Abstract
Imagining helping a person in need can facilitate prosocial intentions. Here we investigated how this effect can change with aging. We found that, similar to young adults, older adults were more willing to help a person in need when they imagined helping that person compared to a baseline condition that did not involve helping, but not compared to a conceptual helping control condition. Controlling for heightened emotional concern in older adults revealed an age-related difference in the effect of imagining on willingness to help. While we observed age-related condition effects, we also found that the subjective vividness of scene imagery predicted willingness to help for both age groups. Our findings provide insight into the relations among episodic simulation, healthy aging, emotion, and prosociality. Implications for effects of episodic memory and aging on social decision-making are discussed.
Keywords: future thinking, episodic memory, social cognition, morality, prosociality
Episodic simulation is a form of future thinking or prospection that allows humans to imagine specific future events (cf., Schacter, & Addis, & Buckner, 2008; Szpunar, Spreng, & Schacter, 2014). Episodic simulation is tightly linked to episodic memory (Atance & O’Neil, 2001; Schacter et al., 2012; Szpunar, 2010), and it is well established that episodic memory declines with aging (Denise & Gutchess, 2005; Craik & Salthouse, 2000; Hartshorne & Germine, 2015; Hedden & Gabrieli, 2004). Recent studies have shown that older adults exhibit a parallel reduction in episodic simulation, generating fewer episodic details than young adults when they imagine future experiences (Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2008; Cole, Morrison, & Conway, 2013; Gaesser, Sacchetti, Addis, & Schacter, 2011; Madore, Gaesser, & Schacter, 2014; Gallo, Korthauer, McDonough, Teshale, & Johnsohn, 2011; Rendell, et al., 2012; for review, see Schacter, Gaesser, & Addis, 2013).
Studies concerning episodic simulation in both young and old adults have focused mainly on elucidating underlying mechanisms; considerably less has been learned about the adaptive functions of episodic simulation (see Schacter, 2012 for review). Recently, interest in social functions of episodic simulations has emerged in studies that have examined how age-related differences in episodic memory and episodic simulation impact social problem solving (Madore & Schacter, 2014; Sheldon, McAndrews, & Moscovitch, 2011).
Here we expand the domain of research on aging and social functions of episodic simulation. Our specific motivation comes from experiments showing that episodic simulation increases prosocial intentions in young adults (Gaesser & Schacter, 2014; Gaesser, DiBiase, & Kensinger, in press; Gaesser, Horn, & Young, 2015). When young adults were presented with a situation where a person was in need of help, their prosocial intentions to help increased after they had imagined a specific future event of helping that person, compared with a conceptual helping condition where they estimated ways in which the person could be helped. Moreover, episodic simulation increased the subjective vividness and accessibility of the imagined helping scene, in turn heightening the perceived plausibility that one would help in that situation. Such a mechanism aligns with previous research on imagination inflation (Hyman & Pentland, 1996; Mazzoni & Memon, 2003; Garry & Polaschek, 2000) and related work showing a link between the sensory vividness of imagined future events and subjective of likelihood (D’Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2012; Szpunar & Schacter, 2013).
In light of the foregoing findings, a straightforward prediction is that older adults’ prosocial intentions will not benefit from episodic simulation to the same extent as young adults. However, because older adults are still able to imagine future events and subjectively experience sensory vividness, we would expect some prosocial benefit from episodic simulation. Moreover, age-related differences in prosocial intentions attributable to episodic simulation may be masked by age-related differences in emotion. Indeed, older adults may even be equally or more willing to help in general compared to young adults as a consequence of age-related changes in heightened emotional processing (Charles & Carstensen, 2010; Mather & Carstensen, 2005). For example, Richter and Kunzmann (2011) found that compared to younger adults, older adults displayed more sympathy when they watched a film clip of a person discussing an emotionally difficult topic (e.g., death in the family, divorce, moving to a new city). Similarly, Sze, Gyurak, Goodkind, and Levenson (2012) found linear increases across the lifespan for emotional concern and prosocial decisions directed at the person in need (see also Bailey, Ruffman, & Rendell, 2013).
In the present experiment, we investigated whether prosocial intentions would increase after imagining future helping events relative to control conditions. Based on older adults’ reduced episodic simulation, older adult participants may be less likely to express willingness to help a person in need when they imagine helping the person compared to younger adults. However, based on heightened emotional processing in older adults, older adults may be equally or more willing to help a person in need when they imagine helping the person compared to young adults.
Method
Participants
We recruited 31 younger adult participants (M = 21.97, SD = 2.51, between the ages of 18–27) and 39 older adult participants (M = 74.00, SD = 6.58, between the ages of 65–86), with a total of 70 participants. Data collection continued until we had collected 30 participants with full data sets that we could then analyze (see Supplemental Material for power analysis, inclusion criteria, recruitment procedures, and neuropsychology scores). Older adults had experienced more years of education (M = 18, SD = 2.61) than young adults (M = 15.41, SD = 1.43), t (57) = 4.21, p < .001. However, years of education was unrelated with the main dependent variable of interest: willingness to help (r (57) = .09, p = .521. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were fluent in English, and no history of neurological impairment.
Procedure
Participants were presented with short stories adapted from the news and social media of everyday events where a person needs help. For each scenario, participants were instructed to imagine a specific future episode of them helping the person (Imagine Helping condition), write down how the person could be helped (Conceptual Helping condition), or identify the media source they thought the story came from based on the writing style (No Helping condition). The No Helping condition, modeled after media control conditions used in empathy research (e.g., Coke, Batson, & McDavis, 1978) to provide a neutral baseline condition, aimed to prevent participants from generating a helping episode while controlling for exposure to the story of need. The Conceptual Helping condition provided a control condition for recruiting semantic retrieval, social cognition, basic imagery, and, critically, conceptual priming of helping responses (Macrae & Johnston, 1998; Nelson & Norton, 2005). By comparison, the Imagine Helping condition required simulating an episode that is specific in time and place. Indeed, the Imagine Helping condition was directly informed by previous neuroimaging and behavioral research using similar instructions and protocol to successfully prompt subjects to generate events set in a specific time and place (e.g., Addis et al., 2008, Addis et al., 2011; Gaesser et al., 2011; Rendell et al., 2012).
Participants reviewed instructions and completed at least 3 practice trials, one for each condition, to familiarize themselves with the task. Stories were presented one at a time for 10 seconds each, followed by the condition prompt for 60 seconds. If necessary, practice trials were repeated for participants to facilitate task comprehension with feedback provided by the experimenter after each trial. Following the practice trials, participants were randomly shown 9 trials, 3 for each of the conditions.
After completion of all trials, participants filled out a self-paced paper-and-pencil survey, rating each story on a Likert scale from 1 to 7 (see Supplemental Material for rating scales). For each of the stories, participants were asked how likely they would be to help out in the situation (1 = not at all, 7 = very willing). Participants also rated the sensory quality of scene imagery by rating their imagined events for scene coherence (the imagined scene in your mind was?; 1 vague - 7 coherent and clear) and scene detail (the imagined scene in your mind was?; 1 simple – 7 detailed) of their imagined events that were averaged to form a composite scene imagery index (see Gaesser et al., in press, for a similar approach; and Supplemental Material for relevant analysis in the present study). Examples were provided for scene imagery measures to facilitate comprehension and encourage participants to use the full range of the scales (see below). Participants’ ratings for emotional concern consisted of an average of the degree to which they experienced different emotions associated with the same underlying construct: emotional concern or empathy for others (e.g., sympathetic, compassionate, softhearted, moved, tender, and warm; see Batson, Early, Salvarani, 1997 and Batson, 2011 for extensive use and justification of this measure) on a scale from 1 to 7 (1= not at all, 7 = extremely) for each person in need. Ratings for theory of mind (akin to perspective taking and mentalizing) for the person in need were also collected, did you consider the person’s thoughts and feelings (1 = not at all, 7 = strongly considered).
Lastly, participants were asked to provide a very brief description (e.g., one sentence or two sentences) of how they imagined helping (e.g., I imagined helping to create a flyer with a picture of a dog), how someone could help (e.g., someone could contact the fire department), or how they identified the media for each of the stories (e.g., It was probably from a blog or social media. The syntax was somewhat unusual or incorrect.) in response to each experimental trial depending on which condition was presented for that story. These descriptions were used to ensure that subjects complied with task instructions (i.e. a participant imagined how they would help the person in the Imagine Helping condition). After finishing the study participants were debriefed and thanked for their time. Data were analyzed using conventional hypothesis testing as well as including newer statistical approaches such as confidence intervals (Cumming, 2013).
Results
We first conducted an Age (Young, Older) by Condition (Imagining Helping, Conceptual Helping, No Helping) ANOVA on willingness to help as an initial test of the relationship between episodic simulation, agining, and prosocial intentions. There was a main effect of Condition, F (1,59) = 45.12, p < .001, ηp2 = .433, and no main effect of Age, F (1,59) = .02, p = .897, ηp2 < .001, or interaction, F (1,59) = 1.64, p = .205, ηp2 = .027. Pairwise t-tests revealed participants were in general more willing to help a person in need when they imagined helping the person (Imagine Helping condition, M = 5.11; SE = .16, 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) = 4.79, 5.42) than when they were merely exposed to the plight of others by considering the writing style and media source of the stories of need (No Helping condition, M = 3.62; SE = .19, 95% CI = 3.23, 4.01), t (60) = 8.68, p< .001, ηp2 = .56. Participants were also more willing to help when they wrote about how the person could be helped (Conceptual Helping condition, M = 4.83; SE = .15, 95% CI = 4.53, 5.13) compared to when they were exposed to the plight of others by considering the writing style and media source of the stories of need (No Helping condition), t (60) = 6.78, p < .001, ηp2 = .434. Participants were marginally more willing to help a person in need when they imagined helping the person (Imagine Helping condition) compared to when they wrote about how the person could be helped (Conceptual Helping condition), t (60) = 1.90, p < .063, ηp2 = .057.
To more directly contrast these findings with results based on previous research with young adults, we examined these condition effects in young adults only. Replicating previous research, young adults indicated they were more willing to help a person in need when they imagined helping the person (Imagine Helping condition, M = 5.20; SE = .24, 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) = 4.70, 5.70) than when they were merely exposed to the plight of others by considering the writing style and media source of the stories of need (No Helping condition, M = 3.61; SE = .25, 95% CI = 3.11, 4.12), t (29) = 6.62, p< .001, ηp2 = .602). Young adults’ willingness to help also increased when they wrote about how the person could be helped (Conceptual Helping condition, M = 4.69; SE = .21, 95% CI = 4.26, 5.12) compare to when they were exposed to the plight of others by considering the writing style and media source of the stories of need (No Helping condition), t (29) = 4.47, p < .001, ηp2 = .408. Importantly, however, their willingness to help was greater when they imagined helping the person (Imagine Helping condition) compared to when they wrote about how the person could be helped (Conceptual Helping condition), t (29) = 2.16, p < .039, ηp2 = .139.
Exploratory analyses for older adults only and willingness to help revealed that they were also more willing to help a person in need when they imagined helping the person (Imagine Helping condition, M = 5.02; SE = .21, 95% CI = 4.60, 5.44) compared to being exposed to the plight of others (No Helping condition, M = 3.62; SE = .30, 95% CI = 3.01, 4.24), t (30) = 5.62, p < .001, ηp2 = .513. Similarly, older adults were more willing to help when they wrote about how the person could be helped (Conceptual Helping condition, M = 4.96; SE = .21, 95% CI = 4.53, 5.40) compared to, being exposed to the plight of others (No Helping condition, t(30) = 5.06, p < .001, ηp2 = .461. Nonetheless, older adults were not significantly more willing to help in the imagine helping condition compared to conceptual helping condition, t (30) = .34, p = .74, ηp2 = .004, suggesting that older adults’ prosocial intentions may not benefit from imagining a helping event as in young adults. However, because the Age x Condition interaction was not significant, this finding requires interpretative caution.
Next, we examined the role of emotional concern on an effect of episodic simulation on willingness to help. Consistent with past work on emotional concern, scale reliability analysis showed that individual emotions directed at the welfare of a person in need (i.e., sympathetic, compassionate, softhearted, moved, tender, and warm; see Batson, Early, & Salvarani, 1997; Batson, 2011) were highly related (Cronbach’s Alpha = .958), and thus were averaged to form an emotional concern index. Given previous evidence that older adults tend to exhibit heightened emotional and empathic responses compared to young adults, we assessed whether age-related differences in emotional concern obscured age-related differences in the effect of episodic simulation on prosocial intentions.
Older adults reported significantly greater emotional concern for people in need than did young adults (Young M = 2.80, SE = .20, 95% CI = 2.40, 3.19; Older M = 4.15, SE = .20, 95% CI = 3.76, 4.54), t (1,59) = 4.78, p < .001, ηp2 = .289; see Supplemental Material for additional analyses). To control for age-related differences in emotional concern affecting willingness to help, we ran a hierarchical regression analysis with willingness to help in the Imagine Helping condition as the outcome variable, entered emotional concern as the first predictor variable, and then entered age group (young, old) as the second predictor variable. This analysis revealed that emotional concern was a significant predictor of willingness to help after imagining a future helping event (Emotional concern B = .35; R-squared = .136, F (1, 58) = 9.33, p = .003). Critically, when controlling for differences in emotional concern young adults were indeed significantly more willing to help after imagining a future helping event compared to older adults (Age B = −.74, R-squared = .208, change in R-squared = .072, F (1, 58) = 5.26, p = .025). Note that when emotional concern was not controlled in a simple regression analysis with age group (young, old) as the predictor and willingness to help in the Imagine Helping condition as the outcome variable, age did not predict willingness to help (Age B = −.178, p = .577).
Finally, no age-related differences emerged for subjective scene imagery of imagined helping events (t (59) = .54, p = .592). Subjective scene imagery was similarly associated with willingness to help in both young adults (r (28) = .68, p < .001) and older adults (r (29) = .48, p = .006). Partial correlations revealed that scene imagery remained associated with willingness to help in both young adults (pr (27) = .59, p < .001) and older adults (pr (28) = .49, p = .006) when scene imagery and emotional concern were entered in the same regression model as predictors of willingness to help. Directly comparing the difference in correlation coefficients across age groups did not show a difference in the strength of the association between scene imagery and willingness to help for young and older adults (z = 1.12, p = .263; Preacher, 2002). No age-related differences emerged for subjective theory of mind for imagined helping events (t (59) = .28, p = .782); however, there was a trending effect for a difference in the association between theory of mind and willingness to help (Young, r (28) = .71, p < .001; Old, r (29) = .35, p = .052; z = 1.92, p = .055), suggesting that the vividness of scene imagery may have a more similar impact on willingness to help across ages than theory of mind.
Discussion
Our results replicate previous findings that episodic simulations focused on helping individuals in need significantly increase subsequent prosocial intentions to help those individuals. Older adults, like young adults, showed increased prosocial intentions following episodic simulation compared with the no helping control, but unlike younger adults, did not show a significant increase in prosocial intentions compared with the conceptual helping condition. Perhaps this pattern occurred because older adults’ prosocial intentions in the Imagine Helping condition were driven to a similar extent by conceptual processing or priming of prosociality as in the conceptual helping condition, and may be related to the more general age-related shift to a more conceptual mode of thinking about the future (as reflected by findings of increased semantic details in older adults future thinking; Schacter et al., 2013). Although the Age x Condition interaction was not statistically significant, and thus interpretations of age-related differences require caution, controlling for heightened emotional concern in older adults revealed that younger adults exhibited greater willingness to help than older adults following episodic simulation. While these results point to a possible age-related condition effect, we also found evidence for a shared mechanism contributing an increase in willingness when imagining future helping events: the subjective experience of scene imagery.
Consistent with previous research, we found that measures of subjective scene imagery (how detailed and coherently an imagined event is experienced) predicted willingness to help for young adults (Gaesser & Schacter, 2014; Gaesser et al., 2015). Building on these results, we found that this relationship was also present in older adults, revealing that subjective scene imagery similarly predicted willingness to help in both older and young adults, whereas theory of mind may differentially contribute to willingness to help across age groups. These data suggest that, regardless of age, as scene imagery of the helping event becomes subjectively more vivid, the perceived plausibility of the imagined helping event increases, enhancing decisions to help others in need. We think that deficits in objective measures of episodic detail (i.e., the Autobiographical Interview; Levine et al., 2002) that have been consistently observed when older adults imagine future events (Addis et al., 2008; Cole et al., 2013; Gaesser et al., 2011; Madore et al., 2014; Gallo et al., 2011; Rendell, et al., 2012; Schacter et al., 2013) likely play a role in the condition differences in prosocial intentions observed here. It is an open question whether objective measures of episodic detail, like subjective measures, also predict willingness to help. While we did collect brief descriptions of events generated by subjects to ensure that they generated condition appropriate events, these descriptions were too brief to allow assessment of objective levels of detail with any sort of precision; however, this approach should be pursued in future research. Relatedly, whereas subjective scene imagery is similarly associated with willingness to help for both young and older adults, the results provide some evidence that theory of mind may be less strongly linked to older adults’ willingness to help directly than young adults; however, future work will be needed to fully address this possibility.
The present findings align with previous work that found increased emotional concern for the welfare of a person in need in older adults compared to young adults (Bailey et al., 2013; Richter & Kunzmann, 2011; Sze et al., 2012). Although we did not observe an overall increase in prosocial responses for older adults compared to young adults, our findings are consistent with the notion that older adults show heighted socio-emotional responses and place an increased emphasis on emotion in decision making (Charles & Carstensen, 2010; Mather & Carstensen, 2005). We recently found that imagining future helping events that elicited positive affect in particular (compared negative or neutral) bolstered willingness to help in young adults (Gaesser et al., in press). Given that older adults tend to imagine future events as more positively valenced than young adults (Gallo et al., 2011), an interesting possibility for future research is to examine how this age-related shift in affective valence of imagined future events impacts older adults’ prosocial decisions.
The present study has possible implications for understanding the effect of memory on prosocial decisions in older adults. Because episodic simulation and episodic memory rely on many of the same cognitive processes and content (Schacter et al., 2012), an interesting question for future research to explore is whether the effect of imagining future helping events on prosocial intentions will extend to remembering past helping events for older adults. Our prior research has found that imagining future and remembering past events increases prosocial responses to the same degree in young adults (Gaesser & Schacter, 2014). Moreover, perceived scene imagery ratings for remembered past events correlated with willingness to help, similar to imagined future events, suggesting a common mechanism underlying the prosocial effect of episodic simulation and memory. Considering that older adults’ abilities to remember past events and imagine future events largely decline in parallel (Addis et al., 2008; Gaesser et al., 2011; Madore, et al., 2014; Gallo, et al., 2011; Rendell, et al., 2012; Schacter, et al., 2013), older adults may show a similar prosocial response when remembering past helping events as they do when imagining future helping events—though future work should be careful to tease apart age-related differences in episodic processes from differences in narrative style and communicative goals (see Schacter & Madore, 2016 for relevant discussion).
Although imagining and remembering fostered prosocial intentions to a similar extent in young adults, in our earlier study we observed a flexible advantage in the range of prosocial events for imagining facilitated compared to remembering (Gaesser & Schacter, 2014). Specifically, there were some helping scenarios that the younger adults had not experienced before and therefore could not remember. On these failed memory trials, willingness to help dropped to baseline levels. An important issue to explore is whether this empathic gap in younger adults between remembering and imagining also exists in older adults. Given that older adults have lived longer than younger adults and thus are likely to have encountered a larger variety of helping situations, to the extent that older adults are able to retrieve these events, the flexible advantage for imagining helping events compared to remembering helping events on prosocial decisions may be lessened in older adults compared to younger adults. While older adults’ episodic memory is generally considered to decline compared to younger adults (Denise & Gutchess, 2005; Craik & Salthouse, 2000; Hartshorne & Germine, 2015; Hedden & Gabrieli, 2004), their prosocial decisions may in fact benefit from their broader experiences and the richer variety of memories they can draw on when evaluating prosocial decisions.
Here we investigated the effect of episodic simulation on hypothetical decisions about helping someone in need. Future work with older adults will be needed to investigate whether these decisions will translate into actual prosocial behavior. While prosocial intentions do not always translate into prosocial action (FeldmanHall, Mobbs, Evans, Hoscox, Navrady, & Dalgleish, 2012), initial work with young adults indicates that imagining helping can increase costly prosocial behavior, increasing the number of charitable donations an individual makes at a financial cost to themselves. In sum, while there is still work to be done, our findings provide a starting point for future studies investigating how social decision-making in older adults is influenced by both emotion and imagery.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank E. Herder for helpful discussions, and/or comments on drafts of the manuscript. This work was partially supported by a Science of Prospection Award from the John Templeton Foundation to B.G and by National Institute on Aging grant AG08441 to D.L.S.
References
- Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL. Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science. 2008;19(1):33–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atance CM, O’Neill DK. Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(1):533–539. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01804-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey PE, Ruffman T, Rendell PG. Age-related differences in social economic decision making: The ultimatum game. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2013;68(3):356–363. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batson CD. Altruism in humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Batson CD, Early S, Salvarani G. Perspective taking: Imagining how another feels versus imagining how you would feel. Personality Social and Psychology Bulletin. 1997;23:751–758. [Google Scholar]
- Charles ST, Carstensen LL. Social and emotional aging. Annual Review of Psychology. 2010;61:383–409. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole SN, Morrison CM, Conway MA. Episodic future thinking: Linking neuropsychological performance with episodic detail in young and old adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2013;66:1687–1706. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.758157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coke JS, Batson CD, McDavis K. Empathic mediation of helping: A two-stage model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1978;36:752–766. [Google Scholar]
- Craik FIM, Salthouse TA. The handbook of aging and cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Cumming G. The new statistics: A how-to guide. Australian Psychologist. 2013;48(3):161–170. [Google Scholar]
- D’Argembeau A, Van der Linden M. Predicting the phenomenology of episodic future thoughts. Consciousness and Cognition. 2012;21(3):1198–206. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FeldmanHall O, Mobbs D, Evans D, Hiscox L, Navrady L, Dalgleish T. What we say and what we do: the relationship between real and hypothetical moral choices. Cognition. 2012;123(3):434–441. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaesser B, DiBiase HD, Kensinger EA. A role for affect in the link between episodic simulation and prosociality. Memory. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1254246. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaesser B, Horn M, Young LL. When can imagining the self increase willingness to help others? Investigating whether the self-referential nature of episodic simulation fosters prosociality. Social Cognition. 2015;33(6):562–584. [Google Scholar]
- Gaesser B, Sacchetti DC, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Characterizing age-related changes in remembering the past and imagining the future. Psychology and Aging. 2011;26(1):80–84. doi: 10.1037/a0021054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaesser B, Schacter DL. Episodic simulation and episodic memory can increase intentions to help others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111(12):4415–4420. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402461111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallo DA, Korthauer LE, McDonough IM, Teshale S, Johnson EL. Age-related positivity effects and autobiographical memory detail: Evidence from a past/future source memory task. Memory. 2011;19(6):641–652. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.595723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garry M, Polaschek DLL. Imagination and Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2000;9(1):6–10. [Google Scholar]
- Hartshorne JK, Germine LT. When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the lifespan. Psychological Science. 2015;26:433–443. doi: 10.1177/0956797614567339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman IE, Jr, Pentland J. The Role of Mental Imagery in the Creation of False Childhood Memories. Journal of Memory and Language. 1996;35(2):101–117. [Google Scholar]
- Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17(4):677–689. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macrae CN, Johnston L. Help, I need somebody: Automatic action and inaction. Social Cognition. 1998;16(4):400–417. [Google Scholar]
- Madore KP, Schacter DL. An episodic specificity induction enhances means-end problem solving in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging. 2014;29:913–924. doi: 10.1037/a0038209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madore KP, Gaesser B, Schacter DL. Constructive episodic simulation: Dissociable effects of a specificity induction on remembering, imagining, and describing in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2014;40:609–622. doi: 10.1037/a0034885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2005;9(10):496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzoni G, Memon A. Imagination can create false autobiographical memories. Psychological Science. 2003;14(2):186–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1432-1327.1999.00020.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson LD, Norton MI. From student to superhero: Situational primes shape future helping. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2005;41:423–430. [Google Scholar]
- Park DC, Gutchess AH. Long-term memory and aging: a cognitive neuroscience approach. In: Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park DC, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of aging. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 218–245. [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ. Calculation for the test of the difference between two independent correlation coefficients [Computer software] 2002 Available from http://quantpsy.org.
- Rendell PG, Bailey PE, Henry JD, Philips LH, Gaskin S, Kliegel M. Older adults have greater difficulty imagining the future rather than atemporal experiences. Psychology and Aging. 2012;27(4):1089–1098. doi: 10.1037/a0029748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richter D, Kunzmann U. Age differences in three facets of empathy: Performance-based evidence. Psychology and Aging. 2011;26(1):60–70. doi: 10.1037/a0021138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DC, Umanath S. Event memory: A theory of memory for laboratory, autobiographical, and fictional events. Psychological Review. 2015;122(1):1–23. doi: 10.1037/a0037907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL. Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. American Psychologist. 2012;67:603–613. doi: 10.1037/a0029869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Episodic simulation of future events: Concepts, data, and applications. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:39–60. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Addis DR, Hassabis D, Martin VC, Spreng RN, Szpunar KK. The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron. 2012;76:2610–2621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Gaesser B, Addis DR. Remembering the past and imagining the future in the elderly. Gerontology. 2013;59:143–151. doi: 10.1159/000342198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Madore KP. Remembering the past and imagining the future: Identifying and enhancing the contribution of episodic memory. Memory Studies. 2016;9:245–255. doi: 10.1177/1750698016645230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheldon S, McAndrews MP, Moscovitch M. Episodic memory processes mediated by the medial temporal lobes contribute to open-ended problem solving. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2439–2447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sze JA, Gyurak A, Goodkind MS, Levenson RW. Greater emotional empathy and prosocial behavior in late life. Emotion. 2012;12(5):1129–1140. doi: 10.1037/a0025011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szpunar KK. Episodic future thought: An emerging concept. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2010;5(2):142–162. doi: 10.1177/1745691610362350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szpunar KK, Schacter DL. Get real: effects of repeated simulation and emotion on the perceived plausibility of future experiences. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 2013;142(2):323–7. doi: 10.1037/a0028877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szpunar KK, Spreng RN, Schacter DL. A taxonomy of prospection: Introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2014;111:18414–18421. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417144111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.