Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Aug 22;6:e25062. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25062

High-intensity training enhances executive function in children in a randomized, placebo-controlled trial

David Moreau 1,, Ian J Kirk 1, Karen E Waldie 1
Editor: Heidi Johansen-Berg2
PMCID: PMC5566451  PMID: 28825973

Abstract

Background:

Exercise-induced cognitive improvements have traditionally been observed following aerobic exercise interventions; that is, sustained sessions of moderate intensity. Here, we tested the effect of a 6 week high-intensity training (HIT) regimen on measures of cognitive control and working memory in a multicenter, randomized (1:1 allocation), placebo-controlled trial.

Methods:

318 children aged 7-13 years were randomly assigned to a HIT or an active control group matched for enjoyment and motivation. In the primary analysis, we compared improvements on six cognitive tasks representing two cognitive constructs (N = 305). Secondary outcomes included genetic data and physiological measurements.

Results:

The 6-week HIT regimen resulted in improvements on measures of cognitive control [BFM = 3.38, g = 0.31 (0.09, 0.54)] and working memory [BFM = 5233.68, g = 0.54 (0.31, 0.77)], moderated by BDNF genotype, with met66 carriers showing larger gains post-exercise than val66 homozygotes.

Conclusions:

This study suggests a promising alternative to enhance cognition, via short and potent exercise regimens.

Funding:

Funded by Centre for Brain Research.

Clinical trial number:

NCT03255499.

Research organism: Human

eLife digest

Exercise has beneficial effects on the body and brain. People who perform well on tests of cardiovascular fitness also do well on tests of learning, memory and other cognitive skills. So far, studies have suggested that moderate intensity aerobic exercise that lasts for 30 to 40 minutes produces the greatest improvements in these brain abilities.

Recently, short high-intensity workouts that combine cardiovascular exercise and strength training have become popular. Studies have shown that these brief bouts of strenuous exercise improve physical health, but do these benefits extend to the brain? It would also be helpful to know if the effect that exercise has on the brain depends on an individual’s genetic makeup or physical health. This might help to match people to the type of exercise that will work best for them.

Now, Moreau et al. show that just 10 minutes of high-intensity exercise a day over six weeks can boost the cognitive abilities of children. In the experiments, over 300 children between 7 and 13 years of age were randomly assigned to one of two groups: one that performed the high-intensity exercises, or a ‘control’ group that took part in less active activities – such as quizzes and playing computer games – over the same time period. The children who took part in the high-intensity training showed greater improvements in cognitive skills than the children in the control group. Specifically, the high-intensity exercise boosted working memory and left the children better able to focus on specific tasks, two skills that are important for academic success.

Moreau et al. further found that the high-intensity exercises had the most benefit for the children who needed it most – those with poor cardiovascular health and those with gene variants that are linked to poorer cognitive skills. This suggests that genetic differences do alter the effects of exercise on the brain, but also shows that targeted exercise programs can offer everyone a chance to thrive.

Moreau et al. suggest that exercise need not be time consuming to boost brain health; the key is to pack more intense exercise in shorter time periods. Further work could build on these findings to produce effective exercise routines that could ultimately form part of school curriculums, as well as proving useful to anyone who wishes to improve their cognitive skills.

Introduction

Possibly the most reliable means to induce cognitive improvements behaviorally, physical exercise has become known for its effects on the brain in addition to its well-documented impact on the body (see for a review Moreau and Conway, 2013). Individuals with higher cardiovascular fitness typically show higher performance on a wide range of cognitive measures, from cognitive control (Pontifex et al., 2011) to working memory (Erickson et al., 2013) and executive functioning (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Hillman et al., 2008). In addition, long-term sport practice is associated with higher working memory capacity (Moreau, 2013), spatial ability (Moreau, 2012), and more efficient visual processing of movements (Güldenpenning et al., 2011). In neuroimaging studies, greater fitness indices have also been linked to differences in white matter integrity (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2014; Voss et al., 2013), as well as with larger hippocampal (Weinstein et al., 2012) and cortical areas (Erickson et al., 2009; Makizako et al., 2015). These results are corroborated by increased long-term potentiation (LTP) in the visual system of physically fit individuals compared to the non-fit (Smallwood et al., 2015), a key finding given the primary role of LTP in major cognitive processes such as learning and memory (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993, Bliss and Lomo, 1973).

Importantly, these correlational findings are further supported by longitudinal designs. Exercise interventions can elicit cognitive improvements in diverse populations ranging from children (Davis et al., 2011) to the elderly (Fabre et al., 2002), as well as in individuals with various clinical conditions such as developmental coordination disorders (Tsai et al., 2012) and schizophrenia (Pajonk et al., 2010). Furthermore, improvements appear to be dose-dependent (Davis et al., 2007; Vidoni et al., 2015), and are not restricted to atypical or clinical populations—adults at their cognitive peak show similar benefits (Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman, 2013; Moreau and Conway, 2013; Voss et al., 2011).

In school settings, exercise interventions have shown to be associated with higher levels of academic achievement (Coe et al., 2006), and exercise regimens in children typically lead to improvements in various aspects of cognition, including executive function, cognitive control and memory (see for a review Tomporowski et al., 2015a). Interventions implemented in early stages of life allow capitalizing on higher cortical plasticity, potentially maximizing their impact (Cotman and Berchtold, 2002). The appeal of early interventions has motivated a whole line of research exploring the effect of physical exercise regimens on behavior, cognitive function, and scholastic performance (Castelli et al., 2007; Davis et al., 2007, Davis et al., 2011; Donnelly et al., 2016; Jackson et al., 2016; Pontifex et al., 2013). Consistent with these findings, Sibley et al. found a robust effect of physical exercise on cognitive function in children, in a meta-analytic assessment of the literature at the time (Sibley and Etnier, 2003).

Most studies in this line of research have evaluated the impact of a rather specific type of regimen, based on aerobic exercise. Usually defined as a sustained regimen performed at moderate intensity (e.g., McArdle et al., 2006), aerobic exercise has come to be accepted as the form of exercise typically associated with neural changes and cognitive enhancement (Hillman et al., 2008; Thomas et al., 2012), for at least two reasons. First, current interventions are rooted in early findings in the animal literature, which typically investigated the effects of physical exercise in rodents—animals who naturally favor aerobic forms of exercise (Gould et al., 1999; Shors et al., 2001). Second, the most dramatic gains in cognition have been observed in the elderly (Erickson et al., 2015; but see also Etnier et al., 2006; Young et al., 2015, for a more nuanced view), a population for which moderate-intensity exercise is seemingly most adequate. Subsequent studies have stemmed from this line of research, therefore expanding the initial paradigm to a wider range of populations.

Yet current trends of research suggest other promising directions. For example, regimens based on resistance training have shown sizeable effects on cognition (Best et al., 2015; Liu-Ambrose et al., 2012; van Uffelen et al., 2007), despite underlying mechanisms of improvements being potentially different from those elicited by aerobic exercise (Goekint et al., 2010). More complex forms of motor training that combine high physical and cognitive demands also appear encouraging (Moreau et al., 2015; Tomporowski et al., 2015b). Moreover, a compelling body of research in the field of exercise physiology indicates that interventions based on short, intense bursts of exercise can induce physiological changes that mirror those following aerobic exercise on a wide variety of outcomes. These include measures of cardiovascular function (Chrysohoou et al., 2015; Gayda et al., 2016), overall fitness (Benda et al., 2015), and general health (Milanović et al., 2015). In some cases, physiological improvements following high-intensity training (HIT) can even go beyond those typically following aerobic regimens (Rognmo et al., 2004). For example, HIT appears to be particularly effective to increase the release of neurotrophic factors essential to neuronal transmission, modulation and plasticity—potentially surpassing aerobic exercise regimens (Ferris et al., 2007). This body of research is promising, as it suggests a plausible mechanism by which intense bursts of exercise could meaningfully influence cognitive function and behavior.

A few studies have partially tested this idea. Short bouts of exercise have been shown to alleviate some of the difficulties typically associated with Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children (Piepmeier et al., 2015), demonstrating the potential of this type of intervention to enhance cognitive abilities. The benefits reported in this study were not limited to children diagnosed with ADHD, however—typical children also exhibited cognitive improvements. More strikingly perhaps, Pontifex and colleagues found that a single 20 min bout of exercise was sufficient to improve cognitive function and scholastic performance in children (Pontifex et al., 2013). This is an impressive finding, given that exercise-induced cognitive improvements typically occur after longer training periods (Etnier et al., 2006; Sibley and Etnier, 2003). Importantly, these effects should be distinguished from short-term improvements immediately following acute bouts of exercise (Tomporowski, 2003), which typically dissipate after a few hours. The two types of outcomes (short-term consequences of single acute sessions vs. more durable benefits) are sometimes conflated, resulting in misleading conclusions (Jackson et al., 2016). The hypothesized mechanisms are, however, different—heightened state of alertness induced by neurotransmitter increases for the former (Tomporowski, 2003), and slower but more durable neurophysiological adaptations in the case of the latter (Erickson et al., 2011, Erickson et al., 2015; Moreau and Conway, 2013; Voss et al., 2013).

One aspect that remains to be formerly investigated relates to the specific influence of exercise intensity. Although focused on short sessions, the aforementioned studies utilized regimens of moderate intensity—a reported 62–72% (Piepmeier et al., 2015) and 65–75% (Pontifex et al., 2013) of individual maximum heart rate, respectively. Yet based on findings from the physiological literature (e.g., Rognmo et al., 2004), there are clear mechanisms via which exercising at a high intensity could influence cognition in a meaningful manner. Arguably, HIT could elicit improvements above and beyond those typically following short sessions of moderate intensity, and provide a legitimate, time-efficient alternative to longer aerobic exercise regimens (Costigan et al., 2015). Together, the conjunction of promising early findings and clear mechanisms of action has prompted discussions to implement HIT interventions more systematically within the community (Gray et al., 2016).

In an effort to better understand and predict individual responses to physical exercise interventions, several studies have investigated the role of specific genetic polymorphisms on the magnitude of exercise-induced improvements (Erickson et al., 2008, Erickson et al., 2013). Among these, many have focused on the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) val66met polymorphism, given its direct influence on serum BDNF concentration (Lang et al., 2009). BDNF is known to support neuronal growth and has been shown to facilitate learning, a process that in turn induces BDNF production (Berchtold et al., 2001; Cotman and Berchtold, 2002; Kesslak et al., 1998). This dynamic coupling makes BDNF an important underlying factor of exercise-induced cognitive improvements. Consistent with this idea, it has been proposed that individuals whose particular BDNF polymorphism is associated with lower activity-dependent BDNF levels (met66 carriers) might benefit from exercise interventions to a greater extent than individuals whose activity-dependent BDNF levels are higher (val66 homozygotes).

Similarly, a few studies have shown that individuals with lower cardiovascular function might maximize benefits from physical exercise interventions designed to improve cognitive function (Sofi et al., 2011; Strong et al., 2005). The implicit assumption is that although lack of physical exercise might be a limiting factor for individuals whose fitness level is low, more active individuals might be less impacted by an exercise intervention program (Heyn et al., 2004; Lautenschlager et al., 2008; Sniehotta et al., 2006). Despite the intuitive appeal of this assumption, several studies, including one from our group, have failed to find a positive correlation between exercise-induced cognitive improvements and the associated physiological changes (Moreau et al., 2015; Tsai et al., 2014). Arguably, the absence of a clear link between the hypothesized physiological mechanisms of improvements and tangible cognitive gains might stem from the plurality and complexity of variables underlying these changes, rendering coherent associations elusive.

Despite similarities in the physiological mechanisms linking aerobic exercise and HIT on cognition, the precise impact of the latter on cognitive performance remains to be confirmed experimentally. In the present study, we tested the viability of HIT as a substitute for aerobic exercise to induce cognitive improvements in school populations. In particular, we postulated that HIT would result in improvements in measures of cognitive control and working memory, as both constructs have been linked to fitness levels (Pontifex et al., 2011) and appear to be malleable via aerobic regimens (Erickson et al., 2013). The choice of these constructs was also motivated by previous research showing the malleability of both cognitive control and working memory in training studies, thus providing theoretical and empirical support for the plausibility of expected improvements (Hampshire et al., 2012; Mishra et al., 2014). Consistent with recent efforts to better model and understand the mechanisms of cognitive improvement (Young et al., 2015; Moreau and Waldie, 2015), the present study also intended to address interindividual variability so as to isolate the underlying factors of improvement. Based on previous literature (Erickson et al., 2013; Moreau et al., 2015), we hypothesized that exercise training would elicit substantially larger cognitive benefits in individuals whose cardiovascular fitness is low, and in BDNF met66 carriers, whose activity-dependent BDNF levels are naturally limited. Finally, we expected physiological improvements with exercise, as typically induced from aerobic interventions (see for a review Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman, 2013).

Results

Statistical analyses were performed in R (RRID:SCR_001905; Core Team R, 2016). The following R packages were used for our analyses (in alphabetical order): BayesFactor (Morey and Rouder, 2015), car (Fox and Weisberg, 2011), dplyr (Wickham, 2011), ggplot2 (Wickham, 2009), gridExtra (Auguie, 2012), lsr (Navarro, 2015), psych (Revelle, 2015), pwr (Champely, 2015), rjags (Plummer, 2016). All packages were retrieved from CRAN (RRID:SCR_003005; https://cran.r-project.org/). Figures 4–6 were generated in JASP (JASP Team, 2016). R code and data are freely available on GitHub (https://github.com/davidmoreau/2017_eLife; a copy is archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/2017_eLife-1). The repository includes data sets, R scripts, details and script of the HIT workout, the CONSORT flow diagram and the CONSORT checklist.

In this section, we report Bayesian model comparisons, to allow quantifying the degree of evidence for a given model compared to other models tested, as well as Bayesian parameter estimations when relevant. All the equivalent frequentist analyses can be found at the end of the Results section.

Normality of distribution was examined for all continuous variables. If distributions were skewed, we compared results using non-corrected vs. log-transformed data, and looked for discrepancies. Although the analyses we present below are fairly robust to outliers, as priors can be adapted to reflect deviations from normality, we systematically checked consistency using standard approaches to outlier exclusion, to facilitate direct comparisons with frequentist tests. We defined outliers as values more than 3/2 times the upper quartile or less than 3/2 times the lower quartile of a given distribution, and systematically checked consistency of our results with and without inclusion.

Physiological improvements

Participants in the exercise group saw a greater decrease in resting heart rate than controls, as demonstrated by a Bayesian ANCOVA with Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a fixed factor and baseline heart rate as a covariate. The full model was preferred to the model with baseline resting heart rate only: BFM = 40.45, and was the most likely given our data: P(M | Data)=0.93, assessed from equal prior probabilities (Figure 1A). In-depth analyses focused on individuals with elevated resting heart rate at baseline allowed further insights into the potency of our exercise intervention. Specifically, a Bayesian t-test on resting heart rate change showed a sizeable difference between the two groups, with larger gains for the HIT group (BF10 = 3.47, with Mgain = 6.11, SDgain = 11.64 and Mgain = 1.89, SDgain = 8.63, for HIT and Control, respectively; Hedges’ g = 0.41 (0.09, 0.73). Test-retest reliability—assessed via a comparison between pretest and posttest resting heart rate for controls only—was acceptable (r = 0.77, BF10 = 1.75 e+51).

Figure 1. Physiological and effort-dependent measures.

Figure 1.

(A) Violin and box plots showing change in resting heart rate (in BPM) between pretest and posttest sessions, for HIT and control groups. The dashed line shows the point of perfect equivalence between pretest and posttest measurements; values below the line indicate heart rate decreases. (B) Targeted range accuracy, defined as the ratio of maximum measured heart rate per participant (in BPM) to targeted heart rate (expected), averaged across sessions. Dark dots show accuracy based on pretest resting heart rate, whereas light dots show accuracy based on posttest resting heart rate. The blue dashed line represents the point of perfect agreement between individual targeted heart rate and maximum measured heart rate. Values above the line represent higher measured heart rate than expected from baseline. (C) Time series of the maximum heart rate (in BPM) measured for a single workout, averaged over participants, plotted across sessions. Smoothing is modeled via a non-parametric locally weighted regression using a nearest neighbor approach (i.e. local polynomial regression fitting). (D) Time series of the total number of steps for a single workout, averaged over participants, shown across sessions. Smoothing is modeled via a non-parametric locally weighted regression using a nearest neighbor approach (i.e. local polynomial regression fitting).

Physiological data also provided important indications about workout intensity idiosyncrasies. We used resting heart rate at pretest to determine target intensities for each individual, such that:

HRTarget=HRReset+δ(HRMaxHRRest) (1)

where HRMax = 220 Age, and δ is set to. 80. This yielded an individual target range (HRTarget or above) while exercising. We then compared this range with the maximum intensity measured during each workout, to obtain an index of accuracy, or agreement, between target zone and actual effort. Results showed that participants did exercise at a suitable intensity overall, as expressed by the deviation from individual target heart rate values (MDev = 1.65, SDDev = 5.87; Figure 1B). Importantly, effort intensity was maintained stable across time, as demonstrated by moderate evidence favoring the null model over an alternate model that included time as a predictor of maximum heart rate in a Bayesian linear regression analysis [BFM = 2.87, P(M | Data)=0.74, Figure 1C]. Because individual resting heart rates tended to decrease throughout the intervention, sustained effort indicates that individuals incrementally increased workout volume, which was confirmed by additional measures such as step count [BFM = 2.979e + 10, P(M | Data) ≈ 1, for the model that included Session as a covariate, Figure 1D]. Together, these results support the notion that the intervention was adaptive, allowing workout intensities tailored to each individual.

Physiological improvements are informative in two key aspects: they provide corroborating evidence for the hypothesized changes associated with exercise, and they allow identifying idiosyncratic parameters often characteristic of training interventions. However, the main goal of a cognitive intervention is to elicit cognitive gains, which were the primary outcomes of the present intervention. In the following sections, we first identify latent constructs from cognitive assessments, before discussing the impact of the intervention on these two constructs.

Exploratory factor analysis

An exploratory factor analysis using principal component extraction and promax rotation was performed on all six cognitive measures at pretest. Although less common than orthogonal rotations, oblique rotations such as promax allow factors to correlate; this property is especially appropriate when the factors extracted are assumed to be correlated to some degree−a reasonable assumption given our design. The corresponding scree plot and eigenvalues (i.e. the variance in all variables accounted for by each factor) suggested a two-component solution (see factor loadings in Table 1 and Table 1—source data 1 and 2). Subsequent test of the two-factor model confirmed that the number of factors was sufficient (χ2 (4)=0.59, p=0.96; Bayesian Information Criterion, BIC = −22.05). We refer to these two components hereafter as Cognitive Control and Working Memory. The correlation between the two factors was r = 0.32. Uniqueness values indicated that the tasks spanned an adequate range within the sample space of each construct (Table 1).

Table 1. Exploratory factor analysis for cognitive measurements at baseline. .

F1 (Cognitive Control) and F2 (Working Memory) refer to the factor loadings of each measure from an exploratory factor analysis with promax rotation (N = 287). Uniqueness represents the variance of each item not accounted for by the two factors.

Table 1—source data 1. Scree plot for the exploratory factor analysis on all cognitive measures.
The plot shows the eigenvalues associated with each factor plotted against each factor, and supports the decision to retain two factors.
Table 1—source data 2. Path diagram for the exploratory factor analysis on all cognitive measures.
F1 (Cognitive Control) and F2 (Working Memory) refer to the factors extracted from an exploratory factor analysis on all six cognitive measures, with promax rotation (N = 287).
Measure CC WM Uniqueness
Flanker 0.89 0.21
Go/no-go 0.71 0.48
Stroop 0.55 0.71
Backward digit span 0.70 0.51
Backward Corsi blocks 0.27 0.91
Visual 2-back 0.33 0.90

Note: Only factor loadings greater than. 25 are included in the table.

Cognitive improvements

Here, we report cognitive improvements broken down by constructs, defined based on the factors extracted from the exploratory factor analysis.

A Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA on Cognitive Control scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor, showed moderate evidence for the interaction model over the main effect model [BFM = 3.38, p(M | Data)=0.46; Table 2]. Participants in the HIT group showed larger improvements than controls from pretest to posttest (Mgain = 0.25, SDgain = 0.6 and Mgain = 0.08, SDgain = 0.47, respectively, Hedges’ g = 0.31 [0.09, 0.54]; Figure 2A, see also Figures 4–6). A Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA on Working Memory scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor showed strong evidence for the interaction model over the main effect model [BFM = 5233.68, p(M | Data) ≈1; Table 3]. Participants in the HIT group showed larger improvements than controls from pretest to posttest (Mgain = 0.48, SDgain = 0.83 and Mgain = 0.12, SDgain = 0.44, respectively, Hedges’ g = 0.54 [0.31, 0.77]; Figure 2B).

Table 2. Model comparisons for the Cognitive Control construct (CC) with condition as a fixed factor. .

The table shows the probability of each model given the data P(M | Data), the corresponding Bayes Factor, BF10 and the percentage of error. The unconditional probability for each model is 0.2.

Models P(M | Data) BFM BF10 Error (%)
 Null 1.01e −5 4.05e −5 1 -
 Session 0.43 3.06 43120.06 0.98
 Condition 2.32e −6 9.28e −6 0.23 3.93
 Main effects 0.11 0.49 11143.87 4.52
 Interaction 0.46 3.38 43792.87 5.52

Figure 2. Cognitive improvements.

Figure 2.

Violin and box plots showing gains in Cognitive Control (A) and Working Memory (B) between pretest and posttest sessions, for HIT and control groups.

Table 3. Model comparisons for the Working Memory construct (WM) with condition as a fixed factor. .

The table shows the probability of each model given the data P(M | Data), the corresponding Bayes Factor, BF10 and the percentage of error. The unconditional probability for each model is 0.2.

Models P(M | Data) BFM BF10 Error (%)
 Null 2.92e −13 1.17e −12 1 -
 Session 4.49e −4 0 1.54e + 9 1.23
 Condition 1.84e −13 17349e −13 0.63 0.69
 Main effects 3.15e −4 0 1.08e + 9 3.13
 Interaction 1 5232.68 3.42e + 12 2.91

Because the cognitive improvements we reported are presumably based on physiological changes, we directly tested the relationship between the two types of variables. A Bayesian regression analysis showed that change in resting heart rate was a reliable predictor of cognitive gains in the HIT group, with respect to Cognitive Control (BF10 = 6.34, p(M | Data)=0.86). This was not the case in the Control group (BF10 = 0.20, p(M | Data)=0.16). The contrast was weaker when comparing Working Memory gains in the HIT group (BF10 = 0.56, p(M | Data)=0.36) with those of the Control group (BF10 = 0.18, p(M | Data)=0.15). Additional Bayesian regression analyses showed that lower resting heart rate at pretest did not predict improvements in either Cognitive Control or Working Memory in the HIT group (BF10 = 0.41, p(M | Data)=0.29. and BF10 = 0.18, p(M | Data)=0.15, respectively). This was also the case when the analyses were restricted to the. 75 quantile of individuals with the lowest resting heart rate at baseline (BF10 = 0.34, p(M | Data)=0.25. and BF10 = 0.62, p(M | Data)=0.38, respectively). Overall, baseline resting heart rate was a fairly noisy measure (M = 85.2, SD = 14.77, over the entire sample) and this might have contributed to the lack of clear impact of resting heart rate change on cognitive function.

Effect of BDNF genotype

A subsample of our data allowed for a better understanding of individual differences in exercise-induced cognitive improvements. Specifically, we looked at the effect of variations in the BDNF polymorphism on cognitive gains in the HIT group, via a comparison between met66 carriers (i.e. met66/ met66 or val66/ met66) and non-carriers (val66 homozygotes). Separate Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs on Cognitive Control and Working Memory scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and BDNF polymorphism (val66 homozygotes vs. met66 carriers) as a between factor showed strong evidence for the interaction model in both cases [BFM = 31.17, p(M | Data)=0.89, and BFM = 675.92, p(M | Data)=0.99, for Cognitive Control and Working Memory, respectively, see Table 4 and Table 5]. These findings suggest that met66 carriers benefited to a greater extent than non-carriers from the exercise intervention (Cognitive Control: Mgain = 0.93, SDgain = 1.20 and Mgain = 0.05, SDgain = 0.13, Hedges’ g = 1.36 [0.52, 2.2]; Working Memory, Mgain = 0.87, SDgain = 0.64 and Mgain = 0.14, SDgain = 0.24, Hedges’ g = 1.83 [0.94, 2.72]; Figure 3). Unequal baseline scores cannot fully account for this effect since evidence for differences in Cognitive Control was limited at pretest (BF10 = 4.03, Error (%)=1.55 e −6 from a Bayesian independent samples t-test) and more substantial, but unable to account for the full effect, for Working Memory (BF10 = 17.47, Error (%)=1.22 e −6 from a Bayesian independent samples t-test). Together, these findings indicate that although genetic variations in the BDNF polymorphism are associated with cognitive differences, the latter are malleable and can be reduced with physical exercise.

Table 4. Model comparisons for the Cognitive Control construct (CC) with BDNF polymorphism as a fixed factor. .

The table shows the probability of each model given the data P(M | Data), the corresponding Bayes Factor, BF10 and the percentage of error. The unconditional probability for each model is 0.2.

Models P(M | Data) BFM BF10 Error (%)
 Null 0.01 0.06 1 -
 Session 0.03 0.12 1.94 1.45
 Condition 0.02 0.10 1.65 2.60
 Main effects 0.04 0.19 3.05 1.71
 Interaction 0.89 31.17 59.49 24.61

Table 5. Model comparisons for the Working Memory construct (WM) with BDNF polymorphism as a fixed factor. .

The table shows the probability of each model given the data P(M | Data), the corresponding Bayes Factor, BF10 and the percentage of error. The unconditional probability for each model is 0.2.

Models P(M | Data) BFM BF10 Error (%)
 Null 2.47e −5 9.90e −5 1 -
 Session 0 0.01 79.86 0.77
 Condition 3.52e −5 1.41e −4 1.42 0.69
 Main effects 0 0.01 155.37 5.84
 Interaction 0.99 675.92 40159.39 3.51

Figure 3. Effect of BDNF allele on cognitive improvements.

Figure 3.

μ and σ2 parameter estimates from the posterior distribution for the difference between BDNF met carriers and non-carriers (met66 – val66 homozygotes) in cognitive gains. Estimates were generated from 10,000 iterations, in one chain, with thinning interval of one (no data point discarded). (A) Trace of μ for Cognitive Control. (B) σ2 estimate for Cognitive Control. (C) Trace of μ for Working Memory. (D) σ2 estimate for Working Memory.

Priors and robustness

All priors used in the reported analyses are default prior scales (Morey and Rouder, 2015). For Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA and ANCOVA, the prior scale on fixed effects is set to 0.5, the prior scale on random effects to 1, and the prior scale on the covariate to 0.354. The latter is also used in Bayesian Linear Regression. The Bayesian t-test uses a Cauchy prior with a width of √2/2 (~0.707), that is half of parameter values lies within the interquartile range [−0.707; 0.707].

It is worth pointing out that the Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA that showed only moderate evidence for the effect of our HIT intervention on Cognitive Control shows stronger evidence with a slight variation on the prior scale. Although this variation in priors is consistent with our data and provides stronger evidence for our claim, we chose to report analyses with default prior scales, as these were the intended parameters a priori. For transparency, we plotted below the prior and posterior distribution for the comparison between Conditions (HIT vs. Control) for Cognitive Control (Figure 4), as well as the Bayes Factor robustness check (Figure 5). Both indicate that our findings are robust and supported by a wide range of priors, as corroborated by a sequential analysis (Figure 6).

Figure 4. Prior and posterior distributions for the comparison between Conditions (HIT vs. Control) for Cognitive Control.

Figure 4.

The graph shows the density of each distribution as a function of effect size, with the prior centered on the null effect.

Figure 5. Bayes factor robustness check for the comparison between Conditions (HIT vs. Control) for Cognitive Control.

Figure 5.

The figure shows our default prior, as well as wide and ultrawide priors. Importantly, the curve shows stronger evidence for our hypothesis with narrower priors, indicating that our conclusions are not based on a restricted range of priors.

Figure 6. Sequential analysis.

Figure 6.

The graph shows the strength of evidence (as expressed by BF10) as N increases.

Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) Parameters

Broadly speaking, MCMC methods approximate the true posterior density p(θ | y) by constructing a Markov chain on the state space θ ∈ Θ. The probability of the subsequent state in a given chain can be defined as:

P(Xn+1=in+1|Xn=In), I  θ (2)

where {X0, X1,..} is a sequence of random variables and θ is the state space. Accordingly, the state at time step n + 1 is dependent only on the state at time n. This process is best represented with a random walk where each vertex is defined by θ, and weighted by the transition probabilities:

pij=P(Xn+1=j|Xn=i), i,jθ (3)

In the analyses reported in the paper, MCMC was used to generate posterior samples via the Metropolis-Hastings algorithm (see for details Rubinstein and Kroese, 2011). All analyses were set at 10,000 iterations, with diagnostic checks for convergence. One chain per analysis was used for all analyses reported in the paper, with a thinning interval of 1 (i.e., no iteration was discarded).

Frequentist analyses

We reported Bayesian analyses throughout the paper. Because we understand that some readers may wish to compare these results with the equivalent frequentist analyses, we are providing all of these herein, in the order of presentation in the paper. Note that an a priori power analysis based on previous studies (Erickson et al., 2013; Moreau et al., 2015) indicated the need for a minimum N of 129 participants per group to detect an effect of d = 0.35 on the primary outcome measures, with 1 – β = 0.80 and α = 0.05. The actual sample size of the present study (N = 152 and N = 153, for HIT and control groups, respectively) allowed an a priori power of. 86, given d and α constant.

  • ANCOVA on change in resting heart rate, with Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a fixed factor and baseline heart rate as a covariate. Main group effect: F(1, 301)=9.84, p=0.002, η2 = 0.02 (.00,. 08), Figure 1A. Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant: F(1, 302)=0.29, p=0.59.

  • Welch two-sample t-test on resting heart rate change, by Condition (HIT vs. Control), after split on resting heart rate at pretest: t(1, 146.4)=2.57, p=0.01. Mgain = 6.11, SDgain = 11.64 and Mgain = 1.89, SDgain = 8.63, for HIT and Control, respectively; Hedges’ g = 0.41 (0.09, 0.73).

  • Linear regressions showed sustained effort across training sessions (β110.04, p>0.05; Figure 1C), but incremental workout load (β118.81, p<0.01; Figure 1D).

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Cognitive Control scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 295)=7.17, p=0.008, η2 = 0.02 (.00,. 07). Participants in the HIT group showed larger improvements than controls from pretest to posttest (Mgain = 0.25, SDgain = 0.6 and Mgain = 0.08, SDgain = 0.47, respectively, Hedges’ g = 0.31 [0.09, 0.54]). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant: F(1, 295)=0.35, p=0.56 and F(1, 295)=0.82, p=0.37, at pretest and posttest respectively.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Working Memory scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 287)=21.89, p<0.001, η2 = 0.06 (.00,. 11). Participants in the HIT group showed larger improvements than controls from pretest to posttest (Mgain = 0.48, SDgain = 0.83 and Mgain = 0.12, SDgain = 0.44, respectively, Hedges’ g = 0.54 [0.31, 0.77]). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not significant at pretest: F(1, 287)=0.16, p=0.69, but was statistically significant at posttest: F(1, 287)=19.75, p<0.001.

  • Regression analysis on Cognitive Control gains with change in resting heart rate as a predictor: HIT, F(1, 149)=7.93, p<0.01, R2 = 0.05, RMSE = 0.58. Control group, F(1, 144)=0.24, p=0.63, R2 = 0.002, RMSE = 0.48.

  • Regression analysis on Working Memory gains with change in resting heart rate as a predictor: HIT, F(1, 137)=2.47, p=0.12, R2 = 0.02, RMSE = 0.82. Control group, F(1, 147)=0.08, p=0.77, R2 = 0.001 RMSE=0.44.

  • Regression analysis on Cognitive Control gains with baseline resting heart rate as a predictor (HIT group only): F(1, 149)=1.83, p=0.18, R2 = 0.01, RMSE = 0.59. When restricted to the. 75 quantile: F(1, 44)=0.37, p=0.54, R2 = 0.01, RMSE = 0.73.

  • Regression analysis on Working Memory gains with baseline resting heart rate as a predictor (HIT group only): F(1, 137)=0.02, p=0.90, R2 = 0, RMSE = 0.83. When restricted to the. 75 quantile: F(1, 40)=1.79, p=0.19, R2 = 0.04, RMSE = 0.40.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Cognitive Control scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and BDNF polymorphism (val vs. met) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 30)=12.73, p=0.001, η2 = 0.22 (.00,. 50). Met66 carriers benefited to a greater extent than non-carriers from the exercise intervention (respectively Mgain = 0.93, SDgain = 1.20 and Mgain = 0.05, SDgain = 0.13, Hedges’ g = 1.36 [0.52, 2.2]). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was statistically significant a pretest: F(1, 30)=25.20, p<0.001, but not at posttest F(1, 30)=2.16, p=0.15.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Working Memory scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and BDNF polymorphism (val vs. met) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 30)=23.01, p<0.001, η2 = 0.24 (.01,. 47). Met66 carriers benefited to a greater extent than non-carriers from the exercise intervention (respectively Mgain = 0.87, SDgain = 0.64 and Mgain = 0.14, SDgain = 0.24, Hedges’ g = 1.83 [0.94, 2.72]). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was statistically significant a pretest: F(1, 30)=14.48, p<0.001, but not at posttest F(1, 30)=0.33, p=0.57.

  • Welch two-sample t-test on Cognitive Control score at pretest, by BDNF polymorphism (val66 vs. met66): t(1, 8.3)=1.68, p=0.13, Hedges’ g = 1.01 (0.20, 1.81).

  • Welch two-sample t-test on Working Memory score at pretest, by BDNF polymorphism (val66 vs. met66): t(1, 8.7)=2.30, p<0.05, Hedges’ g = 1.31 (0.47, 2.14).

We present below analyses for each cognitive tasks included in this study. Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 6.

Table 6. Mean cognitive scores (SDs) for the two conditions at pretest and posttest.

Scores are scaled and centered for each task (z-transformed by row).

HIT Control
Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest
Flanker −0.14 (1.20) 0.16 (0.66) −0.06 (1.18) 0.04 (0.85)
Go/no-go −0.09 (1.11) 0.08 (0.96) 0.01 (1.04) 0.01 (0.88)
Stroop −0.11 (1.19) 0.16 (0.38) −0.09 (1.31) 0.04 (0.83)
Backward digit span −0.14 (1.07) 0.25 (0.55) −0.13 (1.34) 0.02 (0.82)
Backward Corsi blocks −0.13 (1.55) 0.31 (0.35) −0.17 (0.92) 0.00 (0.73)
Visual 2-back −0.24 (1.62) 0.33 (0.51) −0.06 (0.74) −0.03 (0.69)
  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Flanker scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 301)=4.00, p<.05, η2 = 0.01 (.00,. 05). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant: F(1, 301)=0.38, p=0.56 and F(1, 301)=1.14, p=0.29, at pretest and posttest respectively.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Go/no-go scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 301)=4.52, p=0.03, η2 = 0.01 (.00,. 05). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant: F(1, 297)=1.24, p=0.26 and F(1, 297)=0.02, p=0.88, at pretest and posttest respectively.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Stroop scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 301)=2.63, p=0.11, η2 <0.01 (.00,. 04). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant: F(1, 301)=0.05, p=0.83 and F(1, 301)=2.53, p=0.11, at pretest and posttest respectively.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Backward digit span scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 301)=5.66, p=0.02, η2 = 0.02 (.00,. 06). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant at pretest but was statistically significant at posttest: F(1, 301)=0.63, p=0.43 and F(1, 301)=6.41, p=0.01, respectively.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Backward Corsi blocks scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 297)=4.49, p=0.03, η2 = 0.01 (.00,. 05). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant at pretest but was statistically significant at posttest: F(1, 297)=0.07, p=0.79 and F(1, 297)=34.69, p<0.001, respectively.

  • Repeated measures ANOVA on Visual 2-back scores, with Session (pretest vs. posttest) as a within factor and Condition (HIT vs. Control) as a between factor. Interaction effect: F(1, 292)=16.22, p<0.001, η2 = 0.05 (.02,. 11). Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was not statistically significant at pretest but was statistically significant at posttest: F(1, 292)=2.27, p=0.13 and F(1, 292)=16.41, p<0.001, respectively.

Additional analyses

Here, we report analyses for which our a priori hypotheses were null effects. These variables were collected either to control for potential confounds, or for exploratory purposes.

There was no difference between groups regarding self-reported enjoyment or motivation (W = 12058, p=0.54 and W = 11497, p=0.86, respectively). This finding allows controlling for expectation effects, and thus stronger causal claims (Boot et al., 2013; Stothart et al., 2014). In addition, participants’ self-reported belief about cognitive malleability (i.e., mindset) indicated a statistically significant difference (p<0.03) in favor of the control group (M = 7.11, SD = 2.65 and M = 6.42, SD = 2.74, respectively, Hedges’ g = 0.26 [0.03, 0.48]). There was no statistically significant difference between groups at either time points (pretest or posttest) in terms of ethnic background, age, gender, handedness, height, weight, diagnosis of learning disorder, brain trauma or epileptic seizures, current or past enrolment in a remediation or a cognitive training program, English as first language, videogaming experience, physical exercise, self-reported happiness, sleep quality, or general health.

Discussion

The present study reported the first experimental evidence that HIT can elicit robust cognitive improvements in children. We confirmed the main hypothesis that exercise could induce gains in both cognitive control and working memory, as assessed from multiple measures. This finding is particularly promising given that the two constructs are reliable predictors of success in many domains, including professional and academic (Deary et al., 2007); in the classroom, cognitive control and working memory have been associated with effective learning and overall achievement (Rohde and Thompson, 2007). Importantly, these effects are also meaningful at the level of single tasks. Our main findings thus emphasize the potency of short but intense exercise interventions to enhance cognition, and suggests that aerobic exercise is not the sole means to elicit cognitive gains, in line with a growing body of research (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2012; Moreau et al., 2015; Pesce et al., 2016; Tomporowski et al., 2015b). HIT appears to be a viable and promising alternative to longer workouts to enhance cognition.

In addition to the main effect of training, we also postulated that specific genetic profiles would be correlated with different responses to training, with BDNF met66 carriers (i.e. met/met or val/met) benefiting from exercise to a greater extent. This hypothesis, confirmed by the present findings, was based on previous literature showing a relationship between BDNF polymorphism and serum BDNF (Lang et al., 2009), and the influence of exercise interventions on the latter (Leckie et al., 2014). BDNF met66 carriers showed greater gains from pretest to posttest on both cognitive constructs. As the substitution from valine to methionine at codon 66 typically results in decreases of activity-dependent secretion of BDNF at the synapse (Egan et al., 2003), BDNF met66 carriers are thought to be particularly impacted by post-exercise BDNF increases (Nascimento et al., 2015). Conversely, val66 homozygotes might benefit less from BDNF increases given above-average baseline levels. This finding is interesting because of its predictive power−controlling for BDNF polymorphism can allow more accurate forecasting of individual training responses, and better estimates of effect size. Given the current trend toward more personalized interventions (Medalia and Richardson, 2005; Moreau and Waldie, 2015), factoring in genetic information has the potential to refine and improve regimens, for each individual.

Importantly, the main effect of exercise on cognitive function is unlikely to be explained by the placebo effect. Self-reported measures of enjoyment and motivation did not differ between groups, supporting the notion that both types of intervention were equally appealing to children, or at least not fundamentally different with respect to intrinsic motivating factors. In addition, we also controlled for mindset—the degree to which individuals believe their cognitive abilities can change over time (Dweck et al., 1995). Previous research suggests that individual mindsets may be of influence in cognitive growths: individuals with a more ‘malleable’ mindset are thought to be more likely to improve in cognitive and academic domains than individuals with a ‘fixed’ mindset (Paunesku et al., 2015). Although we did find a difference between conditions, it was to the advantage of children in the control group, who held a more malleable view about the dynamic properties of cognitive function than did the HIT group. This finding was reported as an additional analysis in the Results section, rather than in the main section, because the effect was not hypothesized a priori. In any case, and with warranted caution regarding post hoc interpretation, this effect would suggest that controls were more likely to improve over time, an assumption that was not corroborated by our main finding. If anything, this strengthens our main claim—greater improvements in the HIT condition are inconsistent with a differential placebo effect, a critical point in light of recent findings in the field of cognitive training (Foroughi et al., 2016).

Effort-dependent variables provided additional insight about the mechanism underlying improvements. Consistent with previous interventional studies that have investigated the influence of exercise on cognition, we actively monitored several variables throughout the intervention. We could thus ensure that training was adequate, performed at a suitable intensity, and could test directly the dynamic coupling of these variables and their effects on cognitive outcomes. Indeed, we found that almost all participants stayed within the overall targeted range of effort required by the design of the workout and by initial individual measurements. This indicates a high degree of agreement, or fidelity, to the intended protocol—an essential component of the intervention given the underlying assumption that participants would exercise at a high intensity. Performed at a more moderate intensity, the same regimen becomes an aerobic training program, for which substantially longer time commitment might be required to elicit improvements. In addition, it is important to note that participants incrementally increased workout load, thus maintaining appropriate intensity throughout the intervention. Arguably, this adaptive property emerged from the design of the intervention, whereby participants were encouraged to exercise at maximum intensity at the time of the workout—an intrinsically individual and dynamic variable by definition.

In the present study, the effect of exercise could also be appreciated on changes in resting heart rate from pretest to posttest, a finding that confirms previous research in the field (e.g., Moreau et al., 2015). More specifically, exercise appears to be a valuable regulating mechanism, with elevated resting heart rate values likely to normalize as a result of HIT. This effect could not be attributed to regression toward the mean, as it was not found in controls, thus suggesting that the intervention is especially beneficial to individuals who need it most. Together with the finding that HIT benefited more individuals with a genetic polymorphism (BDNF met66 carriers) that was associated with lower cognitive performance, this idea emphasizes the relevance of exercise interventions to individuals with specific genetic or physiological attributes to reduce interindividual differences (Gómez-Pinilla et al., 2001; Leckie et al., 2014). Disparities are genuine, yet targeted interventions allow low-performing individuals to improve dramatically.

Our design allowed delving deeper into the relationship between exercise and cognitive improvements. In particular, we found that change in resting heart rate was predictive of cognitive control, but not working memory, gains. This finding is of interest because we postulated that the mechanisms of improvement were physiological, given that HIT has been shown to elicit neurophysiological changes similar to those following aerobic exercise regimens (Ferris et al., 2007). However, we should point out that baseline resting heart rate did not predict gains in cognitive control or working memory in the HIT group, emphasizing the inherent noise associated with the relationship between physiological and cognitive measures. This lack of clear association between both types of variables is fairly common in the literature, suggesting that the hypothesized mechanisms of improvements are difficult to elucidate (Moreau et al., 2015; Tsai et al., 2014). Potentially, this also suggests that other variables may be of importance, a question we attempted to address with additional measurements, in an exploratory manner (see Additional Analyses in the Results section).

Regardless of the strength of the evidence reported in the present study, one might question the genuine impact of such a short training regimen. Although perhaps counterintuitive, the extreme potency of short, intense bursts of exercise has come to light in recent years (Lucas et al., 2015; Rognmo et al., 2004). In a review of the impact of HIT on public health, Biddle and Batterham, 2015 go as far as to premise their entire argument on the idea that the cardio-metabolic health outcomes are not to be questioned—rather, their only concern was about whether or not such regimens can be widely implemented and sustained over time (Biddle and Batterham, 2015). In a similar vein, Costigan and colleagues concluded that HIT is a time-efficient approach for improving cardiorespiratory fitness in adolescent populations (Costigan et al., 2015). These are strong, unequivocal statements, which reflect current views in exercise physiology—HIT has tremendous health benefits, with little, if any, disadvantages.

More direct evidence for the impact of HIT on brain function comes from neurophysiological studies. In an experiment that directly assessed the impact of short bursts of exercise on BDNF levels, Ferris et al. found that exercise leads to BDNF increases, and that the magnitude of the increase is intensity-dependent (Ferris et al., 2007). This result emphasizes the importance of controlling exercise intensity in HIT studies, given that the main determinant of improvement appears to be the intensity of the workout. Indeed, previous studies have looked at the effect of short, but not intense, bursts of exercise on cognition, and found no clear evidence of improvement (Craft, 1983). The high-intensity component of this type of exercise regimen is intended to allow for higher workout intensity than traditional workouts, despite shorter overall volume. The brevity of exercise, on the other hand, is simply a byproduct of intensity—one cannot maintain a near-maximal exercise intensity for long periods of time, given that this type of regimen depletes energetic resources rapidly (Parolin et al., 1999). In terms of practical implications, this aspect is critical, as it allows designing shorter, more potent workouts.

Despite our findings being in line with previous literature showing that short bursts of exercise can elicit potent cognitive improvements in children (Piepmeier et al., 2015; Pontifex et al., 2013), and, more generally, with a wider, more general literature linking physical exercise and cognition in children (Jackson et al., 2016; Sibley and Etnier, 2003), we should also point out a few limitations of the present study. These open up interesting avenues and directions for future research. First, the duration of training was not experimentally manipulated, and therefore the specific question of dose-dependence was not directly assessed. Therefore, we cannot claim that a 6 week regimen is optimal—larger cognitive improvements could possibly be elicited with longer durations, or, alternatively, similar improvements could be induced with a shorter intervention. Similarly, no follow-up tests were performed to assess durability of improvements post-training; although it should be noted that maintaining benefits is arguably less important with short, potent interventions such as the HIT regimen we proposed. In addition, both the duration of the intervention and the specific experimental protocol were constrained by external factors (e.g. feasibility, academic schedule). With respect to the potency of the intervention, however, this is also promising—as little as six weeks of training can induce noticeable improvements, with possible larger effects if physical exercise is sustained.

Related to this idea, we had to work around constraints typically imposed by interventional studies; namely, the necessity to keep testing sessions time-efficient. Beyond logistic considerations, this was also intended to minimize the influence of cognitive fatigue on our results. Despite time constraints, we aimed to preserve testing diversity (i.e., number of tasks per construct) for a few reasons. First, we strived to provide estimates of constructs that minimize task-specific components and extract meaningful domain-general scores. In psychometric testing of working memory capacity, Foster and colleagues demonstrated that the majority of the variance explained by a single WM task is accounted for in the first few blocks, and that the predictive nature of the task remains largely unchanged for practical purposes when tasks are shortened (Foster et al., 2015). Second, simulation studies have shown that incorporating more tasks within constructs leads to a better signal-to-noise ratio, resulting in more meaningful measures of an underlying ability (Moreau et al., 2016). To further validate this approach, we piloted different versions of our testing tasks and determined that the validity of the versions we retained for the present study was acceptable, given appropriate reliability across task lengths.

Second, because training occurred in schools, the environment was possibly less standardized and controlled than laboratory settings, highlighting typical tradeoffs between impoverished but highly reliable environments and ecological but less controlled settings. Our view remains that ecological validity is of primary importance when training cognitive abilities, because of the wide range of applications stemming from this line of research (Moreau and Conway, 2014). Accordingly, fixed, predictable training regimens are unlikely to favor durable improvements of cognitive function (Moreau and Conway, 2014; Moreau et al., 2015; Posner et al., 2015). We have previously stressed the importance of novelty and variety in cognitive training interventions (Moreau and Conway, 2014), and this limitation applies to exercise regimens as well. Importantly, this echoes similar views across research groups worldwide, which suggest that training-induced cognitive improvements are often restricted to specific activities (Harrison et al., 2013; Tomporowski et al., 2012), and are best nurtured within complex, dynamic environments (Diamond and Lee, 2011). Therefore, the regimen we have presented in this paper constitutes a potent short-term intervention, but more variety might be required to elicit long-lasting improvements. In a field that has suffered from setbacks such as lack of replication (e.g. Redick et al., 2013; Thompson et al., 2013) or common methodological flaws (Moreau et al., 2016), it is wise to remain cautious about preliminary studies and emphasize the need for replication.

Despite these limitations, the present findings represent a promising first step toward reliable and affordable exercise-based cognitive interventions, highlighting effective alternatives to aerobic exercise. Together with complementary findings (e.g. Moreau et al., 2015), the type of physical exercise regimen we described in this paper could pave the way for novel exercise interventions particularly suited to school environments, which are often constrained by time and equipment. The high-intensity workout we designed did not require any special equipment or any instructors training, and each 10 min session was all-inclusive with warm-up and stretching. Therefore, this type of regimen could also be generalized to other populations; for instance, individuals whose schedule allows little time for exercise, or those who do not intrinsically enjoy exercising, could appreciate opportunities to shorten workouts while preserving the typical benefits of exercise. Adaptations to older populations also represent interesting opportunities considering the benefits typically associated with exercise regimens in these communities (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003). The present regimen might require adjustments, given that practicality in a specific context (e.g. managing time constraints in school or professional schedules) potentially differs from practical considerations in another (e.g. mitigating risks of injury in older populations). In any case, generalization is of the rationale, not necessarily of the specific workout that was designed for this intervention.

Finally, it is important to acknowledge that physical exercise, regardless of the specific training regimens considered, is not a panacea when it comes to addressing cognitive deficits−in some cases, especially in the presence of specific conditions or disorders, more targeted or individualized interventions might be required (e.g., Moreau and Waldie, 2015), and the ability for exercise regimens to remediate core cognitive deficits might appear inherently limited. However, it remains that physical exercise is one of the most potent and wide-ranging means currently available to enhance cognition non-invasively, with a myriad of positive side effects.

Materials and methods

We report here a multicenter, randomized (1:1 allocation), placebo-controlled trial. Design and reporting are consistent with CONSORT guidelines (http://www.consort-statement.org/). Participants, parents and school principals gave their informed consent for inclusion in this study, and the Ethics Committee at the University of Auckland approved all procedures. The full protocol and statistical analysis plan are available online at https://github.com/davidmoreau/2017_eLife.

Participants

A total of 318 children participated in this study. Thirteen participants were not included in the analyses because of dropouts (N = 7), extensive missing data (N = 4) or problems in data collection (N = 2, see CONSORT flow diagram for details). Our final sample consisted of 305 children (Mage = 9.9 (7–13), SDage = 1.74, 187 female, MBMI = 18.3, SDBMI = 6.26). They were recruited from six schools across New Zealand, providing a sample of various socioeconomic deciles (three public institutions, three private), locations (three urban institutions, three rural), and ethnic backgrounds representative for the country (70% New Zealand European, 20% Pacific, 7% Asian, 3% Other). The number of students involved per school ranged from 5 to 83 (M = 50.8, SD = 31). All participants reported no history of brain trauma or epilepsy, and all had self-reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. A subset of 22 children reported a learning disability diagnosis (dyslexia: 14, ADHD: 3, Autism spectrum disorder: 3, mild developmental delay: 2, Irlen syndrome: 2, dyscalculia: 1, dyspraxia: 1). Respective subsets of 284, 99 and 32 participants underwent all assessments, measurements and genotyping described below. All the variables measured in the experiment are reported hereafter.

Cognitive assessments

Testing was conducted on school premises. All cognitive assessments were computer-based, administered in groups of a maximum of 15 students. This limit on the number of participants tested at a given time was implemented to minimize potential averse effects of group testing. These assessments have shown to be adequate measures of both cognitive control and working memory (Anderson-Hanley et al., 2012; Aron and Poldrack, 2005; Kane et al., 2004; Nee et al., 2007; Pajonk et al., 2010; Rudebeck et al., 2012; Unsworth and Engle, 2007). For each task, we measured accuracy and response time. Different stochastic variations of all tasks were used at pretest and posttest. Unless specified otherwise, the number of trials varied based on individual performance to allow reaching asymptotes, with a minimum and a maximum specified for each task. The reliability of this method for each task was assessed from a separate sample (N = 34, Mage = 10.3 (8–12), 15 females), and deemed acceptable (all ρs > 0.65) based on Spearman-Brown prophecy formula (Brown, 1910; Spearman, 1910). Specifically, reliability was calculated by comparing test scores on the asymptotic version vs, the maximal-length version, for each task (see trial length details below and online repository for source code data). The order below was the order of presentation for every participant at both pretest and posttest (i.e., Flanker – Go/no-go – Stroop – Backward digit span – Backward Corsi blocks – Visual 2-back). Both testing sessions were scheduled at the same time of the day, and lasted approximately one hour.

Flanker

Participants viewed a series of arrows, either pointing to the left of the right of the screen. They were instructed to ignore all stimuli but the arrow at the center of the screen (target), and respond by pressing the left or right key when presented with arrows pointing left or right, respectively. For any given trial, the number of arrows displayed ranged from three to 25, with equal probability for congruent and incongruent trials. All sessions included 20 trials. We recorded accuracy and response time for both congruent and incongruent trials.

Go/no-go

Participants were presented with a series of circles, either uniform or patterned. The uniform circle required a key response (‘go’) whereas the other required no response (‘no-go’). If response was required, the stimulus remained visible indefinitely, until a response was made. When the stimulus required no response, it disappeared after 2000 ms. A self-paced button press triggered the start of the next trial. The interval from the button press to the presentation of the stimulus ranged from 500 ms to 2000 ms (randomly jittered). A session included between 12 and 40 trials.

Stroop

Participants were presented with a series of color words, in a colored font either congruent or incongruent, drawn with equal probability. They were instructed to attend to the color of the font, and to respond by pressing the key corresponding to the appropriate color on the keyboard. Stimuli remained visible until a response was made. A session included between 20 and 50 trials. We recorded accuracy and response time for both congruent and incongruent trials.

Backward digit span

Participants viewed a series of digits from 1 to 9 presented sequentially for 1000 ms, with 500 ms intertrial intervals. They were instructed to respond by entering the corresponding digits on the keyboard at the end of each trial. Hierarchical item randomization allowed the presentation of a maximum of two identical digits consecutively. For each trial, answers could be corrected until submitted. A self-paced button press triggered the start of the next trial. A session included 12 to 40 trials.

Backward Corsi blocks

Participants were presented with a series of locations on a block, sequentially for 1000 ms, with 500 ms intertrial intervals. They were instructed to respond by clicking on the corresponding locations at the end of each trial. Hierarchical item randomization did not allow presentation of identical locations consecutively. For each trial, no correction was allowed once submitted. A self-paced button press triggered the start of the next trial. A session included 12 to 40 trials.

Visual 2-back

Participants viewed a series of pictures presented sequentially for 2000 ms, with 500 ms intertrial intervals. They were instructed to press a key to signal a match, that is, two identical pictures interleaved with one stimulus in between (i.e., 2-back). No action was required in the absence of match. The number of matches ranged from 20 to 35 per session, randomized. A session included 40 to 70 trials.

Physiological measurements

Physiological measures were collected using FitbitChargeHRTM, powered by the MEMS tri-axial accelerometer. This multisensory wristband has shown adequate accuracy and reliability in previous studies for the measures of interest in the present study (e.g., de Zambotti et al., 2016). Measures included minutes of activity, calories burned, intensity, intensity range (sedentary, lightly active, fairly active, very active), steps and heart rate (measured by changes in blood volume using PurePulseTM LED lights).

Questionnaire

Participants provided information about the following: ethnic background, age, gender, handedness, height, weight, diagnosis of learning disorder, brain trauma or epileptic seizures, current or past enrolment in a remediation or a cognitive training program, and whether English was their first language. In addition, self-reported information was gathered to quantify videogaming and physical exercise habits (4-point Likert scale in both cases), as well as to evaluate overall health, happiness, sleep quality, and mindset (6-point Likert scale for each item). The latter was intended to capture beliefs about the malleability of cognitive ability in the context of schoolwork, that is, the extent to which students perceive academic achievement in a predominantly fixed or malleable manner (see for example Paunesku et al., 2015). All measures were collected prior to the intervention, but variables susceptible to change over time were reassessed post-intervention.

Genotyping

DNA collection was performed using Oragene-DNA Self-Collection kits, in a manner consistent with the manufacturer's instructions. DNA was subsequently extracted from all saliva samples according to a standardized procedure (Nishita et al., 2009). All resultant DNA samples were resuspended in Tris-EDTA buffer and were quantified used Nanodrop ND-1000 1-position spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA).

DNA samples were diluted to 50 ng/μL. A modified version of the method described by Erickson et al., 2008 was used for DNA amplification. Amplification was carried out on the 113 bp polymorphic BDNF fragment, using the primers BDNF-F 5-GAG GCT TGC CAT CAT TGG CT-3 and BDNF-R 5-CGT GTA CAA GTC TGC GTC CT-3. Polymerase chain reaction (PCR) was conducted using 10X Taq buffer (2.5 L μL), Taq polymerase (0.125 μL), dNTPs (5 nmol), primers (10 pmol each), Q solution (5 μL), and DNA (100 ng) made up to 25 μL with dH2O. The PCR conditions consisted of denaturation at 95°C for 15 min, 30 cycles on a ThermoCycler (involving denaturation at 94°C for 30 s, annealing at 60°C for 30 s, and extension at 72°C for 30 s) and a final extension at 72°C.

PCR product (6.5 μL) was incubated with Pm1l at 37°C overnight. The digestion products were analyzed using a high-resolution agarose gel (4%) with a Quick Load 100 bp ladder (BioLabs) and a GelPilot Loading Dye (QIAGEN). After immersion in an ethidium bromide solution for 10 min, DNA was visualized under ultraviolet light. Enzyme digestion resulted in a 113 bp fragment for the BDNF met66 allele, and 78 and 35 bp fragments for the val66 allele. This procedure is consistent with the one described by Erickson et al. (2008).

Intervention

Participants were randomly assigned to either an exercise group (N = 152) or a control (N = 153) group (see Table 7). Randomization was computer-based, generated in R (Core Team R, 2016) by one of the authors (D.M.). Group allocation was performed at the individual level. Testers were blind to group allocation.

Table 7. Demographics and sample characteristics at baseline.

HIT Controls Total
Sample (N)
Gender
152
90 f./62 m.
153
97 f./56 m.
305
187 f./118 m.
Age 9.87 (1.81) 9.96 (1.68) 9.91 (1.74)
Handedness (LH/Ambid.) 18/3 14/3 32/6
BMI  18.1 (3.92) 18.51 (7.89) 18.31 (6.25)
LD diagnosis 13 16 29
Previous remediation 8 14 22
Videogaming 2.32 (0.95) 2.43 (0.96) 2.38 (0.97)
Physical exercise 3.06 (0.8) 2.95 (0.78) 3.03 (0.81)
Happiness 4.53 (1.25) 4.61 (1.22) 4.55 (1.27)
Sleep quality 4.07 (1.36) 4.11 (1.39) 4.11 (1.41)
General health 4.88 (1.05) 4.84 (1.01) 4.82 (1.06)

The exercise intervention consisted of a high-intensity workout including the following: warm-up (2 min), short bursts (5 × 20 s, interleaved with incremental breaks (30 s, 40 s, 50 s, 60 s, and a shorter 20 s break after the last workout period), and stretching (2 min). The video-based workout did not require previous experience or knowledge, as it included basic fitness movements. All movements were designed so that participants could maintain their gaze fixed on the screen at all times. All instructions were provided both verbally (audio recording) and visually (on-screen captions). Complete details and script can be found in the online repository. A complete session lasted 10 min, and was scheduled every morning on weekdays. The control condition consisted of a blend of board games, computer games, and trivia quizzes, consistent with current recommendations regarding active control groups (Boot et al., 2013) and findings showing that aerobic exercise interventions typically do not differ from other regimens with respect to participants’ expectations (Stothart et al., 2014). Consistent with this assumption, self-reported feedback indicated no difference in enjoyment or motivation between conditions, and no difference in mindsets regarding cognitive malleability (Paunesku et al., 2015).

Frequency and duration were matched between conditions. The intervention was 6 weeks long, with five sessions per week, for a total of 30 sessions. This translates to 300 min of actual exercise. There was no difference between groups regarding the number of sessions completed (M = 29.05, SD = 1.63, overall). Due to the nature of the intervention, class size was limited to 20 participants in both conditions. Participants were supervised at all times, to ensure a high degree of fidelity to the intended protocol. Participants did not differ between groups in any of the self-reported measures described previously, which include physical exercise habits. Note that participants did not exercise on the days of pretest and posttest, to prevent acute effects of physical exercise on cognitive performance (see Tomporowski, 2003).

Acknowledgements

DM and KEW are supported by philanthropic donations from the Campus Link Foundation, the Kelliher Trust and Perpetual Guardian (as trustee of the Lady Alport Barker Trust). DM is also supported by the Neurological Foundation of New Zealand.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

David Moreau, Email: d.moreau@auckland.ac.nz.

Heidi Johansen-Berg, University of Oxford, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • Centre for Brain Research 9133-3706255 to David Moreau, Karen E Waldie.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing.

Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: We report here a multicenter, randomized (1:1 allocation), placebo-controlled trial. Design and reporting are consistent with CONSORT guidelines (http://www.consortstatement. org/). Participants, parents and school principals gave their informed consent for inclusion in this study, and the Ethics Committee at the University of Auckland approved all procedures (protocol #015078).

Additional files

Reporting standard 1. CONSORT flow diagram.
Reporting standard 2. CONSORT check list.
elife-25062-repstand2.pdf (686.5KB, pdf)

References

  1. Anderson-Hanley C, Arciero PJ, Brickman AM, Nimon JP, Okuma N, Westen SC, Merz ME, Pence BD, Woods JA, Kramer AF, Zimmerman EA. Exergaming and older adult cognition: a cluster randomized clinical trial. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2012;42:109–119. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aron AR, Poldrack RA. The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1285–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Auguie B. gridExtra: Functions in Grid Graphics. R package version 0.9.12012
  4. Benda NM, Seeger JP, Stevens GG, Hijmans-Kersten BT, van Dijk AP, Bellersen L, Lamfers EJ, Hopman MT, Thijssen DH. Effects of High-Intensity Interval training versus continuous training on physical fitness, cardiovascular function and quality of life in heart failure patients. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0141256. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Berchtold NC, Kesslak JP, Pike CJ, Adlard PA, Cotman CW. Estrogen and exercise interact to regulate brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA and protein expression in the Hippocampus. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;14:1992–2002. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01825.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Best JR, Chiu BK, Liang Hsu C, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Long-Term effects of resistance exercise training on cognition and brain volume in older women: results from a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2015;21:745–756. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Biddle SJ, Batterham AM. High-intensity interval exercise training for public health: a big HIT or shall we HIT it on the head? International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2015;12:95. doi: 10.1186/s12966-015-0254-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bliss TV, Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993;361:31–39. doi: 10.1038/361031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. The Journal of Physiology. 1973;232:331–356. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boot WR, Simons DJ, Stothart C, Stutts C. The Pervasive Problem with placebos in psychology: why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects. Perspectives on Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science. 2013;8:445–454. doi: 10.1177/1745691613491271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown W. Some experimental results in the correlation of mental abilities. British Journal of Psychology, 1904-1920. 1910;3:296–322. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1910.tb00207.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Castelli DM, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Erwin HE. Physical fitness and academic achievement in third- and fifth-grade students. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2007;29:239–252. doi: 10.1123/jsep.29.2.239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Holtrop JL, Voss MW, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. Aerobic fitness is associated with greater white matter integrity in children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:584. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Champely S. Pwr: Basic Functions for Power Analysis. R package version 1.1-22015
  15. Chrysohoou C, Angelis A, Tsitsinakis G, Spetsioti S, Nasis I, Tsiachris D, Rapakoulias P, Pitsavos C, Koulouris NG, Vogiatzis I, Dimitris T. Cardiovascular effects of high-intensity interval aerobic training combined with strength exercise in patients with chronic heart failure. A randomized phase III clinical trial. International Journal of Cardiology. 2015;179:269–274. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2014.11.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Coe DP, Pivarnik JM, Womack CJ, Reeves MJ, Malina RM. Effect of physical education and activity levels on academic achievement in children. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2006;38:1515–1519. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000227537.13175.1b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Science. 2003;14:125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Core Team R . R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  19. Costigan SA, Eather N, Plotnikoff RC, Taaffe DR, Lubans DR. High-intensity interval training for improving health-related fitness in adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2015;49:1253–1261. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2014-094490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cotman CW, Berchtold NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences. 2002;25:295–301. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02143-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Craft DH. Effect of prior exercise on cognitive performance tasks by hyperactive and normal young boys. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1983;56:979–982. doi: 10.2466/pms.1983.56.3.979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, Boyle CA, Waller JL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA, Gregoski M. Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children's cognitive functioning: a randomized controlled trial. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2007;78:510–519. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2007.10599450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, McDowell JE, Austin BP, Miller PH, Yanasak NE, Allison JD, Naglieri JA. Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychology. 2011;30:91–98. doi: 10.1037/a0021766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. de Zambotti M, Baker FC, Willoughby AR, Godino JG, Wing D, Patrick K, Colrain IM. Measures of sleep and cardiac functioning during sleep using a multi-sensory commercially-available wristband in adolescents. Physiology & Behavior. 2016;158:143–149. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Deary IJ, Strand S, Smith P, Fernandes C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence. 2007;35:13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333:959–964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, Lambourne K, Szabo-Reed AN. Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2016;48:1197–1222. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dweck CS, Chiu C-yue, Hong Y-yi. Implicit theories and their role in Judgments and reactions: a word from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry. 1995;6:267–285. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0604_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Bertolino A, Zaitsev E, Gold B, Goldman D, Dean M, Lu B, Weinberger DR. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell. 2003;112:257–269. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00035-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Erickson KI, Kim JS, Suever BL, Voss MW, Francis BM, Kramer AF. Genetic contributions to age-related decline in executive function: a 10-year longitudinal study of COMT and BDNF polymorphisms. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2008;2:11. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.011.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Chaddock L, Hu L, Morris KS, White SM, Wójcicki TR, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus. 2009;19:1030–1039. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey E, Vieira VJ, Martin SA, Pence BD, Woods JA, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. PNAS. 2011;108:3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Erickson KI, Banducci SE, Weinstein AM, Macdonald AW, Ferrell RE, Halder I, Flory JD, Manuck SB. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism moderates an effect of physical activity on working memory performance. Psychological Science. 2013;24:1770–1779. doi: 10.1177/0956797613480367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Erickson KI, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. Physical activity, brain, and cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;4:27–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Etnier JL, Nowell PM, Landers DM, Sibley BA. A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Research Reviews. 2006;52:119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fabre C, Chamari K, Mucci P, Massé-Biron J, Préfaut C. Improvement of cognitive function by mental and/or individualized aerobic training in healthy elderly subjects. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 2002;23:415–421. doi: 10.1055/s-2002-33735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ferris LT, Williams JS, Shen CL. The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2007;39:728–734. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31802f04c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Foroughi CK, Monfort SS, Paczynski M, McKnight PE, Greenwood PM. Placebo effects in cognitive training. PNAS. 2016;113:7470–7474. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601243113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Foster JL, Shipstead Z, Harrison TL, Hicks KL, Redick TS, Engle RW. Shortened complex span tasks can reliably measure working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition. 2015;43:226–236. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0461-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Fox J, Weisberg S. An R Companion to Applied Regression. California: Sage; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  41. Gayda M, Ribeiro PA, Juneau M, Nigam A. Comparison of different forms of exercise training in patients with cardiac disease: where does High-Intensity Interval Training Fit? Canadian Journal of Cardiology. 2016;32:485–494. doi: 10.1016/j.cjca.2016.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Goekint M, De Pauw K, Roelands B, Njemini R, Bautmans I, Mets T, Meeusen R. Strength training does not influence serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2010;110:285–293. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1461-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gómez-Pinilla F, Ying Z, Opazo P, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. Differential regulation by exercise of BDNF and NT-3 in rat spinal cord and skeletal muscle. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;13:1078–1084. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01484.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gomez-Pinilla F, Hillman C. Comprehensive Physiology. Vol. 3. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2013. The Influence of Exercise on Cognitive Abilities; pp. 403–428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gould E, Beylin A, Tanapat P, Reeves A, Shors TJ. Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:260–265. doi: 10.1038/6365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gray SR, Ferguson C, Birch K, Forrest LJ, Gill JM. High-intensity interval training: key data needed to bridge the gap from laboratory to public health policy. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2016;50:1231–1232. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Güldenpenning I, Koester D, Kunde W, Weigelt M, Schack T. Motor expertise modulates the unconscious processing of human body postures. Experimental Brain Research. 2011;213:383–391. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2788-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hampshire A, Highfield RR, Parkin BL, Owen AM. Fractionating human intelligence. Neuron. 2012;76:1225–1237. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Harrison TL, Shipstead Z, Hicks KL, Hambrick DZ, Redick TS, Engle RW. Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychological Science. 2013;24:2409–2419. doi: 10.1177/0956797613492984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Heyn P, Abreu BC, Ottenbacher KJ. The effects of exercise training on elderly persons with cognitive impairment and dementia: a meta-analysis. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2004;85:1694–1704. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jackson WM, Davis N, Sands SA, Whittington RA, Sun LS. Physical activity and cognitive development. Journal of Neurosurgical Anesthesiology. 2016;28:373–380. doi: 10.1097/ANA.0000000000000349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. JASP Team JASP. 0.8.0.02016
  54. Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, Engle RW. The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133:189–217. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kesslak JP, So V, Choi J, Cotman CW, Gomez-Pinilla F. Learning upregulates brain-derived neurotrophic factor messenger ribonucleic acid: a mechanism to facilitate encoding and circuit maintenance? Behavioral Neuroscience. 1998;112:1012–1019. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.4.1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lang UE, Hellweg R, Sander T, Gallinat J. The Met allele of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is associated with increased BDNF serum concentrations. Molecular Psychiatry. 2009;14:120–122. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, Greenop KR, Almeida OP. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300:1027. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.9.1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Leckie RL, Oberlin LE, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Szabo-Reed A, Chaddock-Heyman L, Phillips SM, Gothe NP, Mailey E, Vieira-Potter VJ, Martin SA, Pence BD, Lin M, Parasuraman R, Greenwood PM, Fryxell KJ, Woods JA, McAuley E, Kramer AF, Erickson KI. BDNF mediates improvements in executive function following a 1-year exercise intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:985. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Voss MW, Khan KM, Handy TC. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:1690–1698. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lucas SJ, Cotter JD, Brassard P, Bailey DM. High-intensity interval exercise and cerebrovascular health: curiosity, cause, and consequence. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2015;35:902–911. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2015.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Makizako H, Liu-Ambrose T, Shimada H, Doi T, Park H, Tsutsumimoto K, Uemura K, Suzuki T. Moderate-intensity physical activity, hippocampal volume, and memory in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2015;70:480–486. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. McArdle WD, Katch F, Katch VL. Essentials of Exercise Physiology. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  63. Medalia A, Richardson R. What predicts a good response to cognitive remediation interventions? Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2005;31:942–953. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Milanović Z, Sporiš G, Weston M. Effectiveness of high-intensity Interval training (HIT) and continuous endurance training for VO2max improvements: a systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled trials. Sports Medicine. 2015;45:1469–1481. doi: 10.1007/s40279-015-0365-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Mishra J, de Villers-Sidani E, Merzenich M, Gazzaley A. Adaptive training diminishes distractibility in aging across species. Neuron. 2014;84:1091–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Moreau D. The role of motor processes in three-dimensional mental rotation: shaping cognitive processing via sensorimotor experience. Learning and Individual Differences. 2012;22:354–359. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.02.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Moreau D. Motor expertise modulates movement processing in working memory. Acta Psychologica. 2013;142:356–361. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Moreau D, Morrison AB, Conway AR. An ecological approach to cognitive enhancement: complex motor training. Acta Psychologica. 2015;157:44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Moreau D, Kirk IJ, Waldie KE. Seven Pervasive Statistical flaws in cognitive training interventions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016;10:153. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Moreau D, Conway ARA. Cognitive enhancement: a comparative review of computerized and athletic training programs. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2013;6:155–183. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2012.758763. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Moreau D, Conway AR. The case for an ecological approach to cognitive training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18:334–336. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Moreau D, Waldie KE. Developmental Learning disorders: from generic interventions to individualized remediation. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:2053. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Morey RD, Rouder JN. BayesFactor: computation of Bayes factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.12-22015
  74. Nascimento CM, Pereira JR, Pires de Andrade L, Garuffi M, Ayan C, Kerr DS, Talib LL, Cominetti MR, Stella F. Physical exercise improves peripheral BDNF levels and cognitive functions in mild cognitive impairment elderly with different bdnf val66Met genotypes. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease : JAD. 2015;43:81–91. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Navarro DJ. Learning Statistics with R: A Tutorial for Psychology Students and Other Beginners. Adelaide, Australia: University of Adelaide; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  76. Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J. Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;7:1–17. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Nishita DM, Jack LM, McElroy M, McClure JB, Richards J, Swan GE, Bergen AW. Clinical trial participant characteristics and saliva and DNA metrics. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2009;9:71. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-9-71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Pajonk FG, Wobrock T, Gruber O, Scherk H, Berner D, Kaizl I, Kierer A, Müller S, Oest M, Meyer T, Backens M, Schneider-Axmann T, Thornton AE, Honer WG, Falkai P. Hippocampal plasticity in response to exercise in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:133–143. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Parolin ML, Chesley A, Matsos MP, Spriet LL, Jones NL, Heigenhauser GJ. Regulation of skeletal muscle glycogen phosphorylase and PDH during maximal intermittent exercise. The American Journal of Physiology. 1999;277:E890–E900. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1999.277.5.E890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Paunesku D, Walton GM, Romero C, Smith EN, Yeager DS, Dweck CS. Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychological Science. 2015;26:784–793. doi: 10.1177/0956797615571017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Pesce C, Masci I, Marchetti R, Vazou S, Sääkslahti A, Tomporowski PD. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: mediated and moderated effects. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:349. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Piepmeier AT, Shih C-H, Whedon M, Williams LM, Davis ME, Henning DA, Park S, Calkins SD, Etnier JL. The effect of acute exercise on cognitive performance in children with and without ADHD. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2015;4:97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2014.11.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Plummer M. Rjags: Bayesian Graphical Models Using MCMC 2016
  84. Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Johnson CR, Chaddock L, Voss MW, Cohen NJ, Kramer AF, Hillman CH. Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:1332–1345. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Pontifex MB, Saliba BJ, Raine LB, Picchietti DL, Hillman CH. Exercise improves behavioral, neurocognitive, and scholastic performance in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2013;162:543–551. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.08.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Tang Y-Y. Enhancing attention through training. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;4:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.12.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Redick TS, Shipstead Z, Harrison TL, Hicks KL, Fried DE, Hambrick DZ, Kane MJ, Engle RW. No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2013;142:359–379. doi: 10.1037/a0029082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Revelle W. Northwestern University; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  89. Rognmo Ø, Hetland E, Helgerud J, Hoff J, Slørdahl SA. High intensity aerobic interval exercise is superior to moderate intensity exercise for increasing aerobic capacity in patients with coronary artery disease. European Journal of Cardiovascular Prevention & Prevention and Cardiac Rehabilitation and Exercise Physiology. 2004;11:216–222. doi: 10.1097/01.hjr.0000131677.96762.0c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Rohde TE, Thompson LA. Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability. Intelligence. 2007;35:83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.05.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Rubinstein RY, Kroese DP. Simulation and the Monte Carlo Method. John Wiley & Sons; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  92. Rudebeck SR, Bor D, Ormond A, O'Reilly JX, Lee AC. A potential spatial working memory training task to improve both episodic memory and fluid intelligence. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e50431. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Shors TJ, Miesegaes G, Beylin A, Zhao M, Rydel T, Gould E. Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature. 2001;410:372–376. doi: 10.1038/35066584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in Children: a Meta-Analysis. Pediatric Exercise Science. 2003;15:243–256. doi: 10.1123/pes.15.3.243. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Smallwood N, Spriggs MJ, Thompson CS, Wu CC, Hamm JP, Moreau D, Kirk IJ. Influence of physical activity on human sensory long-term potentiation. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2015;21:831–840. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715001095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Sniehotta FF, Scholz U, Schwarzer R. Action plans and coping plans for physical exercise: A longitudinal intervention study in cardiac rehabilitation. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2006;11:23–37. doi: 10.1348/135910705X43804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sofi F, Valecchi D, Bacci D, Abbate R, Gensini GF, Casini A, Macchi C. Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2011;269:107–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2010.02281.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Spearman C. Correlation calculated from faulty data. British Journal of Psychology, 1904-1920. 1910;3:271–295. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1910.tb00206.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Stothart CR, Simons DJ, Boot WR, Kramer AF. Is the effect of aerobic exercise on cognition a placebo effect? PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e109557. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Strong WB, Malina RM, Blimkie CJ, Daniels SR, Dishman RK, Gutin B, Hergenroeder AC, Must A, Nixon PA, Pivarnik JM, Rowland T, Trost S, Trudeau F. Evidence based physical activity for school-age youth. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2005;146:732–737. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.01.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Thomas AG, Dennis A, Bandettini PA, Johansen-Berg H. The effects of aerobic activity on brain structure. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3:86. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Thompson TW, Waskom ML, Garel KL, Cardenas-Iniguez C, Reynolds GO, Winter R, Chang P, Pollard K, Lala N, Alvarez GA, Gabrieli JD. Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e63614. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Tomporowski PD. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychologica. 2003;112:297–324. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Tomporowski PD, Naglieri JA, Lambourne K. Exercise psychology and children’s intelligence. In: Acevedo E. O, editor. Oxford Handbook of Exercise Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 409–429. [Google Scholar]
  105. Tomporowski PD, McCullick B, Pendleton DM, Pesce C. Exercise and children's cognition: The role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2015a;4:47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2014.09.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  106. Tomporowski PD, McCullick B, Pesce C. Enhancing Children’s Cognition with Physical Activity Games. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2015b. [Google Scholar]
  107. Tsai CL, Wang CH, Tseng YT. Effects of exercise intervention on event-related potential and task performance indices of attention networks in children with developmental coordination disorder. Brain and Cognition. 2012;79:12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Tsai CL, Chen FC, Pan CY, Wang CH, Huang TH, Chen TC. Impact of acute aerobic exercise and cardiorespiratory fitness on visuospatial attention performance and serum BDNF levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;41:121–131. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Unsworth N, Engle RW. On the division of short-term and working memory: an examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:1038–1066. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. van Uffelen JG, Chin A Paw MJ, Hopman-Rock M, van Mechelen W. The effect of walking and vitamin B supplementation on quality of life in community-dwelling adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized, controlled trial. Quality of Life Research : An International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care and Rehabilitation. 2007;16:1137–1146. doi: 10.1007/s11136-007-9219-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Vidoni ED, Johnson DK, Morris JK, Van Sciver A, Greer CS, Billinger SA, Donnelly JE, Burns JM. Dose-Response of aerobic exercise on cognition: a community-based, pilot randomized controlled trial. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0131647. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Voss MW, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2011;111:1505–1513. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Alves H, Chaddock L, Szabo AN, Mailey EL, Wójcicki TR, White SM, Gothe N, McAuley E, Sutton BP, Kramer AF. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34:2972–2985. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Weinstein AM, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Chaddock L, Szabo A, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey E, McAuley E, Kramer AF, Erickson KI. The association between aerobic fitness and executive function is mediated by prefrontal cortex volume. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2012;26:811–819. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wickham H. Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York: Springer; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  116. Wickham H. The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. Journal of Statistical Software. 2011;40:1–29. doi: 10.18637/jss.v040.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  117. Young J, Angevaren M, Rusted J, Tabet N. Aerobic exercise to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2015;4:CD005381. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005381.pub4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter


In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Thank you for submitting your article "High-intensity Training Enhances Executive Function in Children" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Richard Ivry (Senior Editor) and three reviewers, one of whom, Heidi Johansen-Berg (Reviewer #1), is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Phillip Tomporowski (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

The reviewers found the topic interesting, found much value in the study and found the paper to be well written. They were impressed by the large sample and thorough statistical treatment of the data. However, some key methodological details were missing, making it impossible to judge the paper in its current form. Given the importance of the topic, the reviewers agreed to invite a revision to clarify these issues so that they could then take a decision on whether the study meets the standards required for publication in eLife.

Essential revisions:

1) The study design lacks details. Details are needed on how many schools were involved, how many children per school, how children/schools were randomised by condition and whether the statistical analyses take clustering effects (of school) into account.

2) The methods employed to measure children's cognitive performance lack sufficient detail. How were the tests administered; individually or group?

What was the testing environment?

Were the tests administered in the same order?

Could the authors comment on the role of proactive and retroactive interference?

There were a number of tests. What was the over-all duration of testing?

Could the authors comment on the possibility of changes in level of children's motivation? While an activity-control group was employed in this experiment, it clearly does not mean that the children are blinded to conditions, raising the possibility of expectancy effects altering children's motivation to perform during post testing. The authors attempt to address this issue (subsection “Intervention”). However, their assumptions concerning the motivational effects of exercise vs. non-exercise conditions were drawn from studies conducted with adults (Stothardt et al., 2014) or high school students (Paunesku, et al., 2015). There are clear developmental differences between the expectancies of 9-year old children's and those of adults.

Please justify the use of different number of trials based on individual asymptotes during each task, given that this approach is rarely used in the field.

Also, could the small number of trials (i.e. only 20 trials for flanker task, a minimum 12 trials for go/no go and working memory tasks) generate valid measure of cognitive control and working memory? Did each individual participant perform the same number of trials at pre- and post-test? If different numbers of trials were used, does such difference correlate with the performance change?

3) The exercise intervention lacks detail. Given the focus of the research is an implicit comparison between aerobic and HIT, it would seem important to detail the exercise regimen. Who instructed the children?

What was the average class size?

What instruction was provided?

What are "core" exercises?

Is practice required to perform these core exercises?

How were exercises timed?

What type of feedback was provided by instructors?

Were fidelity measures included in the training protocol? Without this information, it would be very difficult to replicate this experiment.

Also, the study used 80% of heart rate reserve (HRR) as the target HR during HIT workout. As far as we can tell, Figure 1C shows that the maximum HR during each workout appears to be approximately 140-150 in BPM, which is less likely to be equal to exercise intensity at 80% HRR. Instead, the HR data would suggest exercise intensity at light-to-moderate intensity in this age group according to the classification of physical activity intensity (ACSM, 2014). Please explain. Details of the HIT workout may also help explain why HR during the HIT workout did not meet the desired intensity.

4) As all data presented graphically are change scores it would be helpful to include summary scores on cognitive tests and fitness measures at for each timepoint so the reader can get a feel for how the children are performing on these tasks and what the differences translate to in terms of the raw scores.

5) Despite the significant findings, some readers may find it implausible that such a small amount of additional activity over a few weeks results in changes in cognitive performance. It would be helpful for the authors to say more about why this might be expected. Is there a threshold effect, for example, such that a small amount of additional activity may reach threshold for cognitive benefits?

It would also be helpful if they could provide data to support how much more daily/weekly energy expenditure the HIT intervention would deliver compared to the control activities. Such information regarding actual exercise volume/time (i.e. calories or 300 minutes) achieved over 6 weeks may be added to the limitation section, but meanwhile this information could also be used to suggest how much physical activity this intervention could contribute to the minimum daily/weekly recommendation for health benefit.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "High-intensity Training Enhances Executive Function in Children" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by a Senior Editor, a Reviewing Editor and three reviewers.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below:

Essential revisions:

We are sorry for that it has taken some time to review the manuscript. In addition to consulting with the original reviewers we also sought input from a statistical reviewer to assess specifically the degree to which the study complies with the CONSORT guidelines for reporting of randomised controlled trials. Some changes are required to meet those guidelines:

In general, the reviewers agreed that transparency for this study is good, all the analysis scripts are made available through GitHUB. The randomisation procedure is clear and blinding has been considered.

Though the CONSORT checklist is reported, the paper has not been written in the spirit of the guidelines. This is a randomised trial where the effect of an intervention was compared to a control intervention for cognitive improvement in children. While the trial design is mentioned on the first page, CONSORT requires the trial be identified as randomised in the title. The Abstract should then focus on the design of the trial rather than be written in a narrative style.

Also the key result with point estimate and confidence interval needs to be presented in the Abstract. I realise that this is not the usual style for eLife but some concession should be made so that the CONSORT requirements are met.

CONSORT requires the primary and secondary outcomes to be specified. This report does not state which outcome is the primary outcome (usually this is the measurement that the sample size calculation was calculated for). The sample size statement in the protocol states 129 per group are required to detect an effect size of 0.35 with power of 80% at 5% significance level. This suggests that the study is powered for a single primary research question, but a primary outcome is not listed instead there are many outcomes listed under several sections. So it is not clear which outcome the difference of 0.35 relates to. While the statistical analysis proposed is Bayesian the arguments over repeated testing still hold if you want to present evidence that the intervention does improve outcomes. Results are frequently presented with statistical hypothesis tests and p-values, but the term 'statistically significant' is often abbreviated to 'significant' which is to be avoided. The authors need to be careful to always present the weight of the evidence in a p-value and also report a corresponding effect size with 95% confidence interval.

As a single primary outcome was not specified a priori it would be helpful if the Abstract communicated the size of the trial and the number of outcomes that were considered in the full protocol, so the statistical significance can be put in context.

eLife. 2017 Aug 22;6:e25062. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25062.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The study design lacks details. Details are needed on how many schools were involved, how many children per school, how children/schools were randomised by condition and whether the statistical analyses take clustering effects (of school) into account.

More information about the number of schools involved and characteristics of the participants have been added in the Participants section. Of particular relevance here:

“They were recruited from six schools across New Zealand, providing a sample of various socioeconomic deciles (three public institutions, three private), locations (three urban institutions, three rural), and ethnic backgrounds representative for the country (70% New Zealand European, 20% Pacific, 7% Asian, 3% Other). The number of students involved per school ranged from 5 to 83 (M = 50.8, SD = 31).”

Concerning randomization, we have added the sentence: “Group allocation was performed at the individual level”, to make it explicit that this was not a cluster randomized trial, but a trial in which every child was assigned to a group randomly.

More generally, details about the intervention have been added to the Materials and methods section and can be found with the source code we have provided (especially HIT_workout_script.txt). We discuss these thoroughly hereafter.

2) The methods employed to measure children's cognitive performance lack sufficient detail. How were the tests administered; individually or group?

What was the testing environment?

This information has been added to the Cognitive Assessments section:

“Testing was conducted on school premises. All cognitive assessments were computer-based, administered in groups of a maximum of 15 students. This limit on the number of participants tested at a given time was implemented to minimize potential averse effects of group testing.”

Were the tests administered in the same order?

In the original manuscript, we stated: “The order below was the order of presentation for every participant at both pretest and posttest. Both testing sessions were scheduled at the same time of the day.” To ensure that this is explicit, we have added the order of presentation in parentheses. This passage now reads: “The order below was the order of presentation for every participant at both pretest and posttest (i.e., Flanker – Go/no-go – Stroop – Backward digit span – Backward Corsi blocks – Visual 2-back).”

Could the authors comment on the role of proactive and retroactive interference?

Because this comment comes immediately after the question about the order of tasks, we assumed that it was in relation to proactive and retroactive interference between tasks. The design was fixed with regard to tasks order, as it was more suitable to the hypotheses of the study. More specifically, we opted for a fixed order for the following reasons:

1) The main problem related to order effects is that it can confound treatment effects if the treatment and control conditions present problems in different orders. A fixed order solves most of the related problems;

2) Couterbalancing is important to establish the separate validity of each of the tasks, whereas we were interested in generating constructs to be compared across time, rather than comparing tasks with each other within a time point. In addition, counterbalancing is critical when a limited number of version for a given test exist a priori; in contrast, all our tasks when randomly generated;

3) Finally, with six tasks the number of permutations is 6! = 720. Even Latin Square designs fail to provide appropriate solutions, and introduce additional confounds.

For the reasons stated above, we opted for a fixed-order design, consistent with traditional methods in psychometric testing.

There were a number of tests. What was the over-all duration of testing?

We have added an estimate of duration for the cognitive testing session in the Materials and methods section: “Both testing sessions were scheduled at the same time of the day, and lasted approximately one hour.” As an aside, cognitive testing was reduced to the minimum possible without threatening tests validity (see our comment on the number of trials below).

Could the authors comment on the possibility of changes in level of children's motivation? While an activity-control group was employed in this experiment, it clearly does not mean that the children are blinded to conditions, raising the possibility of expectancy effects altering children's motivation to perform during post testing. The authors attempt to address this issue (subsection “Intervention”). However, their assumptions concerning the motivational effects of exercise vs. non-exercise conditions were drawn from studies conducted with adults (Stothardt et al., 2014) or high school students (Paunesku, et al., 2015). There are clear developmental differences between the expectancies of 9-year old children's and those of adults.

This is an excellent point, one that needs to be accounted for in any training design. We measured self-reported enjoyment and motivation after the intervention, and compared these between groups. This was initially reported in the Intervention section, but this is technically a finding that better belongs to the Results section, where it has now been edited:

“There was no difference between groups regarding self-reported enjoyment or motivation (W = 12058, p =.54 and W = 11497, p =.86, respectively). This finding allows controlling for expectation effects, and thus stronger causal claims (Boot, Simons, Stothart, and Stutts, 2013; Stothart, Simons, Boot, and Kramer, 2014)

This is in addition to the significant difference in terms of malleability in favor of the Control group, which suggests a potential for greater improvements in Controls:

“In addition, participants’ self-reported belief about cognitive malleability (i.e., mindset) indicated a significant difference (p <.03) in favor of the control group (M = 7.11, SD = 2.65 and M = 6.42, SD = 2.74, respectively).”

This further strengthens our main claim, namely that HIT elicits greater cognitive improvements than the control intervention, despite a more fixed mindset. We do not speculate on this effect in the main text of the paper, however, given that we did not hypothesize the effect a priori.

Please justify the use of different number of trials based on individual asymptotes during each task, given that this approach is rarely used in the field.

Also, could the small number of trials (i.e. only 20 trials for flanker task, a minimum 12 trials for go/no go and working memory tasks) generate valid measure of cognitive control and working memory? Did each individual participant perform the same number of trials at pre- and post-test? If different numbers of trials were used, does such difference correlate with the performance change?

We made sure that reliability was adequate for the versions we proposed in the present experiment, via validation on an independent data set (Spearman-Brown prophecy formula). Trial length did not correlate with performance; information about reliability has been added to the Materials and methods section, to justify using the cognitive tasks in this manner:

“Unless specified otherwise, the number of trials varied based on individual performance to allow reaching asymptotes, with a minimum and a maximum specified for each task. […] Specifically, reliability was calculated by comparing test scores on the asymptotic version vs., the maximal-length version, for each task (see trial length details below and online repository for source code data).”

We corroborate this view with additional literature in the Discussion section:

“Despite time constraints, we aimed to preserve testing diversity (i.e., number of tasks per construct) for a few reasons. First, we strived to provide estimates of constructs that minimize task-specific components and extract meaningful domain-general scores. […] To further validate this approach, we piloted different versions of our testing tasks and determined that the validity of the versions we retained for the present study was acceptable, given appropriate reliability across task lengths.”

Finally, we included to the linked online repository the file Reliability_indices.csv, which contains all indices across tasks (subsection “Cognitive Assessments”).

3) The exercise intervention lacks detail. Given the focus of the research is an implicit comparison between aerobic and HIT, it would seem important to detail the exercise regimen. Who instructed the children?

This information has been added to the Intervention section:

“All instructions were provided both verbally (audio recording) and visually (on-screen captions). Complete details and script can be found in the online repository.”

What was the average class size?

This information has been added to the Intervention section:

“Due to the nature of the intervention, class size was limited to 20 participants in both conditions.”

What instruction was provided?

The complete script of the HIT workout is freely available online (https://github.com/davidmoreau/HIT). The file HIT_workout_script.txt includes all instructions and triggers of the video.

What are "core" exercises?

In the initial version of the manuscript, we used the terminology “core” of the session to describe the high-intensity part of the training. For clarity, this term has now been replaced by the more explicit “short bursts”, which we believe describes more accurately this part of the protocol.

Is practice required to perform these core exercises?

This is an important aspect of the workout that was not emphasized enough in the initial version of the manuscript. We have now added:

“The video-based workout did not require previous experience or knowledge, as it included basic fitness movements. […] All instructions were provided both verbally and on-screen. Complete details and script can be found in the online repository.”

For the interested reader, and for the purpose of replicability, all instructions and second-by-second details of the workout can be found in the online repository, as mentioned:

“R code and data are freely available on GitHub (https://github.com/davidmoreau). The repository includes data sets, R scripts, details and script of the HIT workout, the CONSORT flow diagram and the CONSORT checklist.”

How were exercises timed?

Timing was maintained via screen-based instructions, and ensured by adult supervision. We made this explicit in the manuscript with the following: “Participants were supervised at all times, to ensure a high degree of fidelity to the intended protocol”. Adequate fidelity to the protocol was also ensured post-session with the minutes of activity measure provided by activity trackers.

What type of feedback was provided by instructors?

No feedback was provided by the instructors, to avoid introducing confounds in the intervention. This was meant to allow more rigorous matching between conditions (HIT vs. control).

Were fidelity measures included in the training protocol? Without this information, it would be very difficult to replicate this experiment.

Fidelity to the intended protocol was fundamental to the present experiment, given that the rationale is about intensity of physical exercise. We reiterate this point in the Discussion:

“This indicates a high degree of agreement, or fidelity, to the intended protocol—an essential component of the intervention given the underlying assumption that participants would exercise at a high intensity. […] Arguably, this adaptive property emerged from the design of the intervention, whereby participants were encouraged to exercise at maximum intensity at the time of the workout—an intrinsically individual and dynamic variable by definition.”

More specifically, this was the rationale for monitoring exercise via activity trackers, and for presenting the accuracy ratio, a measure of fidelity to the protocol (see figure below). To make sure that this is further emphasized in the paper, we have added the following to the Discussion:

“We could thus ensure that training was adequate, performed at a suitable intensity, and could test directly the dynamic coupling of these variables and their effects on cognitive outcomes. Indeed, we found that almost all participants stayed within the overall targeted range of effort required by the design of the workout and by initial individual measurements.”

We further detail the importance of controlling for the intensity of exercise in this type of design:

“This result emphasizes the importance of controlling exercise intensity in HIT studies, given that the main determinant of improvement appears to be the intensity of the workout. […] The high-intensity component of this type of exercise regimen is intended to allow for higher workout intensity than traditional workouts, despite shorter overall volume.”

Also, the study used 80% of heart rate reserve (HRR) as the target HR during HIT workout. As far as we can tell, Figure 1C shows that the maximum HR during each workout appears to be approximately 140-150 in BPM, which is less likely to be equal to exercise intensity at 80% HRR. Instead, the HR data would suggest exercise intensity at light-to-moderate intensity in this age group according to the classification of physical activity intensity (ACSM, 2014). Please explain. Details of the HIT workout may also help explain why HR during the HIT workout did not meet the desired intensity.

In the initial version of the manuscript, Figure 1C showedδ HRMax averaged over all participants for a given session, as a function of session. This was thought to better convey both HRMax and δ in a single graph (HRTarget = HRRest + δ HRMaxδ HRRest). However, we acknowledge that this may be confusing for the reader, for the reasons outlined by the reviewers. Therefore, we have now rescaled the ordinate to show uncorrected HRMax (i.e. measured HRMax) values across sessions. Combined with Figure 1D, Figure 1C shows the adaptive property of the workout – namely, maximum heart rate is maintained over time, despite decreasing resting heart rate, to accommodate increasing workout volume. The complete script of the HIT workout is freely available online (https://github.com/davidmoreau/HIT). The file HIT_workout_script.txt includes all instructions and triggers of the video.

4) As all data presented graphically are change scores it would be helpful to include summary scores on cognitive tests and fitness measures at for each timepoint so the reader can get a feel for how the children are performing on these tasks and what the differences translate to in terms of the raw scores.

This is an excellent point—we have added a new table that presents descriptive for each task and measurement (Table 6).

5) Despite the significant findings, some readers may find it implausible that such a small amount of additional activity over a few weeks results in changes in cognitive performance. It would be helpful for the authors to say more about why this might be expected. Is there a threshold effect, for example, such that a small amount of additional activity may reach threshold for cognitive benefits?

We appreciate the opportunity to clarify our contribution. We have further described the hypothesized mechanisms of improvements in the Introduction and the Discussion.

The Introduction now states:

“In some cases, physiological improvements following high-intensity training (HIT) can even go beyond those typically following aerobic regimens (Rognmo, Hetland, Helgerud, Hoff, and Siørdahl, 2004). […] Together, the conjunction of promising findings and clear mechanisms of action has prompted discussions to implement HIT interventions more systematically within the community (Gray, Ferguson, Birch, Forrest, and Gill, 2016).”

The Discussion details further the postulated mechanisms of improvement:

“Regardless of the strength of the evidence reported in the present study, one might question the genuine impact of such a short training regimen. […] In terms of practical implications, this aspect is critical, as it allows designing shorter, more potent workouts.”

It would also be helpful if they could provide data to support how much more daily/weekly energy expenditure the HIT intervention would deliver compared to the control activities. Such information regarding actual exercise volume/time (i.e. calories or 300 minutes) achieved over 6 weeks may be added to the limitation section, but meanwhile this information could also be used to suggest how much physical activity this intervention could contribute to the minimum daily/weekly recommendation for health benefit.

We have added this information to the Materials and methods section:

“Frequency and duration were matched between conditions. The intervention was 6-weeks long, with five sessions per week, for a total of 30 sessions. This translates to 300 min of actual exercise.”

The Discussion section provides more details about the cardio-metabolic outcomes of HIT interventions, and therefore puts in context the overall low volume of exercise in the intervention:

“In a review of the impact of HIT on public health, Biddle and Batterham (2015) go as far as to premise their entire argument on the idea that the cardio-metabolic health outcomes are not to be questioned – rather, their only concern was about whether or not such regimens can be widely implemented and sustained over time (Biddle & Batterham, 2015). In a similar vein, Costigan and colleagues concluded that HIT is a time-efficient approach for improving cardiorespiratory fitness in adolescent populations (Costigan, Eather, Plotnikoff, Taaffe, and Lubans, 2015).

However, it is important to note that exercise recommendations are often expressed in terms of time commitment rather than workout intensities, and that these figures are typically framed in the context of aerobic exercise activities (e.g., Manore et al., 2014). Therefore, estimates based on these figures and applied to non-aerobic interventions may be fundamentally inaccurate.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Essential revisions:

We are sorry for that it has taken some time to review the manuscript. In addition to consulting with the original reviewers we also sought input from a statistical reviewer to assess specifically the degree to which the study complies with the CONSORT guidelines for reporting of randomised controlled trials. Some changes are required to meet those guidelines:

In general, the reviewers agreed that transparency for this study is good, all the analysis scripts are made available through GitHUB. The randomisation procedure is clear and blinding has been considered.

Though the CONSORT checklist is reported, the paper has not been written in the spirit of the guidelines. This is a randomised trial where the effect of an intervention was compared to a control intervention for cognitive improvement in children. While the trial design is mentioned on the first page, CONSORT requires the trial be identified as randomised in the title.

The title has been edited to:

“High-intensity Training Enhances Executive Function in Children in a Randomized, Placebo-Controlled Trial”.

The Abstract should then focus on the design of the trial rather than be written in a narrative style.

Also the key result with point estimate and confidence interval needs to be presented in the Abstract. I realise that this is not the usual style for eLife but some concession should be made so that the CONSORT requirements are met.

The Abstract has been edited to meet the CONSORT requirements, while ensuring it remains within the 150-word limit. In particular, primary and secondary outcomes are now explicit:

“318 children aged 7-13 years were randomly assigned to a HIT or an active control group matched for enjoyment and motivation. In the primary analysis, we compared improvements on six cognitive tasks representing two cognitive constructs (N = 305). Secondary outcomes included genetic data and physiological measurements.”

In addition, the main result is now presented in the Abstract, in the “Results” subsection:

“The 6-week HIT regimen resulted in improvements on measures of cognitive control [BFM = 3.38, g = 0.31 (0.09, 0.54)] and working memory [BFM = 5233.68, g = 0.54 (0.31, 0.77)], moderated by BDNF genotype, with met66carriers showing larger gains post-exercise than val66 homozygotes.”

CONSORT requires the primary and secondary outcomes to be specified. This report does not state which outcome is the primary outcome (usually this is the measurement that the sample size calculation was calculated for). The sample size statement in the protocol states 129 per group are required to detect an effect size of 0.35 with power of 80% at 5% significance level. This suggests that the study is powered for a single primary research question, but a primary outcome is not listed instead there are many outcomes listed under several sections. So it is not clear which outcome the difference of 0.35 relates to. While the statistical analysis proposed is Bayesian the arguments over repeated testing still hold if you want to present evidence that the intervention does improve outcomes.

Thank you for pointing this out. The primary and secondary outcomes are now clearly labeled in the Abstract and in text (subsection “Cognitive Improvements”). In addition, the power analysis now explicitly refers to the primary outcome measures:

“Note that an a priori power analysis based on previous studies (Erickson et al., 2013; Moreau et al., 2015) indicated the need for a minimum N of 129 participants per group to detect an effect of d = 0.35 on the primary outcome measures, with 1 – β =.80 and α =.05.” This allowed us to define our sample size based on the effects observed in Erickson et al., 2013 and Moreau et al., 2015. In addition to focusing on well-defined constructs, rather than multiple single measures, we also used Bayesian methods (both hypothesis testing and parameter estimation) that are robust to typical problems associated with multiple comparisons (Dienes, 2008; Gelman, 2008). Of course, this argument falls short when presenting the frequentist analyses, but those are reported for transparency and to reach a wider audience, and therefore are “equivalent” in the sense of being the typical alternative tests, yet not necessarily in the exact interpretation of the results. With this in mind, the interaction effects for Cognitive Control and Working Memory (p =.008 and p <.001, respectively) still remain largely significant after a Bonferroni correction.

Results are frequently presented with statistical hypothesis tests and p-values, but the term 'statistically significant' is often abbreviated to 'significant' which is to be avoided. The authors need to be careful to always present the weight of the evidence in a p-value and also report a corresponding effect size with 95% confidence interval.

Thank you for pointing out this oversight. The term ‘significant’, when referring to statistical significance, has now been systematically replaced with the latter (see “Frequentist Analyses” section). In addition, all effect sizes (g, η2) are now accompanied by 95% confidence intervals (we have replaced Cohen’s d with Hedges’ g, to correct for bias when sample sizes are unequal between groups). RMSE have also been added to all R2 values. Finally, the error term in Bayesian analyses, expressed in percentage, is also a measure of uncertainty (about the model, in this case). These are included in all tables for model comparison (2, 3, and 4).

As a single primary outcome was not specified a priori it would be helpful if the Abstract communicated the size of the trial and the number of outcomes that were considered in the full protocol, so the statistical significance can be put in context.

This information has been added to the Abstract in its new format:

“318 children aged 7-13 years were randomly assigned to a HIT or an active control group matched for enjoyment and motivation. In the primary analysis, we compared improvements on six cognitive tasks representing two cognitive constructs (N = 305). Secondary outcomes included genetic data and physiological measurements.”

Word space (150) limits our ability to provide details in the Abstract, but all the outcomes measured are listed in the main manuscript (Materials and methods) and in the Statistical Analysis Plan.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Table 1—source data 1. Scree plot for the exploratory factor analysis on all cognitive measures.

    The plot shows the eigenvalues associated with each factor plotted against each factor, and supports the decision to retain two factors.

    Table 1—source data 2. Path diagram for the exploratory factor analysis on all cognitive measures.

    F1 (Cognitive Control) and F2 (Working Memory) refer to the factors extracted from an exploratory factor analysis on all six cognitive measures, with promax rotation (N = 287).

    Reporting standard 1. CONSORT flow diagram.
    Reporting standard 2. CONSORT check list.
    elife-25062-repstand2.pdf (686.5KB, pdf)

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES