Abstract
Objective
This research was aimed to evaluate the behaviors of short‐ or long‐term antidepressant effects of ketamine in rats exposed to chronic unpredictable stress (CUS).
Background
Ketamine, a glutamate noncompetitive NMDA receptor antagonist, regulates excitatory amino acid functions, such as anxiety disorders and major depression, and plays an important role in synaptic plasticity and learning and memory.
Methods
After 42 days of CUS model, male rats received either a single injection of ketamine (10 mg/kg; day 43) or 15 daily injections (days 43–75). The influence of ketamine on behavioral reactivity was assessed 24 hr (short‐term) or 7 weeks after ketamine treatment (long‐term). Behavioral tests used to assess the effects of these treatments included the sucrose preference (SP), open field (OF), elevated plus maze (EPM), forced swimming (FS), and water maze (WM) to detect anxiety‐like behavior (OF and EPM), forced swimming (FS), and water maze (WM).
Results: Short‐term ketamine administration resulted in increases of body weight gain, higher sensitivity to sucrose, augmented locomotor activity in the OF, more entries into the open arms of the EPM, along increased activity in the FS test; all responses indicative of reductions in depression/despair in anxiety‐eliciting situations. No significant differences in these behaviors were obtained under conditions of long‐term ketamine administration (p > .05). The CUS + Ketamine group showed significantly increased activity as compared with the CUS + Vehicle group for analysis of the long‐term effects of ketamine (*p < .05). Nor were significant differences obtained in learning and memory performance in rats receiving ketamine (p > .05).
Conclusion
Taken together these findings demonstrate that a short‐term administration of ketamine induced rapid antidepressant‐like effects in adult male rats exposed to CUS conditions, effects that were not observed in response to the long‐term treatment regime.
Keywords: antidepressant, behavioral test, chronic unpredictable stress, depression, ketamine
1. INTRODUCTION
Depression is the most common mental disorder in community settings and a major cause of disability worldwide. It includes a wide range of mental health problems manifested by the absence of a positive affect (a loss of interest and enjoyment in ordinary events and experiences), despondent mood and a number of associated emotional, cognitive, physical, and behavioral symptoms (Health, 2010; Krishnan & Nestler, 2008). It has been reported that the prevalence of depression in most countries ranges from 8 to 12%, which results in enormous personal suffering, as well as substantial social and economic burdens (Kessler et al., 2003; Pincus & Pettit, 2001). The world health organization estimates that by 2020 depression is projected to become the second most common cause of disability‐adjusted life years worldwide (Murray & Lopez, 1997).
Normally, antidepressants such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and selective norepinephrine inhibitors (SNRIs) are used in the treatment of this disorder (Ahuja, 2008). It is generally believed that the increase in neuronal monoamine neurotransmitter transport that occurs in response to these agents can improve the adaptability of neurons in the limbic brain regions that control mood and depression, to achieve their therapeutic effect (Warner‐Schmidt et al., 2010). However, these typically prescribed antidepressants have significant limitations, including low effective rates upon initial use (effective rates to the first treatment account for only one‐third, and effective rates with multiple trials accounts for only up to two‐thirds of treated patients), substantial latencies for a therapeutic response (weeks to months) and serious side effect (such as decreased libido and weight increases) (Trivedi et al., 2006). Such shortcomings are particularly problematic for an illness that is associated with high rates of suicide, further highlighting a major unmet need for the development of novel, rapid‐acting, and more efficacious antidepressant agents.
Several lines of evidence suggest that dysfunction of the glutamate neurotransmitter system is also associated with the pathophysiology of mood disorders, such as major depressive disorder (MDD) and bipolar disorder (BP) (Hashimoto, 2009, 2011; Sanacora, Zarate, Krystal, & Manji, 2008; Tokita, Yamaji, & Hashimoto, 2012; Zarate et al., 2010). Results from postmortem studies reveal that glutamate levels are reduced in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in MDD, which indicates that glutamate neurotransmission in MDD patient shows abnormal functioning with regard to both pathophysiology and glutamate receptor expression (Zhao et al., 2012). Ketamine, a noncompetitive glutamate NMDA receptor antagonist, regulates excitatory amino acid function and plays an important role in synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory. It has been reported that subanesthetic doses of ketamine (10 mg/kg) reduce depression symptoms in rats subjected to the depression model of learned helplessness (Beurel, Song, & Jope, 2011). Moreover, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled randomized clinical trials have also demonstrated the efficacy of a single dose of ketamine in treating patients with refractory MDD and BP (Berman et al., 2000; Diazgranados et al., 2010; Zarate et al., 2006). The rapid onset of ketamine action makes it a highly attractive drug for patients with mood disorders, although the mechanistic action is unknown (Mathews, Henter, & Zarate, 2012). Discovery of the rapid antidepressant actions of ketamine, which acts by a mechanism completely different from typical monoamine reuptake inhibitors, represents a major advance in the field of depression. However, as the widespread use of ketamine is limited by the potential for toxicity and abuse, studies are being conducted in animal models to elucidate the mechanisms underlying the actions of ketamine as an approach to develop safe, rapidly acting agents.
In an attempt to further understand some of the parameters of ketamine's effects as an antidepressant, the purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of ketamine in the rat model of chronic unpredictable mild stress (CUS). This model is widely used as a preclinical animal model of depression (Cryan & Holmes, 2005; Nestler et al., 2002). The specific goals of this study were to examine whether ketamine could improve depression‐like behaviors seen in rats subjected to CUS, and to determine the rapidity and duration of these antidepressant effects of ketamine.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Animals
Male Sprague–Dawley (SD) rats (250–350 g) obtained from the animal center at WeiFang Medical University were used for this study. The age of the rats at the start of experiment was day 35–49, which roughly approximates adolescence in humans (Andersen & Navalta, 2004; Spear, 2000). Procedures involving animal use were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guidelines (Use of Laboratory Animals) and were approved by the WeiFang Medical University Animal Care and Use Committees. The rats were allowed a 1‐week period of acclimatization to their housing conditions before commencing stress exposure. During the acclimatization period food and water were provided adlibitum. Rats were housed in temperature (23 ± 2°C) and humidity (50 ± 5%) controlled rooms and maintained on a 12 hr/12 hr light/dark cycle.
2.2. Chronic unpredictable stress paradigm
To establish the chronic mild unpredictable stress animal model, 10 different stressors, with two stressors per day were administered. The stressors used were as reported previously (Li et al., 2011; Spear, 2000) and included: lights off for 3 hr (10 am to 1 pm), lights on overnight, strobe light overnight, 45° tilted cages, food and water deprivation, crowded housing, isolation housing, tail clip, wet cage, and noise. The stressors were administered over a 42‐day period. Throughout the duration of the experiment, all rats remained healthy with no obvious indications of sickness and no deaths.
2.3. Drug specification and experimental design
After establishing the CUS model, rats received ketamine (10 mg/kg, ip.) once daily for either 1 day or 15 consecutive days. The influence of ketamine on behavioral reactivity was then assessed at either 1 day (short‐term) or at 7 weeks (long‐term) after the onset of the first ketamine administration. This subanesthetic dose of ketamine was chosen as it is regarded to represent a recreational dose for use in rodents with a LD50 of 600 mg/kg at 4 hr and was consistent with dosages reported in the literature (Enomoto & Floresco, 2009; Quirk, Sosulski, Feierstein, Uchida, & Mainen, 2009; Sun et al., 2011). A placebo group received injections of physiological saline (0.9% sodium chloride, ip), and an untreated (normal) control group received no treatments during the 15 days of ketamine/saline injection. So 28 rats were randomly assigned into three groups as follows: “CUS + Ketamine,” “CUS + Vehicle,” and “Control” group.
2.4. Behavioral tests
Rats were subjected to two behavioral assays on the final day of ketamine or saline administration to verify the presence of stress in this CUS model, the Sucrose Preference Test (SPT) and the Water Maze Test (WMT) (see Figure 1 for experimental groups/testing sequence). These behavioral tests were conducted at 4 hours after injection, as the metabolites of ketamine are cleared by urinary excretion by 4 hours after injection (Sun et al., 2011). The influence of ketamine on the five behavioral tests described below was assessed at 24 hr (short‐term) and at 7 weeks (long‐term) after ketamine treatment. Behavioral assessments were recorded using a camera (SMART, Panlab SL, and Barcelona, Spain) and were scored by two raters who were blind as to the treatment condition of the groups. No statistically significant differences were obtained between scores of the raters (inter‐rater reliability = 0.91). All tests were performed between 800 and 1700 hr.
2.5. Sucrose preference test
Sucrose preference is considered to provide an index of anhedonia (Shi et al., 2012; Warner‐Schmidt & Duman, 2008). The rats were trained to adapt to a 1% sucrose solution (w/v) for 48 hr at the beginning of the experiment. For this two‐bottle choice test (Bolanos, Barrot, Berton, Wallace‐Black, & Nestler, 2003), one bottle was filled with 1% sucrose solution and the other filled with water and both were then placed in the cage as described previously (Suo et al., 2013). The positions of water and sucrose bottles were altered during the training sessions. For the SPT, rats were deprived of water for 21 hr and placed individually in a cage containing two identical bottles for a 3‐hr period. The amount of sucrose and water consumed (mL) was recorded. A sucrose preference percent (%) was calculated by measuring sugar water intake divided by the sum of sugar water intake and water intake.
2.6. Elevated plus maze test
The elevated plus maze (EPM) is based on a rat's natural fear of open, unprotected and elevated spaces (Suo et al., 2013). Each rat was first placed in the central zone of the EPM with its head oriented to the closed arm. The rat was allowed to freely explore the maze for 5 min as described previously (Franceschelli, Sens, Herchick, Thelen, & Pitychoutis, 2015; Suo et al., 2013) and their behavior was recorded using a video camera placed above the EPM. The EPM was cleaned with 70% ethanol between tests to prevent interference resulting from any residual odors of the previous rat. The number of entries into and time spent in the open and closed arms were recorded. A retention time (time spent in the open arm) and the ratio number of entries into the open arm were calculated by Smart electronic equipment.
2.7. Open field test
On days 56 and 113 of the experiment (15 and 57 days after their final ketamine injection), selected rats from each group were evaluated in the open field test during the dark cycle of their photoperiod (1300‐1700 hr) (Walsh & Cummins, 1976). This apparatus, which has been described previously (Ma et al., 2013, 2011), consisted of a square box with dimensions, 80 × 80 × 50 cm, and the field was divided into 25 squares with computer virtual grid lines for analysis using the Smart3.0 software. The rat's behavior was recorded within 5 min. For each trial, rats were allowed to activity for 5 min. At the start of the test, the rat was placed in the corner of the open field. The numbers of grids crossed (all four limbs), up‐right postures and carding, fecal grains and entrances into the central zone of the apparatus were all recorded in this test. The apparatus was cleaned with 70% ethanol between each test to eliminate any residual olfactory cues of the previously tested rat.
2.8. Forced swimming test
The FST was performed using methods previously described by Sadeghi, Peeri, & Hosseini, (2016). The equipment for this test consisted of a glass barrel (height = 46 cm, diameter = 20 cm) filled with 36 cm of water and maintained at 23 ± 1°C. Prior to testing, the rats were placed in the water for a 15‐min period to adapt to the apparatus. Twenty‐four hours later, they were again placed in the cylinders for the 5 min FST. The latency of immobility, as defined by the time at which the rat was no longer engaged in actively swimming, was then recorded. After testing, rats were removed, placed in a normal heat preservation breeding cage with padding, and covered with an absorbent towel.
2.9. Water maze test
The WMT is used to measure spatial navigation learning and memory in rats.(Sterneck et al., 1998). All groups of rats were trained for 3 days, and the WMT was performed as described previously (Sun et al., 2011). A black circular tank (150 cm in diameter) was filled to a depth of 25 cm with water and maintained at 23 ± 1°C. For each trial, rats were allowed to search for the hidden platform for a 90 s period. The platform was hidden 1 cm below the water surface in the center of one quadrant of the pool. If a rat did not locate the platform after 90 s, it would be guided to the platform and allowed to remain on the platform for 20 s to recognize the location. The rats received three such consecutive trials each day with an intertrial interval of 30 s. Four prominent signs (square, heart‐shape, moon and triangle) were placed around the tank at four fixed points, with one in each quadrant. The water was changed each day's experiment. The escape latency time for the rat to locate and climb onto the platform was recorded.
2.10. Statistical analyses
Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 18.0 and Graphpad Prism 5. All data were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or median (10th to 90th percentile) for the groups. Behavioral data were analyzed using a mixed‐design analyses of variance (ANOVAS) followed by Fisher least significant difference (LSD) post hoc test. Results were considered statistically significant when p < .05.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Effects of ketamine on body weight gain
Reductions in body weight can be used as an index of physiological responses indicative of chronic stress. In our experiment, CUS + Vehicle rats gained weight at a lower rate than the control group, with the result being that body weights of rats in the CUS + Vehicle group on day 42 were significantly decreased (*p < .001; Figure 2). While rats treated with ketamine showed significantly reduced weight gains as compared with normal controls (# p < .05), their weight gains were significantly increased as compared with CUS + Vehicle rats not receiving ketamine (# p < .05) throughout the duration of the experiment. The differences between these two latter groups were enhanced as a function of the duration of the experiment – day 70 (*p < .001), day 84 (*p < .002), and day 98 (*p < .001).
3.2. Effects of ketamine's on reward‐related behavior
The sucrose preference test provides an index of stress‐induced anhedonia (Figure 3c). Exposure of rats to the 42‐day stress regime was successful in eliciting an anhedonia‐like condition as sucrose consumption in stressed rats was significantly lower than that of the untreated control group (F (2, 27) = 16.572, *p < .001: CUS + Ketamine group versus Control group, *p < .001 and CUS + Vehicle group versus Control group, *p < .001; Figure 3a). In the short‐term, ketamine treatment significantly increases the percent of sucrose intake as compared with the CUS + Vehicle group (*p < .036). Although sucrose intake of the ketamine group was less than that of the CUS + Vehicle group, no statistically significant was found.
3.3. Effects of ketamine on anxiety‐like behavior
The OF and EPM tests were used to assess anxiety‐like behavior in rats. In the OF, reductions in carding and crossing numbers indicate higher levels of anxiety/depression. The results of the OF test in response to the short‐term ketamine treatment indicated that carding and fecal grains in CUS + Ketamine group were significantly increased as compared to CUS + Vehicle group, but the scores for crossing and up‐right posture were not statistically significant (carding: F(2, 27) = 4.28, *p < .025;fecal grains: F(2, 27) = 3.89, *p < .034). Analysis of results for long‐term ketamine administration indicated no overall statistically significant differences among the three groups in the OF test (Tables 1, 2).
Table 1.
Control | CUS + Vehicle | CUS + Ketamine | F | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Crossing | 50.30 ± 16.30 | 34.50 ± 19.49 | 37.50 ± 16.68 | 2.26 | .125 |
Up‐right | 17.90 ± 6.92 | 15.13 ± 4.85 | 14.60 ± 7.03 | 0.74 | .487 |
Carding | 6.10 ± 2.88 | 2.88 ± 1.89 | 3.20 ± 2.90 | 4.28 | .025a |
Fecal grains | 3.80 ± 3.82 | 0.01 ± 0.01 | 4.50 ± 4.62 | 3.89 | .034a |
p < .05 exposed to repeated ANOVA was significantly different.
Table 2.
Control | CUS + Vehicle | CUS + Ketamine | F | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Crossing | 44.70 ± 16.99 | 36.88 ± 27.72 | 39.44 ± 26.95 | 0.25 | .778 |
Up‐right | 16.00 ± 6.50 | 13.75 ± 8.80 | 14.00 ± 8.05 | 0.24 | .790 |
Carding | 3.40 ± 2.50 | 2.62 ± 2.26 | 2.33 ± 1.41 | 0.63 | .535 |
Fecal grains | 2.60 ± 2.84 | 1.25 ± 2.38 | 2.22 ± 3.07 | 0.54 | .591 |
Values are means ± standard deviations.
For the EPM, the CUS + Ketamine group showed significantly higher ratios of entries into the open arm (F(2, 23) = 6.530, *p < .006) and longer durations within the open arms (F(2, 23) = 6.250, *p < .007) as compared with the CUS + Vehicle group in the short‐term condition (Figure 4a, c). However, results between the CUS + Ketamine and CUS + Vehicle groups were not significantly different on day 115 (Figure 4b, d). CUS + Vehicle rats receiving, short‐ or long‐term ketamine treatment spent significantly less time in the open arms of the EPM as compared with the Control group (short‐term: *p < .005; long‐term: *p < .017). The CUS + Ketamine group showed no significantly of entries into the open arm. (Figure 4b, d).
3.4. Effects of ketamine on behavioral despair
Ketamine affects responses observed in the FS test, with decreases in immobility (increased activity) being indicative of reductions in despair. While analysis of the short‐term effects of ketamine in the FS test indicated that these CUS + Ketamine rats were more active (decreased immobility) as compared to the other two groups, but the results were not statistically significant. For analysis of the long‐term effects of ketamine, the CUS + Ketamine group showed significantly increased activity as compared with the CUS + Vehicle group (*p < .001), and the CUS + Vehicle group also decreased activity as with the Control group (*p < .005) (F(2, 23) = 9.261, *p < .001; Figure 5b).
3.5. Effects of ketamine on learning and memory
As memory can also be affected with depression, we assessed memory function in these rats with use of the Water Maze Test. When tested in the water maze test, reductions in escape latencies and distance traveled to the platform provide an index of memory function. The escape latency time(s) for the rats to locate and climb onto the platform showed no statistically significant differences, although the CUS + Vehicle group rats spent more time to find the platform. Further analysis (LSD) showed that escape latencies of the CUS + Ketamine groups were increased in comparison to CUS + Vehicle group in the last day (CUS + Ketamine: 61.58 ± 23.51, CUS + Vehicle: 44.48 ± 21.17), but the result was not statistically significant. Moreover, the distance traveled indicated the differences in space exploration between short‐ and long‐term administration of ketamine (Figure 6a1‐3, b1‐3).
4. DISCUSSION
Several lines of evidence have suggested that ketamine rapidly enhances the structure and function of cortical synapses known to play a role in mood. It has been proposed that the antidepressant effects of ketamine involve a rapid activation of the mammalian rapamycin pathway, including increases in extracellular signals regulating kinase, protein kinase B, and brain‐derived neurotrophic factor in the hippocampus and producing increases in the number of new spines in the prefrontal cortex (Autry et al., 2011). Importantly, age of the subject and ketamine dose have proved to be significant factors in producing these antidepression effects. Subanesthetic doses of ketamine are thought to enhance glutamatergic signaling and dopamine release in the prefrontal cortex by reducing excitatory input upon gamma amino butyric acid neurons, subsequently leading to hyperactivity of corticolimbic pathways (Li et al., 2010; Moghaddam, Adams, Verma, & Daly, 1997). As adolescents have higher concentrations of NMDA receptors and typically metabolize drugs more rapidly than adults (Hein, 1987), it seems possible that the subanesthetic doses of ketamine used in the adolescent rats of our experiment involved similar mechanisms to produce the increased levels of exploratory behavior observed.
Significant differences in weight gains were obtained between CUS + Vehicle exposed rats and the Control group (Figure 6). Stressful situations are known to influence feeding behavior as it has been reported that chronic exposure to stressors alters body weights in rats (Bekris, Antoniou, Daskas, & Papadopoulou‐Daifoti, 2005; Dess, Raizer, Chapman, & Garcia, 1988). During CUS model, the hypothalamus‐pituitary‐adrenal (HPA) axis and glucocorticoids (GC) remain elevated, and the main adrenal cortical hormone in rats is corticosterone (mineralocorticoid) (Cryan, Mombereau, & Vassout, 2005). Cortisone injections enhance the body's sensitivity to insulin sensitivity to reduce food intake, leading to the weight loss (Nilsson et al., 2002). This result was consistent with our report indicating that CUS induced growth retardation, and is supported by related findings which show reduced body weights and caloric intakes in depressed animals and humans (Iniguez, Warren, & Bolanos‐Guzman, 2010; Simansky & Eberle‐Wang, 1993). In our study, the ketamine‐treated group showed significantly elevated body weights as compared with the CUS + Vehicle group throughout days 42 to112 of the experiment. From days 84 to 112, the decline in body weights within the CUS + Vehicle group was particularly significant. As described above, the central nervous system plays an important role in the control of appetite and maintenance of body weight (Coppari, Ramadori, & Elmquist, 2009). Accordingly, the effects of ketamine observed within our study likely reflect the effects exerted upon hormonal and neuronal signals regulating appetite and body weight control in young animals (Sun et al., 2011). The rapid recovery of body weight in stressed rats treated with ketamine suggests that NMDA receptors are involved in modulating feeding behavior during stressful situations. Interestingly, in adult humans and rats, chronic ketamine exposure disrupts appetite and weight gain (Cvrcek, 2008; Parise et al., 2013). Further studies will be necessary to elucidate the possible effects of ketamine when administered at different ages and using different doses/times of injection as well as studies directed at examining the mechanisms of action.
In rodents, the CUS paradigms produces anhedonia, the loss of interest in normally pleasurable and rewarding activities, which are a core symptom of depression (Ma et al., 2013; Willner, Muscat, & Papp, 1992). As based upon the results of our sucrose preference test, the CUS paradigm used in this report induced anhedonia, as rats exposed to chronic stress consumed significantly lower amounts of sucrose (Figure 3a). However, in response to ketamine, CUS rats showed increases in preferences for sucrose, indicating that they were now more responsive to the rewarding effects of sucrose. Anhedonia might provide insights into the underlying neurobiology of depression and changes in depression following treatments with antidepressants. With the use of anhedonia, it will be possible to identify areas and mechanisms associated with the sensitization of brain reward pathways (Subhan, Deslandes, Pache, & Sewell, 2000), resulting from enhanced firing activity dopamine neurons within the ventral tegmental area (Sekine, Suzuki, Ramachandran, Blackburn, & Ashby, 2007) and dopamine neurotransmission in the striatum (Stein, 2008). Interestingly, antidepressants alleviate (Katz & Carroll, 1977) or have no effects (Matthews et al., 1996) on responding for rewarding brain stimulation. However, it is important to note that NMDA receptor antagonists, such as AP‐1 and MK801, increase the total amount of food consumed in rodents (Treece, Ritter, & Burns, 2000).
Under certain conditions, behavioral activity can be used as an index of anxiety (Kovacs & de Wied, 1978). Our results showed that exposure to CUS produced a decrease in rats’ exploratory activity, an effect which was altered by short‐term ketamine treatment. This increase in exploratory activity can be interpreted as a reduction in the degree of anxiety in these ketamine‐treated rats. The work suggested the behavior of up‐right was on behalf of the rat's ability for adapting a strange environment (Casu et al., 2005). In contrast to our findings, the work of Marin, Cruz, & Planeta, (2007), revealed that animals exposed to chronic restraint stress displayed higher exploratory behavior in the open field test. It is possible that the categories, intensities and number of exposures to the stressors were important factors in determining how chronic stress affected the exploratory activity in response to a novel environment.
Anxiety‐like behavior, locomotor activity and risk assessment were evaluated with use of the EPM (Myers & Greenwood‐Van Meerveld, 2010). CUS rats showed increased amounts of anxiety‐like behavior, which was characterized by spending less time in the open arms and decreased locomotor activity in the EPM. In contrast, ketamine‐treated rats showed behavioral responses indicative of antianxiety‐like behavior. Taking these results obtained from both the OF and EPM tests, it seems that ketamine might be operating as anantidepressant drug.
In the FS test, rats adopt an immobile posture after an initial period of vigorous activity of swimming in a beaker from which they cannot escape, and this immobility has been interpreted as reflecting despair/depression (Aan het Rot et al., 2010; Murrough et al., 2013). Ketamine‐treated rats demonstrated lower levels of despair when subjected to the FS test, as indicated by showing increased immobility latencies as compared with that of the CUS + Vehicle group following the long‐term treatment with ketamine. A related study failed to find any such antidepressant effects of ketamine (Hayase, Yamamoto, & Yamamoto, 2006). However, in that study, rats were forced to swim for an extended period of time, which may have reduced the influence of ketamine on the responses of these rats (Parise et al., 2013).
Collating the findings from the array of behavioral assays used in this report, we demonstrate that ketamine yielded rapid antidepressant‐like effects in rats exposed to CUS conditions, effects which were greater that observed with long‐term treatment. Parise et al. revealed that ketamine produced rapid antidepressant responses in rodents, however, these antidepressant or anxiolytic effects were not sustained (Parise et al., 2013) and findings from other studies have indicated that the antidepressant responses to ketamine only lasted for a few days (Autry et al., 2011; Maeng et al., 2008). There is also evidence from clinical reports that acute ketamine injection yielded rapid antidepressant and anxiolytic effects (Aan Het Rot, Zarate, Charney, & Mathew, 2012; Sappington, Corssen, Becker, & Tavakoli, 1977), however, these antidepressant and anxiolytic symptoms relapsed within days (Aan Het Rot et al., 2012; Kindler et al., 2013). While, a study by Murrough JW has reported that repeated ketamine administration was being explored as a treatment paradigm for the long‐lasting maintenance of antidepressant response (Aan het Rot et al., 2010; Murrough et al., 2013). For analysis of the long‐term effects of ketamine, the CUS + Ketamine group showed significantly increased activity as compared with the CUS + Vehicle group. Thus, our studies offered a new strategy consisting of a 14‐day ketamine administration that ameliorates CUS. The repeated ketamine administration might lead to robust, perhaps permanent, changes in cortical synapses for keeping longer lasting antidepressant effects (Parise et al., 2013). Meanwhile, we cannot discard the idea that repeated ketamine administration caused PV‐loss in the PFC (Yang, Han, Zhang, Ren, & Hashimoto, 2016), suggesting a detrimental side effects of repeated ketamine injections which might lead to the effectless of the antidepressant response to the long‐term treatments. And the potential metabolic side effects that may result from these treatments, especially on the dose and repeated injection time and frequency will require future research.
A single training session with use of the WM test was used to differentiate the drug's effects on different stages of memory. While the conventional protocols of WM test involved repetitive multitraining sessions in several days, such a protocol makes it difficult to differentiate the drug's effects on different memory aspects, as suggested by Moosavi, Yadollahi Khales, Rastegar, & Zarifkar, (2012). Results obtained from previous studies had indicated that ketamine impaired memory and learning behavior as the rats treated with ketamine spent more time to reach the hidden platform (Walker & Gold, 1991). Moreover, subanesthetic posttraining doses of ketamine (5 mg/kg) showed no effects on memory consolidation and larger doses (10, 20, and 50 mg/kg) did not influence the retrieval of memory when tested in a T‐maze (Wang, Fu, Wilson, & Ma, 2006). However, Getova and Doncheva reported that a subanesthetic administration of ketamine actually improved learning and memory as revealed in an active avoidance test (Getova & Doncheva, 2011). Our current results suggest that a subanesthetic dose of either short‐ or long‐term ketamine exerted no significant effects upon learning and working memory as assessed in the WM test (Figure 6). In fact, the escape latencies of the CUS + Ketamine group were somewhat longer than that obtained in the CUS + Vehicle group. Given that ketamine is a noncompetitive antagonist of the N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate (NMDA) receptor, which is considered to be one of the vitally important receptors involved with synaptic plasticity and neuronal learning (Rezvani, 2006), these findings are expected. It seems likely that these impairments in working memory by ketamine were not attributable to the dysfunction of motivational, motor, short‐term memory or spatial memory processes (Storer & Demeyere, 2014). While the water maze is generally considered to be a test of spatial learning and working memory in rodents (Brody & Holtzman, 2006), performance in this task could also be influenced by a number of other factors.
In summary, our study utilized a depression model in rats for use in evaluating the antidepressant effectiveness of short‐term and long‐term ketamine administration. We demonstrate that a 14‐day ketamine treatment not only induced a rapid onset, but also a persistent long‐term antidepressant effect in rats. The mechanisms underlying this effect, as well as any potential metabolic side effects that may result from these treatments, will require future research.
5. CONCLUSION
We conclude from this study that the use of a subanesthetic dose of ketamine to CUS exposed rats’ results in greater increases of weight gain, higher sensitivity to sucrose, augmented spontaneous locomotor behavior to anxiety‐eliciting situations, and some deficits in memory. Ketamine induced rapid antidepressant‐like effects in adult male rats exposed to CUS conditions; however, these antidepressant‐like effects were not present under conditions of long‐term ketamine treatment.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
None declared.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by the Science and Technology Innovation Fund of Weifang Medical University K1301011, the Research Award Foundation Program of Outstanding Young Scientist of Shandong Province BS2014YY043, and education teaching reform and research foundation of Weifang Medical University 2015Y024. The content of article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agency and institutes.
Jiang Y, Wang Y, Sun X, et al. Short‐ and long‐term antidepressant effects of ketamine in a rat chronic unpredictable stress model. Brain Behav. 2017;7:e00749 https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.749
REFERENCES
- Aan het Rot, M. , Collins, K. A. , Murrough, J. W. , Perez, A. M. , Reich, D. L. , Charney, D. S. , & Mathew, S. J. (2010). Safety and efficacy of repeated‐dose intravenous ketamine for treatment‐resistant depression. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 139–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aan Het Rot, M. , Zarate, C. A. Jr. , Charney, D. S. , & Mathew, S. J. (2012). Ketamine for depression: Where do we go from here? Biological Psychiatry, 72, 537–547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahuja, N. (2008). A short textbook of Psychiatry (PP374–389) vol.7 New Delhi: Jaypee. [Google Scholar]
- Andersen, S. L. , & Navalta, C. P. (2004). Altering the course of neurodevelopment: A framework for understanding the enduring effects of psychotropic drugs. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 22(5–6), 423–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Autry, A. E. , Adachi, M. , Nosyreva, E. , Na, E. S. , Los, M. F. , Cheng, P. F. , … Monteggia, L. M. (2011). NMDA receptor blockade at rest triggers rapid behavioural antidepressant responses. Nature, 475, 91–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bekris, S. , Antoniou, K. , Daskas, S. , & Papadopoulou‐Daifoti, Z. (2005). Behavioural and neurochemical effects induced by chronic mild stress applied to two different rat strains. Behavioral Brain Research, 161, 45–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berman, R. M. , Cappiello, A. , Anand, A. , Oren, D. A. , Heninger, G. R. , Charney, D. S. , & Krystal, J. H. (2000). Antidepressant effects of ketamine in depressed patients. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 351–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beurel, E. , Song, L. , & Jope, R. S. (2011). Inhibition of glycogen synthase kinase‐3 is necessary for the rapid antidepressant effect of ketamine in mice. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 1068–1070.21502951 [Google Scholar]
- Bolanos, C. A. , Barrot, M. , Berton, O. , Wallace‐Black, D. , & Nestler, E. J. (2003). Methylphenidate treatment during pre‐ and periadolescence alters behavioral responses to emotional stimuli at adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1317–1329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody, D. L. , & Holtzman, D. M. (2006). Morris water maze search strategy analysis in PDAPP mice before and after experimental traumatic brain injury. Experimental Neurology, 197, 330–340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casu, M. A. , Pisu, C. , Sanna, A. , Tambaro, S. , Spada, G. P. , Mongeau, R. , & Pani, L. (2005). Effect of delta9‐tetrahydrocannabinol on phosphorylated CREB in rat cerebellum: An immunohistochemical study. Brain Research, 1048(1–2), 41–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coppari, R. , Ramadori, G. , & Elmquist, J. K. (2009). The role of transcriptional regulators in central control of appetite and body weight. Nature Clinical Practice Endocrinology & Metabolism, 5, 160–166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cryan, J. F. , & Holmes, A. (2005). The ascent of mouse: Advances in modelling human depression and anxiety. Nature Reviews. Drug Discovery, 4, 775–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cryan, J. F. , Mombereau, C. , & Vassout, A. (2005). The tail suspension test as a model for assessing antidepressant activity: Review of pharmacological and genetic studies in mice. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(4–5), 571–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cvrcek, P. (2008). Side effects of ketamine in the long‐term treatment of neuropathic pain. Pain Medicine, 9, 253–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dess, N. K. , Raizer, J. , Chapman, C. D. , & Garcia, J. (1988). Stressors in the learned helplessness paradigm: Effects on body weight and conditioned taste aversion in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 44(4–5), 483–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diazgranados, N. , Ibrahim, L. , Brutsche, N. E. , Newberg, A. , Kronstein, P. , Khalife, S. , … Zarate, C. A. Jr (2010). A randomized add‐on trial of an N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate antagonist in treatment‐resistant bipolar depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 793–802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enomoto, T. , & Floresco, S. B. (2009). Disruptions in spatial working memory, but not short‐term memory, induced by repeated ketamine exposure. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 33(4), 668–675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschelli, A. , Sens, J. , Herchick, S. , Thelen, C. , & Pitychoutis, P. M. (2015). Sex differences in the rapid and the sustained antidepressant‐like effects of ketamine in stress‐naïve and “depressed” mice exposed to chronic mild stress. Neuroscience, 2), 49–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Getova, D. P. , & Doncheva, N. D. (2011). Effects of ketamine on memory and nociception in rats. Folia Medica, 53, 53–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashimoto, K. (2009). Emerging role of glutamate in the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder. Brain Research Reviews, 61, 105–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashimoto, K. (2011). The role of glutamate on the action of antidepressants. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35, 1558–1568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayase, T. , Yamamoto, Y. , & Yamamoto, K. (2006). Behavioral effects of ketamine and toxic interactions with psychostimulants. BMC Neuroscience, 7, 25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Health, N. C. C. F. M. (2010). Depression: the treatment and management of depression in adults, Updated ed. Leicester (UK): British Psychological Society. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hein, K. (1987). The use of therapeutics in adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 8, 8–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iniguez, S. D. , Warren, B. L. , & Bolanos‐Guzman, C. A. (2010). Short‐ and long‐term functional consequences of fluoxetine exposure during adolescence in male rats. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 1057–1066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz, R. J. , & Carroll, B. J. (1977). Intracranial reward after Lilly 110140 (fluoxetine HCl): Evidence for an inhibitory role for serotonin. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 51, 189–193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler, R. C. , Berglund, P. , Demler, O. , Jin, R. , Koretz, D. , Merikangas, K. R. , … Wang, P. S. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS‐R). JAMA, 289, 3095–3105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kindler, J. , Homan, P. , Jann, K. , Federspiel, A. , Flury, R. , Hauf, M. , … Hubl, D. (2013). Reduced neuronal activity in language‐related regions after transcranial magnetic stimulation therapy for auditory verbal hallucinations. Biological Psychiatry, 73, 518–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovacs, G. L. , & de Wied, D. (1978). Effects of amphetamine and haloperidol on avoidance behavior and exploratory activity. European Journal of Pharmacology, 53, 103–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan, V. , & Nestler, E. J. (2008). The molecular neurobiology of depression. Nature, 455, 894–902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, N. , Lee, B. , Liu, R. J. , Banasr, M. , Dwyer, J. M. , Iwata, M. , … Duman, R. S. (2010). mTOR‐dependent synapse formation underlies the rapid antidepressant effects of NMDA antagonists. Science, 329, 959–964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, N. , Liu, R. J. , Dwyer, J. M. , Banasr, M. , Lee, B. , Son, H. , … Duman, R. S. (2011). Glutamate N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate receptor antagonists rapidly reverse behavioral and synaptic deficits caused by chronic stress exposure. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 754–761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma, X. C. , Dang, Y. H. , Jia, M. , Ma, R. , Wang, F. , Wu, J. , … Hashimoto, K. (2013). Long‐lasting antidepressant action of ketamine, but not glycogen synthase kinase‐3 inhibitor SB216763, in the chronic mild stress model of mice. PLoS ONE, 8, e56053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma, X. C. , Jiang, D. , Jiang, W. H. , Wang, F. , Jia, M. , Wu, J. , … Gao, C. G. (2011). Social isolation‐induced aggression potentiates anxiety and depressive‐like behavior in male mice subjected to unpredictable chronic mild stress. PLoS ONE, 6, e20955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maeng, S. , Zarate, C. A. Jr , Du, J. , Schloesser, R. J. , McCammon, J. , Chen, G. , & Manji, H. K. (2008). Cellular mechanisms underlying the antidepressant effects of ketamine: Role of alpha‐amino‐3‐hydroxy‐5‐methylisoxazole‐4‐propionic acid receptors. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 349–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marin, M. T. , Cruz, F. C. , & Planeta, C. S. (2007). Chronic restraint or variable stresses differently affect the behavior, corticosterone secretion and body weight in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 90, 29–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathews, D. C. , Henter, I. D. , & Zarate, C. A. (2012). Targeting the glutamatergic system to treat major depressive disorder: Rationale and progress to date. Drugs, 72, 1313–1333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews, K. , Baldo, B. A. , Markou, A. , Lown, O. , Overstreet, D. H. , & Koob, G. F. (1996). Rewarding electrical brain stimulation: Similar thresholds for flinders sensitive line hypercholinergic and flinders resistant line hypocholinergic rats. Physiology & Behavior, 59, 1155–1162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moghaddam, B. , Adams, B. , Verma, A. , & Daly, D. (1997). Activation of glutamatergic neurotransmission by ketamine: A novel step in the pathway from NMDA receptor blockade to dopaminergic and cognitive disruptions associated with the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2921–2927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moosavi, M. , Yadollahi Khales, G. , Rastegar, K. , & Zarifkar, A. (2012). The effect of sub‐anesthetic and anesthetic ketamine on water maze memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. European Journal of Pharmacology, 677(1–3), 107–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray, C. J. , & Lopez, A. D. (1997). Alternative projections of mortality and disability by cause 1990‐2020: Global burden of disease study. Lancet, 349(9064), 1498–1504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murrough, J. W. , Perez, A. M. , Pillemer, S. , Stern, J. , Parides, M. K. , Aan het Rot, M. , … Iosifescu D. V. (2013). Rapid and longer‐term antidepressant effects of repeated ketamine infusions in treatment‐resistant major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 250–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers, B. , & Greenwood‐Van Meerveld, B. (2010). Divergent effects of amygdala glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptors in the regulation of visceral and somatic pain. American Journal of Physiology. Gastrointestinal and Liver Physiology, 298, G295–G303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestler, E. J. , Gould, E. , Manji, H. , Buncan, M. , Duman, R. S. , Greshenfeld, H. K. , … Zalcman, S. (2002). Preclinical models: Status of basic research in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 503–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson, C. , Jennische, E. , Ho, H. P. , Eriksson, E. , Bjorntorp, P. , & Holmang, A. (2002). Increased insulin sensitivity and decreased body weight in female rats after postnatal corticosterone exposure. European Journal of Endocrinology, 146, 847–854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parise, E. M. , Alcantara, L. F. , Warren, B. L. , Wright, K. N. , Hadad, R. , Sial, O. K. , … Bolanos‐Guzman, C. A. (2013). Repeated ketamine exposure induces an enduring resilient phenotype in adolescent and adult rats. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 750–759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pincus, H. A. , & Pettit, A. R. (2001). The societal costs of chronic major depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 62(Suppl 6), 5–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quirk, M. C. , Sosulski, D. L. , Feierstein, C. E. , Uchida, N. , & Mainen, Z. F. (2009). A defined network of fast‐spiking interneurons in orbitofrontal cortex: Responses to behavioral contingencies and ketamine administration. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 3, 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rezvani, A. H. . (2006). Involvement of the NMDA System in Learning and Memory. [PubMed]
- Sadeghi, M. , Peeri, M. , & Hosseini, M. J. (2016). Adolescent voluntary exercise attenuated hippocampal innate immunity responses and depressive‐like behaviors following maternal separation stress in male rats. Physiology & Behavior, 163, 177–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanacora, G. , Zarate, C. A. , Krystal, J. H. , & Manji, H. K. (2008). Targeting the glutamatergic system to develop novel, improved therapeutics for mood disorders. Nature Reviews. Drug Discovery, 7, 426–437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sappington, A. A. , Corssen, G. , Becker, A. T. , & Tavakoli, M. (1977). Effects of ketamine on patient responsiveness during the various phases of a single induced anxiety session. The Alabama Journal of Medical Sciences, 14, 121–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sekine, Y. , Suzuki, K. , Ramachandran, P. V. , Blackburn, T. P. , & Ashby, C. R. Jr (2007). Acute and repeated administration of fluoxetine, citalopram, and paroxetine significantly alters the activity of midbrain dopamine neurons in rats: An in vivo electrophysiological study. Synapse (New York, N. Y.), 61, 72–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shi, H. S. , Zhu, W. L. , Liu, J. F. , Luo, Y. X. , Si, J. J. , Wang, S. J. , … Lu, L. (2012). PI3K/Akt signaling pathway in the basolateral amygdala mediates the rapid antidepressant‐like effects of trefoil factor 3. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37, 2671–2683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simansky, K. , & Eberle‐Wang, K. (1993). Serotonergic mechanisms and ingestion: Pharmacological facts and physiological promises. Appetite, 21, 220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age‐related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 417–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein, D. J. (2008). Depression, anhedonia, and psychomotor symptoms: The role of dopaminergic neurocircuitry. CNS Spectrums, 13, 561–565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterneck, E. , Paylor, R. , Jackson‐Lewis, V. , Libbey, M. , Przedborski, S. , Tessarollo, L. , … Johnson, P. F. (1998). Selectively enhanced contextual fear conditioning in mice lacking the transcriptional regulator CCAAT/enhancer binding protein delta. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 10908–10913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storer, L. , & Demeyere, N. (2014). Disruptions to number bisection after brain injury: Neglecting parts of the Mental Number Line or working memory impairments? Brain and Cognition, 86, 116–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subhan, F. , Deslandes, P. N. , Pache, D. M. , & Sewell, R. D. (2000). Do antidepressants affect motivation in conditioned place preference? European Journal of Pharmacology, 408, 257–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun, L. , Lam, W. P. , Wong, Y. W. , Lam, L. H. , Tang, H. C. , Wai, M. S. , … Yew, D. T. (2011). Permanent deficits in brain functions caused by long‐term ketamine treatment in mice. Human and Experimental Toxicology, 30, 1287–1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suo, L. , Zhao, L. , Si, J. , Liu, J. , Zhu, W. , Chai, B. , … Lu, L. (2013). Predictable chronic mild stress in adolescence increases resilience in adulthood. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2013, 1387–1400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tokita, K. , Yamaji, T. , & Hashimoto, K. (2012). Roles of glutamate signaling in preclinical and/or mechanistic models of depression. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 100, 688–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treece, B. R. , Ritter, R. C. , & Burns, G. A. (2000). Lesions of the dorsal vagal complex abolish increases in meal size induced by NMDA receptor blockade. Brain Research, 872(1–2), 37–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trivedi, M. H. , Rush, A. J. , Wisniewski, S. R. , Nierenberg, A. A. , Warden, D. , Ritz, L. , … Fava, M. (2006). Evaluation of outcomes with citalopram for depression using measurement‐based care in STAR*D: Implications for clinical practice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 28–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker, D. L. , & Gold, P. E. (1991). Effects of the novel NMDA antagonist, NPC 12626, on long‐term potentiation, learning and memory. Brain Research, 549, 213–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh, R. N. , & Cummins, R. A. (1976). The open‐field test: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 482–504. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, J. H. , Fu, Y. , Wilson, F. A. , & Ma, Y. Y. (2006). Ketamine affects memory consolidation: Differential effects in T‐maze and passive avoidance paradigms in mice. Neuroscience, 140, 993–1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warner‐Schmidt, J. L. , Chen, E. Y. , Zhang, X. , Marshall, J. J. , Morozov, A. , Svenningsson, P. , & Greengard, P. (2010). A role for p11 in the antidepressant action of brain‐derived neurotrophic factor. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 528–535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warner‐Schmidt, J. L. , & Duman, R. S. (2008). VEGF as a potential target for therapeutic intervention in depression. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 8, 14–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willner, P. , Muscat, R. , & Papp, M. (1992). Chronic mild stress‐induced anhedonia: A realistic animal model of depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 16, 525–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang, C. , Han, M. , Zhang, J. C. , Ren, Q. , & Hashimoto, K. (2016). Loss of parvalbumin‐immunoreactivity in mouse brain regions after repeated intermittent administration of esketamine, but not R‐ketamine. Psychiatric Research, 239, 281–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zarate, C. Jr , Machado‐Vieira, R. , Henter, I. , Ibrahim, L. , Diazgranados, N. , & Salvadore, G. (2010). Glutamatergic modulators: The future of treating mood disorders? Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 18, 293–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zarate, C. A. Jr , Singh, J. B. , Carlson, P. J. , Brutsche, N. E. , Ameli, R. , Luckenbaugh, D. A. , … Manji, H. K. (2006). A randomized trial of an N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate antagonist in treatment‐resistant major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 856–864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao, J. , Bao, A. M. , Qi, X. R. , Kamphuis, W. , Luchetti, S. , Lou, J. S. , & Swaab, D. F. (2012). Gene expression of GABA and glutamate pathway markers in the prefrontal cortex of non‐suicidal elderly depressed patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 138, 494–502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]