Abstract
Theories of episodic memory have long hypothesized that recollection of a specific instance from one’s life is mediated by recovery of a neural state of spatiotemporal context. This paper reviews recent theoretical advances in formal models of spatiotemporal context and a growing body of neurophysiological evidence from human imaging studies and animal work that neural populations in the hippocampus and other brain regions support a representation of spatiotemporal context.
Keywords: Temporal context, Spatial context, Mathematical models of memory, Time cells, Place cells
Introduction
Since its earliest conception, episodic memory has been hypothesized to be associated with the recovery of the spatiotemporal context in which a memory was formed [1]. For this to be the case, the brain must contain a representation of spatiotemporal context that codes information about the spatial location and temporal relationships between events. The hippocampal place code provides dramatic evidence for a representation of spatial context in the brain [2, 3, 4]. A representation of spatiotemporal context should also change gradually over time to enable the expression of temporal relationships, tapping into a longstanding tradition in mathematical psychology in which a gradually-changing state of context mediates associations between stimuli [5, 6, 7]. Later work agumented these earlier formal models of contextual drift by hypothesizing that episodic memory is associated with recovery of a gradually-changing state of temporal context, enabling a concise account of behavioral contiguity effects, especially in the free recall task [8, 9, 10]. Neurophysiological work has long shown that the firing rate of neural populations in the hippocampus and elsewhere change gradually over time even when experimentally-controlled variables are equated [11, 12, 13, 14].
This paper reviews recent progress towards an understanding of the behavioral and neural evidence for a representation of spatiotemporal context. There is by now overwhelming neurophysiological evidence for a representation of spatial context—spatial correlates of neural firing in the hippocampus and related brain regions—from a range of species (see [15, 16, 17] for recent reviews). Because it is less well-known, this paper will focus on empirical evidence for temporal aspects of a context representation based on recent work from both animal and human studies. In addition to empirical developments, there have also been recent theoretical developments in our understanding of spatial and temporal context and their relationship to one another.
Empirical status of retrieved temporal context models
Retrieved context models hypothesize that a gradually-changing state of temporal context is recovered when an episodic memory is retrieved. This provides a natural account of behavioral effects that show associations between items presented close together in time. The importance of this contiguity effect in human episodic memory has recently been challenged [18]. However, that position seems inconsistent with recent empirical findings demonstrating that essentially all normally-functioning individuals show a temporal contiguity effect [19] in free recall studies and that the temporal contiguity effect in free recall is correlated with remember vs know judgments in recognition memory [20], suggesting that the processes supporting contiguity are not limited only to recall tasks. Moreover, detailed free recall modeling has shown that this approach is sufficient to account for detailed properties of the spacing effect in free recall [21] and patterns of recall and recognition performance associated with normal aging [22].
Recent evidence for a gradually-changing state of temporal context in the brain
There is now overwhelming evidence that neural population vectors in the rodent brain change gradually over macroscopic periods of time ranging from minutes up to at least days, providing a potential temporal context signal for temporally mediated associations. In recent years, hippocampal region CA2 has been identified as possibly having a central role in temporal variation, in contrast to other hippocampal regions that show both spatial and temporal correlates [23]. Gradual neural drift has been observed in animal preparations with a range of imaging modalities, including optical recordings [24] and activity-dependent markers [25, 26]. Notably, these last two studies showed that markers of gradually-changing neural activity in the amygdala and hippocampus coincided with temporally-graded behavioral associations for fear conditioning, consistent with the hypothesis that temporally-graded associations are mediated by a gradually-changing state of temporal context in the hippocampus [14, 24].
In addition to animal work, human studies have also shown that neural processes measurable with fMRI change gradually over time and are affected by the history of recent stimuli and, in turn, affect behavioral performance in a range of tasks. The representational similarity in the entorhinal cortex and other brain regions between studied events predicts the judged time between those events [27]. Similarly, pattern dissimilarity in the hippocampus during study predicts successful performance in the judgment of recency task [28]. Similarly, the degree to which hippocampal patterns reflect the content of intervening items—the temporal context—predicts success in judging the time that has elapsed between pairs of stimuli [29, 30]. A study in which sequences of stimuli were presented showed that the multivoxel pattern changed gradually over time and contained information about recent stimuli [31]. Subsequent work suggests that this temporal context signal is present not only in the hippocampus, but also other brain regions throughout the “core recollection network” [32]. Another study showed repetition suppression to stimuli presented in repeated temporal contexts [33], minimizing any concerns that the analyses in [31] were contaminated by a slow hemodynamic response.
fMRI evidence from the free recall task confirms the observation that recently-experienced stimulus categories contribute to the information contained in the BOLD signal [34]. Moreover, this information about the preceding temporal context in the BOLD signal predicts transitions among words preceded by similar temporal contexts in free recall. Taken together, the animal and human work leave little doubt that the brain contains a representation of the recent past that comports with the basic properties required by retrieved temporal context models.
Is temporal context recovered in episodic retrieval?
The other main contention of retrieved temporal context models—that temporal context is recovered as part of episodic memory retrieval—has not yet been definitively observed. There is very strong evidence that episodic memory results in a recovery of brain states observed during study. It is also clear that this recovery can cause associations that bridge across different experiences with a stimulus, as predicted by retrieved context models of memory. It is also known that behavioral markers of contextual retrieval correlate with hippocampal activity [35]. However, it has not yet been definitively established that gradually-changing brain states are in fact recovered during episodic memory. That is, suppose that the brain state is a superposition of a part that changes, and another part that reflects currently available information. The available neurophysiological data collected in the last few years is consistent with the hypothesis that only the part that reflects currently-available stimuli is recovered. Earlier studies attempted to directly test the hypothesis that a gradually-changing temporal context was retrieved [36, 37, 38], but each of these studies have some limitations that prevent a definitive answer to this question.
Although it is not direct neural evidence for recovery of temporal context, a growing body of evidence demonstrates that the hippocampus and PFC participate in building transitive associations that bridge across temporally disjoint events, as predicted by retrieved temporal context models. After study of a-b and, much later, b-c, a transitive association is observed to the extent that a and c become associated to one another. Transitive associations are a natural prediction of retrieved temporal context models. If, during study of b-c, b recovers its previous temporal context, which includes information from a, then this provides a natural mechanism for associations between a and c. Recent fMRI evidence shows that transitive associations are observed for rewarding stimuli dependent on connections with the hippocampus [39], reviewed in [40]. Other evidence shows that the degree to which transitive a-c associations are formed between neutral stimuli is dependent on the degree of hippocampal-PFC connectivity [41] (reviewed in [42]). This adds to a body of work indicating that temporal context affects hippocampal representations [43]. MEG evidence suggests that the retrieval of transitive associations takes place after recovery of item information, suggesting an alignment with the time course of retrieval in recognition memory [44]. It should be noted that transitive associations are not a unique prediction of retrieved temporal context models [45, 46].
Although there has not yet been a definitive study, technology exists to measure the recovery of a neural temporal context. First, any neural measure that generates a multivariate response can in principle be used to estimate a neural jump back in time. Suppose that we measure the multivariate response during retrieval of a stimulus originally presented in a randomly-assembled study list that includes many other stimuli. If the remembered stimulus was originally presented at serial position i, then retrieved temporal context models predict that the pattern after retrieval should resemble the neural pattern during study of stimuli presented close in time to serial position i. This strategy has been pursued previously with human single unit and ECoG studies [37, 36, 38], but with empirically-ambiguous results. Modern techniques for reactivating tagged neural ensembles [47] provides another possible avenue for establishing a causal connection between retrieved temporal context and behavioral associations. It is known that ensembles change gradually over many days [24] and that temporally-modulated representations covary with temporally-graded associations [26]. There is no conceptual or technical obstacle to a study in which the neurons active in a particular environment associated with fear are tagged. If reactivation of that ensemble in environments learned close together in time results in temporally-graded fear generalization, this would establish a causal connection between retrieval of temporal context and temporally-graded behavioral contiguity effects.
Advances in the theory of temporal and spatial context
In parallel with empirical progress, recent theoretical developments have led to an integrative hypothesis for spatial and temporal context that unifies a wide range of behavioral and neural phenomena. A large body of behavioral and theoretical work has suggested that the representations supporting memory should be temporally scale-invariant, exhibiting similar sensitivity to temporal relationships over a range of time scales [55, 56, 57]. Inspired by the idea that memory ought to be scale-invariant, this theoretical approach develops a set of equations to describe a compressed representation of recent experience [58, 59]. Because the temporal history changes gradually from one moment to the next, this neural representation leads to similar predictions regarding temporally-graded associations. Moreover, because it contains information about the temporal history leading up to the present, this representation can also be used to account for phenomena that depend on explicit temporal information, including judgment of recency tasks and scale-invariant timing behavior [50]. Rather than merely changing gradually over time, this compressed timeline provides a record of what stimuli happened at what point in the past (Fig. 1A).
At the neural level, such a timeline would consist of cells that fire conjunctively when a particular stimulus was experienced at a certain point in the past. In this way, a set of these cells would enable reconstruction of what happened when in the past. These predictions resemble the properties of hippocampal “time cells” that fire sequentially during a circumscribed period within a delay period [60, 49]. Consistent with the hypothesis that the compressed representation of the past is accessed in many different forms of memory, time cells are not only observed in the hippocampus [51], but also the entorhinal cortex [52], striatum [54, 61], and medial PFC [53, 62]. Notably, in all of these studies, time cells show a characteristic compression as qualitatively predicted by theory (Fig. 1B).
The mathematical specifics of the compressed representation [58, 59] endow it with numerous desirable theoretical properties. The compressed timeline, manifest as a set of units with firing properties like time cells, is generated from an intermediate representation. This intermediate representation stores at each moment the Laplace transform of the input history as a function of time. By modulating the equations governing the Laplace transform with velocity, one can construct the Laplace transform with respect to position. By providing different input functions and modulating the Laplace transform with various functions, one can generate a “particle zoo” of place cells observed in the hippocampal formation during spatial navigation tasks in rodents [63] (Fig. 2A). As such, the same mathematical framework can be used to construct a representation of temporal context, of spatial context, and of conjoint spatiotemporal context [65].
This unified account of spatial context, temporal context and spatiotemporal context aligns well with recent fMRI studies virtual reality suggesting not only that similar regions participate in recovery of spatial and temporal context [66, 67, 64] (see also Fig. 2B). These findings have been extended to real world memory [68], where hippocampal ensembles capture not only the spatial distance between memories and the temporal distance between memories, but also the interaction of spatial and temporal distance. Rodent work demonstrating that entorhinal grid cells have time-cell-like properties in time cell experiments [52], suggesting that spatial and temporal context representations are supported in some cases by the same neurons. Moreover, because of the mathematical properties of the representation, it can be extended further to generate compressed representations of the future [69].
Conclusions
Recent years have shed light on retrieved temporal context models of episodic memory. There is now strong evidence from animal and human work that brain states change gradually over time reflecting recent stimuli and that these brain states predict behavior, as predicted by retrieved context models. Although the recovery of these gradually-changing states—a jump back in time—has not been definitively established, a growing body of evidence is consistent with predictions of retrieved temporal context models, especially with regard to transitive associations that bridge across episodes. In addition, theoretical developments have produced a richer and more detailed hypothesis about the nature of temporal context. These new models account for a much wider range of behavioral phenomena, provide a natural explanation of sequentially-activated time cells and provide a natural theoretical unification between temporal context, spatial context, and spatiotemporal context.
Acknowledgments
Supported by NSF IIS 1631460, NIBIB R01EB022864, and NIMH R01MH112169.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Tulving E. Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving E, Donaldson W, editors. Organization of Memory. Adademic Press; New York: 1972. pp. 381–403. [Google Scholar]
- 2.O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research. 1971;34(1):171–175. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science. 1993;261:1055–8. doi: 10.1126/science.8351520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Burgess N, O’Keefe J. Neuronal computations underlying the firing of place cells and their role in navigation. Hippocampus. 1996;6(6):749–62. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:6<749::AID-HIPO16>3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Estes WK. Statistical theory of spontaneous recovery and regression. Psychological Review. 1955;62:145–154. doi: 10.1037/h0048509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mensink G-JM, Raaijmakers JGW. A model for contextual fluctuation. Journal of Mathematical Psychology. 1989;33:172–186. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Murdock BB. Context and mediators in a theory of distributed associative memory (TODAM2) Psychological Review. 1997;104(2):839–862. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Howard MW, Kahana MJ. A distributed representation of temporal context. Journal of Mathematical Psychology. 2002;46(3):269–299. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sederberg PB, Howard MW, Kahana MJ. A context-based theory of recency and contiguity in free recall. Psychological Review. 2008;115:893–912. doi: 10.1037/a0013396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Polyn SM, Norman KA, Kahana MJ. A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. Psychological Review. 2009;116:129–156. doi: 10.1037/a0014420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Manns JR, Howard MW, Eichenbaum HB. Gradual changes in hippocampal activity support remembering the order of events. Neuron. 2007;56(3):530–540. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mankin EA, Sparks FT, Slayyeh B, Sutherland RJ, Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK. Neuronal code for extended time in the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:19462–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214107109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hyman JM, Ma L, Balaguer-Ballester E, Durstewitz D, Seamans JK. Contextual encoding by ensembles of medial prefrontal cortex neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2012;109:5086–91. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114415109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ziv Y, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Hamel EO, Ghosh KK, Kitch LJ, El Gamal A, Schnitzer MJ. Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16(3):264–6. doi: 10.1038/nn.3329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rowland DC, Roudi Y, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Ten years of grid cells. Annual review of neuroscience. 2016;39:19–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Finkelstein A, Las L, Ulanovsky N. 3-d maps and compasses in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2016;39:171–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Moser M-B, Rowland DC, Moser EI. Place cells, grid cells, and memory. Cold Spring Harbor perspectives in biology. 2015;7(2):a021808. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hintzman DL. Is memory organized by temporal contiguity? Memory & Cognition. 2016;44(3):365–375. doi: 10.3758/s13421-015-0573-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Healey MK, Kahana MJ. Is memory search governed by universal principles or idiosyncratic strategies? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2014;143(2):575. doi: 10.1037/a0033715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sadeh T, Moran R, Goshen-Gottstein Y. When items ‘pop into mind’: variability in temporal-context reinstatement in free-recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2015;22(3):779–90. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0746-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Siegel LL, Kahana MJ. A retrieved context account of spacing and repetition effects in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 2014;40(3):755–764. doi: 10.1037/a0035585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Healey MK, Kahana MJ. A four-component model of age-related memory change. Psychological Review. 2016;123(1):23–69. doi: 10.1037/rev0000015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mankin EA, Diehl GW, Sparks FT, Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK. Hippocampal CA2 activity patterns change over time to a larger extent than between spatial contexts. Neuron. 2015;85(1):190–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rubin A, Geva N, Sheintuch L, Ziv Y. Hippocampal ensemble dynamics timestamp events in long-term memory. eLife. 2015;4:e12247. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25•.Rashid AJ, Yan C, Mercaldo V, Hsiang H-LL, Park S, Cole CJ, De Cristofaro A, Yu J, Ramakrishnan C, Lee SY, et al. Competition between engrams influences fear memory formation and recall. Science. 2016;353(6297):383–387. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0594. Provides evidence for temporally-varying neural ensembles in amygdala, coincident with temporally-graded fear memory. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26••.Cai DJ, Aharoni D, Shuman T, Shobe J, Biane J, Song W, Wei B, Veshkini M, La-Vu M, Lou J, Flores SE, Kim I, Sano Y, Zhou M, Baumgartel K, Lavi A, Kamata M, Tuzsynski M, Mayford M, Golshani P, Silva A. A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature. 2016;534(7605):115–118. doi: 10.1038/nature17955. This technical tour-de-force demonstrates that hippocampal ensembles change over hours coincident with temporally-graded fear memory. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lositsky O, Chen J, Toker D, Honey CJ, Shvartsman M, Poppenk JL, Hasson U, Norman KA. Neural pattern change during encoding of a narrative predicts retrospective duration estimates. eLife. 2016;5:e16070. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Jenkins LJ, Ranganath C. Distinct neural mechanisms for remembering when an event occurred. Hippocampus. 2016;26:554–559. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.DuBrow S, Davachi L. Temporal memory is shaped by encoding stability and intervening item reactivation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(42):13998–4005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2535-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.DuBrow S, Davachi L. Temporal binding within and across events. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2016;134:107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hsieh L-T, Gruber MJ, Jenkins LJ, Ranganath C. Hippocampal activity patterns carry information about objects in temporal context. Neuron. 2014;81(5):1165–1178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hsieh L-T, Ranganath C. Cortical and subcortical contributions to sequence retrieval: Schematic coding of temporal context in the neocortical recollection network. NeuroImage. 2015;121:78–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wang F, Diana RA. Temporal context processing within hippocampal subfields. Neuroimage. 2016;134:261–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Chan SCY, Applegate MC, Morton NW, Polyn SM, Norman KA. Lingering representations of stimuli influence recall organization. Neuropsychologia. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kragel JE, Morton NW, Polyn SM. Neural activity in the medial temporal lobe reveals the fidelity of mental time travel. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35(7):2914–2926. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3378-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Manning JR, Polyn SM, Litt B, Baltuch G, Kahana MJ. Oscillatory patterns in temporal lobe reveal context reinstatement during memory search. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA. 2011;108(31):12893–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015174108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Howard MW, Viskontas IV, Shankar KH, Fried I. Ensembles of human MTL neurons “jump back in time” in response to a repeated stimulus. Hippocampus. 2012;22(9):1833–1847. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Yaffe RB, Kerr MSD, Damera S, Sarma SV, Inati SK, Zaghloul KA. Reinstatement of distributed cortical oscillations occurs with precise spatiotemporal dynamics during successful memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111(52):18727–32. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417017112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gerraty RT, Davidow JY, Wimmer GE, Kahn I, Shohamy D. Transfer of learning relates to intrinsic connectivity between hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and large-scale networks. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(34):11297–11303. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0185-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Shohamy D, Daw ND. Integrating memories to guide decisions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;5:85–90. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schlichting ML, Zeithamova D, Preston AR. CA1 subfield contributions to memory integration and inference. Hippocampus. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Schlichting ML, Preston AR. Memory integration: neural mechanisms and implications for behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;1:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schapiro AC, Turk-Browne NB, Norman KA, Botvinick MM. Statistical learning of temporal community structure in the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2016;26(1):3–8. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kurth-Nelson Z, Barnes G, Sejdinovic D, Dolan R, Dayan P. Temporal structure in associative retrieval. Elife. 2015;4:e04919. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Banino A, Koster R, Hassabis D, Kumaran D. Retrieval-based model accounts for striking profile of episodic memory and generalization. Scientific Reports. 6 doi: 10.1038/srep31330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schapiro AC, Turk-Browne NB, Botvinick MM, Norman KA. Complementary learning systems within the hippocampus: A neural network modeling approach to reconciling episodic memory with statistical learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2017;372(1711):20160049. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ramirez S, Tonegawa S, Liu X. Identification and optogenetic manipulation of memory engrams in the hippocampus. Neural circuits underlying emotion and motivation: Insights from optogenetics and pharmacogenetics. 2015:108. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tiganj Z, Shankar KH, Howard MW. AAAI 2017 Spring Symposium Series - Science of Intelligence: Computational Principles of Natural and Artificial Intelligence. 2017. Scale invariant value computation for reinforcement learning in continuous time. [Google Scholar]
- 49.MacDonald CJ, Lepage KQ, Eden UT, Eichenbaum H. Hippocampal “time cells” bridge the gap in memory for discontiguous events. Neuron. 2011;71(4):737–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50•.Howard MW, Shankar KH, Aue W, Criss AH. A distributed representation of internal time. Psychological Review. 2015;122(1):24–53. doi: 10.1037/a0037840. Presents a new formulation for temporal context and detailed behavioral models for a wide range of tasks including conditioning, interval timing, short-term and long-term memory, and episodic recall. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Salz DM, Tiganj Z, Khasnabish S, Kohley A, Sheehan D, Howard MW, Eichenbaum H. Time cells in hippocampal area CA3. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:7476–7484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0087-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kraus BJ, Brandon MP, Robinson RJ, Connerney MA, Hasselmo ME, Eichenbaum H. During running in place, grid cells integrate elapsed time and distance run. Neuron. 2015;88(3):578–589. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tiganj Z, Kim J, Jung MW, Howard MW. Sequential firing codes for time in rodent mPFC. Cerebral Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Mello GB, Soares S, Paton JJ. A scalable population code for time in the striatum. Current Biology. 2015;25(9):1113–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Chater N, Brown GDA. From universal laws of cognition to specific cognitive models. Cognitive Science. 2008;32(1):36–67. doi: 10.1080/03640210701801941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brown GDA, Neath I, Chater N. A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review. 2007;114(3):539–76. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Balsam PD, Gallistel CR. Temporal maps and informativeness in associative learning. Trends in Neuroscience. 2009;32(2):73–78. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Shankar KH, Howard MW. A scale-invariant internal representation of time. Neural Computation. 2012;24(1):134–193. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Shankar KH, Howard MW. Optimally fuzzy temporal memory. Journal of Machine Learning Research. 2013;14:3753–3780. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, Buzsaki G. Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science. 2008;321(5894):1322–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1159775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Akhlaghpour H, Wiskerke J, Choi JY, Taliaferro JP, Au J, Witten I. Dissociated sequential activity and stimulus encoding in the dorsomedial striatum during spatial working memory. eLife. 2016;5:e19507. doi: 10.7554/eLife.19507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bolkan SS, Stujenske JM, Parnaudeau S, Spellman TJ, Rauffenbart C, Abbas AI, Harris AZ, Gordon JA, Kellendonk C. Thalamic projections sustain prefrontal activity during working memory maintenance. Nature Neuroscience. doi: 10.1038/nn.4568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Howard MW, MacDonald CJ, Tiganj Z, Shankar KH, Du Q, Hasselmo ME, Eichenbaum H. A unified mathematical framework for coding time, space, and sequences in the hippocampal region. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(13):4692–707. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5808-12.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64•.Deuker L, Bellmund JL, Schröder TN, Doeller CF. An event map of memory space in the hippocampus. eLife. 2016;5:e16534. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16534. Elegant virtual reality experiment that shows that retrieved memories from close together in virtual space and experienced close together in time retrieve similar multivoxel patterns of activity. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Howard MW, Eichenbaum H. Time and space in the hippocampus. Brain Research. 2015;1621:345–354. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.10.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Kyle CT, Smuda DN, Hassan AS, Ekstrom AD. Roles of human hippocampal subfields in retrieval of spatial and temporal context. Behavioural brain research. 2015;278:549–558. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Stokes J, Kyle C, Ekstrom AD. Complementary roles of human hippocampal subfields in differentiation and integration of spatial context. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2015;27(3):546–559. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68••.Nielson DM, Smith TA, Sreekumar V, Dennis S, Sederberg PB. Human hippocampus represents space and time during retrieval of real-world memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(35):11078–11083. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507104112. fMRI study that studies memory for real-world experiences. Participants viewed pictures from their own lives. Neural distance in the hippocampus as participants viewed remembered pictures corresponded to the real-world spatial and temporal distance between the experiences. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Shankar KH, Singh I, Howard MW. Neural mechanism to simulate a scale-invariant future. Neural Computation. 2016;28:2594–2627. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]