Skip to main content
Neuroscience Bulletin logoLink to Neuroscience Bulletin
. 2014 Jan 6;30(3):387–393. doi: 10.1007/s12264-013-1395-y

Motor assessment of developing common marmosets

Yiwen Wang 1,2, Qin Fang 2, Neng Gong 2,
PMCID: PMC5562599  PMID: 24395453

Abstract

Motor development has been extensively studied in human infants and children, with several established scales for the evaluation of motor functions. However, the study of the neuronal mechanisms underlying human motor development is hampered by the lack of good animal models. The common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), a small New World monkey, has recently attracted much attention as a potential nonhuman primate model for understanding human physiology and diseases. However, little is known about its gross motor development. In the present study, we found that marmosets have a critical period for motor development in postnatal weeks 2 to 5, and acquire most of their motor skills by 8 weeks of age. We also developed methods to assess their motor functions, which will be useful for the evaluation of motor performance in marmoset models of human diseases. In addition, we found that marmosets exhibit a “head-to-tail” sequence of motor development similar to that found in humans, further supporting the notion that they provide a good animal model for studying the neuronal mechanisms underlying human motor development.

Keywords: marmoset, animal model, motor development, motor behavior

References

  • [1].Piper MC, Pinnell LE, Darrah J, Maguire T, Byrne PJ. Construction and validation of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS) Can J Public Health. 1992;83(Suppl2):S46–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [2].Thelen E. Motor development. A new synthesis. Am Psychol. 1995;50:79–95. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Heriza C. Motor development: traditional and contemporary theories. Contemporary Management of Motor Control Problems: Proceedings of the II STEP Conference. Alexandria, Va: Foundation for Physical Therapy; 1991. pp. 99–126. [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Yang SH, Cheng PH, Banta H, Piotrowska-Nitsche K, Yang JJ, Cheng EC, et al. Towards a transgenic model of Huntington’s disease in a non-human primate. Nature. 2008;453:921–924. doi: 10.1038/nature06975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Kordower JH, Emborg ME, Bloch J, Ma SY, Chu Y, Leventhal L, et al. Neurodegeneration prevented by lentiviral vector delivery of GDNF in primate models of Parkinson’s disease. Science. 2000;290:767–773. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Mansfield K. Marmoset models commonly used in biomedical research. Comp Med. 2003;53:383–392. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Sasaki E, Suemizu H, Shimada A, Hanazawa K, Oiwa R, Kamioka M, et al. Generation of transgenic non-human primates with germline transmission. Nature. 2009;459:523–527. doi: 10.1038/nature08090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Hook MA, Rogers LJ. Development of hand preferences in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) and effects of aging. J Comp Psychol. 2000;114:263–271. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.114.3.263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].Kaplan G, Rogers LJ. Head-cocking as a form of exploration in the common marmoset and its development. Dev Psychobiol. 2006;48:551–560. doi: 10.1002/dev.20155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Verhave PS, Vanwersch RA, van Helden HP, Smit AB, Philippens IH. Two new test methods to quantify motor deficits in a marmoset model for Parkinson’s disease. Behav Brain Res. 2009;200:214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].Iwanami A, Yamane J, Katoh H, Nakamura M, Momoshima S, Ishii H, et al. Establishment of graded spinal cord injury model in a nonhuman primate: the common marmoset. J Neurosci Res. 2005;80:172–181. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Marshall JW, Ridley RM. Assessment of cognitive and motor deficits in a marmoset model of stroke. ILAR J. 2003;44:153–160. doi: 10.1093/ilar.44.2.153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [13].Tardif SD, Mansfield KG, Ratnam R, Ross CN, Ziegler TE. The marmoset as a model of aging and age-related diseases. ILAR J. 2011;52:54–65. doi: 10.1093/ilar.52.1.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [14].McGraw MB. The Neuromuscular Maturation of the Human Infant. London: Mac Keith Press; 1943. [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Gesell A. The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In: Carmichael L, editor. Manual of Child Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1946. [Google Scholar]
  • [16].Bernstein NA. The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press Ltd.; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Schoner G, Kelso JA. Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science. 1988;239:1513–1520. doi: 10.1126/science.3281253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neuroscience Bulletin are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES