuroimaging differences between posterior cortical atrophy and typical amnestic Alzheimer’s disease patients at an early disease stage. Sci Rep (2016) 6:29372. 10.1038/srep29372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Greene JD. Apraxia, agnosias, and higher visual function abnormalities. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2005) 76(Suppl 5):v25–34. 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M. Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJ. editors. Analysis of Visual Behaviour. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press; (1982). p. 549–86. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Goodale MA, Milner AD. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci (1992) 15:20–5. 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Galton CJ, Patterson K, Xuereb JH, Hodges JR. Atypical and typical presentations of Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical, neuropsychological, neuroimaging and pathological study of 13 cases. Brain (2000) 123(Pt 3):484–98. 10.1093/brain/123.3.484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Crutch SJ, Schott JM, Rabinovici GD, Murray M, Snowden JS, van der Flier WM, et al. Consensus classification of posterior cortical atrophy. Alzheimers Dement (2017):1–15. 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.01.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Whitwell JL, Jack CR, Jr, Kantarci K, Weigand SD, Boeve BF, Knopman DS, et al. Imaging correlates of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurobiol Aging (2007) 28:1051–61. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Migliaccio R, Agosta F, Rascovsky K, Karydas A, Bonasera S, Rabinovici GD, et al. Clinical syndromes associated with posterior atrophy: early age at onset AD spectrum. Neurology (2009) 73:1571–8. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c0d427 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Nestor PJ, Caine D, Fryer TD, Clarke J, Hodges JR. The topography of metabolic deficits in posterior cortical atrophy (the visual variant of Alzheimer’s disease) with FDG-PET. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2003) 74:1521–9. 10.1136/jnnp.74.11.1521 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Schmidtke K, Hull M, Talazko J. Posterior cortical atrophy: variant of Alzheimer’s disease? A case series with PET findings. J Neurol (2005) 252:27–35. 10.1007/s00415-005-0594-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Spehl TS, Hellwig S, Amtage F, Weiller C, Bormann T, Weber WA, et al. Syndrome-specific patterns of regional cerebral glucose metabolism in posterior cortical atrophy in comparison to dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease – a [F-18]-FDG pet study. J Neuroimaging (2015) 25:281–8. 10.1111/jon.12104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wang XD, Lu H, Shi Z, Cai L, Liu S, Liu S, et al. A pilot study on clinical and neuroimaging characteristics of Chinese posterior cortical atrophy: comparison with typical Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One (2015) 10:e0134956. 10.1371/journal.pone.0134956 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Whitwell JL, Graff-Radford J, Singh TD, Drubach DA, Senjem ML, Spychalla AJ, et al. 18F-FDG PET in posterior cortical atrophy and dementia with lewy bodies. J Nucl Med (2017) 58:632–8. 10.2967/jnumed.116.179903 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, Emre M, O’Brien JT, Feldman H, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology (2005) 65:1863–72. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Graff-Radford J, Murray ME, Lowe VJ, Boeve BF, Ferman TJ, Przybelski SA, et al. Dementia with Lewy bodies: basis of cingulate island sign. Neurology (2014) 83:801–9. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Imamura T, Ishii K, Sasaki M, Kitagaki H, Yamaji S, Hirono N, et al. Regional cerebral glucose metabolism in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease: a comparative study using positron emission tomography. Neurosci Lett (1997) 235:49–52. 10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00713-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Tronel C, Largeau B, Santiago Ribeiro MJ, Guilloteau D, Dupont AC, Arlicot N. Molecular targets for PET imaging of activated microglia: the current situation and future expectations. Int J Mol Sci (2017) 18(4):802. 10.3390/ijms18040802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kreisl WC, Lyoo CH, Liow JS, Snow J, Page E, Jenko KJ, et al. Distinct patterns of increased translocator protein in posterior cortical atrophy and amnestic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging (2017) 51:132–40. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.12.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Vivash L, O’Brien TJ. Imaging microglial activation with TSPO PET: lighting up neurologic diseases? J Nucl Med (2016) 57:165–8. 10.2967/jnumed.114.141713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Cecchi M, Moore DK, Sadowsky CH, Solomon PR, Doraiswamy PM, Smith CD, et al. A clinical trial to validate event-related potential markers of Alzheimer’s disease in outpatient settings. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) (2015) 1:387–94. 10.1016/j.dadm.2015.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Stothart G, Kazanina N, Naatanen R, Haworth J, Tales A. Early visual evoked potentials and mismatch negativity in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis (2015) 44:397–408. 10.3233/JAD-140930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Braak H, Braak E. Staging of Alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol Aging (1995) 16:271–8. 10.1016/0197-4580(95)00021-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Quiroz YT, Ally BA, Celone K, McKeever J, Ruiz-Rizzo AL, Lopera F, et al. Event-related potential markers of brain changes in preclinical familial Alzheimer disease. Neurology (2011) 77:469–75. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227b1b0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Muller KI, Bekkelund SI. Visual impairment and posterior cortical atrophy preceding rapid progressive dementia. BMJ Cas Rep (2013) 2013:1–4. 10.1136/bcr-2012-007624 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Mares I, Custodio P, Fonseca J, Bentes C, Guerreiro M, Guimaraes N, et al. To read or not to read: a neurophysiological study. Neurocase (2015) 21:793–801. 10.1080/13554794.2015.1013137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Rosenbloom MH, Alkalay A, Agarwal N, Baker SL, O’Neil JP, Janabi M, et al. Distinct clinical and metabolic deficits in PCA and AD are not related to amyloid distribution. Neurology (2011) 76:1789–96. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821cccad [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Saint-Aubert L, Lemoine L, Chiotis K, Leuzy A, Rodriguez-Vieitez E, Nordberg A. Tau PET imaging: present and future directions. Mol Neurodegener (2017) 12:19. 10.1186/s13024-017-0162-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ossenkoppele R, Schonhaut DR, Scholl M, Lockhart SN, Ayakta N, Baker SL, et al. Tau PET patterns mirror clinical and neuroanatomical variability in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain (2016) 139:1551–67. 10.1093/brain/aww027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Xia C, Makaretz SJ, Caso C, McGinnis S, Gomperts SN, Sepulcre J, et al. Association of in vivo [18F]AV-1451 Tau PET imaging results with cortical atrophy and symptoms in typical and atypical Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol (2017) 74:427–36. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2016.5755 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Day GS, Gordon BA, Jackson K, Christensen JJ, Rosana PM, Su Y, et al. Tau-PET binding distinguishes patients with early-stage posterior cortical atrophy from amnestic Alzheimer disease dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord (2017) 31(2):87–93. 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Janelidze S, Stomrud E, Palmqvist S, Zetterberg H, van WD, Jeromin A, et al. Plasma beta-amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular disease. Sci Rep (2016) 6:26801. 10.1038/srep26801 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Mattsson N, Andreasson U, Zetterberg H, Blennow K, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . Association of plasma neurofilament light with neurodegeneration in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol (2017) 74:557–66. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2016.6117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Laforce R, Jr, Tosun D, Ghosh P, Lehmann M, Madison CM, Weiner MW, et al. Parallel ICA of FDG-PET and PiB-PET in three conditions with underlying Alzheimer’s pathology. Neuroimage Clin (2014) 4:508–16. 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Whitwell JL, Jack CR, Jr, Kantarci K, Weigand SD, Boeve BF, Knopman DS, et al. Imaging correlates of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurobiol Aging (2007) 28:1051–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Andrade K, Kas A, Valabregue R, Samri D, Sarazin M, Habert MO, et al. Visuospatial deficits in posterior cortical atrophy: structural and functional correlates. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2012) 83:860–3. 10.1136/jnnp-2012-302278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, Kertesz A, Mendez M, Cappa SF, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology (2011) 76:1006–14. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821103e6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim AS. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci (2009) 21:489–510. 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Fransson P, Marrelec G. The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. Neuroimage (2008) 42:1178–84. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Lehmann M, Ghosh PM, Madison C, Laforce R, Jr, Corbetta-Rastelli C, Weiner MW, et al. Diverging patterns of amyloid deposition and hypometabolism in clinical variants of probable Alzheimer’s disease. Brain (2013) 136:844–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Serrano-Pozo A, Frosch MP, Masliah E, Hyman BT. Neuropathological alterations in Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med (2011) 1:a006189. 10.1101/cshperspect.a006189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Imperiale F, Agosta F, Canu E, Mandic-Stojmenovic G, Stojkovic T, Stefanova E, et al. Functional and structural brain alterations in patients with posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology (2016) 86(16):189. [Google Scholar]
- 88.Sitek EJ, Narozanska E, Peplonska B, Filipek S, Barczak A, Styczynska M, et al. A patient with posterior cortical atrophy possesses a novel mutation in the presenilin 1 gene. PLoS One (2013) 8:e61074. 10.1371/journal.pone.0061074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Saint-Aubert L, Payoux P, Hannequin D, Barbeau EJ, Campion D, Delisle MB, et al. MR, (18)F-FDG, and (18)F-AV45 PET correlate with AD PSEN1 original phenotype. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord (2013) 27:91–4. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318251d87c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Tremolizzo L, Susani E, Mapelli C, Isella V, Bertola F, Ferrarese C, et al. First report of PSEN2 mutation presenting as posterior cortical atrophy. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord (2015) 29:249–51. 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Rossi G, Bastone A, Piccoli E, Morbin M, Mazzoleni G, Fugnanesi V, et al. Different mutations at V363 MAPT codon are associated with atypical clinical phenotypes and show unusual structural and functional features. Neurobiol Aging (2014) 35:408–17. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Depaz R, Haik S, Peoc’h K, Seilhean D, Grabli D, Vicart S, et al. Long-standing prion dementia manifesting as posterior cortical atrophy. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord (2012) 26:289–92. 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318231e449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Carrasquillo MM, Khan Q, Murray ME, Krishnan S, Aakre J, Pankratz VS, et al. Late-onset Alzheimer disease genetic variants in posterior cortical atrophy and posterior AD. Neurology (2014) 82:1455–62. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.van der Flier WM, Schoonenboom SN, Pijnenburg YA, Fox NC, Scheltens P. The effect of APOE genotype on clinical phenotype in Alzheimer disease. Neurology (2006) 67:526–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Schott JM, Crutch SJ, Carrasquillo MM, Uphill J, Shakespeare TJ, Ryan NS, et al. Genetic risk factors for the posterior cortical atrophy variant of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement (2016) 12:862–71. 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.01.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]