Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Child Dev. 2017 Feb 28;89(2):360–369. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12771

Locating Economic Risks for Adolescent Mental and Behavioral Health: Poverty and Affluence in Families, Neighborhoods, and Schools

Rebekah Levine Coley a, Jacqueline Sims a, Eric Dearing a, Bryn Spielvogel a
PMCID: PMC5573665  NIHMSID: NIHMS847188  PMID: 28245340

Abstract

Research has identified risks of both poverty and affluence for adolescents. This study sought to clarify associations between income and youth mental and behavioral health by delineating economic risks derived from family, neighborhood, and school contexts within a nationally-representative sample of high school students (N =13,179, average age 16). Attending schools with more affluent schoolmates was associated with heightened likelihoods of intoxication, drug use, and property crime, but youth at poorer schools reported greater depressive and anxiety symptoms, engagement in violence, and, for males, more frequent violence and intoxication. Neighborhood and family income were far less predictive. Results suggest that adolescent health risks derive from both ends of the economic spectrum, and may be largely driven by school contexts.

Keywords: Poverty, Affluence, Mental health, Health risk behaviors


Typical development is defined by healthy transitions through adolescence. Yet a substantial number of youth experience serious psychological distress or engage in risk behaviors such as substance use and delinquency, with the potential for sustained psychological, behavioral, and social problems in adulthood (Martin et al., 2010; Mason et al., 2010). Research has long pointed to family poverty as a risk factor for heightened mental and behavioral health problems among youth (Dearing, 2008), and recent reviews and meta-analyses reiterate these economic gradients, identifying small but persistent negative associations between family income or SES and adolescent depression, delinquency, and aggression (Letourneau, Duffet-Leger, Levac, Watson, & Young-Morris, 2011; Reiss, 2013).

Although much scholarship concentrates on developmental threats related to poverty, recent evidence suggests that affluence may also confer risk for adolescents. Innovative work by Luthar and colleagues has largely driven this argument (Luthar & Sexton, 2004; Luthar, Barkin, & Crossman, 2013; Rosin, 2015). A series of studies assessing youth in suburban affluent communities found heightened engagement in substance use and internalizing problems (particularly among girls) and sometimes heightened engagement in delinquency (particularly among boys) compared to national norms (Luthar & D’Avanzo, 1999; Luthar & Goldstein, 2008). The authors argued that numerous contextual stressors contributed to the elevated mental and behavioral health problems among affluent youth, including isolation from parents, excessive achievement pressures, and peer approval for risk behaviors.

Although this work was groundbreaking in drawing attention to risks for affluent youth, it was limited by its focus on a few affluent communities. Specifically, Luthar’s initial work sampled youth from single schools within single communities, and assessed group differences in outcomes by categorizing youth in suburban, predominantly white communities with high median income as “affluent,” and those in urban schools with many low-income and racial minority students as “inner-city” or “low SES” (see Luthar et al., 2013 for summary). Without a greater representation of communities from across the income distribution it was not possible to precisely identify points of risk or the functional form of associations (i.e., linear versus non-linear relations). Moreover, this approach limited generalizability and failed to disentangle the unique effects of affluence at family, community, and school levels. Because the economic contexts of families, neighbors, and schoolmates may all affect social norms of acceptable and unacceptable behaviors, provide resources or barriers to healthy development, and increase or buffer against stress, it is essential for research to consider their independent contributions to youth functioning.

Indeed, extensive evidence highlights the central role that neighborhood contexts play in adolescent development (see Leventhal, Dupéré, & Shuey, 2015 for review). Prior research finds that high levels of poverty (Haynie, Silver, & Teasdale, 2006; Wickrama & Bryant, 2003) or low levels of affluence (Beyers, Bates, Pettit, & Dodge, 2003) among families clustered within neighborhoods are associated with higher rates of depression and delinquency. Yet some recent work suggests the potential for deleterious consequences of higher neighborhood income. For example, Lund and Dearing (2013) found that neighborhood income was positively associated with delinquency among boys and internalizing symptoms among girls. Similarly, others have reported links between neighborhood disadvantage and lower rates of substance use among adolescents (Chuang, Ennett, Bauman, & Foshee, 2005; Snedker, Herting, & Walton, 2009). In the experimental Moving to Opportunity study, boys reported increased behavior problems and property crime following opportunities to move to less poor neighborhoods (Kling, Ludwig, & Katz, 2005; Sanbonmatsu et al., 2011), whereas there were benefits for girls’ substance use and mental health outcomes (Gennetian et al., 2012; Kling, Liebman, & Katz, 2007).

Researchers also contend that the economic strata of schoolmates may affect youth mental and behavioral health outcomes, although empirical evidence in this area is more limited (see Crosnoe, 2009 for review). Some have argued that schools may, in fact, be a primary force behind some “neighborhood effects” (Lund & Dearing, 2013). Prior research has linked greater schoolmate income with heightened alcohol and drug use, although this research is limited in the use of proxies for school income, such as Census neighborhood measures aggregated to the school level (Botticello, 2009), or parental education (O’Malley, Johnston, Bachman, Schulenberg, & Kumar, 2006). Other research found negative links between average schoolmate family income and youth depressive symptoms (Goodman, Huang, Wade, & Kahn, 2003).

From this base, we argue that it is essential to disentangle how the economic contexts provided by families, neighbors, and schoolmates are uniquely associated with youth mental and behavioral health outcomes. Moreover, we argue that the field would benefit from estimates that are more widely generalizable to the population, with the functional form of relations examined across the income distribution. In the current study, we examined a large, nationally representative sample of high school youth; considered unique associations between family, neighborhood, and school income and youth mental and behavioral health; and considered both linear and nonlinear income effects as well as several alternative specifications of income, broadly examining the full distribution in each of these economic contexts.

Method

Sampling and Data Collection

Data were drawn from youth, parent, and administrator interviews conducted in 1994–1995 and appended 1990 U.S. Census data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health). Drawing from the wave 1 in-home survey sample—a stratified, nationally representative sample of 7th through 12th graders across the U.S. (response rate of 79%)—the analytic sample included all participants in grades 9 through 12 attending high schools at wave 1 with valid survey weights and neighborhood and school identifiers (excluding 11 cases with cross-classification in school and neighborhood contexts), N = 13,179 youth in 76 schools.

Measures

Youth mental and behavioral health outcomes

Youth self-reported on their mental and behavioral health using well-validated measures, all coded such that higher scores indicate more severe symptomology or higher engagement. Measures included depressive symptoms (19 items from the CES-D, α = 0.87), anxiety symptoms (8 items representing physiological symptoms of anxiety, α =.71), intoxication (days per month), illicit drug use (use of marijuana, cocaine, inhalants, and other illegal drugs in past month, α = 0.63), property crime (stealing, burglarizing, or damaging property [0 “never” to 5 “5 or more times”] in the past year, α = 0.87), and violence (fighting and carrying, brandishing, or using a weapon [0 “never” to 2 “2 or more times”] in the past year, α = 0.67).

Family, neighborhood, and school income

Family income was derived from parent reports of total household income in the prior year. Neighborhood income was assessed by average household income in each Census block. Family income was aggregated to the school level to assess school income (all expressed in units of $10,000 in 1994 dollars). (See Alternate Model Specifications section for other variable operationalizations.) Income variables were only moderately correlated (r = 0.39 to 0.46), with adequate variability across contexts. For example, 20% of youth in the highest quintile of family income were in the bottom two quintiles of school income, whereas 17% were in the bottom two quintiles of neighborhood income. Similarly, 10% of youth in the highest quintile of school income were in the lowest two quintiles of neighborhood income.

Covariates

Following expert advice (Leventhal et al., 2015), we controlled for key demographic factors and preferences to help adjust for selection bias into income strata at each level. Covariates included youth age, race and ethnicity, and number of household members; parent age (youngest), education (highest), marital status (never, previously, or currently married), immigrant status, and indicators reflecting parents’ primary reason for living in their current neighborhood (either the quality of schools or neighborhood safety); and school urbanicity.

Sample characteristics

Youth averaged 16 years, 51% were female, 48% identified as White, 19% Black, 18% Hispanic, 8% Asian, 1% Native American, and 6% multiracial or other (Table 1). In 1994 dollars, family income averaged $45,600 dollars, school income $46,500, and neighborhood income $35,400. Eleven percent of the schools had average incomes more than twice the national household median, with the top two schools having incomes 3.5 and 4.3 times the national median (similar to suburban samples in Luthar’s studies, which ranged from about 1.8 to 4 times the national median). On average, youth reported low levels of mental health symptoms and risk behaviors, with adolescent girls reporting slightly higher mental health problems (depressive and anxiety symptoms) than boys, and slightly lower behavior health problems, both in terms of a greater propensity to report 0 engagement and a lower frequency of engagement in intoxication, drug use, property crime, and violence.

Table 1.

Sample Descriptives

Females (n = 6,676) Males (n = 6,503)
Dependent Variables
 Depressive symptoms 0.75(0.51) 0.59(0.43)
 Anxiety symptoms 1.34(1.82) 0.91(1.31)
 Intoxication % 0 68.01 63.66
 Intoxication count 1.64(2.57) 3.17(4.66)
 Illicit drug use % 0 84.27 80.37
 Illicit drug use count 7.39(7.65) 12.27(12.23)
 Property crime % 0 76.33 61.84
 Property crime count 3.17(2.35) 3.95(3.16)
 Violence % 0 85.20 72.02
 Violence count 1.40(0.64) 1.76(1.18)
Income variables
 Family income ($10,000s) 4.59(3.45) 4.52(3.31)
 Neighborhood income ($10,000s) 3.51(1.50) 3.57(1.54)
 School income ($10,000s) 4.67(1.71) 4.62(1.79)
Covariates
 Age 16.12(1.58) 15.83(1.65)
 White 48.02 48.19
 African American 19.18 19.15
 Hispanic 18.32 18.14
 Asian 8.14 8.13
 American Indian 0.79 0.81
 Multiracial and other 5.67 5.58
 Immigrant household 15.57 16.66
 Parent age 42.15(5.98) 42.44(5.91)
 Number of household members 2.50(1.54) 3.38(1.41)
 Parent single 4.03 4.76
 Parent previously married 28.91 27.58
 Parent married 67.09 68.08
 Parent < high school 12.24 13.43
 Parent high school 34.02 35.42
 Parent some college 19.31 18.02
 Parent college degree or more 34.42 33.13
 Moved to neighborhood for school 18.17 18.25
 Moved to neighborhood for safety 17.72 17.71
 Urban school 23.42 22.16
 Rural school 19.53 20.17
 Suburban school 57.05 57.67

Note: N = 13,179. Mean(standard deviation) or % reported in each cell. Depressive symptoms were reported in the past week, anxiety symptoms in the past year, intoxication and illicit drug use in the past month, and property crime and violence in the past year.

Analytic Technique

Multilevel models, with standard error adjustments at the neighborhood level and with youth nested within schools, assessed associations between income and youth outcomes. Given research suggesting sex-specific vulnerabilities to risks associated with poverty and affluence (Kling et al., 2007; Lund & Dearing, 2013; Luthar & Latendresse, 2005a, 2005b), models were estimated separately by sex. Links with depressive and anxiety symptoms were assessed utilizing OLS regression models. Intoxication, drug use, property crime, and violence were overdispersed count variables with large proportions of zeroes. Thus, zero-inflated negative binomial models were estimated, which concurrently predict the odds of being a true zero as well as the predicted count for respondents who are not true zeroes. Initial models tested for nonlinear income effects at the family, neighborhood, and school level through inclusion of linear and quadratic income variables. Nonsignificant quadratics were cut for the sake of parsimony. All analyses included covariates and were conducted in Mplus 7.4 using the wave 1 grand sample weight and full information maximum likelihood estimation to provide unbiased parameter estimates and account for missing data (0 to 11.54%).

Results

Multilevel Regression Results

The top panel of Table 2 presents multilevel model results for girls. In 0-inflated negative binomial models, the exponentiated (ERR) zero-inflated coefficients are interpreted as the change in odds of having 0 engagement, while the exponentiated count coefficients are interpreted as the difference in level of engagement per unit shift in the predictor. For models predicting depressive and anxiety symptoms, we provide general linear regression estimates in the same panel as the count estimates. To ease interpretation of results, we translated coefficients into predicted values of the outcome variables at multiple points along the income distribution, reporting results for levels that approximate −1 SD, +1 SD, and +3 SDs from the mean (which translate to around the 13th, 89th, and 98th percentiles of each income distribution) to highlight both ends of the income distribution (see Table 3).

Table 2.

Multilevel OLS and Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial Models Predicting Females’ and Males’ Mental and Behavioral Health

Depressive Symptoms
Anxiety Symptoms
Intoxication
Illicit Drug Use
Property Crime
Violence
Coef(SE) Coef(SE) Coef(SE) ERR Coef(SE) ERR Coef(SE) ERR Coef(SE) ERR
Females
Zero Inflated
  Family Income −0.015(0.012) 0.985 0.005(0.017) 1.005 −0.006(0.014) 0.994 0.027(0.020) 1.027
   Quadratic -- -- -- --
  Neighborhood Income −0.052(0.031) 0.949 −0.033(0.034) 0.968 0.044(0.031) 1.045 0.074(0.051) 1.077
   Quadratic -- -- -- −0.027(0.007) 0.973**
  School Income −0.102(0.021) 0.903** −0.084(0.018) 0.919** −0.046(0.014) 0.955** 0.126(0.018) 1.134**
   Quadratic -- -- -- --
Count/GLM
  Family Income −0.005(0.003) −0.011(0.012) −0.041(0.029) 0.960 −0.026(0.013) 0.974* −0.002(0.006) 0.998 0.002(0.007) 1.002
   Quadratic -- -- -- -- -- --
  Neighborhood Income −0.009(0.007) −0.005(0.032) 0.024(0.039) 1.024 −0.047(0.031) 0.954 0.015(0.023) 1.015 −0.011(0.018) 0.989
   Quadratic -- -- -- -- −0.008(0.004) 0.992* --
  School Income −0.023(0.005)** −0.022(0.009)* 0.001(0.024) 1.001 0.025(0.016) 1.025 0.012(0.010) 1.012 −0.004(0.011) 0.996
   Quadratic -- -- -- -- -- --
Males
Zero Inflated
  Family Income −0.080(0.018) 0.923** −0.026(0.015) 0.974 −0.032(0.014) 0.969* 0.058(0.022) 1.060**
   Quadratic 0.003(0.002) 1.003* -- -- −0.004(0.002) 0.996**
  Neighborhood Income 0.000(0.030) 1.000 0.001(0.037) 1.001 0.007(0.031) 1.007 0.082(0.031) 1.085**
   Quadratic -- -- -- --
  School Income −0.064(0.026) 0.938* −0.024(0.018) 0.976 −0.168(0.045) 0.845** 0.155(0.044) 1.168**
   Quadratic -- -- 0.011(0.004) 1.011** −0.008(0.003) 0.992*
Count/GLM
  Family Income −0.002(0.002) −0.005(0.006) −0.008(0.020) 0.992 −0.005(0.015) 0.995 0.005(0.007) 1.005 0.011(0.010) 1.011
   Quadratic -- -- -- -- -- --
  Neighborhood Income −0.014(0.007)* −0.009(0.016) 0.058(0.057) 1.060 −0.045(0.043) 0.956 −0.011(0.027) 0.989 −0.011(0.015) 0.989
   Quadratic -- -- -- -- -- --
  School Income −0.031(0.006)** −0.027(0.007)** −0.042(0.016) 0.959** −0.006(0.012) 0.994 −0.030(0.035) 0.970 −0.019(0.006) 0.981**
   Quadratic 0.002(0.000)** -- -- -- -- --

Note: Female n = 6,676; Male n = 6,503. Coef = Coefficient, SE = Standard Error, ERR = Event Rate Ratio.

*

p <.05,

**

p <.01.

All analyses controlled for youth age, race/ethnicity, and number of household members; parental age, immigrant status, marital status, education, and neighborhood selection preferences; and school urbanicity. Depressive symptoms were reported in the past week, anxiety symptoms in the past year, intoxication and illicit drug use in the past month, and property crime and violence in the past year.

Table 3.

Predicted Values of Outcomes for Significant Family, Neighborhood, and School Income Effects

Depressive Symptoms Anxiety Symptoms Intoxication Illicit Drug Use Property Crime Violence

Females
 Probability Engagement
  Neighborhood Income
   $20,000 -- -- -- 16.23%
   $50,000 -- -- -- 13.37%
   $80,000 -- -- -- 17.00%
  School Income
   $30,000 29.25% 13.37% 22.07% 16.75%
   $65,000 36.94% 17.06% 24.89% 11.56%
   $100,000 45.37% 21.51% 27.95% 7.83%
 Count/Level
  Family Income
   $10,000 -- -- -- 5.36 -- --
   $80,000 -- -- -- 4.49 -- --
   $150,000 -- -- -- 3.76 -- --
  Neighborhood Income
   $20,000 -- -- -- -- 2.94 --
   $50,000 -- -- -- -- 3.07 --
   $80,000 -- -- -- -- 2.77 --
  School Income
   $30,000 0.80 1.37 -- -- -- --
   $65,000 0.72 1.29 -- -- -- --
   $100,000 0.64 1.22 -- -- -- --
Males
 Probability Engagement
  Family Income
   $10,000 29.53% -- 41.17% 32.09%
   $80,000 41.93% -- 46.53% 24.16%
   $150,000 48.52% -- 51.96% 23.71%
  Neighborhood Income
   $20,000 -- -- -- 29.59%
   $50,000 -- -- -- 24.62%
   $80,000 -- -- -- 20.24%
  School Income
   $30,000 33.80% -- 36.19% 33.08%
   $65,000 38.85% -- 50.18% 22.54%
   $100,000 44.16% -- 58.04% 17.12%
 Count/Level
  Neighborhood Income
   $20,000 0.62 -- -- -- -- --
   $50,000 0.57 -- -- -- -- --
   $80,000 0.53 -- -- -- -- --
  School Income
   $30,000 0.65 0.96 1.93 -- -- 1.73
   $65,000 0.55 0.87 1.67 -- -- 1.62
   $100,000 0.49 0.78 1.45 -- -- 1.52

For girls, the most consistent results emerged in relation to school income, which was associated with all six outcomes. School income showed negative linear associations with girls’ depressive and anxiety symptoms, with the highest levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms seen among girls in the lowest income schools. For example, predicted scores for depressive symptoms ranged from .80 for girls with average schoolmate income of $30,000 to .64 in schools with average incomes of $100,000, about 1/3 of a SD difference. School income also was linearly associated with a likelihood of 0 engagement in violence, indicating a higher likelihood of involvement in poorer schools. As the average schoolmate income rose from $30,000 to $100,000, predicted rates of engagement in violence dropped from 17% to 8%. On the other hand, linear associations between school income and the likelihood of 0 engagement in intoxication, drug use, and property crime all indicated a higher likelihood of engagement in more affluent schools. Considering girls at schools with mean incomes of $30,000 and $100,000, 29% and 45% reported intoxication; 13% and 22% reported drug use; and 22% and 28% reported property crime.

In contrast, family income was significantly associated with only one outcome among girls, with a negative linear association indicating the highest level of illicit drug use among users in poorer families: girls in families with $10,000 income reported 5.36 uses per month, lowering to 3.76 times per month in families with $150,000 income. Neighborhood income was significantly linked with only two outcomes with both associations following a nonlinear pattern: girls showed the highest level of property crime in upper-middle income neighborhoods (rising from 2.94 to 3.07 then dropping back to 2.77 incidents as neighborhood income rose from $20,000 to $50,000 to $80,000), but also the lowest likelihood of engagement in violence (decreasing from 16% to 13%, then rising back to 17% as neighborhood income rose).

Results for boys are presented in the bottom halves of Tables 2 and 3. Again, school income was the most common predictor of behavioral and mental health problems, having significant associations with five outcomes among boys. School income showed negative curvilinear associations with levels of depressive symptoms, and negative linear links with anxiety symptoms, with the highest levels in poorer schools. For example, depressive symptoms dropped from .65 to .55 to .48 as average school income increased from $30,000 to $65,000 to $100,000, more than 1/3 of a SD difference. Similarly, associations between school income and engagement in interpersonal violence indicted higher violence in poorer schools: as school income increased, the predicted likelihood of boys’ involvement in violence dropped nonlinearly from 33% to 23% to 17%, and the frequency of engagement among engagers dropped linearly, from 1.73 to 1.52. Frequency of engagement in intoxication among boys also dropped linearly as school income increased, with average rates of 1.93 days to 1.45 days per month as school income rose from $30,000 to $100,000.

A different pattern emerged between school income and boys’ likelihood of any engagement in intoxication and property crime. Associations indicated that the likelihood of any engagement was highest in affluent schools: rates of intoxication engagement rose linearly from 34% in poorer schools to 44% in more affluent schools, and engagement in property crime rose nonlinearly from 36% to 50% to 58%.

Some significant associations between family and neighborhood income and boys’ behavioral health problems were also evident. Family income was positively associated with a heightened likelihood of intoxication and property crime, with rates of engagement in intoxication rising in a curvilinear fashion from 30% to 42% to 49%, and engagement in property crime rising linearly from 41% to 47% to 52% as family income shifted from $10,000 to $80,000 to $150,000. In contrast, a curvilinear association indicated that the highest rates of engagement in violence occurred among poor youth, with rates declining from 32% to 24% for boys in poor versus upper-middle class families, then remaining at 24% for affluent youth. Finally, neighborhood income showed small negative linear associations with levels of boys’ depressive symptoms and likelihood of engagement in violence: as average neighborhood income rose from $20,000 to $80,000, boys’ predicted engagement in violence declined from 30% to 20%, and depressive symptoms dropped from .62 to .53, a shift of 1/5 of a SD.

Additional Model Specifications

A number of additional model specifications were estimated to examine alternate measures of income. These included (1) variables assessing % affluent neighbors and % affluent schoolmates (delineated as 2.5 SDs above the national median household income) to better tap into affluence; (2) measures of family, neighborhood, and school SES, created by standardizing and averaging measures of family income, parental education, and parental job prestige at each level; (3) SES composites created using % affluent, % college degree, and % professional job variables at the neighborhood and school levels; and (4) models including only family income (excluding neighborhood and school). Results were generally consistent with those presented above, although in some models family income or SES gained significant negative associations with mental health outcomes.

Models also were estimated with students nested within neighborhoods rather than schools, again with similar results. We also assessed the full sample, including interactions between sex and family, neighborhood, and school income. About half of the connections between school income and behavioral health outcomes differed significantly between girls and boys, and hence we focus on the separate sex models. We also assessed interactions between youth age and family, neighborhood, and school income, as well as two-way interactions between family, neighborhood, and school income: in both sets, no robust pattern of interactions emerged.

Discussion

Research has long pointed to family and community poverty as a risk factor for heightened behavioral and mental health problems. Yet scientists and the media have recently raised concerns over elevated levels of mental health problems, substance use, and delinquency among economically advantaged youth (Luthar & Sexton, 2004; Luthar et al., 2013; Rosin, 2015), pointing to extreme levels of competition, achievement stress, peer pressure, and social isolation created by a culture of affluence. However, much of the empirical evidence for such claims derives from research in a small number of schools populated by affluent children and located in affluent communities; hence this work was not able to discern whether associations were driven by family, neighborhood, or school contexts. Isolating the unique role of these three proximal contexts and explicitly testing for both linear and nonlinear connections between income and youth outcomes suggested by prior work were primary goals of the current study.

Using a nationally representative sample of high school youth, this research found risks associated with both poverty and affluence, with patterns varying by context, outcome, and youth gender. The primary pattern indicated that schoolmate income was by far the most consistent correlate of adolescents’ mental and behavioral health outcomes, significantly associated with depressive and anxiety symptoms, intoxication, drug use, violence, and property crime. These results highlight the central role of schools and peers in driving social norms and expectations affecting mental and behavioral health (Crosnoe, 2009; Luthar et al., 2013).

However, patterns of associations with school income differed across arenas of functioning. School income was positively and mostly linearly associated with adolescents’ likelihood of engagement in numerous risk behaviors, including intoxication, drug use, and property crime, with the highest likelihood of engagement seen among youth attending the most affluent schools. These results replicate and extend patterns delineated by Luthar and colleagues (1999; 2005; 2008; 2012), specifying that risk derives primarily from schools populated by affluent youth, rather than from adolescents’ family or neighborhood income. In contrast, poorer schools served as a risk for other outcomes: levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms, the likelihood of engagement in interpersonal violence, and frequency of boys’ intoxication and violence all declined as school income increased, again with mostly linear associations. Our results replicate prior research finding heightened rates of depressive symptoms or clinically significant levels of internalizing problems among youth from poor schools (Goodman et al., 2003; Lyman & Luthar, 2014). Yet these linear associations did not replicate other of Luthar’s results finding heightened rates (in comparison to national norms) of clinically significant levels of internalizing problems in some affluent schools (e.g., Luthar & Barkin, 2012). Future work should seek to assess the specific processes linking school income with youth functioning and help decipher diverse patterns across mental and behavioral health outcomes.

In contrast to the results associated with school income, both family and neighborhood income showed relatively few links with youth outcomes, and results lacked a clear pattern, with the exception that higher family income was associated with a greater likelihood of engagement in intoxication, property crime, and violence among boys in a linear or curvilinear manner. Overall, however, few curvilinear associations emerged between family, neighborhood, or school income and youth outcomes. Moreover, patterns were generally consistent across younger and older high school students, and were robust to a variety of operationalizations of income and SES. In interpreting these results and comparing them to prior research, it is essential to recall that prior work often used samples at the very extremes of the income distribution: much research on neighborhoods, for example, has focused solely on high poverty, primarily African American urban neighborhoods (e.g., Kling et al., 2007), whereas Luthar’s work focused primarily on affluent, mostly White suburban communities (Luthar et al., 2013). In addition to conflation of family, neighborhood, and school income, the roles of urbanicity and race and ethnicity are difficult to disentangle from poverty and affluence in this prior work.

The use of a large, representative national sample in this study allowed us to consider diverse youth across the U.S. Further research could explicitly address the potential for distinct associations between income and youth functioning across urbanicity, region, and race and ethnicity to consider the role of macroeconomic and cultural contexts (e.g., Votruba-Drzal, Miller, & Coley, 2016). Future research should also seek to replicate these results with newer national data (not currently available), as historical context may moderate these descriptive results, and should attend to potential long-term repercussions of adolescent economic contexts. It is also essential for additional research employing methodologies that allow causal interpretations. It is important to note, however, that correlational research that identifies descriptive patterns of risk, such as this, is essential: regardless of causal mechanisms, a precise identification of where risk lies across the income distribution is critical for science, policy, and practice with youth.

Beyond these cautions, this research, the first of which we are aware to assess the unique associations of family, neighborhood, and school income with youth health outcomes, suggests that school economic contexts are particularly important for youth well-being. Attending schools with higher-income schoolmates was associated with a heightened likelihood of engagement in substance use and property crime, whereas poorer schools were linked with elevated mental health problems and interpersonal violence, suggesting that risks are derived from both ends of the economic spectrum. Our findings challenge a disproportionate focus on the risks experienced by individuals from low-resource schools, and call attention to threats also associated with greater aggregate socioeconomic resources at the school level. As income inequality has grown and become more bifurcated and geographically concentrated in the U.S. (Pendall & Hedman, 2015), so too has the likelihood that youth are surrounded by peers in similar economic circumstances to themselves, perhaps increasing the risks associated with both poverty and affluence. These patterns heighten the relevance of our results for school-based programs and policies seeking to support mental and behavioral health among youth, with particular attention to local patterns of risks. Recent efforts in school-based “whole child,” socioemotional learning, and positive youth development initiatives provide viable models (Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, & Schellinger, 2011; Greenberg et al., 2003) in efforts to support the well-being of youth across the economic spectrum.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded with generous support from Boston College through an Ignite Grant. This research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health website (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis.

References

  1. Beyers JM, Bates JE, Pettit GS, Dodge KA. Neighborhood structure, parenting processes, and the development of youths’ externalizing behaviors: A multilevel analysis. American journal of community psychology. 2003;31(1–2):35–53. doi: 10.1177/0192513X12437693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Botticello AL. School contextual influences on the risk for adolescent alcohol misuse. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2009;43:85–97. doi: 10.1007/s10464-008-9226-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Crosnoe R. Low-income students and the socioeconomic composition of public high schools. American Sociological Review. 2009;74:709–730. doi: 10.1177/000312240907400502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chuang YC, Ennett ST, Bauman KE, Foshee VA. Neighborhood influences on adolescent cigarette and alcohol use: mediating effects through parent and peer behaviors. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2005;46(2):187–204. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Dearing E. The psychological costs of growing up poor. In: Kaler SG, Rennert OM, editors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Special Issue: Scientific Approaches to Understanding and Reducing Poverty. Vol. 1136. 2008. pp. 324–332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Durlak JA, Weissberg RP, Dymnicki AB, Taylor RD, Schellinger KB. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development. 2011;82:405–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Gennetian LA, Sanbonmatsu L, Katz LF, Kling JR, Sciandra M, Ludwig J, … Kessler RC. The Long-Term Effects of Moving to Opportunity on Youth Outcomes. Cityscape. 2012;14(2):137–167. doi: 10.1007/s11292-013-9189-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Goodman E, Huang B, Wade TJ, Kahn RS. A multilevel analysis of the relation of socioeconomic status to adolescent depressive symptoms: does school context matter? The Journal of pediatrics. 2003;143(4):451–456. doi: 10.1067/S0022-3476(03)00456-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Greenberg MT, Weissberg RP, O’Brien MT, Zins JE, Fredericks L, Resnik H, Elias MJ. Enhancing school-based prevention and youth development through coordinated social, emotional, and academic learning. American Psychologist. 2003;58(6/7):466–474. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.6-7.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Haynie DL, Silver E, Teasdale B. Neighborhood characteristics, peer networks, and adolescent violence. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2006;22(2):147–169. doi: 10.1007/s10940-006-9006-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Kling JR, Liebman JB, Katz LF. Experimental analysis of neighborhood effects. Econometrica. 2007;75(1):83–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0262.2007.00733.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Kling JR, Ludwig J, Katz LF. Neighborhood effects on crime for female and male youth: Evidence from a randomized housing voucher experiment. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2005:87–130. doi: 10.3386/w10777. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Letourneau NL, Duffett-Leger L, Levac L, Watson B, Young-Morris C. Socioeconomic status and child development: A meta-analysis. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2011;21(3):211–224. doi: 10.1177/1063426611421007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Leventhal T, Dupéré V, Shuey EA. Children in neighborhoods. In: Borstein MH, Leventhal T, Lerner RM, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Volume 4. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2015. pp. 493–533. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lund TJ, Dearing E. Is growing up affluent risky for adolescents or is the problem growing up in an affluent neighborhood? Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2013;23(2):274–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2012.00829.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Luthar SS, Barkin SH. Are affluent youth truly “at risk”? Vulnerability and resilience across three diverse samples. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24(02):429–449. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Luthar SS, Barkin SH, Crossman EJ. “I can, therefore I must”: Fragility in the upper-middle classes. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(402):1529–1549. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Luthar SS, D’Avanzo K. Contextual factors in substance use: A study of suburban and inner-city adolescents. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:845–867. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Luthar SS, Goldstein AS. Substance use and related behaviors among suburban late adolescents: The importance of perceived parent containment. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:591–614. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Luthar SS, Latendresse SJ. Children of the affluent: Challenges to well-being. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005a;14:49–53. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00333.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Luthar SS, Latendresse SJ. Comparable “risks” at the SES extremes: Pre-adolescents’ perceptions of parenting. Development and Psychopathology. 2005b;17:207–230. doi: 10.1017/s095457940505011x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S095457940505011X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Luthar SS, Sexton CC. The high price of affluence. In: Kail R, editor. Advances in Child Development and Behavior. London: Academic Press; 2004. pp. 125–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lyman EL, Luthar SS. Futher evidence of the “costs of privilege”: Perfectionism in high-achieving youth at socioeconomic extremes. Psychology in the Schools. 2014;51:912–930. doi: 10.1002/pits.21791. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Martin M, Conger R, Schofield T, Dogan S, Widaman K, Donnellan MB, Neppl T. Evaluation of the interactionist model of socioeconomic status and problem behavior: A developmental cascade across generations. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:695–713. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mason WA, Hitch JE, Kosterman R, McCarty CA, Herrenkohl TI, Hawkins JD. Growth in adolescent delinquency and alcohol use in relation to young adult crime, alcohol use disorders, and risky sex: a comparison of youth from low-versus middle-income backgrounds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51(12):1377–1385. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02292.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. O’Malley PM, Johnston LD, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, Kumar R. How substance use differs among American secondary schools. Prevention Science. 2006;7:409–420. doi: 10.1007/s11121-006-0050-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Pendall R, Hedman C. Research Report. Washington, DC: Urban Institute; 2015. Worlds apart: Inequality between America’s most and least affluent neighborhoods. [Google Scholar]
  28. Reiss F. Socioeconomic inequalities and mental health problems in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Social Science & Medicine. 2013;90:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rosin H. The Silicon Valley Suicides: Why are so many kids with bright prospects killing themselves in Palo Alto? [Electronic version] The Atlantic 2015 Dec; [Google Scholar]
  30. Sanbonmatsu L, Ludwig J, Katz LF, Gennetian LA, Duncan GJ, Kessler RC, … Lindau ST. Moving to Opportunity for fair housing demonstration program--Final impacts evaluation. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, Office of Policy Development and Research; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  31. Snedker KA, Herting JR, Walton E. Contextual Effects and Adolescent Substance Use: Exploring the Role of Neighborhoods*. Social Science Quarterly. 2009;90(5):1272–1297. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00677.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Votruba-Drzal E, Miller P, Coley RL. Poverty, urbanicity, and children’s development of academic skills. Child Development Perspectives. 2016;10:3–9. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12152. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Wickrama KAS, Bryant CM. Community context of social resources and adolescent mental health. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65(4):850–866. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00850.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES