Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Aug 21.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012 Oct 12;19(2):173–180. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712001166

Influence of Cognitive Function on Speech and Articulation Rate in Multiple Sclerosis

Jonathan D Rodgers 1, Kris Tjaden 2, Lynda Feenaughty 2, Bianca Weinstock-Guttman 1,3, Ralph H B Benedict 1,3
PMCID: PMC5564302  NIHMSID: NIHMS892450  PMID: 23058309

Abstract

We examined cognitive predictors of speech and articulation rate in 50 individuals with multiple sclerosis (MS) and 23 healthy controls. We measured speech and articulation rate from audio-recordings of participants reading aloud and talking extemporaneously on a topic of their choice (i.e., self-generated speech). Articulation rate was calculated for each speech sample by removing lexically irrelevant vocalizations and pauses of >200 ms. Speech rate was similarly calculated including pauses. Concurrently, the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in Multiple Sclerosis (MACFIMS) battery, as well as standardized tests of sentence intelligibility and syllable repetition were administered. Analysis of variance showed that MS patients were slower on three of the four rate measures. Greater variance in rate measures was accounted for by cognitive variables for the MS group than controls. An information processing speed composite, as measured by the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) and the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), was the strongest predictor among cognitive tests. A composite of memory tests related to self-generated speech, above and beyond information processing speed, but not to oral reading. Self-generated speech, in this study, was not found to relate more strongly to cognitive tests than simple reading. Implications for further research are discussed.

Keywords: Multiple sclerosis, Articulation disorders, Speech production measurement, Neuropsychology, Cognitive science, Neurobehavioral manifestations

INTRODUCTION

Multiple sclerosis (MS) affects diverse functional systems including ambulation, fine motor, cognitive, and speech communication abilities (Benedict et al., 2011; Hartelius, Buder, & Strand, 1997). This autoimmune disease involves lymphocyte attack on myelinated fibers leading to widespread white and gray matter demyelinating lesions and atrophy (Compston & Coles, 2008). Cognitive impairment is common, appearing in approximately 50% of cases (Arnett & Strober, 2011; Benedict & Bobholz, 2007). In addition, abnormalities in spoken communication are found in 40 to 50% of MS patients (Hartelius, Runmarker, & Andersen, 2000; Yorkston et al., 2003). Differences in spoken communication can also negatively impact work status and quality of life (Baylor, Yorkston, Bamer, Britton, & Amtmann, 2010; Yorkston et al., 2003).

An important consideration in the clinical management of MS is how impairment in one functional system may impact or interact with another. Cognitive-speech motor interaction is an emerging area of interest. Arnett and colleagues (2008) examined relationships between cognition and oral-motor speed, using the Maximum Repetition Rate of Syllables task (Kent, Kent, & Rosenbek, 1987). They found robust correlation between syllable production rate and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT; Benton, Sivan, Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1994), the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT; Gronwall, 1977), and the oral-response version of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT; Smith, 1982). However, this study was limited by the use of a syllable repetition task for which inter-judge measurement reliability is low (Gadesmann & Miller, 2008), although this concern is not ubiquitous (Kent, Duffy, Kent, Vorperian, & Thomas, 1999; Tjaden & Watling, 2003). In addition, this task involves repeating nonsense syllables, and movement strategies for nonsense speaking tasks are quite different from those used for real-world spoken communication (Weismer, 2006; Westbury & Dembowski, 1993). Additionally, Mackenzie and Green (2009) showed that a composite of language-based cognitive tasks selected from the Arizona Battery for Communication Disorders of Dementia (Bayles & Tomoeda, 1993) was significantly correlated with sentence intelligibility as indexed by the Sentence Intelligibility Test (Yorkston & Beukelman, 1984). However, the study did not explore cognitive abilities beyond those directly tied to linguistic ability. In addition, the sample size was small (n = 24) and limited to patients referred for dysarthria treatment, such that problems with intelligibility may have been overrepresented.

The present study investigated the cognitive-speech motor relationship using connected speech tasks and neuropsychological tests from multiple cognitive domains. We hypothesized that (a) cognitive variables would explain more variance in rate measures for participants with MS than healthy controls and (b) in MS, self-generated speech would demonstrate higher cognitive demand as evidenced by cognitive variables having greater explanatory power for rate measures of the self-generated speech task as compared to the reading task.

METHODS

Participants

MS participants included 50 individuals with a mean age of 49.0 ± 8.9 years and mean education of 14.8 ± 2.5 years. Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS; Kurtzke, 1983) scores ranged from 0 to 6.5 with a mean of 4.0 ± 1.9. The sample was 94% Caucasian, 60% female, and disease duration was 13.3 ± 8.0 years (see Table 1 for detail). There were 24 relapsing-remitting patients and 26 secondary-progressive patients as determined by their treating neurologist. Inclusion criteria were (a) diagnostic criteria for MS based on Polman et al. criteria (2005), (b) no history of significant drug/alcohol abuse, (c) no sensory impairments that might interfere with cognitive or speech testing, (d) no history of ADHD or learning disability, (e) no comorbid medical conditions that may affect cognition or motor function, (f) absence of a recent relapse or corticosteroid use (within 4 weeks), (g) absence of a level of physical or neurological impairment that would make cognitive/language testing invalid, (h) native speaker of standard American English, (i) no neuropsychiatric disease predating MS (e.g., mood disorder), (j) no current or prior use of antipsychotic medication, (k) functional hearing with the ability to pass a pure tone audiometric screening (bilateral pure tone thresholds of at least 40 dB at 500 Hz and 1–4 KHz).

Table 1.

Between-group comparisons: Demographic and cognitive measures

Variable MS Controls F p d


n = 50 n = 23


Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Age 49.00 (8.88) 47.13 (9.06) 0.69 n.s.
Education (years) 14.76 (2.48) 15.89 (2.01) 3.53 n.s.
Sex: male/female, % female 20/30, 60% 8/15, 65.2%
Race: C/AA/O, % Caucasian 47/2/1, 94% 21/2/0, 91%
Depression (BDI-FS) 2.96 (2.82) 0.83 (1.30) 11.92 .001 0.9
Information Processing Speed
 Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) 47.44 (15.34) 61.83 (10.67) 16.50 < .001 1.0
 Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3-second (PASAT3) 42.00 (13.24) 48.65 (9.77) 4.63 .035 0.5
Memory
 California Verbal Learning Test-Total (CVLT2-TL) 52.44 (13.36) 60.39 (8.49) 6.84 .011 0.7
 California Verbal Learning Test-Delay (CVLT2-DR) 11.02 (3.68) 13.04 (1.82) 6.22 .015 0.6
 Brief Visual Motor Test Revised-Total (BVMTR-TL) 18.88 (7.58) 23.70 (5.16) 7.63 .007 0.7
 Brief Visual Motor Test Revised-Total (BVMTR-DR) 7.40 (2.67) 9.48 (1.68) 11.79 .001 0.9
Executive Function
 Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) 33.32 (11.56) 41.43 (13.53) 6.96 .010 0.7
 Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Sorting Test-Correct Sorts (DKEFS-CS) 8.88 (3.27) 11.52 (2.15) 12.49 .001 0.9

Note. All p values were based on two-tail tests. n.s. = not significant.

Control participants (n = 23) were 8 men and 15 women with a mean age of 47.1 ± 9.1 years and mean education level of 15.9 ± 2.0 years. Control participants met the same inclusion criteria as MS participants. The study was approved by the Health Sciences Institutional Review Board at SUNY Buffalo. Study participants were paid for participation.

Measures of Cognitive Function

Measures of cognitive function were obtained using the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in MS (MAC-FIMS; Benedict et al., 2002) that has well established reliability and validity, including strong correlation with brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) metrics (Benedict, Ramasamy, Munschauer, Weinstock-Guttman, & Zivadinov, 2009; Benedict et al., 2006).

The MACFIMS measures a broad array of cognitive functions. The oral form of the SDMT (Smith, 1982) measures cognitive processing speed. Working memory and processing are assessed using the PASAT (Gronwall, 1977). Verbal memory is assessed using the California Verbal Learning Test, 2nd Edition (CVLT2; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 2000). The Brief Visual Memory Test, Revised (BVMTR; Benedict, 1997) measures visual learning and memory. The COWAT (Benton et al., 1994) assesses verbal fluency. Finally, the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System-Sorting Test (DKEFS; Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001) is used to assess categorical reasoning.

Speech Tasks, Procedures, and Measures

All speech tasks are part of the standard, clinical motor-speech examination used in speech pathology (Duffy, 2005). Speech materials were recorded and transduced using an Isomax ear-mounted microphone (model #E610P5L2) and digitized at a sampling rate of 22.05 kHz using the computer program TF32 (Milenkovic, 2002).

Descriptive speech measures

The Sentence Intelligibility Test (Yorkston, Beukelman, & Tice, 1996) and the Maximum Repetition Rate of Syllables and Multisyllabic Combinations task (Kent et al., 1987) were obtained for the purpose of characterizing the study population. The Sentence Intelligibility Test involves audio-recording a talker as she/he reads 11 sentences aloud from a printed script. After recording, three speech-language pathologists were presented the sentences via headphones for orthographic transcription. The transcription was compared to the script of the passage and the mean percentage of words matching the script was used. The Maximum Repetition Rate of Syllables and Multisyllabic Combinations task is a syllable repetition task (Kent et al., 1987) involving the rapid repetition of the syllables “puh,” “tuh,” and “kuh” individually, as well as repetition of all three syllables in succession (i.e., “puh-tuh-kuh”). The score used was the average number of syllables produced per second for the individual syllables and the sequence. A more detailed description of procedures for collection is outlined in Tjaden and Watling (2003).

Experimental speech measures

Experimental speech materials consisted of a Reading Passage and a Self-Generated Passage used to prompt production of connected speech. Previous research suggests measures using real-world connected speech provide insight into factors related to speech-motor behavior for individuals with progressive neurologic disease (Huber & Darling, 2011; Lowit, Brendel, Dobinson, & Howell, 2006). The Reading Passage selected for use was the Grandfather Passage (Duffy, 2005). This 115-word passage contains most speech sounds in varying combinations and takes approximately 35 to 45 s to read aloud for individuals with normal reading skill. Participants were given a printed script of the Reading Passage to read aloud. The script was printed on a single white 8.5 × 11 page using a plain, Calibri, 16-point, black font. The examiner first read the passage aloud while participants followed along in silence, then the participant read. For the Self-Generated Passage, participants were instructed to talk about a topic of personal interest for several minutes. Topics were suggested by the examiner from a list that included favorite foods, pets, hobbies, a memorable vacation, jobs, and sports. Once the participant had selected a topic, audio-recording commenced and continued for 2 min. If the participant stopped speaking before a sufficient sample length was recorded, the examiner prompted with a question or comment.

Extemporaneous or self-generated speech is considered to be a more cognitively demanding than simply reading aloud from a printed script (Huber & Darling, 2011; Tasko & McClean, 2004). A printed script is thought to invoke more automatic literacy-based neural pathways thereby reducing the demands on working memory. Self-generated speech, in contrast, requires planning what to say, creating or recalling content, and monitoring the conversation for understanding. Speech measures described below were obtained for the entire Reading Passage and the initial 45 s of the Self-Generated Passage. This duration of the Self-Generated Passage was selected for study to correspond to the mean duration of the Reading Passage for all participants. The initial 45 s was selected as best matching the conditions in the Reading Passage.

Procedures for calculating rate measures were similar to those reported in Tjaden and Wilding (2011). The printed script of the Reading Passage and a transcribed script of each Self-Generated Passage was used to obtain syllable counts. A trained research assistant generated the initial orthographic transcription of each Self-Generated Passage which was then reviewed and edited by a second research assistant. In the case of any disagreements, repeated listening and discussion were used to reach consensus concerning content as well as syllable counts. Using TF32 (Milenkovic, 2002) speech acoustic software, stretches of speech and pauses were identified for both tasks. Pauses included silent pauses, operationally defined as a silent period between words of greater than 200 milliseconds (Tjaden & Wilding, 2004), and filled pauses, operationally defined as a non-lexical vocalization or sound hesitation such as “um” “uh” and so forth of any length (Clark & Fox Tree, 2002; Goldman-Eisler, 1968). For each task, overall speech rate and articulation rate were calculated. Speech rate was calculated by counting the number of syllables spoken and dividing by the passage duration yielding speech rate in syllables per second. Articulation rate refers to the rate of speech per unit time excluding both silent and filled pauses. Thus, when calculating articulation rate in syllables per second, all filled and silent pause durations were subtracted from the passage duration.

Speech rate and articulation rate measures were repeated for a random sample of approximately 10% of the Reading Passages and Self-Generated Passages to determine measurement reliability. For both intra- and inter-judge measurement reliability, analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated no significant difference in speech rate or articulation rate measures for the Reading Passage (p >.05). Similar findings held for the Self-Generated Passage.

Depression

The Beck Depression Inventory-Fast Screen (BDI-FS; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 2000) was administered. The BDI-FS is a seven-item, self-report measure of depression. This measure has been validated for use with MS (Benedict, Fishman, McClellan, Bakshi, & Weinstock-Guttman, 2003).

General Procedures

After recruitment and screening, study participants were tested on cognitive tasks (MACFIMS), completed depression screening, provided speech samples, and were examined by a neurologist to obtain an EDSS. Examiners included trained graduate students and neuropsychology fellows. Cognitive tests were administered under the supervision of a board-certified neuropsychologist. Tests and speech recordings were conducted in a standard clinical testing room.

Speech recording was conducted on the same day as cognitive testing whenever possible (69.9% of subjects). Of those patients not completing all measures on the same day (30.1%), the mean number of days to complete was 23.7 ± 45.4 days. No significant differences on NP or speech variables were detected between subjects with a delay, and those without (using ANOVA, p >.05). No significant differences were detected in the average delay of subjects in the healthy control group versus the MS groups (using ANOVA, p >.05).

Data Analysis

Data analysis was conducted with SPSS 19.0. Variables were assessed for deviation from normality by examining graphic representations and skewness/kurtosis statistics. All variables approximated a normal distribution. Between-group comparisons on demographic, cognitive, sentence intelligibility, and syllable repetition rates for the Maximum Repetition Rate of Syllables and Multisyllabic Combinations task were made to establish differences between MS and healthy controls. Although the number of between-group comparisons puts the findings at significant risk for type-1 error, no Bonferroni correction was used as these comparisons were descriptive in nature and not used to test hypotheses.

Following the between-group comparisons, all of the cognitive tests from the MACFIMS were normalized into Z-scores using a regression-based normative procedure as described in Parmenter, Testa, Schretlen, Weinstock-Guttman, and Benedict (2010). The regression model was derived from 152 healthy controls, an expanded sample from those used in the original study by Parmenter et al. (2010). Age and gender were the normalizing factors. To minimize the number of variables used to test hypotheses, we calculated three cognitive composites from the mean Z-scores for the cognitive tests. These composites and their component tests were selected based on a principle component analysis of the MACFIMS (Benedict et al., 2006). The executive composite consisted of the COWAT and DKEFS-sorting correct sorts. The memory composite consisted of learning and delayed trials from the CVLT2 and BVMTR. The information processing speed composite included the SDMT and PASAT3-second.

The first hypothesis, that cognitive variables would explain more variance in rate measures for participants with MS than healthy controls, was tested using stepwise regression models. Four models were constructed, with speech rate and articulation rate measures for the Self-Generated Passage and the Reading Passage each as the dependent variable. Demographic variables (age and depression) were first entered as a block. Then the executive, memory, and information processing speed composites were included using forward stepwise progression with the composite accounting for the greatest variance added first, followed by the subsequent composites until no further significant (p < .05) composites remained. One follow-up analysis was used. To directly assess the possible influence of articulation, syllable repetition rate (“puh-tuh-kuh”) was included as an intermediary step between incidental variables and the cognitive composites.

The second hypothesis, that the Self-Generated Passage would be more strongly related to cognition than the Reading Passage for MS patients, was tested using the information processing speed composite as the dependent variable in a hierarchical regression using speech rate and articulation rate from the Self-Generated Passage and Reading Passage as predictors. Again, age and depression were entered as block 1, followed by differing orders of the rate measures.

RESULTS

Between-Group Differences

Demographic variables

There were no statistically significant MS/control differences on age, education, gender, or race (see Table 1). On BDI-FS the MS group had higher scores (F[1,71] = 11.92; p = .001), with a mean of 3.0 ± 2.8 versus 0.8 ± 1.3 for normal controls.

Cognitive tests

Comparisons on cognitive test results revealed expected differences favoring controls (see Table 1). Most cognitive variables were statistically significant (using p < .01). Effect sizes ranged from d = 0.5 for PASAT3 to d = 1.0 for SDMT, representing medium to large effects, and replicating prior work with the same test battery (Benedict et al., 2006).

Descriptive speech measures

Speech measure comparisons are summarized in Table 2. Despite a marginally significant difference on the Sentence Intelligibility Test (F[1,70] = 6.24; p = .015), speech for both groups was almost 100% intelligible (97–98%). Thus, the group difference in these scores is not clinically meaningful. For the syllable repetition task, only the multi-syllable rate “puh-tuh-kuh” was significant (F[1,71] = 8.51; p = .005) for MS versus the control group, with faster rates for the control group.

Table 2.

Between-group comparisons for speech measures

Variable MS Controls F p d


n = 50 n = 23


M (SD) M (SD)
Experimental Speech Measures
Reading Passage-Speech Rate 3.56 (0.69) 4.07 (0.35) 11.25 .001 0.8
Reading Passage-Articulation Rate 4.32 (0.64) 4.72 (0.44) 7.49 .008 0.7
Self-Generated Passage-Speech Rate 3.39 (0.70) 3.85 (0.52) 7.60 .007 0.7
Self-Generated Passage-Articulation Rate 4.57 (0.72) 4.80 (0.72) 1.67 .200 0.3
Descriptive Speech Measures
Sentence Intelligibility Test 96.78% (1.49) 97.68% (1.21) 6.24 .015 0.6
Syllable Repetition Task “Puh-Tuh-Kuh” 4.99 (1.31) 5.91 (1.12) 8.51 .005 0.7
Syllable Repetition Task “Puh” 5.37 (1.14) 5.47 (1.16) 0.12 .731 0.1
Syllable Repetition Task “Tuh” 5.11 (1.25) 5.32 (1.06) 0.44 .508 0.2
Syllable Repetition Task “Kuh” 4.81 (1.11) 5.25 (1.13) 2.42 .124 0.4

Note. Speech rates, articulation rates, “puh-tuh-kuh” and individual syllable measures are calculated in syllables per second. All p-values were based on two-tailed tests.

Experimental speech tasks

MS patients had slower speech and articulation rates for the Reading Passage (see Table 2) with effects sizes of d = .8 and .7, respectively. Similar results were obtained for the Self-Generated Passage with effect sizes of d = .7 and .3, respectively. However, articulation rates for the Self-Generated Passage were not statistically significant (F[1,71] = 1.67; p = .200).

Hypothesis Testing Results

Cognition and rate measures

The results of the stepwise regression models for the MS group predicting the experimental speech rate measures are reported in Table 3. Age and depression were not significant contributors to any model.

Table 3.

Stepwise regression for MS: Cognition and rate measures

Order retained R2 ΔR2 F change (df) p (F change)
Reading Passage-Speech Rate
  Age and Depression .024 .024 .59 (2, 47) = .561
 1. Information Processing Speed Composite .365 .341 24.68 (1, 46) < .001
 – Executive and Memory Composites, not retained p >.05
Reading Passage-Articulation Rate
  Age and Depression .018 .018 .46 (2, 47) = .650
 1. Information Processing Speed Composite .316 .298 20.01 (1, 46) < .001
 – Executive and Memory Composite, not retained p >.05
Self-Generated Passage-Speech Rate
  Age and Depression .061 .061 1.53 (2, 47) = .228
 1. Information Processing Speed Composite .314 .253 16.96 (1, 46) < .001
 2. Memory Composite .381 .067 4.84 (1, 45) = .033
 – Executive Composite, not retained, p >.05
Self-Generated Passage-Articulation Rate
  Age and Depression .006 .006 .14 (2, 47) = .871
 1. Information Processing Speed Composite .120 .114 5.94 (1, 46) = .019
 2. Memory Composite .196 .076 4.27 (1, 45) = .045
 – Executive Composite, not retained, p >.05

Note. The age and depression variables were force entered as a block. The subsequent order of composites was determined by forward stepwise regression with p < .05 required to retain a composite and composites retained in order of greatest variance accounted for.

For the Reading Passage, the information processing speed composite (SDMT and PASAT) accounted for substantial variance in speech and articulation rates, 34% and 30%, respectively. The executive and memory composites added negligible variance above and beyond information processing speed. For the Self-Generated Passage, the information processing speed composite again accounted for substantial variance in speech rate (25%) and a small amount of variance in articulation rate (11%). In the Self-Generated Passage, the memory composite (CVLT2 and BVMTR) was additionally retained in both models. This composite accounted for an additional 7% of variance in speech rate and 8% in articulation rate. The combination of information processing speed and memory composites accounted for a total of 32% (speech rate) and 19% (articulation rate) of the variance in the Self-Generated Passage. In contrast to the MS models, the stepwise models for the normal controls resulted in no cognitive composites retained at p < .05.

Follow-up analysis of the MS group with syllable repetition rate, “puh-tuh-kuh,” did not substantially change the results. Above and beyond incidental variables and “puh-tuh-kuh” rates, in the Reading Passage the information processing speed composite still accounted for 21% of variance (F change = 17.06; p < .001) in speech rate and 17% of variance (F change = 13.12; p = .001) in articulation rate. In the speech rate measure for the Self-Generated Passage, the additional contribution of the information processing speed composite was 12% (F change = 10.23; p = .003) and the memory composite still accounted for 5% of additional variance (F change = 4.77; p = .034). For articulation rate in the Self-Generated Passage, adding the articulation measure made both composites non-significant and not retained in the model.

Cognitive demand and rate measures

The models evaluating the cognitive demand of speech tasks are summarized in Table 4. The association between information processing speed and speech variables was much stronger for reading than it was for self-generated speech, contrary to our hypothesis. For speech rate, above and beyond age and depression, the Reading Passage accounted for 35% of the variance while the Self-Generated Passage accounted for only 3%. When the Self-Generated Passage was entered first, it accounts for 27% of the variance with the Reading Passage still accounting for an additional 11%. The results for articulation rate paralleled those for speech rate. Entered first, the Reading Passage accounted for 30% of the variance with a negligible amount of additional variance from the Self-Generated Passage. Self-Generated Passage entered first accounted for 11% of variance and the Reading Passage added 19%.

Table 4.

Influence of speech task: Information processing speed composite as the dependent variable

Order Block R2 ΔR2 F change (df) p (F change)
 1. Age and Depression .004 .004 .10 (2, 47) .907
Speech rate
 2. Reading Passage .352 .348 24.68 (1, 46) < .001
 3. Self-Generated Passage .381 .029 2.14 (1, 45) .151
 2. Self-Generated Passage .272 .268 16.96 (1, 46) < .001
 3. Reading Passage .381 .109 7.91 (1, 45) .007
Articulation rate
 2. Reading Passage .306 .302 20.01 (1, 46) < .001
 3. Self-Generated Passage .308 .002 .14 (1, 45) .709
 2. Self-Generated Passage .118 .114 5.94 (1, 46) .019
 3. Reading Passage .308 .190 12.36 (1, 45) .001

Note. Order column represents the order of variables entered into hierarchical regression models. For all models, age and depression were entered first, followed by differing orders of the speech or articulation rate measures from the Self-Generated or Reading Passages.

DISCUSSION

In this prospective study, we found differences on rate measures for both oral reading and self-generated speech produced by MS patients compared to healthy controls. The effect sizes were of medium magnitude. Regression models predicting rate from cognitive abilities were statistically significant for patients but not healthy controls. Findings therefore support the hypothesis that cognitive changes may be a factor in speech-motor performance in MS, as measured by rate of speech. In addition, high sentence intelligibility scores suggest that cognitive rather than physiological (i.e., dysarthria or speech-motor) variables contributed to the slower speech and articulation rates for MS patients. This is further supported by closely similar results when a commonly used measure of articulation was included as a follow-up analysis.

Generally, these results replicate the findings of Benedict et al. (2011) who reported robust correlations between aspects of processing speed, ambulation, and manual speed/dexterity in MS. We extend those findings, tentatively, to speech-motor behavior during connected speech, in the form of speech rate and articulation rate measures. Benedict et al. (2011) hypothesized that shared neural networks involving frontal white matter mediate both gross motor control and cognitive processing, and that neuropathology in these shared networks may be a possible source of concurrent impairments. This relationship can also be seen in work with structural imaging and post-mortem studies of frontal and subcortical regions tied to processing speed being critical in regulating aspects of gait (e.g., step-length and support time; Rosano, Brach, Studenski, Longstreth, & Newman, 2007; Rosano et al., 2012). Although these studies link gross motor skills to cognitive processing through shared neuropathology, there is limited research coupling cognition and speech-motor behavior in MS. The current study begins to elucidate this link. Impairments in information processing speed and real-world speech-motor function, (i.e., rate of connected, complex speech) may also reflect shared, common regions of brain pathology, a hypothesis we are considering for future research. Like Arnett et al. (2008), we believe that oral motor impairment also impacts cognitive performance where speech is required.

Our data did not support the idea that self-generated speech is more difficult or relevant for cognition than simple reading. One possible explanation is that allowing participants to select a topic for the Self-Generated Passage from among several on a list (i.e., hobbies, jobs) yielded a familiar topic which the participants had prior experience discussing. This suggestion is borne out by the small contribution of the memory composite to rate measures in the Self-Generated Passage, but not the Reading Passage. Future studies could evaluate this idea by studying Self-Generated Passages where participants are and are not allowed to choose their own topic.

Limitations to the study include a small sample size (MS group [n = 50]) for conducting a multiple regression analysis. We attempted to minimize the number of variables through the use of composites. Also, no statistical correction was used in the regression models, indicating a risk of type-1 error. This was considered justified as this study is an initial exploration of the cognitive-speech motor relationship with real-world rate measures. Future research into these relationships should expand the sample size and potentially use more conservative statistical procedures (e.g., Bonferroni correction). There was also a time delay between collection of cognitive tests and speech measures, although subsequent analysis indicated this was not related to performance. Also, for the Self-Generated Passage, we did not evaluate the content or complexity of speech, which may provide additional evidence for the increased or decreased demand of the task.

This study benefited from using well-validated speech (Tjaden & Wilding, 2004; Yorkston et al., 1996) and cognitive measures with demonstrated psychometric properties (Benedict et al., 2002, 2006) and strong relationships to brain MRI (Benedict et al., 2004). It also tested a range of cognitive constructs often compromised in MS. We provided an expansion of the existing work on the interplay between cognitive variables and speaking rate measures in MS. We conclude that cognitive abilities, particularly information processing speed, may be related to speech and articulation rate during connected speech for MS patients. Further imaging studies should evaluate the possibility of shared regional pathology that may explain the observed relationship between speech production and cognitive ability. Further research is also needed to evaluate the perceptual and clinical relevance of the speaking rate aberrancies for participants with MS. These aberrancies may contribute to reduced speech naturalness which may, in turn, have implications for potential employability, social relationships (Baylor et al., 2010; Klugman & Ross, 2002), and perceived competence (Allard & Williams, 2008). Finally, this study continued to demonstrate the utility of the SDMT as a component of comprehensive measurement of cognitive impairment in MS, as seen in prior studies (Benedict et al., 2011; Drake et al., 2010).

Acknowledgments

Research supported by NIH-NIDCD R01DC004689. PI: K. Tjaden. The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors would like to acknowledge Ms. Marietta Hoogs for her contribution to recruitment and data collection.

References

  1. Allard ER, Williams DF. Listeners’ perceptions of speech and language disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2008;41:108–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2007.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnett PA, Smith MM, Barwick FH, Benedict RHB, Ahlstrom BP. Oralmotor slowing in multiple sclerosis: Relationship to neuropsychological tasks requiring an oral response. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2008;14:454–462. doi: 10.1017/s1355617708080508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnett PA, Strober LB. Cognitive and neurobehavioral features in multiple sclerosis. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics. 2011;11:411–424. doi: 10.1586/ern.11.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bayles KA, Tomoeda CK. Arizona battery for communication disorders of dementia. Tucson, AZ: Canyonlands; 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baylor C, Yorkston K, Bamer A, Britton D, Amtmann D. Variables associated with communicative participation in people with multiple sclerosis: A regression analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2010;19:143–153. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0087). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. BDI-fast screen for medical patients: Manual. San Antonia, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  7. Benedict RHB. Brief visuospatial memory test-revised: professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  8. Benedict RHB, Bobholz JH. Multiple sclerosis. Seminars in Neurology. 2007;27:78–85. doi: 10.1055/s-2006-956758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Benedict RHB, Cookfair D, Gavett R, Gunther M, Munschauer F, Garg N, Weinstock-Guttman B. Validity of the minimal assessment of cognitive function in multiple sclerosis (MACFIMS) Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2006;12:549–558. doi: 10.1017/s1355617706060723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Benedict RHB, Fischer JS, Archibald CJ, Arnett PA, Beatty WW, Bobholz J, … Munschauer F. Minimal neuropsychological assessment of MS patients: A consensus approach. Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2002;16:381–397. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.3.381.13859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Benedict RHB, Fishman I, McClellan MM, Bakshi R, Weinstock-Guttman B. Validity of the Beck Depression Inventory-Fast Screen in multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis Journal. 2003;9:393–396. doi: 10.1191/1352458503ms902oa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Benedict RHB, Holtzer R, Motl RW, Foley FW, Kaur S, Hojnacki D, Weinstock-Guttman B. Upper and Lower Extremity Motor Function and Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2011;17:643–653. doi: 10.1017/s1355617711000403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Benedict RHB, Ramasamy D, Munschauer FE, Weinstock-Guttman B, Zivadinov R. Memory impairment in multiple sclerosis: Correlation with deep gray matter and mesial temporal atrophy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry. 2009;80:201–206. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.148403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Benedict RHB, Weinstock-Guttman B, Fishman I, Sharma J, Tjoa CW, Bakshi R. Prediction of neuropsychological impairment in multiple sclerosis: Comparison of conventional magnetic resonance imaging measures of atrophy and lesion burden. Archives of Neurology. 2004;61:226–230. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.2.226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Benton AL, Sivan AB, Hamsher K, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment: A clinical manual. 2. New York: Oxford University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  16. Clark HH, Fox Tree J. Using Uh and Um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition. 2002;84:73–111. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00017-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Compston A, Coles A. Multiple sclerosis. Lancet. 2008;372:1502–1517. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(08)61620-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  19. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test: Adult version. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  20. Drake AS, Weinstock-Guttman B, Morrow SA, Hojnacki D, Munschauer FE, Benedict RHB. Psychometrics and normative data for the Multiple Sclerosis Functional Composite: Replacing the PASAT with the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Multiple Sclerosis. 2010;16:228–237. doi: 10.1177/1352458509354552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Duffy JR. Motor speech disorders: Substrates, differential diagnosis, and management. 2. St. Louis: Elsevier Science; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  22. Gadesmann M, Miller N. Reliability of speech diadochokinetic test measurement. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2008;43:41–54. doi: 10.1080/13682820701234444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Goldman-Eisler F. Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. New York: Academic Press; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  24. Gronwall D. Paced auditory serial addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1977;44:367–373. doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hartelius L, Buder EH, Strand EA. Long-term phonatory instability in individuals with multiple sclerosis. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 1997;40:1056–1072. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4005.1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hartelius L, Runmarker B, Andersen O. Prevalence and characteristics of dysarthria in a multiple-sclerosis incidence cohort: Relation to neurological data. Folia Phoniatrica Et Logopaedica. 2000;52:160–177. doi: 10.1159/000021531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Huber JE, Darling M. Effect of Parkinson’s disease on the production of structured and unstructured speaking tasks: Respiratory physiologic and linguistic considerations. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2011;54:33–46. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0184). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kent RD, Duffy J, Kent JF, Vorperian HK, Thomas JE. Quantification of motor speech abilities in stroke: Time-energy analyses of syllable and word repetition. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology. 1999;7:83–90. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kent RD, Kent JF, Rosenbek JC. Maximum performance tests of speech production. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1987;52:367–387. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5204.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Klugman TM, Ross E. Perceptions of the impact of speech, language, swallowing, and hearing difficulties on quality of life of a group of South African persons with multiple sclerosis. Folia Phoniatrica Et Logopaedica. 2002;54:201–221. doi: 10.1159/000063194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple-sclerosis: An Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) Neurology. 1983;33:1444–1452. doi: 10.1212/wnl.33.11.1444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lowit A, Brendel B, Dobinson C, Howell P. An investigation into the influences of age, pathology, and cognition on speech production. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology. 2006;14:253–262. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mackenzie C, Green J. Cognitive-linguistic deficit and speech intelligibility in chronic progressive multiple sclerosis. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 2009;44:401–420. doi: 10.1080/13682820802697879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Milenkovic P. TF32. University of Wisconsin-Madison; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  35. Parmenter BA, Testa SM, Schretlen DJ, Weinstock-Guttman B, Benedict RHB. The utility of regression-based norms in interpreting the minimal assessment of cognitive function in multiple sclerosis (MACFIMS) Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2010;16:6–16. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709990750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Polman CH, Reingold SC, Edan G, Filippi M, Hartung HP, Kappos L, … Wolinsky JS. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2005 Revisions to the “McDonald Criteria”. Annals of Neurology. 2005;58:840–846. doi: 10.1002/ana.20703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rosano C, Brach J, Studenski S, Longstreth WT, Newman AB. Gait variability is associated with subclinical brain vascular abnormalities in high-functioning older adults. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;29:193–200. doi: 10.1159/000111582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rosano C, Studenski SA, Aizenstein HJ, Boudreau RM, Longstreth WT, Newman AB. Slower gait, slower information processing and smaller prefrontal area in older adults. Age and Ageing. 2012;41:58–64. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Smith A. Symbol digit modalities test: Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  40. Tasko SM, McClean MD. Variations in articulatory movement with changes in speech task. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2004;47:85–100. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Tjaden K, Watling E. Characteristics of diadochokinesis in multiple sclerosis and Parkinson’s disease. Folia Phoniatrica Et Logopaedica. 2003;55:241–259. doi: 10.1159/000072155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Tjaden K, Wilding GE. Rate and loudness manipulations in dysarthria: Acoustic and perceptual findings. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2004;47:766–783. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/058). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Tjaden K, Wilding GE. Effects of speaking task on intelligibility in Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2011;25:155–168. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2010.520185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Weismer G. Philosophy of research in motor speech disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2006;20:315–349. doi: 10.1080/02699200400024806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Westbury J, Dembowski JS. Articulatory kinematics of normal diadochokinetic performance. Annual Bulletin of the Research Institute of Logopedics and Phoniatrics. 1993;27:13–36. [Google Scholar]
  46. Yorkston K, Beukelman D, Tice R. Sentence intelligibility test. Lincoln, NE: Institute for Rehabilitation Science and Engineering at Madonna Rehabilitation Hospital; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  47. Yorkston KM, Beukelman DR. Assessment of intelligibility of dysarthric speech. Austin, TX: PRO-ED; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  48. Yorkston KM, Klasner ER, Bowen J, Ehde DM, Gibbons LE, Johnson K, Kraft G. Characteristics of multiple sclerosis as a function of the severity of speech disorders. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology. 2003;11:73–84. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES