Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Aug 18;7:8719. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-09292-0

Impact of varying social experiences during life history on behaviour, gene expression, and vasopressin receptor gene methylation in mice

Carina Bodden 1,2,, Daniel van den Hove 3,4,#, Klaus-Peter Lesch 3,4,5,#, Norbert Sachser 1,2,#
PMCID: PMC5562890  PMID: 28821809

Abstract

Both negative and positive social experiences during sensitive life phases profoundly shape brain and behaviour. Current research is therefore increasingly focusing on mechanisms mediating the interaction between varying life experiences and the epigenome. Here, male mice grew up under either adverse or beneficial conditions until adulthood, when they were subdivided into groups exposed to situations that either matched or mismatched previous conditions. It was investigated whether the resulting four life histories were associated with changes in anxiety-like behaviour, gene expression of selected genes involved in anxiety and stress circuits, and arginine vasopressin receptor 1a (Avpr1a) gene methylation. Varying experiences during life significantly modulated (1) anxiety-like behaviour; (2) hippocampal gene expression of Avpr1a, serotonin receptor 1a (Htr1a), monoamine oxidase A (Maoa), myelin basic protein (Mbp), glucocorticoid receptor (Nr3c1), growth hormone (Gh); and (3) hippocampal DNA methylation within the Avpr1a gene. Notably, mice experiencing early beneficial and later adverse conditions showed a most pronounced downregulation of Avpr1a expression, accompanied by low anxiety-like behaviour. This decrease in Avpr1a expression may have been, in part, a consequence of increased methylation in the Avpr1a gene. In summary, this study highlights the impact of interactive social experiences throughout life on the hippocampal epigenotype and associated behaviour.

Introduction

An individual’s phenotype is the result of a complex interplay between its genotype and the environment. Experiences during early phases of life have long been known to profoundly modulate behavioural and transcriptomic profiles1. Over the past years, a growing body of evidence also revealed that experiences during later phases of life can significantly impact upon an individual’s epigenotype, thereby mediating behavioural variations2. More recently, the focus has moved from investigating the influence of experiences during single phases of life to the study of whole life histories both in rodents and humans35.

In our previous work in mice, we first demonstrated that adverse social experiences during different phases of life modulate not only anxiety-like behaviour and exploratory locomotion69, but also neuronal morphology10, fear conditioning and extinction as well as corresponding neurophysiologic response patterns11. In subsequent studies, we combined several established paradigms to create adverse and beneficial experiences in a whole-life-history approach3, 5. As a main result, limited adversity during adulthood was associated with decreased anxiety-like and increased exploratory behaviour, however, only in combination with beneficial experiences during early phases of life. In line with findings from human and non-human primate research, it was argued that some lifetime adversity may be beneficial3, 12, 13.

Although numerous studies have already reported long-term effects of early social experiences, the mechanisms mediating these effects are not well understood. In this context, it is also not clear how experiences during later phases of life exert effects on brain function and behaviour14, 15. Recent evidence indicates that environmental influences can induce changes in the epigenome, which is comprised of DNA methylation and chromatin structure, thereby inducing transcriptional alterations1619. Although reversible in nature, these epigenetic changes often persist beyond infancy even into adulthood, representing a molecular pathway through which long-term-programming effects are achieved16. Yet again, epigenetic regulation appears to be dynamic, where effects of early life experiences may be changed again by environmental variation later in life. This makes the study of the interplay between genes and experiences across the lifespan extremely important for environmental and pharmacologic intervention20, 21.

Over the past years, an increasing number of rodent studies demonstrated that epigenetic mechanisms can modulate various signalling pathways that are involved in the regulation of emotional behaviours and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) responses to stress1, 17, 2123. One of these pathways is serotonergic neurotransmission, which plays a key modulatory role in central nervous system processes, underlying for example anxiety, fear, and aggression24. Among the elements of the serotonin system identified as crucial regulators of emotional behaviours are the serotonin receptors 1a and 2a (Htr1a, Htr2a)25, 26 as well as monoamine oxidase A (Maoa)27. Elements known for their modulatory function of the HPA axis include the glucocorticoid receptor (GR; Nr3c1)17, 28, corticotropin releasing hormone receptor 1 (Crhr1)29, 30, and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Bdnf)31, 32. Furthermore, growth hormone (Gh) and myelin basic protein (Mbp) appeared as candidate genes in previous studies3335. Both play important roles in the brain as they are involved in neural processes such as myelination. Additionally, central pathways for vasopressin and oxytocin have been implicated in the neurobiologic mechanisms underlying anxiety and social behaviours. Hence, arginine vasopressin receptor 1a (Avpr1a) and oxytocin receptor (Oxtr) have garnered much attention since there is also increasing evidence for effects of social interactions, in particular social defeat, on their expression levels3638. Since social experiences often involve testosterone release, which acts through androgenic pathways, the androgen receptor (Ar) has raised interest. Notably, Ar expression levels appear to be profoundly influenced by sexual behaviour as well as winning fights39, 40. Moreover, the social status (‘dominant’ vs ‘submissive’) was found to be reflected in gene expression levels of neuropeptide Y (Npy) and Bdnf 41.

So far, it is well-known that experiences during specific phases of life profoundly influence an individual’s epigenome, however, the impact of whole life histories is still unknown. Based on our previous work3, 5, we hypothesized that variations in life histories bring about not only variations in the behavioural but also in the epigenetic profile. Consequently, we investigated the influence of different combinations of adverse and beneficial experiences throughout life on gene expression and DNA methylation as well as anxiety-like behaviour and exploratory locomotion in male C57BL/6 J mice.

Methods

Animals and housing conditions

In the present study, we used male C57BL/6 J mice (n = 63), which were bred in the Department of Behavioural Biology at the University of Münster, Germany. All animals were housed in transparent standard Makrolon cages type III (38 × 22 × 15 cm) with sawdust as bedding material and a paper towel as nesting material. Food pellets and tap water were provided ad libitum. Housing rooms were maintained at a 12 h light/dark cycle with lights on at 8:00 a.m., a temperature of about 22 °C, and a relative air humidity of about 50%.

Ethics statement

All procedures complied with the regulations covering animal experimentation within the EU (European Communities Council DIRECTIVE 2010/63/EU). They were conducted in accordance with the institutional and national guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals and approved by the local government authorities (Landesamt für Natur, Umwelt und Verbraucherschutz Nordrhein-Westfalen, LANUV NRW, reference number: 84–02.05.20.12.212).

Experimental design

Four different life histories (group size AA: 13, AB: 12, BA: 13, BB: 12) were experimentally induced according to the paradigm published earlier3. In line with a follow-up study5, an additional sham-handled group was included to control for handling effects (group size SH: 13). Mice were randomly assigned to one of the five experimental groups. Briefly, life histories were divided into an early and a late phase (see Fig. 1). The early phase comprised the prenatal and suckling period as well as adolescence, while the subsequent phase of early adulthood was referred to as the late phase. During both the early and late phase, mice were exposed to either adverse (‘A’) or beneficial (‘B’) experiences, or a sham-handling procedure (‘SH’). Thereby, animals underwent either consistent (AA, BB) or changing (AB, BA) social experiences from the prenatal stage until adulthood.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Experimental design. Mice were assigned to one of the four life histories or the sham-handled group. They were provided with either matching or mismatching adverse (‘A’) and/or beneficial (‘B’) conditions from the prenatal phase to adulthood, or a sham-handling (‘SH’) procedure at corresponding times. Anxiety-like and exploratory behaviour were assessed. Afterwards, brains were dissected and subjected to gene expression and methylation analyses (Figure modified after3, 5).

Adverse experiences (A) during the early phase were created by two different consecutive treatments. First, during the prenatal and suckling period (AA and AB), an adverse environment was created by exposing pregnant and lactating female C57BL/6 J mice to soiled bedding from unfamiliar males. These olfactory cues signal the danger of infant killing and thus, simulate a dangerous environment6, 9. Second, after being separated from the mother on postnatal day (PND) 22, the male offspring were housed in same-sex groups of two to five animals until PND 35 ± 2. From then on, mice were housed individually. During adolescence, adverse experiences (AA and AB) were generated by confronting the male subjects with an adult male mouse from the aggressive NMRI strain42 in order to provide loser experiences7 (5 times, 10 min duration, PND 37 ± 2–61 ± 2).

Beneficial conditions (B) during the early phase (BA and BB) were also created by two consecutive treatments. First, pregnant and lactating female C57BL/6 J mice were exposed to neutral bedding in order to simulate a safe environmental setting. Second, with the onset of adolescence, beneficial experiences for the male offspring (BA and BB) were provided by repeated encounters with a female mouse in oestrus, allowing for mating experiences, which has been shown to reduce anxiety-like behaviour43 (5 times, 10 min duration, PND 37 ± 2–61 ± 2).

During adulthood, the male test subjects were transferred to a custom-made cage system, where they experienced either an escapable adverse (AA and BA) or a beneficial situation (AB and BB; PND 70 ± 2). The cage system provided the opportunity to escape from potential agonistic encounters in the main cage to another ‘safe’ cage via a water basin (filled to a height of approximately 3 cm)8. After an acclimatization phase of 24 hours, in which the experimental male was allowed to freely explore the cage system alone, either an aggressive male mouse from the NMRI strain (applied to AA and BA mice) or a female C57BL/6 J mouse in oestrus (applied to AB and BB mice) was additionally placed into the main cage (PND 71 ± 2; for details see ref. 3). On 6 out of the following 8 days, (PND 72 ± 2–79 ± 2), the experimental animal was either placed back into the main cage where the aggressive NMRI male resided (AA and BA) or shortly handled and returned to the female (AB and BB). As a result, AA and BA mice repeatedly experienced escapable social defeat, while AB and BB animals were constantly living with a mating partner.

SH animals were not provided with any environmental modifications in their life history (neutral environment). Instead of social encounters during adolescence and adulthood, these animals were sham-handled by the experimenter at the corresponding times.

Behavioural tests

Behavioural testing took place between PND 75 ± 2 and 77 ± 2 days. In total, 63 males (sample size: AA: 13, AB: 12, BA: 13, BB: 12, SH: 13) were investigated for their anxiety-like and exploratory behaviour in the Elevated plus-maze test (EPM)44, the Dark-light test (DL)45, and the Open-field test (OF)46. The order of tests was the same for each animal with one test per day. All behavioural tests lasted 5 min and were performed during the light phase in a room different from the housing room. The test equipment was cleaned with 70% ethanol between subjects. The animal’s movements were recorded by a webcam (Logitech Webcam Pro 9000, Logitech Europe S.A., Lausanne, Switzerland) and analysed by the video tracking system ANY-maze (Version 4.75, Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, USA). Please note that the behavioural data of the present work are a subset of the data published earlier5. For more details on procedures, see Supplementary Methods.

Elevated plus-maze test

The parameters measured were the percentage of time spent on the open arms as well as the percentage of entries into the open arms to assess anxiety-like behaviour, and the sum of entries into open and closed arms as an indicator of exploratory locomotion.

Dark-light test

The parameters analysed were the latency to enter the light compartment and the percentage of time spent in the light compartment as indicators of anxiety-like behaviour, and the number of entries into the light compartment to assess exploratory locomotion.

Open-field test

The parameters analysed were the time spent in the centre of the arena (defined as the area of the OF being located at least 20 cm distant from the walls) to measure anxiety-like behaviour and the distance travelled for assessing exploratory locomotion.

Gene expression and DNA methylation analysis

Concerning gene expression analysis, we concentrated on 13 candidate genes that have been associated with HPA axis (re)activity, anxiety-like and social behaviours, brain myelination, and neuronal and synaptic plasticity. In total, brains of 61 mice were used (n = 8–13/group).

Tissue preparation

On PND 79 ± 2, two hours after the last handling in the cage system, mice were anesthetized using 2.5% isoflurane in oxygen and decapitated. Brains were immediately removed and frozen on dry ice. Afterwards, the left and right part of the hippocampus and the amygdala, respectively, were dissected on a cooling plate and subsequently crushed on dry ice to homogenize the tissue. Half of the crushed tissue was used for RNA extraction, and the other half was used for DNA extraction. Due to technical reasons, the sample sizes partly differed (gene expression analysis: AA: 13, AB: 12, BA: 11–12, BB: 11, SH: 13; DNA methylation analysis: AA: 13, AB: 8–12, BA: 11, BB: 11, SH: 12–13).

Gene expression analysis

Gene expression was analysed via quantitative real-time PCR (qRT-PCR) employing the Bio-Rad CFX384 Real-Time PCR Detection System (in technical triplicates). Mean efficiencies were calculated by LinReg47. Reference genes for normalization were tested for stability using geNorm48. Guanosine diphosphate (Gdp) dissociation inhibitor 2 (Gdi2), TATA-binding protein (Tbp), and Smad nuclear interacting protein 1 (Snip1) were used for normalization. Reference gene stability was very high (geNorm M < 0.2) and expression levels were neither affected by life history nor by the early or late phase. Relative expression data were calculated by qbase+ (Biogazelle, Zwijnaarde, Belgium), taking into account the normalization factors obtained from geNorm and the mean efficiencies from LinReg. More details on procedures as well as primer sequences can be found as Supplementary Methods and Table S1.

DNA methylation analysis

DNA bisulphite treatment was performed using the EpiTect® Fast DNA Bisulfite Kit (Qiagen) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. The PyroMark CpG Assays (Mm_AVPR1a_01_PM [PM00220241], Mm_AVPR1a_02_PM [PM00220248], Mm_AVPR1a_04_PM [PM00220262]) were obtained from Qiagen. Further PCR and sequencing primers (Mm_AVPR1a_TF) were designed with the PyroMark Assay Design 2.0.1.15 Software (Qiagen; see Fig. 2 for gene map). Pyrosequencing was conducted using the PyroMark Q96 MD™ Pyrosequencer (Biotage, Uppsala, Sweden) and PyroMark Gold Q96 CDT Reagents Kit (Qiagen). Results were analysed with the PyroMark CpG software (Qiagen). For more details on procedures as well as sequences analysed, see Supplementary Methods and Table S2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Map of the Avpr1a gene. Triangles indicate sequences analysed for DNA methylation. Vertical grey lines mark independent CpG sites (1–16). TSS = transcription start site; ATG = translation start codon; TGA = stop codon; CTCF = 11-zinc finger protein or CCCTC-binding factor (Figure created based on information from UCSC Genome Browser69, Ensembl70, and Qiagen).

Statistics

General Linear Models (GLM) were used to analyse data obtained from tests for anxiety-like behaviour and exploratory locomotion as well as gene expression and DNA methylation data. Residuals were graphically examined for homoscedasticity and outliers and the Lilliefors corrected Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test was applied. Though not all parameters met the assumptions of parametric analysis, GLM were used since ANOVA is not very sensitive to moderate deviations from normality, as indicated by simulation studies using a variety of non-normal distributions, which showed that the false positive rate is only minimally affected by this violation of the assumption4951. Specifically, the following kinds of models were established and fitted to the different dependent variables:

Model (A) Life history analysis. Univariate ANOVA of several dependent variables (anxiety-like and exploratory behaviours, gene expression, DNA methylation) with fixed between-subject factor ‘life history’. This analysis served to detect main effects of whole life histories.

Model (B) Early vs late phase analysis (without group SH). Univariate ANOVA of several dependent variables (anxiety-like and exploratory behaviours, gene expression and DNA methylation) with fixed between-subject factors ‘early phase’ (adverse: AA and AB; beneficial: BA and BB), ‘late phase’ (adverse: AA and BA; beneficial: AB and BB), and the interaction of ‘early phase’ and ‘late phase’. This analysis served to disentangle specific effects of the early vs late phase. A significant interaction would be indicative of a mismatch effect.

All main effects and interaction terms were tested on local significance level alpha = 0.05, respectively. If there were significant main or interaction effects, post hoc pairwise comparisons of different levels were conducted using Bonferroni adjustment. Data are presented as bars with means and standard error (s.e.m).

All statistical analyses were conducted using the statistical software IBM SPSS Statistics (IBM Version 23, Release 2015). Graphs were created using the software SigmaPlot for Windows (Version 12.5, Build 12.5.0.38, Systat Software, Inc. 2011).

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article (and its Supplementary Information files).

Results

Anxiety-like behaviour and exploratory locomotion

Effects of life history

A significant main effect of life history on anxiety-like behaviour was detected as revealed by the time in the centre of the OF (Model A, F(4,58) = 2.632, p = 0.043; see Supplementary Table S3). Regarding exploratory locomotion, there was a significant main effect of life history as reflected by the sum of entries in the EPM (F(4,58) = 2.783, p = 0.035), entries into the light compartment of the DL (F(4,58) = 2.868, p = 0.031; Fig. 3b), and distance travelled in the OF (F(4,58) = 3.293, p = 0.017). More specifically, AB mice entered the light compartment significantly less often than BA mice (p = 0.045) and travelled less in the OF than SH animals (p = 0.014; see Supplementary Table S4).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Anxiety-like behaviour and exploratory locomotion. (a) Percentage of entries into open arms of Elevated plus-maze (EPM) test and (b) Entries into light compartment of Dark-light (DL) test displayed by mice grown up in an early adverse (AA and AB) or beneficial (BA and BB) environment and provided with later matching (AA and BB) or mismatching (AB and BA) conditions in adulthood. Sham-handled mice (SH) served as control. There was a significant main effect of the early phase on the percentage of entries into the open arms (p = 0.042) as well as a significant main effect of life history (p = 0.031) and the late phase (p = 0.005) on the entries into the light compartment. Data are given as means + s.e.m. Statistics: ANOVA, Bonferroni post-hoc testing. *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01. n = 12–13.

Effects of early vs late phase

The additional early vs late phase analysis revealed a significant main effect of the early phase on anxiety-like behaviour, as demonstrated by the percentage of entries into open arms in the EPM (Model B, F(1,46) = 4.398, p = 0.042; Fig. 3a). More specifically, early adverse conditions increased anxiety-like behaviour. Furthermore, in particular the late phase was found to significantly influence exploratory locomotion, with late escapable adversity increasing the sum of entries into open and closed arms in the EPM (F(1,46) = 10.313, p = 0.002), the number of entries into the light compartment of the DL (F(1,46) = 8.912, p = 0.005; Fig. 3b), and the distance travelled in the OF (F(1,46) = 5.409, p = 0.025).

Gene expression

Effects of life history

There were significant main effects of life history on hippocampal gene expression levels of Avpr1a (Model A, F(4,56) = 9.501, p < 0.001; Fig. 4a), Htr1a (F(4,56) = 3.533, p = 0.012; Fig. 4b), Maoa (F(4,56) = 3.196, p = 0.020; Fig. 4c), and Mbp (F(4,56) = 2.832, p = 0.033; Fig. 4d). Remarkably, expression patterns in BA mice stood out compared to mice of other life histories, as demonstrated by post hoc analyses. Differences between life histories were most pronounced regarding Avpr1a expression. BA mice had significantly lower Avpr1a expression levels in comparison to AB (p < 0.001), BB (p < 0.001), and SH mice (p < 0.001). Moreover, AA mice differed in their Avpr1a expression in terms of decreased levels compared to both AB (p = 0.047) and SH mice (p = 0.043). Concerning Htr1a, BA mice differed from SH mice in terms of higher expression levels (p = 0.006). Maoa and Mbp expression levels were found to be decreased in BA mice compared to BB animals (Maoa: p = 0.020; Mbp: p = 0.043).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Gene expression and DNA methylation in the hippocampus. (a–e) Quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction (qRT-PCR) analysis of (a) Avpr1a, (b) Htr1a, (c) Maoa, (d) Mbp, and (e) Nrc31 mRNA as well as (f) pyrosequencing analysis of bisulphite-treated DNA at CpG site 13 within the Avpr1a gene of mice grown up in an early adverse (AA and AB) or beneficial (BA and BB) environment and provided with later matching (AA and BB) or mismatching (AB and BA) conditions in adulthood. Sham-handled mice (SH) served as control. There were significant main effects of life history (Gene expression: Avpr1a: p < 0.001; Htr1a: p = 0.012; Maoa: p = 0.020; Mbp: p = 0.033; DNA methylation: Avpr1a CpG site 13: p = 0.028) and of the late phase (Gene expression: Avpr1a: p < 0.001; Maoa: p = 0.014; Mbp: p = 0.013; Nr3c1: p = 0.013; DNA methylation: Avpr1a CpG site 13: p = 0.013). Data are given as means + s.e.m. Statistics: ANOVA, Bonferroni post-hoc testing. *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.001. n = 11–13 (ae), n = 8–13 (f). CNRQ = Calibrated Normalized Relative Quantities.

Regarding gene expression of the analysed genes in the amygdala, no main effects of life history were found (see Supplementary Table S3).

Effects of early vs late phase

The additional early vs late phase analysis revealed significant main effects of the late phase on hippocampal gene expression of Avpr1a (Model B, F(1,44) = 24.323, p < 0.001; Fig. 4a), Maoa (F(1,44) = 6.514, p = 0.014; Fig. 4c), Mbp (F(1,44) = 6.769, p = 0.013; Fig. 4d), and Nr3c1 (F(1,44) = 6.658, p = 0.013; Fig. 4e), with late escapable adversity being associated with lower expression levels when compared to late beneficial conditions. A significant early-by-late phase interaction was found regarding Gh expression (F(1,43) = 5.394, p = 0.025). Post-hoc testing showed that late adversity was only associated with increased Gh expression, when following on beneficial but not when following on adverse early conditions (p = 0.010). Furthermore, early beneficial conditions only led to increased Gh expression, when followed by adverse late experiences but not when followed by beneficial late conditions (p = 0.019). The early vs late phase analysis did not reveal any significant main effects of the early phase on expression levels of the genes analysed.

Regarding gene expression in the amygdala, the early vs late phase analysis showed that the late phase significantly influenced Npy expression (F(1,44) = 4.248, p = 0.045), with late adverse experiences predicting increased expression levels.

DNA methylation

Since the most pronounced effects of life history were found concerning Avpr1a gene expression, we additionally investigated methylation within the Avpr1a gene at 16 CpG sites in the hippocampus (see Fig. 2).

Effects of life histories

A significant main effect of life history on methylation was detected for CpG site 13 (Model A, F(4,54) = 2.957, p = 0.028), with increased methylation in BA mice compared to AB mice (p = 0.045; Fig. 4f).

Effects of early vs late phase

The early vs late phase analysis revealed significant main effects of the late phase on DNA methylation at three CpG sites (Model B, CpG site 6: F(1,38) = 4.758, p = 0.035; CpG site 8: F(1,42) = 4.052, p = 0.050; CpG site 13: F(1,42) = 6.768, p = 0.013; Fig. 4f). While late adversity predicted increased methylation at CpG sites 6 and 13, methylation was decreased at CpG site 8 compared to late beneficial experiences. No significant main effect of the early phase was found, neither an early-by-late phase interaction.

Discussion

On the behavioural level, we confirmed our earlier finding that life history profoundly modulates the behavioural profile3, 5. More precisely, we could validate that a beneficial early life followed by escapable adversity in later life (BA) is associated with relatively low levels of anxiety-like and relatively high levels of exploratory behaviour. The experimental design also allowed for testing the mismatch hypothesis, which predicts adverse consequences for the individual if the environmental experiences during early phases of life differ from those during later life stages52. However, the present results contradict the mismatch hypothesis since BA mice experienced mismatching conditions over the lifespan, but displayed diminished anxiety-like behaviour, which is in line with our previous findings. Strikingly, in addition to the behavioural effects of life history, we found significant alterations in gene expression levels. Again, mice of the BA group displayed a profoundly different expression pattern than mice of other life histories. Further analysis, performed to disentangle the effects of the early versus late phase, revealed that expression levels of several genes were significantly influenced by the late phase or, in one case, by an early-by-late phase interaction, whereas no effects of the early phase could be detected. Although it has been shown that early epigenetic marks can persist into adulthood, there is also evidence for plasticity, leaving the possibility that early effects were reversed by late effects16, 20, 21. In this study, differences in gene expression were only detected in the hippocampus. This brain region is fundamental in processing emotions, stress responses as well as spatial memory, and shows an impressive capacity for structural reorganization that is maintained even into adulthood1, 22 (for a review see ref. 53).

A key modulatory role in central nervous system processes, mediating anxiety and fear is provided by the serotonin system. In the hippocampus, HTR1A is the most abundant serotonin receptor and an important regulator of anxiety25, 54, 55. Here, we found a significant effect of life history on Htr1a expression, with expression levels being highest in BA mice. That environmental stimuli, e.g. prenatal stress or unpredictable chronic stress, alters both Htr1a gene expression and receptor binding has already been demonstrated in rats and mice25, 26, 56. In these studies, stressed animals showed a decrease of Htr1a expression or binding. A further key element in the serotonin system is MAOA which inactivates neuroactive monoamines such as serotonin and has been strongly implicated in panic disorders24, 57. In the present study, the lowest Maoa expression levels were found in BA mice. There is already evidence that the prevailing environment can modulate Maoa expression; e.g. in rats, stress during adolescence was associated with increased Maoa expression in the prefrontal cortex and at the same time increased anxiety-like behaviour on the EPM27. Altogether, these findings suggest that the effects of life history on anxiety-like behaviour are mediated, at least partly, by changes in the regulation of serotonergic pathways, especially reflected by differential Htr1a and Maoa expression.

Varying social experiences during life were also reflected by changes in hippocampal GR (Nr3c1) expression. Specifically, beneficial conditions during the late phase predicted elevated GR expression compared to adverse experiences in adulthood. Increased GR expression, which was associated with a dampened stress response, was already reported for the adult offspring of mothers performing high compared to low quality of maternal care1, 23. As the hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor system is strongly implicated in the regulation of HPA activity, increased glucocorticoid receptor expression has been suggested to mediate increased glucocorticoid feedback sensitivity1.

The most pronounced effects of life history were detected concerning Avpr1a expression. AVPR1A is a G-protein coupled receptor that binds arginine vasopressin (AVP). While there are three receptor subtypes for AVP (V1A, V1B, and V2), the V1A receptor (AVPR1A), has been suggested to play the dominant role in regulating behaviour and is implicated in the modulation of stress, anxiety, and sociability5860. The finding that BA mice showed decreased levels of anxiety-like behaviour and at the same time the lowest levels of Avpr1a expression suggests an association between life history, anxiety-like behaviour, and Avpr1a expression. Interestingly, a relationship between anxiety-like behaviour and Avp as well as Avpr1a has already been demonstrated in several studies58, 6163. Namely, Avp was found to exert anxiogenic effects and thus increased anxiety-like behaviours63. Similarly, it was shown that decreased Avpr1a expression was linked to decreased anxiety-like behaviour58, 61, 62. Moreover, evidence emerged that, with decreasing Avp expression, active coping increases63. Since in the present study, BA animals were provided with a situation in adulthood with which they had to cope by escaping from their offender through a tunnel-system, it is reasonable to assume that decreased Avpr1a expression levels were indeed linked to active coping styles. This is also underscored by the finding that the late phase rather than the early phase significantly influenced the Avpr1a gene expression.

Furthermore, the expression of two genes involved in the process of myelination of the brain, Mbp and Gh, was significantly influenced by life history (Mbp) or an early-by-late phase interaction (Gh), respectively. While Mbp expression was lowest in mice of the BA group, Gh expression appeared to be modulated by an early-by-late phase interaction. Only the combination of an early beneficial and a late adverse environment (BA) was associated with increased Gh expression compared with other combinations. Notably, it has been shown earlier that Mbp expression can be modulated by environmental variations, such as prenatal stress, which was linked to increased Mbp expression33, 34, 64. In addition, elevated gene expression levels of Gh have been associated with hippocampal-dependent learning but also with the exposure to an acute stressful event65, 66.

Since the impact of life history was most evident in terms of Avpr1a expression, we additionally investigated methylation in the Avpr1a gene in the hippocampus. The different experiences during life significantly influenced methylation at several CpG sites. Particularly experiences during the late phase left an epigenetic mark, indicating a certain degree of plasticity towards environmental stimuli, which is in line with the late phase effects on gene expression levels. Notably, mice of the BA group showed increased methylation at CpG site 13, reciprocally associated at the same time with decreased Avpr1a expression levels. These findings are consistent with the idea that the effect of different experiences during life on gene expression can be mediated by alterations in DNA methylation and chromatin structure, which needs to be further investigated in future studies on Avpr1a.

Altogether, the present findings indicate that experiences over the lifespan interact with genetic and epigenetic variations to shape the behavioural profile, particularly anxiety-like behaviour and exploratory locomotion. In addition, we show that gene expression profiles of mice were significantly altered by life history – and in particular by experiences during adulthood – with regard to the key elements of the serotonin system, Htr1a and Maoa, and furthermore Mbp, GR (Nrc31), Gh, and Avpr1a. Moreover, also Avpr1a gene methylation was impacted by life history and experiences during adulthood. There is already substantial evidence for an association between Avp and mood as well as cognitive behaviours, making its receptors the targets of novel psychopharmacological agents67, 68. Thus, Avpr1a is a promising candidate gene for future studies on the impact of diverse experiences during life on stress, anxiety and sociability.

Electronic supplementary material

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grants from the German Research Foundation (DFG: SFB/TRR58, Projects A01 and A05) to NS, KPL, and DvdH. The authors’ work was also supported by the European Community (EC: AGGRESSOTYPE FP7/No. 602805 and Eat2beNICE H2020/No. 728018) to KPL, and the 5–100 Russian Academic Excellence Project to KPL. The authors thank G. Ortega for excellent technical assistance.

Author Contributions

The study was conceived and designed by N.S., K.P.L., C.B.; data acquisition, analysis, and drafting of the work was performed by C.B.; C.B., D.v.d.H., K.P.L., N.S. interpreted the data for the work, revised it critically and approved the final version to be published.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Daniel van den Hove, Klaus-Peter Lesch and Norbert Sachser contributed equally to this work.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-09292-0

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Liu D, et al. Maternal Care, Hippocampal Glucocorticoid Receptors, and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Responses to Stress. Science. 1997;277:1659–1662. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5332.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Nestler EJ. Epigenetics: Stress makes its molecular mark. Nature. 2012;490:171–172. doi: 10.1038/490171a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bodden, C. et al. Benefits of adversity?! How life history affects the behavioral profile of mice varying in serotonin transporter genotype. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 4.Kuhn M, et al. Mismatch or allostatic load? Timing of life adversity differentially shapes gray matter volume and anxious temperament. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016;11:537–547. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Remmes, J. et al. Impact of life history on fear memory and extinction. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 6.Heiming RS, et al. Living in a dangerous world decreases maternal care: A study in serotonin transporter knockout mice. Horm. Behav. 2011;60:397–407. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jansen F, et al. Modulation of behavioural profile and stress response by 5-HTT genotype and social experience in adulthood. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;207:21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lewejohann L, et al. Social status and day-to-day behaviour of male serotonin transporter knockout mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;211:220–228. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Heiming, R. S. et al. Living in a dangerous world: the shaping of behavioral profile by early environment and 5-HTT genotype. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 3 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 10.Nietzer SL, et al. Serotonin transporter knockout and repeated social defeat stress: Impact on neuronal morphology and plasticity in limbic brain areas. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;220:42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Narayanan V, et al. Social Defeat: Impact on Fear Extinction and Amygdala-Prefrontal Cortical Theta Synchrony in 5-HTT Deficient Mice. PLoS ONE. 2011;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Seery MD, Leo RJ, Lupien SP, Kondrak CL, Almonte JL. An Upside to Adversity?: Moderate Cumulative Lifetime Adversity Is Associated With Resilient Responses in the Face of Controlled Stressors. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24:1181–1189. doi: 10.1177/0956797612469210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Parker KJ, Buckmaster CL, Sundlass K, Schatzberg AF, Lyons DM. Maternal mediation, stress inoculation, and the development of neuroendocrine stress resistance in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:3000–3005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506571103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Champagne FA, Curley JP. How social experiences influence the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005;15:704–709. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sachser N, Kaiser S, Hennessy MB. Behavioural profiles are shaped by social experience: when, how and why. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2013;368:20120344–20120344. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Champagne FA. Epigenetic influence of social experiences across the lifespan. Dev. Psychobiol. 2010;52:299–311. doi: 10.1002/dev.20436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Weaver ICG, et al. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:847–854. doi: 10.1038/nn1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zhang T-Y, Meaney MJ. Epigenetics and the Environmental Regulation of the Genome and Its Function. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2010;61:439–466. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Zheng Y, Fan W, Zhang X, Dong E. Gestational stress induces depressive-like and anxiety-like phenotypes through epigenetic regulation of BDNF expression in offspring hippocampus. Epigenetics. 2016;11:150–162. doi: 10.1080/15592294.2016.1146850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Branchi I, Cirulli F. Early experiences: Building up the tools to face the challenges of adult life: Complexity of Early Experiences. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014;56:1661–1674. doi: 10.1002/dev.21235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Weaver ICG. Reversal of Maternal Programming of Stress Responses in Adult Offspring through Methyl Supplementation: Altering Epigenetic Marking Later in Life. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:11045–11054. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3652-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Caldji C, et al. Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 1998;95:5335–5340. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Francis D, Diorio J, Liu D, Meaney MJ. Nongenomic Transmission Across Generations of Maternal Behavior and Stress Responses in the Rat. Science. 1999;286:1155–1158. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5442.1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gingrich JA, Hen R. Dissecting the role of the serotonin system in neuropsychiatric disorders using knockout mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2001;155:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s002130000573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.López JF, Chalmers DT, Little KY, Watson SJ. Regulation of serotonin 1A, glucocorticoid, and mineralocorticoid receptor in rat and human hippocampus: implications for the neurobiology of depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 1998;43:547–573. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00484-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.van den Hove DLA, et al. Prenatal stress in the rat alters 5-HT1A receptor binding in the ventral hippocampus. Brain Res. 2006;1090:29–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Márquez C, et al. Peripuberty stress leads to abnormal aggression, altered amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity and increased prefrontal MAOA gene expression. Transl. Psychiatry. 2013;3 doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Brunton PJ, Russell JA. Prenatal Social Stress in the Rat Programmes Neuroendocrine and Behavioural Responses to Stress in the Adult Offspring: Sex-Specific Effects. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2010;22:258–271. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2010.01969.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bonaz B, Rivest S. Effect of a chronic stress on CRF neuronal activity and expression of its type 1 receptor in the rat brain. Am. J. Physiol. - Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 1998;275:R1438–R1449. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.5.R1438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Pérez-Tejada J, et al. Coping with Chronic Social Stress in Mice: Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal/ Sympathetic-Adrenal-Medullary Axis Activity, Behavioral Changes and Effects of Antalarmin Treatment: Implications for the Study of Stress-Related Psychopathologies. Neuroendocrinology. 2013;98:73–88. doi: 10.1159/000353620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Branchi I, et al. Early Social Enrichment Shapes Social Behavior and Nerve Growth Factor and Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Levels in the Adult Mouse Brain. Biol. Psychiatry. 2006;60:690–696. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Coppens, C. M. et al. Social defeat during adolescence and adulthood differentially induce BDNF-regulated immediate early genes. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 5 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 33.Schraut KG, et al. Prenatal stress-induced programming of genome-wide promoter DNA methylation in 5-HTT-deficient mice. Transl. Psychiatry. 2014;4 doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.van den Hove D, et al. Differential Effects of Prenatal Stress in 5-Htt Deficient Mice: Towards Molecular Mechanisms of Gene × Environment Interactions. PLoS ONE. 2011;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jakob S, et al. Differential Effects of Prenatal Stress in Female 5-Htt-Deficient Mice: Towards Molecular Mechanisms of Resilience. Dev. Neurosci. 2014;36:454–464. doi: 10.1159/000363695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Francis DD, Young LJ, Meaney MJ, Insel TR. Naturally occurring differences in maternal care are associated with the expression of oxytocin and vasopressin (V1a) receptors: gender differences. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2002;14:349–353. doi: 10.1046/j.0007-1331.2002.00776.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Litvin Y, Murakami G, Pfaff DW. Effects of chronic social defeat on behavioral and neural correlates of sociality: Vasopressin, oxytocin and the vasopressinergic V1b receptor. Physiol. Behav. 2011;103:393–403. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Murakami G, Hunter RG, Fontaine C, Ribeiro A, Pfaff D. Relationships among estrogen receptor, oxytocin and vasopressin gene expression and social interaction in male mice. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011;34:469–477. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07761.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fuxjager MJ, et al. Winning territorial disputes selectively enhances androgen sensitivity in neural pathways related to motivation and social aggression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2010;107:12393–12398. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001394107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fernandez-Guasti A, Swaab D. & Rodr?guez-Manzo, G. Sexual behavior reduces hypothalamic androgen receptor immunoreactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28:501–512. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00036-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Fiore M, et al. Agonistic encounters in aged male mouse potentiate the expression of endogenous brain NGF and BDNF: possible implication for brain progenitor cells’ activation: Brain response to agonistic behaviour. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003;17:1455–1464. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02573.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Navarro JF. An ethoexperimental analysis of the agonistic interactions in isolated male mice: comparison between OF. 1 and NMRI strains. Psicothema. 1997;9:333–336. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Edinger KL, Frye CA. Sexual experience of male rats influences anxiety-like behavior and androgen levels. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:443–453. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 1987;92:180–185. doi: 10.1007/BF00177912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Crawley J, Goodwin FK. Preliminary report of a simple animal behavior model for the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1980;13:167–170. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90067-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Archer J. Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: A review. Anim. Behav. 1973;21:205–235. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(73)80065-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ruijter JM, et al. Amplification efficiency: linking baseline and bias in the analysis of quantitative PCR data. Nucleic Acids Res. 2009;37:e45–e45. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkp045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Vandesompele, J. et al. Accurate normalization of real-time quantitative RT-PCR data by geometric averaging of multiple internal control genes. Genome Biol. 3, research0034–1 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 49.Glass GV, Peckham PD, Sanders JR. Consequences of failure to meet assumptions underlying the fixed effects analyses of variance and covariance. Rev. Educ. Res. 1972;42:237–288. doi: 10.3102/00346543042003237. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Harwell MR, Rubinstein EN, Hayes WS, Olds CC. Summarizing Monte Carlo results in methodological research: The one-and two-factor fixed effects ANOVA cases. J. Educ. Stat. 1992;17:315–339. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lix LM, Keselman JC, Keselman HJ. Consequences of Assumption Violations Revisited: A Quantitative Review of Alternatives to the One-Way Analysis of Variance ‘F’ Test. Rev. Educ. Res. 1996;66 [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Schmidt MV. Animal models for depression and the mismatch hypothesis of disease. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Leuner B, Gould E. Structural Plasticity and Hippocampal Function. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2010;61:111–140. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Booij L, Tremblay R, Szyf M, Benkelfat C. Genetic and early environmental influences on the serotonin system: consequences for brain development and risk for psychopathology. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40:5–18. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Gordon JA, Hen R. The serotonergic system and anxiety. NeuroMolecular Med. 2004;5:27–40. doi: 10.1385/NMM:5:1:027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Watanabe Y, Sakai RR, McEwen BS, Mendelson S. Stress and antidepressant effects on hippocampal and cortical 5-HT1A and 5-HT2 receptors and transport sites for serotonin. Brain Res. 1993;615:87–94. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91117-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Deckert J, et al. Excess of high activity monoamine oxidase A gene promoter alleles in female patients with panic disorder. Hum. Mol. Genet. 1999;8:621–624. doi: 10.1093/hmg/8.4.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bielsky IF, Hu S-B, Szegda KL, Westphal H, Young LJ. Profound Impairment in Social Recognition and Reduction in Anxiety-Like Behavior in Vasopressin V1a Receptor Knockout Mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:483–493. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Goodson JL, Bass AH. Social behavior functions and related anatomical characteristics of vasotocin/vasopressin systems in vertebrates. Brain Res. Rev. 2001;35:246–265. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00043-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Hammock EAD, Lim MM, Nair HP, Young LJ. Association of vasopressin 1a receptor levels with a regulatory microsatellite and behavior. Genes Brain Behav. 2005;4:289–301. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00119.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bielsky IF, Hu S-B, Ren X, Terwilliger EF, Young LJ. The V1a Vasopressin Receptor Is Necessary and Sufficient for Normal Social Recognition: A Gene Replacement Study. Neuron. 2005;47:503–513. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Ecke LE, et al. CREB-mediated alterations in the amygdala transcriptome: coordinated regulation of immune response genes following cocaine. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;14:1111–1126. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Neumann ID, Landgraf R. Balance of brain oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for anxiety, depression, and social behaviors. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:649–659. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Föcking M, et al. Proteomic Investigation of the Hippocampus in Prenatally Stressed Mice Implicates Changes in Membrane Trafficking, Cytoskeletal, and Metabolic Function. Dev. Neurosci. 2014;36:432–442. doi: 10.1159/000365327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Donahue CP, et al. Transcriptional Profiling Reveals Regulated Genes in the Hippocampus During Memory Formation. Hippocampus. 2002;12:821–833. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Donahue CP, Kosik KS, Shors TJ. Growth hormone is produced within the hippocampus where it responds to age, sex, and stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:6031–6036. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507776103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Holmes A, Li Q, Murphy DL, Gold E, Crawley JN. Abnormal anxiety‐related behavior in serotonin transporter null mutant mice: the influence of genetic background. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2:365–380. doi: 10.1046/j.1601-1848.2003.00050.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Murgatroyd C, Wu Y, Bockmühl Y, Spengler D. Genes learn from stress: How infantile trauma programs us for depression. Epigenetics. 2010;5:194–199. doi: 10.4161/epi.5.3.11375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Chinwalla AT, et al. Initial sequencing and comparative analysis of the mouse genome. Nature. 2002;420:520–562. doi: 10.1038/nature01262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Yates A, et al. Ensembl 2016. Nucleic Acids Res. 2016;44:D710–D716. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkv1157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article (and its Supplementary Information files).


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES