Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2017 Oct;158:729–736. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.07.038

The role of public and private natural space in children's social, emotional and behavioural development in Scotland: A longitudinal study

Elizabeth A Richardson a,, Jamie Pearce a, Niamh K Shortt a, Richard Mitchell b
PMCID: PMC5571194  PMID: 28750342

Abstract

Introduction

Poor mental health in childhood has implications for health and wellbeing in later life. Natural space may benefit children's social, emotional and behavioural development. We investigated whether neighbourhood natural space and private garden access were related to children's developmental change over time. We asked whether relationships differed between boys and girls, or by household educational status.

Methods

We analysed longitudinal data for 2909 urban-dwelling children (aged 4 at 2008/9 baseline) from the Growing Up in Scotland (GUS) survey. The survey provided social, emotional and behavioural difficulty scores (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)), and private garden access. Area (%) of total natural space and parks within 500 m of the child's home was quantified using Scotland's Greenspace Map. Interactions for park area, total natural space area, and private garden access with age and age2 were modelled to quantify their independent contributions to SDQ score change over time.

Results

Private garden access was strongly related to most SDQ domains, while neighbourhood natural space was related to better social outcomes. We found little evidence that neighbourhood natural space or garden access influenced the trajectory of developmental change between 4 and 6 years, suggesting that any beneficial influences had occurred at younger ages. Stratified models showed the importance of parks for boys, and private gardens for the early development of children from low-education households.

Conclusion

We conclude that neighbourhood natural space may reduce social, emotional and behavioural difficulties for 4–6 year olds, although private garden access may be most beneficial.

Abbreviations: CI, Confidence Interval; GUS, Growing Up in Scotland survey; IQR, Interquartile Range; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SF-12, The 12-item Short Form health questionnaire; SGM, Scotland's Greenspace Map; SIMD, Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation

Keywords: Nature, Children, Social development, Emotional development, Behavioural development, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

Highlights

  • Children with gardens had better social, emotional and behavioural scores.

  • Children with more neighbourhood natural space had better social skills.

  • Developmental trajectories did not differ with natural space availability.

1. Introduction

Poor mental health in childhood has implications for health and wellbeing in later life, and presents a considerable burden for families and wider society. In the short term, for example, school attainment may be impaired (Trout et al., 2003), while in the longer term persistent mental health issues, higher mortality rates and wider inequalities may result (Dube et al., 2003, Jokela et al., 2009). Recent decades have seen substantial increases in the prevalence of childhood social, emotional, and behavioural problems (Layard and Dunn, 2009). To address this upward trend, and the consequent growing societal burden now and in the future, it is imperative to identify the determinants of these childhood problems. Individual, family, and household characteristics contribute, but they do not explain all of the variation in risk (Bradshaw and Tipping, 2010, Wilson et al., 2012). Environmental influences – including noise (Forns et al., 2015), air pollution (Forns et al., 2015), and a lack of contact with natural space (Amoly et al., 2014) – have also been identified as possible risk factors for poor mental health in childhood.

Our study examines the role that natural space might play in children's development. Louv (2005) argued that there are substantial negative effects of ‘alienation’ from nature, and that these may be the root cause of increases in childhood developmental problems. Today's children spend less time outdoors in nature than previous generations (Gaster, 1991), and tend to be less physically active and more obese (Anon, 2013). Urbanisation, increasingly indoor pastimes, and parental concerns about safety may all have contributed to declining childhood nature experiences (Strife and Downey, 2009, Valentine and McKendrick, 1997).

A growing body of research has found that children who live or spend time in more natural surroundings typically have fewer social, emotional and behavioural problems than those in less green settings (Amoly et al., 2014, Faber Taylor and Kuo, 2009). A number of causative mechanisms have been suggested. Firstly, experiences of natural environments may directly restore a child's attention by giving fatigued cognitive processes the opportunity to rest (“Attention Restoration Theory”, Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989). In a US study schoolchildren who moved to more natural settings exhibited greater improvement in their attention levels than others (Wells, 2000), and in Barcelona, children with greener surroundings had better memory and attention levels (Dadv and et al., 2015). Secondly, natural environments may support stress reduction through favourable physiological responses (“Psychoevolutionary Theory”; Ulrich, 1983). Wells and Evans (2003) reported that levels of nearby nature buffered the impact of stressful life events on schoolchildren. Thirdly, natural environments may increase opportunities for play (Almanza et al., 2012), which in natural settings is typically more creative, adventurous, social, and challenging than play elsewhere (Hart, 1979). Indeed, increased usage of green space in urban areas has been linked to improved health and wellbeing in Scottish schoolchildren (McCracken et al., 2016). Fourthly, natural space availability may indirectly affect the child via effects on their carer. Exposure to natural spaces has been linked with better mental health in adulthood (Hartig et al., 2014), and the carer's mental health can influence early childhood development (Marryat and Martin, 2010).

Research into the potential role of nature in childhood development focusses on school-aged children. However, considering younger children is critical because of the important capabilities in exploration, imagination, socialisation, and control that develop through increasingly independent play at younger ages (Bee, 1992, Erikson, 1963). Further, different types of natural space may be more or less beneficial for children's development, but this has been little researched. The developmental benefits of play are optimised when children are able to explore the space and construct things (e.g., shelters) with minimal adult intrusion, and to interact with others (Hart, 1979). Expansive public spaces may therefore be more beneficial (e.g. parks rather than private gardens or overall natural space). Indeed, Lithuanian 4–6 year olds had fewer emotional and behavioural problems if they had better availability of parks nearby, although these problems were not related to overall green space (Balseviciene et al., 2014). Alternatively, play with minimal supervision – particularly for young children – may satisfy parents’ safety concerns more if it takes place in a private garden rather than a public space. In this case having access to a private garden may be more important than natural space in the neighbourhood: 3–7 year old children in England with access to a garden had lower levels of social, emotional and behavioural problems, but neighbourhood green space was unrelated (Flouri et al., 2014).

Evidence for the determinants of early childhood development problems is urgently needed to inform public health interventions. Here we expand the evidence base by investigating whether social, emotional and behavioural development for young children (age 4 at baseline) is better for those with more natural space around their homes, and particularly more public park space, or whether access to a private garden is more important. We explore differences by sex and household socioeconomic status, given known differences in how these groups use and are affected by their local environments (Cleland et al., 2010, de Vries et al., 2003).

2. Methods

2.1. Study population

We analysed data from the Growing Up in Scotland (GUS) survey (Scottish Centre for Social Research, 2012). GUS's nationally-representative birth cohort sample was selected in 2005/2006 (n = 5217 achieved interviews) from families with babies of approximately 12 months in receipt of child benefits (97% of families with children in Scotland) – at that time a non-means-tested benefit paid to carers of children under 16 – and was followed up annually thereafter. Sampling stratification ensured a representative selection of areas of differing socioeconomic status within each local authority (Wilson et al., 2012). We selected respondents from wave five (age 5, 2009/2010; n = 3833 achieved interviews) because these children's home postcodes were available through a secure setting. There are over 200,000 postcodes in Scotland, each representing approximately 15 households. We selected the 2909 children (76%) living in areas of Scotland covered by the urban natural space data (see Section 2.3) at wave five. The child's wave four (age 4) and six (age 6) survey data could be included if they had been living at their wave five address then (i.e., non-movers), resulting in an additional 2650 wave four and 2482 wave six observations.

2.2. Outcome variables

Social, emotional and behavioural difficulties were assessed using the 25-item Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997) in waves four, five and six. The SDQ – a behavioural screening tool designed for children between 3 and 16 years old – has been widely used internationally, owing to its good psychometric properties and clinical utility (Theunissen et al., 2015). The questionnaire was self-completed by the main carer, usually the mother.

For each SDQ domain - Hyperactivity Problems, Emotional Problems, Peer Problems, Conduct Problems, and Prosocial Behaviour - the respondent was asked whether each of five items (Table 1) was ‘Not true’, ‘Somewhat true’ or ‘Certainly true’ of the child's behaviour over the last six months. Responses were scored 0, 1, or 2, with 2 being the most negative (or most positive, in the case of Prosocial Behaviour). The scores were summed to give a domain score of 0–10, and a Total Difficulties score (ranging 0–40) was calculated by summing all domains except Prosocial Behaviour. Higher scores indicated worse problems (opposite for Prosocial Behaviour).

Table 1.

Items within the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) domains.

SDQ domain Items
Hyperactivity Problems Restless, overactive, cannot stay still for long
Constantly fidgeting or squirming
Easily distracted, concentration wanders
Thinks things out before acting
Sees tasks through to the end, good attention span
Emotional Problems Often complains of headaches, stomach aches or sickness
Many worries, often seems worried
Often unhappy, downhearted or tearful
Nervous or clingy in new situations, easily loses confidence
Has many fears, is easily scared
Peer Problems Rather solitary, tends to play alone
Has at least one good friend
Generally liked by other children
Picked on or bullied by other children
Gets on better with adults than with other children
Conduct Problems Often has temper tantrums or hot tempers
Generally obedient, usually does what adults request
Often fights with other children or bullies them
Often lies or cheats
Steals from home, school or elsewhere
Prosocial Behaviour Considerate of other people's feelings
Shares readily with other children
Helpful if someone is hurt, upset or feeling ill
Kind to younger children
Often volunteers to help others (parents, teachers, other children)

2.3. Natural space measures

We quantified the area of public parks and total natural space around each child's home, using 2011 data (at around wave six). We obtained ‘Scotland's Greenspace Map’ (SGM; Greenspace Scotland, 2011) in geographical information system shapefile format. The SGM study area covered settlements in Scotland with populations greater than 3000 in 2001, plus a 500 m buffer. Each polygon of a high resolution (centimetre-accuracy) vector map product (Ordnance Survey's MasterMap) had been manually classified into types (e.g., park, playing field, church yard, or school ground) using aerial photography. We used the primary land use class only, unless ‘public park’ had been identified as a secondary land use (all were included as public park).

We found some incomplete mapping and overlapping polygons in the SGM dataset. We identified postcodes within the study area that did not have natural space mapped within 30 m, and used aerial photography (Google Maps) to verify this. We found 740 postcodes (0.5% of a total of 157,282) with incomplete natural space mapping, and excluded these from the analysis. The full list of excluded postcodes is available as Supplemental Material. Overlapping polygons were identified in 1909 locations (mean overlap size 106 m2). One overlapping portion in each case was deleted to prevent artificially-inflated area calculations. Portions of parkland were preferentially retained in the dataset (e.g., if woodland overlapped with park the woodland polygon was deleted).

Agricultural land and some open water had not been mapped in the SGM. As both land uses could provide nature experiences we augmented the dataset accordingly. Agricultural areas were extracted from the European Environment Agency's 2006 CORINE dataset (CORINE classes 12–22) and added to SGM if they occurred in unmapped parts of the study area. Open water areas not already mapped in the study area were added from Ordnance Survey's VectorMap product.

We calculated the area of public parks and total natural space within 500 m (Euclidean distance) of each child's postcode, representing a young child's walk of approximately 10 min. Total natural space included all public and private natural surfaces – vegetation, water, sand, mud and rock – and included private gardens. Geoprocessing was conducted using ArcMap 10.1 software (ESRI, Redlands, CA).

Whether the child had access (sole or shared) to a private garden was obtained from the survey data.

2.4. Covariates

We adjusted for possible confounders of the relationship between the child's SDQ scores and natural space (Bradshaw and Tipping, 2010, Pachter et al., 2006, Wilson et al., 2012). Child covariates were sex, age (decimal years, centred at the grand mean of 4.85), age2 (to capture non-linear temporal trends; mean-centred), and hours of screen time per day (constrained to a maximum of 8 h to address some erroneous values). Household covariates were highest educational attainment (degree or equivalent, vocational qualification below degree, Higher/Standard grades or equivalent, and other or no qualifications), equivalised annual income (continuous), and the carer's mental component summary score on the SF-12 questionnaire (0–100, with higher score indicating better mental health; Ware et al., 1996). Neighbourhood-level disadvantage was measured using national-level quintiles of the 2009 Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation (SIMD; Scottish Government, 2009), for the child's residential ‘datazone’ (administrative unit containing 500–1000 residents). Missing values for the dependent and independent variables (see Table 2) were imputed (five imputations) using multiple imputation with chained equations in Stata SE/14.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).

Table 2.

Individual, household and neighbourhood characteristics for the 2909 children in the sample, as at wave five.

Level Characteristic Mean (95% CI) Count (%)
Individual Age (years) 4.85 (4.85, 4.85)
Sex
Male 1478 (51)
Female 1431 (49)
Screen time (hours) 2.38 (2.32, 2.44)
Missing 708 (24)
SDQ score:
Hyperactivity Problems 3.69 (3.60, 3.77)
Missing 32 (1)
Emotional Problems 1.25 (1.19, 1.30)
Missing 24 (1)
Peer Problems 1.05 (1.00, 1.10)
Missing 23 (1)
Conduct Problems 1.70 (1.65, 1.76)
Missing 23 (1)
Total Difficulties 7.67 (7.50, 7.84)
Missing 37 (1)
Prosocial Behaviour 8.22 (8.16, 8.28)
Missing 24 (1)
Dental caries
Yes 220 (8)
No 2689 (92)
Household Highest educational attainment:
Degree 1108 (38)
Vocational qualification 1109 (38)
Higher/Standard grade 406 (14)
Other/no qualification 135 (5)
Missing 151 (5)
Access to a garden?
Yes 2740 (94)
No 166 (6)
Missing 3 (0)
Equivalised household income (£000 s) 24.08 (23.61, 24.55)
Missing 157 (5)
Carer's mental health score (SF12) 50.34 (50.00, 50.69)
Missing 20 (1)
Neighbourhood SIMD quintile:
Quintile 1 (least deprived) 741 (25)
Quintile 2 424 (15)
Quintile 3 480 (17)
Quintile 4 608 (21)
Quintile 5 (most deprived) 656 (23)
Total natural space (% within 500 m) 63.07 (62.58, 63.55)
Park space (% within 500 m) 4.69 (4.44, 4.94)

2.5. Statistical analyses

We ran random-intercept repeated-measures linear models, with waves nested within individuals nested within the survey's Primary Sampling Units. We entered percentage total natural space, percentage park space, and garden access as main effects, to quantify whether they had a consistent association with each SDQ domain across the study period. We also included their interactions with age and age2, to quantify their contributions to SDQ score trajectories between ages 4 and 6. To assess their independent contributions to any relationship found, park space was modelled concurrently with total natural space, even though the latter included the former. On average only 3% (median) of total natural space was park, and the areas of both correlated weakly (r = 0.15). To capture the individual children's SDQ score trajectories we tested the inclusion of a random slope for age, but as this did not alter the results these models are not presented.

We ran the models in MLwiN (Rasbash et al., 2009) using Stata's runmlwin routine (Leckie and Charlton, 2013). First-order marginal quasi-likelihood estimates were used as initial values for a Markov Chain Monte Carlo estimation (Browne, 2016). The models used a Markov chain length of 40,000, with orthogonal parameterisation and hierarchical centring at the individual level. We did not weight our urban subsample as we were not seeking to produce representative estimates for the wider population. After running whole sample models we stratified by sex and by household educational attainment (degree/equivalent versus lower).

Our models may have been subject to residual confounding, particularly if we had not fully captured socioeconomic disadvantage. We therefore chose to also model a socially-patterned control outcome without a plausible link to natural space: whether the child had fillings (treatment for dental caries) by wave five. Among our sample, prevalence rates of dental caries were highest for children from households with the lowest educational attainment (10%) and the most deprived areas (10%), and lowest for those from the most educated households (5%) and the least deprived neighbourhoods (7%).

To retain the full amount of information in the SDQ scores we modelled them as continuous variables. To test the sensitivity of the results to this decision we also ran logistic versions of the main models, with the scores dichotomised into normal/borderline and abnormal, as defined by Goodman (1997). Abnormal scores were ≥17 for Total Difficulties, ≥4 for Conduct Problems, and Peer Problems, ≥5 for Emotional Problems, ≥7 for Hyperactivity Problems, and ≤4 for Prosocial Behaviour.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive statistics

The children had an average of 63% natural space within 500 m of their home postcode (Table 2). A small proportion of this (7%) was public park. We present model coefficients for an interquartile range (IQR) increase in each type: 16.2% points for total natural space and 6.8 for public parks. Most (94%) of the children had access to a garden. Those without access to a garden had significantly less total natural space around their homes (54.8%, 95% confidence interval 52.3–57.4) than those with garden access (63.6%, 63.1–64.1; t = 8.3, d.f. = 2904, p < 0.001), and significantly more park space (6.5% 5.3–7.7%) c.f. (4.6% 4.3–4.8%; t = −3.4, d.f. = 2904, p < 0.001). Autocorrelation between these predictor variables did not disrupt the models, however.

Natural space availability was socially patterned: garden access was significantly more common for those from the least deprived neighbourhoods (χ2 = 82.5, p < 0.001) and most educated households (χ2 = 37.1, p < 0.001). Children from the least deprived neighbourhoods also had significantly more total natural space (t = 8.3, d.f. = 1395, p < 0.001), and significantly less public park space (t = −5.0, d.f. = 1395, p < 0.001), than those from the most deprived neighbourhoods. Neighbourhood natural space availability, however, did not vary with household educational attainment.

Cronbach's alpha coefficients, indicating the internal consistency of the items within each SDQ scale, were good for Total Difficulties (0.86), Emotional Symptoms (0.74), and Prosocial Behaviour (0.79), acceptable for Hyperactivity Problems (0.60) and poor for Conduct Problems (0.58), and Peer Problems (0.59).

3.2. Main analysis

The models’ age and age2 coefficients (Table 3) described the children's SDQ problem trajectories between waves four and six. Peer Problems and Total Difficulties did not change significantly, whereas Conduct Problems and Prosocial Behaviour improved over the period, and Hyperactivity and Emotional Problems worsened slightly until mean age and improved thereafter. In general, neither parks nor total natural space were associated with the SDQ domains or their change over time, although an IQR increase in total natural space was associated with Prosocial Behaviour scores 0.08 points higher (i.e., better behaviour). Having access to a garden was substantially more important than local natural space: after adjustment for both types of natural space availability those children without garden access had significantly higher scores for Hyperactivity Problems (+0.52), Peer Problems (+0.23), Conduct Problems (+0.27), and Total Difficulties (+1.15). Garden access, however, was not related to SDQ change over time. Increased screen time was related to worse outcomes for all SDQ domains except Conduct Problems.

Table 3.

Coefficients (95% CI) for the relationship between natural space (total, parks and garden access) with SDQ domain scores and their change over time.

Hyperactivity problems Emotional problems Peer Problems Conduct problems Total difficulties Prosocial behaviour
FIXED PART
Total natural space (per IQRa increase)
Total % −0.05 (−0.14, 0.04) −0.04 (−0.10, 0.01) −0.04 (−0.09, 0.01) 0.01 (−0.05, 0.06) −0.13 (−0.30, 0.05) 0.08 (0.02, 0.14)*
Total % x Age 0.30 (−0.45, 1.05) 0.23 (−0.34, 0.81) −0.24 (−0.78, 0.30) −0.04 (−0.56, 0.48) 0.25 (−1.18, 1.69) 0.00 (−0.68, 0.69)
Total % x Age2 −0.03 (−0.10, 0.05) −0.02 (−0.08, 0.04) 0.03 (−0.03, 0.08) 0.01 (−0.05, 0.06) −0.02 (−0.17, 0.13) 0.00 (−0.07, 0.07)
Park space (per IQRa increase)
Park % −0.02 (−0.10, 0.05) −0.03 (−0.07, 0.02) −0.03 (−0.07, 0.01) −0.02 (−0.06, 0.02) −0.10 (−0.24, 0.04) 0.00 (−0.05, 0.05)
Park % x Age 0.42 (−0.21, 1.05) 0.10 (−0.35, 0.55) −0.18 (−0.63, 0.28) −0.24 (−0.70, 0.22) 0.11 (−1.12, 1.35) 0.43 (−0.15, 1.00)
Park % x Age2 −0.04 (−0.11, 0.02) −0.01 (−0.05, 0.04) 0.02 (−0.03, 0.06) 0.02 (−0.02, 0.07) −0.01 (−0.14, 0.11) −0.04 (−0.10, 0.02)
No garden access (ref: Yes)
No access 0.52 (0.20, 0.84)** 0.12 (−0.07, 0.31) 0.23 (0.04, 0.41)* 0.27 (0.09, 0.46)** 1.15 (0.52, 1.78)*** 0.10 (−0.13, 0.33)
No access x Age −0.07 (−2.94, 2.80) 0.59 (−1.47, 2.64) 0.52 (−1.63, 2.68) 0.19 (−1.68, 2.05) 1.19 (−4.57, 6.96) −2.05 (−4.49, 0.39)
No access x Age2 0.03 (−0.27, 0.32) −0.06 (−0.27, 0.16) −0.04 (−0.26, 0.18) −0.02 (−0.21, 0.18) −0.08 (−0.68, 0.51) 0.20 (−0.06, 0.45)
Constant 6.42 (5.90, 6.94)*** 2.52 (2.19, 2.85)*** 2.22 (1.87, 2.56)*** 3.51 (3.19, 3.84)*** 14.68 (13.60, 15.77)*** 6.95 (6.56, 7.35)***
Sex (ref: Male) −0.71 (−0.85, −0.57)*** −0.02 (−0.10, 0.07) −0.16 (−0.24, −0.08)*** −0.26 (−0.35, −0.18)*** −1.14 (−1.42, −0.87)*** 0.48 (0.38, 0.57)***
Age (mean-centred) 1.32 (0.70, 1.94)*** 0.47 (0.01, 0.94)* −0.30 (−0.74, 0.15) −0.58 (−1.02, −0.15)** 0.89 (−0.30, 2.08) 1.17 (0.61, 1.72)***
Age2 (mean-centred) −0.14 (−0.20, −0.08)*** −0.05 (−0.09, 0.00) 0.02 (−0.02, 0.07) 0.04 (0.00, 0.09) −0.12 (−0.24, 0.00) −0.09 (−0.15, −0.03)**
Screen time (hours) 0.09 (0.04, 0.15)** 0.06 (0.01, 0.10)* 0.06 (0.02, 0.09)** 0.05 (0.00, 0.10) 0.24 (0.09, 0.40)** −0.08 (−0.12, −0.04)**
Household education (ref: Degree)
Vocational qualification 0.37 (0.19, 0.54)*** 0.04 (−0.07, 0.14) −0.03 (−0.14, 0.07) 0.11 (0.01, 0.21)* 0.49 (0.15, 0.82)** 0.11 (−0.01, 0.23)
Higher/Standard grade 0.52 (0.24, 0.79)*** 0.07 (−0.09, 0.23) 0.10 (−0.04, 0.24) 0.24 (0.10, 0.38)** 0.91 (0.41, 1.42)** −0.06 (−0.24, 0.12)
Other/no qualifications 0.36 (0.02, 0.71)* 0.16 (−0.09, 0.40) 0.23 (0.00, 0.46)* 0.36 (0.09, 0.63)* 1.08 (0.40, 1.77)** −0.05 (−0.34, 0.25)
Equivalised household income (£000 s) −0.01 (−0.02, −0.01)*** 0.00 (−0.01, 0.00) −0.01 (−0.01, 0.00)*** −0.01 (−0.01, 0.00)*** −0.03 (−0.05, −0.02)*** 0.00 (0.00, 0.01)
Carer's mental health score −0.04 (−0.05, −0.03)*** −0.03 (−0.03, −0.02)*** −0.02 (−0.02, −0.02)*** −0.03 (−0.03, −0.03)*** −0.12 (−0.14, −0.11)*** 0.01 (0.01, 0.02)***
SIMD 2009 quintile (ref: Quintile 1)
Quintile 2 0.08 (−0.16, 0.31) 0.03 (−0.11, 0.17) 0.05 (−0.08, 0.19) 0.02 (−0.12, 0.16) 0.19 (−0.27, 0.64) −0.05 (−0.21, 0.12)
Quintile 3 0.25 (0.02, 0.47)* 0.08 (−0.06, 0.22) 0.08 (−0.06, 0.21) 0.13 (0.00, 0.27) 0.54 (0.09, 0.99)* −0.01 (−0.17, 0.15)
Quintile 4 0.13 (−0.10, 0.35) 0.16 (0.02, 0.30)* 0.21 (0.07, 0.34)** 0.07 (−0.07, 0.20) 0.58 (0.13, 1.03)* 0.13 (−0.03, 0.29)
Quintile 5 (most deprived) 0.35 (0.11, 0.59)** 0.16 (0.01, 0.31)* 0.26 (0.12, 0.40)*** 0.24 (0.10, 0.38)** 1.03 (0.56, 1.50)*** 0.00 (−0.17, 0.17)
RANDOM PART
Level 1: Wave 1.82 (1.74 to 1.89)*** 1.03 (0.99 to 1.07)*** 0.95 (0.91 to 0.99)*** 0.87 (0.83 to 0.90)*** 6.66 (6.38 to 6.94)*** 1.49 (1.43 to 1.55)***
Level 2: Individual 2.93 (2.74 to 3.13)*** 0.96 (0.89 to 1.03)*** 0.84 (0.77 to 0.90)*** 0.95 (0.89 to 1.02)*** 11.53 (10.78 to 12.28)*** 1.25 (1.15 to 1.34)***
Level 3: PSU 0.01 (−0.01 to 0.03) 0.00 (0.00 to 0.01) 0.00 (0.00 to 0.01) 0.00 (0.00 to 0.01) 0.03 (−0.05 to 0.11) 0.01 (−0.01 to 0.02)
*

0.01≤p < 0.05.

**

0.001≤p < 0.01.

***

p < 0.001.

a

Interquartile ranges in percentage points were 16.2 for total and 6.8 for parks.

3.3. Stratification by sex

Boys’ SDQ scores were not related to total natural space, but some domains were related independently to park space and garden access (Table 4). Boys with garden access had reduced Peer Problems (−0.29), Conduct Problems (−0.34) and Total Difficulties (−1.18) compared to those without. Park area was independently related to boys’ Peer Problems and Conduct Problems, although effect sizes were smaller (−0.08 and −0.22 per IQR increase, respectively). In contrast, girls’ scores on some SDQ domains were related independently to total natural space and garden access but not park space. An IQR increase in total natural space around girls’ homes was associated with fewer Hyperactivity Problems (−0.15), Peer Problems (−0.08), and Total Difficulties (−0.31), and more Prosocial Behaviour (+0.14). Garden access was related to larger SDQ differences than an IQR increase in total natural space for Hyperactivity (−0.65) and Total Difficulties (−1.13). SDQ score change over time was not related to natural space or garden access for girls or boys.

Table 4.

Coefficients for the relationship between natural space (total, parks and garden access) with SDQ domain scores, from modelsa stratified by sex or educational attainment.

Hyperactivity problems Emotional problems Peer problems Conduct problems Total difficulties Prosocial behaviour
BOYS
Total (per IQRb increase) 0.03 −0.03 −0.01 0.04 0.03 0.03
Total × Age (per IQR) −0.23 −0.12 0.02 0.17 −0.14 0.01
Total × Age2 (per IQR) 0.03 0.01 0.00 −0.02 0.03 0.00
Parks (per IQR increase) −0.07 −0.03 −0.08** −0.04 −0.22* 0.00
Parks × Age (per IQR) 0.38 −0.22 −0.33 −0.22 −0.39 0.36
Parks × Age2 (per IQR) −0.04 0.02 0.03 0.02 0.04 −0.04
No garden access 0.40 0.11 0.29* 0.34* 1.18* 0.09
No garden access × Age −1.09 0.96 0.47 1.09 1.19 −2.09
No garden access × Age2 0.16 −0.09 −0.04 −0.09 −0.03 0.21
GIRLS
Total (per IQR increase) −0.15* −0.05 −0.08* −0.04 −0.31* 0.14**
Total × Age (per IQR) 0.89 0.59 −0.50 −0.29 0.66 0.00
Total × Age2 (per IQR) −0.09 −0.06 0.05 0.03 −0.07 0.00
Parks (per IQR increase) 0.03 −0.02 0.02 0.00 0.03 0.00
Parks × Age (per IQR) 0.47 0.49 0.00 −0.25 0.72 0.50
Parks × Age2 (per IQR) −0.05 −0.05 0.00 0.02 −0.08 −0.05
No garden access 0.65** 0.13 0.15 0.20 1.13* 0.12
No garden access × Age 1.19 0.32 0.49 −1.06 1.10 −2.04
No garden access × Age2 −0.13 −0.04 −0.04 0.09 −0.13 0.19
LOW EDUCATION HOUSEHOLDS
Total (per IQR increase) −0.04 −0.06 −0.08* −0.01 −0.18 0.06
Total × Age (per IQR) 0.80 0.59 −0.23 0.17 1.33 −0.12
Total × Age2 (per IQR) −0.08 −0.06 0.02 −0.01 −0.13 0.01
Parks (per IQR increase) −0.01 −0.04 −0.02 0.01 −0.06 −0.03
Parks × Age (per IQR) 0.41 0.06 −0.11 −0.18 0.20 0.61
Parks × Age2 (per IQR) −0.05 0.00 0.01 0.02 −0.02 −0.06
No garden access 0.56** 0.11 0.19 0.26* 1.11** 0.09
No garden access × Age −0.90 −0.13 −0.01 0.30 −0.76 −1.89
No garden access × Age2 0.11 0.02 0.01 −0.03 0.11 0.18
HIGH EDUCATION HOUSEHOLDS
Total (per IQR increase) −0.07 −0.02 0.03 0.03 −0.03 0.12*
Total × Age (per IQR) −0.63 −0.36 −0.28 −0.39 −1.68 0.21
Total × Age2 (per IQR) 0.07 0.04 0.03 0.04 0.18 −0.02
Parks (per IQR increase) −0.04 −0.02 −0.05 −0.06 −0.17 0.05
Parks × Age (per IQR) 0.20 0.12 −0.34 −0.39 −0.43 0.15
Parks × Age2 (per IQR) −0.02 −0.01 0.04 0.04 0.04 −0.02
No garden access 0.40 0.09 0.24 0.27 1.00 0.20
No garden access × Age 5.46 5.47* 3.41 1.16 15.81** −3.98
No garden access × Age2 −0.56 −0.55* −0.31 −0.11 −1.56* 0.37
*

0.01≤p < 0.05.

**

0.001≤p < 0.01.

a

Adjusted for age, age2, sex, screen time, household educational attainment, household equivalised income, carer's mental health, and neighbourhood deprivation.

b

Interquartile ranges in percentage points were 16.2 for total and 6.8 for parks.

3.4. Stratification by household education

An IQR increase in total natural space was associated with fewer Peer Problems (−0.08) for children from low education households, and better Prosocial Behaviour (+0.12) for those from high education households (Table 4). Not having access to a garden was associated with significantly higher levels of Hyperactivity (+0.56), Conduct Problems (+0.26), and Total Difficulties (+1.11) for children from low education but not high education households. SDQ score change over time was unrelated to natural space for either group, but garden access was related to non-linear change in Emotional Problems and Total Difficulties over time for children from high education households. As an example, Fig. 1 shows that the Total Difficulties scores of children from high education families without garden access (N.B., only 2% of the group, n = 66) worsened at a faster rate than for those with garden access.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The predicted trajectory of SDQ Total Difficulties score change between ages 4 and 6 for children from high education families, with and without garden access.

3.5. Sensitivity analyses

The sensitivity analyses broadly confirmed the main results. Again, for most SDQ domains there was no association with parks or total natural space, although an IQR increase in park area was related to a 28% reduction in likelihood of abnormal Emotional Problems (OR 0.72). As in the main analyses, garden access was important: children without access to a garden were more likely to have abnormal Hyperactivity Problems (OR 2.60), and Conduct Problems (OR 1.73).

4. Discussion

In our study of 4–6 year old children in urban Scotland we found that certain groups with more park or total natural space around their homes had slightly better social, emotional and behavioural outcomes, compared to those with less. In contrast, having access to a garden was related to sizeable mental health benefits (particularly for Hyperactivity), on a par with the advantage apparent for girls over boys, children from degree-educated households over those from households with no educational qualifications, or a £20,000 to £50,000 increase in equivalised household income. Change over time, however, was not related to neighbourhood natural space or garden access (with one exception), suggesting that any beneficial influence had already occurred by age 4. The findings suggest that neighbourhood natural space may have a modest relationship with social, emotional and behavioural difficulties for young children, and that private natural spaces may enable the most beneficial experiences for this age group.

We did not hypothesise about the relative importance of public versus private natural space for 4–6 year olds, given the equivocal evidence (see Introduction). Play opportunities are crucial for child development, but these can be provided by public parks, other public natural spaces, or private gardens. Indeed, developmental benefits for 4–6 year olds have been reported for public parks (Balseviciene et al., 2014) and for private gardens (Flouri et al., 2014), but the two have not previously been investigated jointly. In our Scottish study, public parks were only related to improved mental health outcomes for boys (Peer Problems and Total Difficulties, with an independent significant association for private gardens), whereas private garden access was related to some improved outcomes for the whole sample, girls, boys, and those from low education households. Having access to a private garden was more frequently and strongly linked to improved mental health outcomes (Hyperactivity, Peer Problems, Conduct Problems, Total Difficulties) than increased availability of neighbourhood natural space. Carers of 4–6 year olds may be more inclined to allow relatively unsupervised play in a private garden than in public natural space, enabling more beneficial nature experiences to accrue in private settings. Prosocial Behaviour, however, was related to neighbourhood natural space (total) but not access to a private garden. This suggests that public spaces have an important role in facilitating socially-beneficial interactions, perhaps because children and adults outside of the child's immediate family and friends are more likely to be encountered.

Differences were found between boys and girls: parks appeared to be of particular importance for boys, while other natural spaces – such as amenity areas or playing fields – may be just as important for girls’ mentally-stimulating play. Compared with girls, boys in this age group engage in more active play when outdoors (Pate et al., 2013), and parks perhaps offer better safe opportunities for active play than other natural spaces. Furthermore, better social outcomes (fewer Peer Problems and/or more Prosocial Behaviour) were found for all groups with more natural space (total and/or parks) around their homes, again suggesting the importance of neighbourhood natural space in facilitating beneficial social interactions for children.

The relationship between garden access and SDQ outcomes differed by household educational status. Children from low-education households had worse outcomes (Hyperactivity, Conduct Problems and Total Difficulties) across the study period if they had no garden access, but garden access was not related to change in these outcomes over time, suggesting that any beneficial influence had already occurred by age 4. In contrast, the Emotional Problems and Total Difficulties scores of children from high-education households without garden access worsened at a faster rate than the scores of those with garden access. Children from low-education households had significantly less natural space in their neighbourhoods, so having a garden may have compensated for this in their earliest development. The results suggest the importance of garden access for social, emotional and behavioural development of children from higher educational status households becomes apparent at a later stage (i.e., after 4 years). We found no evidence that the beneficial relationships between natural space and SDQ outcomes were stronger for lower socioeconomic status children, in contrast to studies that suggest green space can buffer against the deleterious influences of social disadvantage on health (Mitchell et al., 2015).

Our study found that a high proportion of children in urban Scotland have sole or shared access to a private garden; in countries where this is not the case the potential implications for childhood development should be especially considered. Therefore, further research is needed to understand the implications of access to different forms of natural space in a range of national contexts.

In the whole-sample analyses, and those for all groups except children from high income households, we found no evidence that natural space was related to change in the SDQ outcomes between 4 and 6 years of age. The absence of such relationships suggests that any beneficial influence of park space, total natural space, or garden access had already occurred by age 4. In England, Flouri et al. (2014) also found that neighbourhood green space was unrelated to change in SDQ problem domains over time, for 3–7 year olds.

Across the whole sample in our study, neighbourhood natural space was only related to Prosocial Behaviour, whereas studies with older children have suggested whole-sample benefits of neighbourhood greenness or proximity to public green space for Peer Problems, Hyperactivity, and Total Difficulties (Amoly et al., 2014, Markevych et al., 2014). Jointly, therefore, the evidence suggests that individual and household factors (including garden access) are of key importance for the development of young children, while neighbourhood natural space may become more important as children age. The birth cohort analysed here has been followed up at ages 8 and 10, making it possible in future to investigate whether the natural environment has become more important to the children's social, emotional and behavioural development over time.

Independently of neighbourhood natural space and garden access, children with higher screen time had worse outcomes for all SDQ domains (except Conduct Problems). Strong evidence has linked early childhood television exposure with subsequent attentional problems and social disengagement (Christakis et al., 2004, Sigman, 2012). One suggested mechanism is that screen time reduces social interaction that helps children develop social and emotional skills (Sigman, 2012) – encouraging greater interaction with nature could capitalise on the benefits of nature experiences, while reducing the harms from screen use (Louv, 2005).

Our study is the first in Scotland to investigate the role of natural space in early child development, and builds upon previous research by studying younger children, using natural space measures specific to the child's home location, and applying a longitudinal approach. We used a widely-validated measure of child development – the SDQ – which has proven sensitivity for young children (Theunissen et al., 2015). We also took steps to guard against residual confounding by checking for associations with a health outcome not expected to be related to natural space. In a model fully adjusted for covariates, we found that children with more natural space (total or parks) or garden access were no more or less likely than others to have fillings, and concluded that residual confounding was not a significant issue in our models.

Certain limitations must also be acknowledged. First, our sample was unlikely to be representative of urban children in Scotland because it was subject to attrition and selection bias. Lower socioeconomic status families are less likely to continue participating in a longitudinal survey. Also, wave four and six observations were omitted if the address was different from wave five, which will have disproportionately affected low socioeconomic status households, because these were significantly more likely to have moved (results not shown). We adjusted models for household and area socioeconomic status to address this issue. Second, the SDQ was completed by the child's carer, hence was subjective. Nonetheless the reliability of the SDQ in detecting psychosocial problems has been reported elsewhere (Theunissen et al., 2015). Third, we hypothesised that natural spaces would benefit children because of the opportunities they offer for play, but our natural space measure captured quantity rather than usage. Also we could not capture natural space quality, which could be an additional influence on usage and health outcomes. Hence, finding any significant results with our crude measure of quantity is notable. Finally, our analyses were correlational, hence we were unable to prove that better outcomes in 4 to 6 year olds were caused by greener neighbourhoods or garden access.

5. Conclusions

We have enhanced the scarce evidence base about the role of urban natural space in childhood development, producing the first country-wide analysis to date. Our work suggests that natural space, particularly in the form of private gardens, contributes to better social, emotional and behavioural outcomes for 4–6 year old children in urban Scotland. Neighbourhood natural space may have an important role in facilitating the beneficial social interactions of young children. We found little evidence that neighbourhood natural space or garden access influenced the trajectory of developmental change between 4 and 6 years, suggesting that any beneficial influences had occurred at younger ages. Given the growing prevalence of childhood social, emotional and behavioural problems, and their implications for health in later life, our findings have public health importance. Urban planning policies that ensure children have nearby access to nature could help improve children's development. Further longitudinal studies are required to establish the mechanisms underlying the associations found, and to investigate whether the importance of neighbourhood natural space increases with age.

Ethical approval

Wave one of the Growing Up in Scotland study was subject to medical ethical review by the Scotland ‘A’ MREC committee (application reference: 04/M RE 1 0/59). Subsequent annual waves were reviewed via substantial amendment submitted to the same committee.

Competing financial interests declaration

The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to ScotCen for access to the Growing Up in Scotland data (Scottish Centre for Social Research 2012) and Ordnance Survey and Greenspace Scotland for access to the natural space data (© Crown Copyright and Database Right 2015. Ordnance Survey (Digimap Licence)). The work was supported by the European Research Council [ERC-2010-StG Grant 263501].

Contributor Information

Elizabeth A. Richardson, Email: richardson.eliz@gmail.com.

Jamie Pearce, Email: jamie.pearce@ed.ac.uk.

Niamh K. Shortt, Email: niamh.shortt@ed.ac.uk.

Richard Mitchell, Email: richard.mitchell@glasgow.ac.uk.

References

  1. Almanza E., Jerrett M., Dunton G., Seto E., Pentz M.A. A study of community design, greenness, and physical activity in children using satellite, GPS and accelerometer data. Health Place. 2012;18:46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amoly E., Dadvand P., Forns J., López-Vicente M., Basagaña X., Julvez J. Green and blue spaces and behavioral development in Barcelona schoolchildren: the BREATHE project. Environ. Health Perspect. 2014;122:1351–1358. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1408215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Balseviciene B., Sinkariova L., Grazuleviciene R., Andrusaityte S., Uzdanaviciute I., Dedele A. Impact of residential greenness on preschool children's emotional and behavioral problems. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2014;11:6757–6770. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110706757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bee H. The Developing Child. 6th ed. Harper Collins College Publishers; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bradshaw, P., Tipping, S., 2010. Growing Up in Scotland: Children’s social, emotional and behavioural characteristics at entry to primary school.〈doi:http://www.gov.scot/Publications/2010/04/26102809/13〉.
  6. Browne, W.J., 2016. MCMC Estimation in MLwiN, v2.36. 〈doi:http://www.bristol.ac.uk/cmm/media/software/mlwin/downloads/manuals/2-36/mcmc-web.pdf〉.
  7. Christakis D.A., Zimmerman F.J., DiGiuseppe D.L., McCarty C.A. Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics. 2004;113:708–713. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.4.708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cleland V., Timperio A., Salmon J., Hume C., Baur L.A., Crawford D. Predictors of time spent outdoors among children: 5-year longitudinal findings. J. Epidemiol. Community Heal. 2010;64:400–406. doi: 10.1136/jech.2009.087460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Craig, R., Mindell, J., eds. 2013. Health, social care and lifestyles Health Survey for England 2012. London, UK.
  10. Dadvand P., Nieuwenhuijsen M.J., Esnaola M., Forns J., Basagaña X., Alvarez-Pedrerol M. Green spaces and cognitive development in primary schoolchildren. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2015;112:7937–7942. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1503402112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. de Vries S., Verheij R.A., Groenewegen P.P., Spreeuwenberg P. Natural environments—healthy environments? An exploratory analysis of the relationship between greenspace and health. Environ. Plan. A. 2003;35:1717–1731. [Google Scholar]
  12. Dube S.R., Felitti V.J., Dong M., Giles W.H., Anda R.F. The impact of adverse childhood experiences on health problems: evidence from four birth cohorts dating back to 1900. Prev. Med. (Baltim.). 2003;37:268–277. doi: 10.1016/s0091-7435(03)00123-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Erikson E.H. W. W. Norton; Co, New York: 1963. Childhood and Society. [Google Scholar]
  14. Faber Taylor A., Kuo F.E. Children with attention deficits concentrate better after walk in the park. J. Atten. Disord. 2009;12:402–409. doi: 10.1177/1087054708323000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Flouri E., Midouhas E., Joshi H. The role of urban neighbourhood green space in children's emotional and behavioural resilience. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014;40:179–186. [Google Scholar]
  16. Forns J., Dadvand P., Foraster M., Alvarez-Pedrerol M., Rivas I., López-Vicente M. Traffic-related air pollution, noise at school, and behavioral problems in Barcelona schoolchildren: a cross-sectional study. Environ. Health Perspect. 2015;124:529–535. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1409449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gaster S. Urban children's access to their neighborhood: changes over three generations. Environ. Behav. 1991;23:70–85. [Google Scholar]
  18. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Greenspace Scotland, 2011. Scotland’s Greenspace Map: Final Report.; doi:http://greenspacescotland.org.uk/SharedFiles/Download.aspx?Pageid = 133&mid = 129&fileid = 51.
  20. Hart R. Irvington; New York: 1979. Children's Experience of Place. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hartig T., Mitchell R., de Vries S., Frumkin H. Nature and Health. Annu. Rev. Public Health. 2014:35. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182443. (doi:doi:10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182443) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jokela M., Ferrie J., Kivimaki M. Childhood problem behaviors and death by midlife: the British National Child Development Study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2009;48:19–24. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31818b1c76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kaplan R., Kaplan S. Cambridge University Presss; Cambridge: 1989. The Experience of Nature: a Psychological Perspective. [Google Scholar]
  24. Layard R., Dunn J. Penguin; London: 2009. A Good Childhood: Searching for Values in a Competitive Age. [Google Scholar]
  25. Leckie G., Charlton C. runmlwin - A program to run the MLwiN multilevel modelling software from within Stata. J. Stat. Softw. 2013;52:1–40. [Google Scholar]
  26. Louv R. Algonquin Books; Chapel Hill, NC: 2005. Last Child in the Woods: Saving our Children from Nature-deficit Disorder. [Google Scholar]
  27. Markevych I., Tiesler C.M.T., Fuertes E., Romanos M., Dadvand P., Nieuwenhuijsen M.J. Access to urban green spaces and behavioural problems in children: results from the GINIplus and LISAplus studies. Environ. Int. 2014;71:29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2014.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Marryat, L., Martin, C., 2010. Maternal mental health and its impact on child behaviour and development. 〈doi:http://www.gov.scot/resource/doc/310448/0097971.pdf〉.
  29. McCracken D.S., Allen D.A., Gow A.J. Associations between urban greenspace and health-related quality of life in children. Prev. Med. Rep. 2016;3:211–221. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2016.01.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mitchell R.J., Richardson E.A., Shortt N.K., Pearce J.R. Neighborhood Environments and Socioeconomic Inequalities in Mental Well-Being. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pachter L.M., Auinger P., Palmer R., Weitzman M. Do parenting and the home environment, maternal depression, neighborhood, and chronic poverty affect child behavioral problems differently in different racial-ethnic groups? Pediatrics. 2006;117:1329–1338. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pate R.R., Dowda M., Brown W.H., Mitchell J., Addy C. Physical activity in preschool children with the transition to outdoors. J. Phys. Act. Health. 2013;10:170–175. doi: 10.1123/jpah.10.2.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rasbash J., Charlton C., Browne W.J., Healy M., Cameron B., 2009. MLwiN Version 2.1.
  34. Scottish Centre forSocial Research, 2012. Growing Up in Scotland: Sweep 5 Postcodes, 2009–2010: Secure Access. [data collection].
  35. Scottish Government, 2009. Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation 2009: Technical Report.
  36. Sigman A. Time for a view on screen time. Arch. Dis. Child. 2012;97:935–942. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2012-302196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Strife S., Downey L. Childhood development and access to nature: a new direction for environmental inequality research. Organ. Environ. 2009;22:99–122. doi: 10.1177/1086026609333340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Theunissen M.H.C., Vogels A.G.C., De Wolff M.S., Crone M.R., Reijneveld S.A. Comparing three short questionnaires to detect psychosocial problems among 3 to 4-year olds. BMC Pediatr. 2015;15:84. doi: 10.1186/s12887-015-0391-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Trout A.L., Nordness P.D., Pierce C.D., Epstein M.H. Research on the academic status of children with emotional and behavioral disorders: a review of the Literature From 1961 to 2000. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 2003;11:198–210. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ulrich R.S. Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. In: Altman I., Wohlwilleds J.F., editors. Behavior and the Natural Environment. Plenum Press; New York: 1983. pp. 85–125. [Google Scholar]
  41. Valentine G., McKendrick J. Children's outdoor play: exploring parental concerns about children's safety and the changing nature of childhood. Geoforum. 1997;28:219–235. [Google Scholar]
  42. Ware J., Kosinski M., Keller S. A 12-item ShortForm health survey (SF-12): construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Med. Care. 1996;32:220–233. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199603000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Wells N.M. At home with nature: effects of “Greenness” on children's cognitive functioning. Environ. Behav. 2000;32:775–795. [Google Scholar]
  44. Wells N.M., Evans G.W. Nearby nature: a buffer of life stress among rural children. Environ. Behav. 2003;35:311–330. [Google Scholar]
  45. Wilson P., Bradshaw P., Tipping S., Henderson M., Der G., Minnis H. What predicts persistent early conduct problems? Evidence from the Growing Up in Scotland cohort. J. Epidemiol. Community Heal. 2012;67:76–80. doi: 10.1136/jech-2011-200856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES