Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2017 Aug 7;114(34):9128–9133. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1704303114

Effects of maternal investment, temperament, and cognition on guide dog success

Emily E Bray a,b,1, Mary D Sammel c, Dorothy L Cheney d,1, James A Serpell e, Robert M Seyfarth a
PMCID: PMC5576795  PMID: 28784785

Significance

A successful guide dog must navigate a complex world, avoid distractions, and respond adaptively to unpredictable events. What leads to success? We followed 98 puppies from birth to adulthood. Puppies were enrolled in a training program where only ∼70% achieved success as guide dogs. More intense mothering early in life was associated with program failure. In addition, mothers whose nursing style required greater effort by puppies produced more successful offspring. Among young adult dogs, poor problem-solving abilities, perseveration, and apparently greater anxiety when confronted with a novel object were also associated with program failure. Results mirror the results from rodents and humans, reaffirming the enduring effects on adult behavior of maternal style and individual differences in temperament and cognition.

Keywords: guide dogs, nursing style, maternal style, temperament, cognition

Abstract

A continuing debate in studies of social development in both humans and other animals is the extent to which early life experiences affect adult behavior. Also unclear are the relative contributions of cognitive skills (“intelligence”) and temperament for successful outcomes. Guide dogs are particularly suited to research on these questions. To succeed as a guide dog, individuals must accomplish complex navigation and decision making without succumbing to distractions and unforeseen obstacles. Faced with these rigorous demands, only ∼70% of dogs that enter training ultimately achieve success. What predicts success as a guide dog? To address these questions, we followed 98 puppies from birth to adulthood. We found that high levels of overall maternal behavior were linked with a higher likelihood of program failure. Furthermore, mothers whose nursing style required greater effort by puppies were more likely to produce successful offspring, whereas mothers whose nursing style required less effort were more likely to produce offspring that failed. In young adults, an inability to solve a multistep task quickly, compounded with high levels of perseveration during the task, was associated with failure. Young adults that were released from the program also appeared more anxious, as indicated by a short latency to vocalize when faced with a novel object task. Our results suggest that both maternal nursing behavior and individual traits of cognition and temperament are associated with guide dog success.


It is often assumed that, in both human and nonhuman animals, variation in cognitive abilities contributes to variation in problem-solving skills. However, there remains little consensus about what attributes, exactly, comprise such abilities, because performance is affected not just by variation in general “intelligence” (1) or reasoning ability (e.g., refs. 2, 3) but also by variation in more affective attributes, such as impulse control, neophobia, motivation, and exploration (e.g., refs. 46).

Similarly, the long-term effects of early life experiences remain poorly understood. There is now considerable evidence that early exposure to stress has lasting effects on physiology [e.g., humans (refs. 79), rodents (refs. 10, 11), rhesus macaques (ref. 12 and reviewed in ref. 13)]. In rhesus macaques, mothering style is correlated with offspring cortisol and serotonin levels (14, 15), and in baboons, the male offspring of subordinate mothers exhibit higher glucocorticoid levels than the offspring of more dominant mothers (16). In rodents, experiences across the early weeks of life have lasting implications for later temperament measures, such as stress reactivity and fear (17, 18), and cognitive skills, such as spatial memory (19). Similar effects are observed in children, where negative life events in childhood are linked to later reductions in adolescent self-control (20).

Guide dogs are particularly suited to research on the long-term effects of early experience on adult outcomes. Over the first 5 wk of life, puppies remain with their mothers in the same facility, where they are housed in highly controlled conditions and available for systematic observation. By 2 y of age, a relatively quick period of maturation, their adult behavior can be assessed according to a discrete dependent measure: either success in or release from the training program. Achieving success, moreover, requires meeting stringent temperament and cognitive requirements. Guide dogs must follow the commands of their owners, respond appropriately to a rich array of environmental stimuli (e.g., revolving doors, escalators), ignore their impulses (e.g., to chase a squirrel), and react to the unexpected (e.g., barriers along their route). Indeed, many of the traits that we value in guide dogs, such as attention, inhibitory control, and problem solving, are also beneficial in other species, including our own. However, despite being bred and raised with the specific aim of becoming guide dogs, only ∼70% of dogs that enter training ultimately succeed in the program.

In dogs, high levels of maternal care have been linked to physical and social engagement, aggression, and lower levels of anxiety and fear (2123). Furthermore, aspects of young adult temperament, as measured by behavioral observations and questionnaires after 6 mo of age, have routinely been found to affect working dog success (2435). In military and police dogs, high levels of search focus, sharpness, prey drive, and aggression have been linked to success (25, 27, 34). In drug detection dogs, a desire for work, measured via obedience, activity, and concentration, leads to better outcomes (33). In guide dogs specifically, success is associated with high levels of obedience and trainability and low levels of reactivity, hyperactivity, aggression, distraction, and anxious behaviors (e.g., barking) (24, 26, 29, 32, 35). To date, however, no study has examined the direct effect of mother–puppy interactions on program success or failure; examined the direct effect of performance on cognitive tests on subsequent working dog success; or simultaneously explored maternal, cognitive, and temperament effects within the same model, thereby testing each variable while controlling for the others.

To examine these questions, we studied a population of German Shepherds, Labrador Retrievers, and Golden Retrievers bred to enter a guide dog training program at two life stages: puppyhood and adolescence. We began by observing mothers and their litters over the puppies’ first 3 wk of life (36) (Table S1). We then tested the same individuals on 11 cognitive and temperament tasks as young adults, at 14–17 mo of age. Some tests examined variables previously shown to predict adult working dog performance: distractibility, interest in fetching, and other temperament measures (Table S2). Other tests examined variables presumed to be important for guide dogs: temperament factors, such as obedience and attentiveness to task and handler, and cognitive factors, such as flexibility, problem solving, and proficiency in navigating a detour (Table S2). These skills have been linked to variation in adult behavior among humans and other animals but never measured in guide dogs.

Table S1.

Demographics of mothers and puppies in the study

Litter Litter size Puppies included in analyses Mother’s breed Father’s breed Puppies’ breed Labrador Retriever, % Coded puppy breed
Della 6 5 Labrador Retriever Golden Retriever Lab-Golden cross 50 Labrador Retriever
Lizzie 9 5 Golden Retriever Golden Retriever Golden Retriever 0 Golden Retriever
Dagmar 8 6 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Dori 5 2 Golden Retriever Labrador Retriever Lab-Golden cross 50 Golden Retriever
Lolly 2 0 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Dotty 2 1 Golden Retriever Labrador Retriever Lab-Golden cross 50 Golden Retriever
Onyx 8 5 Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever 100 Labrador Retriever
Maude 9 5 Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever 100 Labrador Retriever
Ayesha 10 7 Labrador Retriever* Labrador Retriever Lab-Golden cross × 3 87.5 Labrador Retriever
Foxy 7 5 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Toffee 6 5 Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever 100 Labrador Retriever
Carey 8 5 Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever 100 Labrador Retriever
Aura 7 6 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Naomi 8 6 Labrador Retriever* Labrador Retriever Lab-Golden cross × 3 87.5 Labrador Retriever
Omega 8 7 Golden Retriever Golden Retriever Golden Retriever 0 Golden Retriever
Lea 6 6 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Leah 5 3 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Paris 4 2 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Elise 9 8 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
Xyris 7 6 Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever Labrador Retriever 100 Labrador Retriever
Lisa 4 3 German Shepherd German Shepherd German Shepherd 0 German Shepherd
*

These dogs are Labrador-Golden crosses × 2, meaning their mothers were 50%-50% Labrador-Golden crosses and their sires were 100% Labrador Retrievers, making them 75% Labrador Retriever. Thus, these dogs were classified as Labrador Retrievers.

Table S2.

Summary of young adult tasks that were used in analysis

Order Task Task description Variable Type Measure Description Rho Kappa
1 Isolation The handler releases the dog into the empty lighted testing room, which the dog is then free to explore for 2 min. Time near exit Duration Time, % Dog is near the exit, in the half of the room closest to the door 0.99
Activity score Count 1–39 How many times dog switches between quadrants over the course of the session 0.98
Mobile Duration Time, % Dog is not sitting, standing, or lying in the same spot for more than 3 s while in view 0.97
Vocalizing Duration Time, % Dog is howling, barking, yelping, whining, groaning, or play-growling 0.94
2 Distraction The handler walks the dog to the end of a hallway, facing the experimenter, and releases the dog when the experimenter calls. During the first two trials, the hallway is empty. During the last two trials, six toys and three treat rewards are placed in exact, alternating locations. All trials are capped at 2 min. Competency Duration No. of seconds Amount of time to come when called to the experimenter down an empty hallway (44 × 4 ft), averaged over two trials
Toy distraction Difference score Amount of time to come when called down hallway with six toy and three food distractors minus amount of time to come when called down empty hallway
Toy contact Average 0–6 Average number of toy distractors that dog contacts with any part of its body over two trials
Food ate Average 0–3 Average number of treats that dog eats off of the floor of the hallway over two trials
3 Sustained attention The handler positions the dog to face the experimenter in a standing position. At the start of the trial, the experimenter says “[Dog’s name], sit!” and holds up her right arm with a closed fist in a sit gesture, at which point the handler drops the leash. The trial begins when the dogs sits, and ends when both the dog’s chest and face are oriented away from the experimenter. Each of two test trials is capped at 2 min. Body orient, trial 1 Duration No. of seconds From the time the dog sits to the time that both the dog’s chest and face are oriented away from the experimenter
Body orient, trial 2 Duration No. of seconds Same as above, for trial 2
Face orient, trial 1 Duration No. of seconds Time that dog’s face is oriented toward the experimenter
Face orient, trial 2 Duration No. of seconds Same as above, for trial 2
4 Memory problem solving Over two stages of familiarization trials, dogs eat food treats directly out of food wells, as well as by removing plastic bones to uncover the wells as part of the Nina Ottosson Dog Magic game. In the test trial, the dog watches as the experimenter baits four of the nine wells with food and then places plastic bones over all of the wells. The dog is then released and allowed 2 min to solve the problem and retrieve the rewards. Solving time Duration No. of seconds Amount of time to uncover and eat all four treats successfully
No. correct Count 0–4 Number of correct wells uncovered in 2 min
Accuracy score Difference score Correct wells, % Number of correct wells uncovered in 2 min minus number of incorrect wells uncovered in 2 min
Persistence Duration Time, % Amount of time engaging with the apparatus divided by the solving time
5 Multistep problem solving Over three stages of familiarization trials, dogs eat treats directly out of uncovered wells, as well as spinning the apparatus and removing plastic bones to uncover the treats as part of the Nina Ottosson Dog Tornado game. In the test trial, the dog watches as the experimenter baits a well, twists the apparatus to cover the baited well, and then places a plastic bone in the empty well next to the baited one, thereby rendering the apparatus unable to spin until the bone is dislodged. The dog is released and allowed 2 min to solve the problem and retrieve the reward. Solving time Duration No. of seconds Amount of time to uncover and eat the treat successfully
Perseveration Duration Time, % Amount of time that the dog sniffs, noses, paws, scratches, mouths, and/or licks at the area of the apparatus covering the well with the hidden treat while the bone is still in the adjacent well (and thus blocking the rotation of the apparatus), divided by the total amount of time interacting with the apparatus 0.99
6 Cylinder The dog completes familiarization trials with an opaque cylinder, in which it must retrieve a food reward from the open sides of the apparatus without touching the front on four of the last five trials. In 10 test trials, the dog must solve the identical problem except that the apparatus is a transparent cylinder. Test trial score Count No. correct Correct if dog’s snout enters the open end of the cylinder without the dog first touching the exterior of the cylinder with any part of its snout or paws; incorrect if dog touches the front or back of the cylinder with its snout or paws before finding the treat
7 Detour problem solving The experimenter stands directly in front of the dog with a treat and calls it over three warm-up trials. Then, the handler takes the dog out of the room and the experimenter sets up a serpentine maze of barriers that are 4 ft tall. When the dog reenters the room, the experimenter stands out of view at the end of the maze and calls it, at which point the handler releases the dog to solve the problem. Three test trials are capped at 2 min, and if the dog has not solved the problem on its own by the end of each trial, it is shown the solution. Test trial 1 time Duration No. of seconds Amount of time from start of trial 1 to solving trial 1
Test trial 2 time Duration No. of seconds Same as above, for trial 2
Test trial 3 time Duration No. of seconds Same as above, for trial 3
Test trial score Rating 1 Solved one of three trials within the time limit
2 Solved two of three trials within the time limit
3 Solved three of three trials within the time limit
8 Greeting The dog is held on leash in the center of the testing room. The experimenter knocks on the door, and then enters the room cloaked in a hooded felt cape and standing hunched-back, ∼5 ft from the dog. She waits silently for 15 s, and then calls and encourages the dog. If the dog approaches, the experimenter pets the dog and talks in a friendly manner. At the end of 45 s, the experimenter removes her cape and plays with the dog. Latency to approach Latency No. of seconds Amount of time for dog to approach the experimenter after her entry into the testing room, with dogs that never approach receiving the maximum score of 45
Interact Duration No. of seconds Amount of time that dog is in contact with the experimenter throughout the trial
9 Ball play The experimenter throws a round rubber KONG extreme ball (medium/large) for 30 s as a warm-up, and then throws the ball and encourages the dog to retrieve it. She rethrows the ball as many times as the dog brings it back over 1 min and then repeats the process for a second trial. Retrieval score Rating 1 Dog shows no interest in the ball
2 Dog runs after the ball, touches it, but does not pick it up in its mouth
3 Dog picks up the ball but does not bring it back
4 Dog retrieves the ball and brings it back one to two times
5 Dog retrieves the ball and brings it back three or more times
10 Novel object The handler releases the dog into the empty testing room with two motion-activated battery-operated toy cats (FurReal Friends Daisy Play-With-Me-Kitty) for 2 min. Latency to approach Latency No. of seconds Amount of time to approach one of the cats initially within one foot 0.96
Orient Duration No. of seconds Amount of time that dog spends with face oriented toward a cat 0.97
Latency to vocalize Latency No. of seconds Amount of time until dog makes first sound (howl, bark, yelp, whine, groan, or play-growl) 0.84
11 Umbrella opening The handler holds the dog on leash 64-in from the experimenter. When the dog is facing forward, the experimenter pushes a button to release an auto-open black umbrella, then immediately lowers it to the ground. The dog is then allowed to explore for 45 s. If the dog is not near the umbrella after 15 s, the experimenter verbally coaxes it, and if the dog does not approach after 30 s, the handler will pick up the dog’s tab leash and try to gently guide the dog to the umbrella. Reactivity initial response Rating 1 No detectable reaction other than turning head or perking ears 0.83
2 Flinch or startle without lowering of the body (some movement, including a small step back, is fine)
3 Crouch or ducking (downward movement of body and/or head) without major displacement and maintaining general body orientation
4 Rapid avoidance response away from stimulus
Recovery approach Rating 1 Dog initially approaches the umbrella within 15 s
2 Dog initially approaches the umbrella within 16–30 s, after receiving verbal encouragement
3 Dog initially approaches the umbrella after 30 s, after being led to it on leash
4 Dog never approaches the umbrella over the 45-s trial, despite verbal and physical coaxing
Recovery contact Duration No. of seconds Dog closely sniffing and/or in contact with the umbrella

The tasks presented during young adult testing were similar to the tasks reported in the following studies: task 1 (22, 37, 38), task 2 (26, 39), task 3 (not previously studied), task 4 (40), task 5 (5, 51), task 6 (41, 42), task 7 (44, 45), task 8 (31, 45, 46), task 9 (27, 37, 45), task 10 (6, 47, 48), and task 11 (49, 50).

By 2.5 y of age, all dogs (n = 98) had received an outcome: either success [placed as a guide or breeder, n = 66 (67%)] or failure [released from the program, n = 32 (33%)] (Table S3). Our overall aim was to examine the relation between dogs’ success in the program and both their mothers’ behavior before weaning and their performance in subsequent cognitive and temperament tests as young adults.

Table S3.

Sample size of observed dataset

Observed dataset n
Total included in sample 98
 Placed as guide or breeder 66
 Released from program for behavioral reasons 32
Total excluded from sample 40
 Transferred to external organization 1
 Died 1
 Released from program for medical reasons 26
 Missing novel object data due to camera malfunction 1
 Missing multistep problem-solving data due to failing to pass the warm-up trials 8
 Missing all young adult data due to release before return to headquarters 3

Results

Maternal Style.

From videotapes of mothers and puppies, we extracted seven variables of maternal behavior: time spent in a nursing box with puppies, contact, licking/grooming, lateral nursing (mother lying on side), vertical nursing (mother sitting/standing), ventral nursing (mother lying on stomach), and orienting away from puppies. These behaviors all loaded onto one principal component (PC), Maternal behavior, that explained a significant portion of the variance (54%), remained stable over time, and was correlated with concurrent experimental and hormonal measures of maternal care (36). Mothers that scored high on this component were vigilant, often in proximity to their litter, and regularly interacted with their puppies (further details are provided in SI Materials and Methods). These mothers also showed higher baseline cortisol levels and a greater stress response when briefly separated from their puppies.

Differences in Maternal behavior were associated with several measures of young adult performance (Table S4). Dogs that experienced more maternal care were more active when isolated (task 1b: estimate = 0.57, Wald = 7.35, P = 0.007), slower and more perseverative at multistep problem solving (task 5: estimate = 0.48, Wald = 4.21, P = 0.04), and quicker to vocalize during the novel object task (task 10b: estimate = −0.59, Wald = 5.62, P = 0.02). All other maternal behavior effects varied by breed (SI Materials and Methods and Table S4). Differences in Maternal behavior were also associated with outcome, whereby puppies that experienced lower levels of maternal behavior were more likely to succeed (SI Materials and Methods and Table S5).

Table S4.

Associations between Maternal behavior and young adult test measures

Task no. Task description Estimate German Shepherd Labrador Retriever Golden Retriever
1a Isolation, anxious 0.08
1b Isolation, active 0.57**
2 Distraction 0.14
3 Sustained attention 0.01
4a Memory problem solving, superior performance 0.24
4b Memory problem solving, accuracy −2.16** 0.08 0.44 −1.72**
5 Multistep problem solving, poor performance 0.48*
6 Cylinder −0.01
7 Detour problem solving, poor performance −0.16
8 Greeting 0.08
9 Ball play 0.04
10a Novel object, wary −1.85* −0.14 0.96** −0.89
10b Novel object, quiet −0.59*
11a Umbrella opening, reactivity −0.19
11b Umbrella opening, recovery −2.03* 0.00 1.12* −0.92

Estimate values are listed under each breed in the event of an interaction. Estimates that were significant at P < 0.05 or less are bolded. Predictor variables included the following: Maternal behavior; breed (German Shepherd, Labrador Retriever, or Golden Retriever); litter size, 2–10; maternal parity, 1–5; sex of puppy, 1/0 (male or female); and age at return, 14–17 mo. Litter ID was entered as a random effect. n = 98 (32 release dogs and 66 successes). Statistical tests of significance used GEE (**P < 0.01; *P < 0.05).

Table S5.

Model exploring the association between Maternal behavior and outcome

Predictor variables OR Estimate SE Wald P value
Maternal behavior 3.39 1.22 0.34 12.98 <0.001***
Golden score 0.70 −0.35 0.33 1.11 0.292
Labrador score 0.99 −0.01 0.38 0.00 0.974
Maternal parity 1.07 0.07 0.10 0.46 0.500
Sex of puppy 0.37 −0.99 0.62 2.54 0.111
Age at return 0.71 −0.34 0.21 2.69 0.101

The dependent variable was outcome in the program, 1/0 (released from program or successfully placed as guide or breeder). Predictor variables retained were as follows: Maternal behavior Golden score, Golden Retriever compared with German Shepherd; Labrador score, Labrador Retriever compared with German Shepherd; maternal parity, 1–5; sex of puppy, 1/0 (male or female); and age at return, 14–17 mo. Litter ID was entered as a random effect. n = 98 (32 release dogs, 66 successes). Statistical tests of significance used GEE (***P < 0.001).

Research in other species has shown that specific maternal behaviors, particularly nursing styles, can have long-term effects on offspring development (Introduction), and our Maternal behavior PC included three nursing types that loaded at varying strengths and recalled some differences in nursing styles in other species. Therefore, we analyzed which of the behaviors that loaded strongly onto Maternal behavior were associated with outcome (36). We standardized each variable and entered it singly as a predictor variable. Upon determining which variables were significantly associated with outcome in individual models, we combined those variables into a single logistic regression model. We built a generalized estimating equation (GEE) general linear model (GLM) with outcome as the dependent variable; time in the nursing box, licking/grooming per puppy, vertical nursing per puppy, and ventral nursing per puppy were entered as predictors with breed, maternal parity, sex of puppy, and age at return entered as covariates. Litter identification (ID) was a random effect. Results (Table S6) revealed a main effect of ventral nursing (Wald = 10.20, P = 0.001): Puppies exposed to high levels of ventral nursing were more likely to be released from the program [odds ratio (OR) = 4.22, odds of program release 4.22-fold higher]. There was also a main effect of vertical nursing (Wald = 34.57, P < 0.001), but in the opposite direction: Puppies exposed to more vertical nursing were less likely to be released (OR = 0.25, 75% lower odds of program release).

Table S6.

Model exploring the association between variables comprising Maternal behavior and outcome

Predictor variables OR Estimate SE Wald P value
Ventral nursing per puppy 4.22 1.44 0.45 10.20 0.001**
Vertical nursing per puppy 0.25 −1.39 0.24 34.57 <0.001***
Licking/grooming per puppy 1.02 0.02 0.51 0.00 0.970
Golden score 0.84 −0.17 0.47 0.13 0.716
Labrador score 1.80 0.59 0.55 1.15 0.283
Maternal parity 1.21 0.19 0.18 1.05 0.305
Sex of puppy 0.47 −0.76 0.54 2.02 0.156
Age at return 0.75 −0.29 0.22 1.80 0.180

The dependent variable was outcome in the program: 1/0 (released from program or successfully placed as guide or breeder). Predictor variables were retained as follows: ventral nursing per puppy; vertical nursing per puppy; licking/grooming per puppy; Golden score, Golden Retriever compared with German Shepherd; Labrador score, Labrador Retriever compared with German Shepherd; maternal parity, 1–5; sex of puppy, 1/0 (male or female); and age at return, 14–17 mo. Litter ID was entered as a random effect. n = 98 (32 release dogs, 66 successes). Statistical tests of significance used GEE (***P < 0.001; **P < 0.01).

Young Adult Test Performance.

Dogs participated in 11 tasks of temperament and cognition as young adults, just before entering The Seeing Eye training program (5, 6, 22, 26, 27, 31, 3751) (SI Materials and Methods and Table S2). The 11 tasks yielded scores that could be summarized by 13 PCs and two standardized/z-scored variables (SI Materials and Methods and Table S7).

Table S7.

Using PC analysis, where applicable, to reduce variables per task in the young adult test

Task no. Task Measure Type of measure Scores into measure Proportion variance explained, % Fit
1a Isolation Anxious PC Time near exit (+), vocalizing (+) 54 0.77
1b Isolation Active PC Activity score (+), mobile (+) 46 0.77
2 Distraction Distractibility PC Toy distraction (+), toy contact (+), food eaten (+) 61 0.77
3 Sustained attention Attentive to human PC Body orient trial 1 and 2 (+), face orient trial 1 and 2 (+) 72 0.89
4a Memory problem solving Superior performance PC Solving time (−), no. correct (+), persistence (+) 69 0.97
4b Memory problem solving Accuracy PC Accuracy (+) 31 0.97
5 Multistep problem solving Poor performance PC Solving time (+), perseveration (+) 80 0.89
6 Cylinder Test trial score Z-scored variable Test trial score NA NA
7 Detour problem solving Poor performance PC Test trial score (−), test trial 1 time (+), test trial 2 time (+), test trial 3 time (+) 56 0.85
8 Greeting Willingness to interact PC Latency to approach (−), interact (+) 73 0.64
9 Ball play Retrieval score Z-scored variable Retrieval score NA NA
10a Novel object Wary PC Latency to approach (+), orient (+) 58 0.69
10b Novel object Quiet PC Latency to vocalize (+) 42 0.69
11a Umbrella opening Reactivity PC Initial response (+) 35 0.64
11b Umbrella opening Recovery PC Approach (+), contact (−) 65 0.64

To determine which of the young adult performance tests best predicted program outcome (52, 53) (SI Materials and Methods), we included data from both our primary dataset (n = 98; 66 successes and 32 behavioral releases) and from an additional 32 subjects that had entered the training program but were subsequently released for medical reasons (imputed dataset; Table S8). Three young adult test scores were associated with program outcome in both the observed and the observed plus imputed datasets (Table S8, tasks 5, 10b, and 11a): poor performance on the multistep problem-solving task, latency to vocalize when presented with a novel object, and umbrella-opening reactivity. We therefore selected these three tests for inclusion into a final multivariate logistic regression model (Table S9) that used program outcome from only the observed (nonimputed) dataset as the dependent variable. All three tests remained associated with program outcome (SI Materials and Methods).

Table S8.

Young adult testing ORs between score on each task and release from the program

Observed dataset Imputed plus observed dataset
Task no. Task description n OR Shep Lab Gold n OR Shep Lab Gold
1a Isolation, anxious 109 0.87
1b Isolation, active 109 1.12
2 Distraction 110 0.89
3 Sustained attention 110 1.35
4a Memory problem solving, superior performance 105 1.35
4b Memory problem solving, accuracy 105 2.10* 125 1.21
5 Multistep problem solving, poor performance 100 1.70* 120 1.49
6 Cylinder 108 0.78
7 Detour problem solving, poor performance 110 1.07
8 Greeting 110 0.86
9 Ball play 110 17.12* 0.58 1.26 21.33* 130 1.70 0.63 0.77 1.31
10a Novel object, wary 109 1.35
10b Novel object, quiet 109 0.60** 129 0.61*
11a Umbrella-opening reactivity 110 0.33* 0.98 1.36 0.45 130 0.29* 0.94 1.57* 0.45
11b Umbrella-opening recovery 110 1.14

OR values are listed under each breed in the event of an interaction. OR values that were significant at P < 0.10 or less are bolded. Predictor variables included the following: each task score, respectively; breed (German Shepherd, Labrador Retriever, or Golden Retriever); maternal parity, 1–5; sex of puppy, 1/0 (male or female); and age at return, 14–17 mo. Litter ID was entered as a random effect. Statistical tests of significance used GEE (**P < 0.01; *P < 0.05). The observed dataset included 98 subjects with a known outcome in the program. The imputed plus observed dataset included an additional 32 subjects that entered the training program but were subsequently released for medical reasons. In the judgment of The Seeing Eye’s Director of Canine Development (who had not observed the dogs during testing and had no knowledge of their performance on tests), these subjects would have included 19 successes (59%) and 13 releases (41%) (n = 130; 85 successes and 45 behavioral releases). The imputed plus observed dataset was only used to aid in verifying the tasks most associated with outcome, and not in any predictive models. Shep, Shepherd; Lab, Labrador Retriever; Gold, Golden Retriever.

Table S9.

Model exploring the association between young adult test performance and outcome

Predictor variables OR Estimate SE Wald P value
Multistep problem solving, poor performance 1.75 0.56 0.23 5.63 0.018*
Novel object, quiet 0.43 −0.84 0.33 6.51 0.011*
Golden score 0.43 −0.85 0.31 7.64 0.006**
Labrador score 0.97 −0.03 0.41 0.01 0.941
Maternal parity 1.12 0.11 0.13 0.67 0.414
Sex of puppy 0.31 −1.18 0.59 4.03 0.045*
Age at return 0.59 −0.52 0.25 4.41 0.036*
Interaction 0.23 −1.45 0.45 10.30 0.001**
Umbrella-opening reactivity × German Shepherd 0.84 −0.18 0.46 0.16 0.691
Umbrella-opening reactivity × Labrador Retriever 1.70 0.53 0.26 4.15 0.042*
Umbrella-opening reactivity × Golden Retriever 0.40 −0.92 0.37 6.22 0.013*

The dependent variable was outcome in the program, 1/0 (released from program or successfully placed as guide or breeder). Predictor variables retained were as follows: multistep problem solving, poor performance; long latency to vocalize when presented with a novel object; an interaction between umbrella-opening reactivity and breed (German Shepherd, Labrador Retriever, and Golden Retriever); Golden score, Golden Retriever compared with German Shepherd; Labrador score, Labrador Retriever compared with German Shepherd; maternal parity, 1–5; sex of puppy, 1/0 (male or female); and age at return, 14–17 mo. Litter ID was entered as a random effect. n = 98 (32 release dogs, 66 successes). Statistical tests of significance used GEE (**P < 0.01; *P < 0.05).

Maternal Style and Young Adult Performance Combined.

To compare the predictive strength of maternal style and young adult test performance, we built a single model that incorporated both classes of variables as predictors. Program outcome was the dependent variable, and predictors were the Maternal behavior PC and the three young adult test performance variables listed above. We included as covariates breed, maternal parity, sex of puppy, and age at return. Litter ID was entered as a random effect. Results are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Model exploring the combined effect of Maternal behavior and young adult performance on outcome

Predictor variables OR Estimate SE Wald P value
Maternal behavior 2.61 0.96 0.44 4.62 0.032*
Multistep problem-solving poor performance 1.67 0.51 0.24 4.52 0.034*
Novel object quiet 0.50 −0.69 0.33 4.39 0.036*
Golden score 0.59 −0.53 0.31 2.89 0.089
Labrador score 0.72 −0.33 0.45 0.54 0.461
Maternal parity 1.04 0.04 0.14 0.07 0.792
Sex of puppy 0.42 −0.86 0.57 2.27 0.132
Age at return 0.57 −0.57 0.24 5.94 0.015*
Interaction 0.27 −1.32 0.40 11.00 <0.001***
Umbrella-opening reactivity × German Shepherd 0.75 −0.29 0.52 0.30 0.584
Umbrella-opening reactivity × Labrador Retriever 1.80 0.59 0.27 4.89 0.027*
Umbrella-opening reactivity × Golden Retriever 0.48 −0.73 0.32 5.28 0.022*

The dependent variable was outcome in the program, 1/0 (released from program or successfully placed as guide or breeder). Predictor variables retained were as follows: Maternal behavior; multistep problem-solving poor performance; long latency to vocalize when presented with a novel object; an interaction between umbrella-opening reactivity and breed (German Shepherd, Labrador Retriever, and Golden Retriever); Golden score, Golden Retriever compared with German Shepherd; Labrador score, Labrador Retriever compared with German Shepherd; maternal parity, 1–5; sex of puppy, 1/0 (male or female); and age at return, 14–17 mo. Litter ID was entered as a random effect. n = 98 (32 release dogs, 66 successes). Statistical tests of significance used GEE (***P < 0.001; *P < 0.05).

As in earlier tests, we found a main effect of Maternal behavior (Wald = 4.62, P = 0.03), indicating that puppies raised by mothers with high scores on Maternal behavior were more likely to be released (OR = 2.61, odds of program release 2.61-fold higher). We also found an association with performance on the multistep problem-solving task (Wald = 4.52, P = 0.03), indicating that dogs performing poorly on this task were more likely to be released (OR = 1.67, odds of program release 1.67-fold higher; Movie S1). In addition, young adults of all breeds that were slow to vocalize during the novel object task were less likely to be released (Wald = 4.39, P = 0.04, OR = 0.50, odds of program release 50% lower; Movie S2). Finally, we found an interaction between breed and reactivity to the umbrella-opening task (Wald = 11.00, OR = 0.27, P < 0.001): Labrador Retrievers that showed stronger behavioral responses had higher rates of release from the program (Wald = 4.89, P = 0.03, OR = 1.80, odds of program release 1.80-fold higher), whereas Golden Retrievers with stronger responses had lower rates (Wald = 5.28, P = 0.02, OR = 0.48, odds of program release 52% lower).

Finally, age at return, a demographic factor, was also found to be important. Dogs that returned to headquarters at a younger age had higher rates of program release (Wald = 5.94, P = 0.02, OR = 0.57, odds of program release 43% lower with each additional month remaining with puppy-raising family).

Model Comparisons.

Several measures of maternal behavior and young adult performance were significantly associated with outcome. The associations remained significant even when the predictors were combined into a single model. To determine which combination of measures best predicted outcome, we tested the discrimination, or performance, of each model by calculating the area under the curve (AUC), which quantified each model’s ability to classify a dog correctly as an eventual program release or success (higher AUCs indicate better predictive power) (54, 55) (SI Materials and Methods). The AUCs and 95% confidence intervals for all models are listed in Table S10. Values for all models were above 0.5, indicating that all combinations of maternal and young adult measures were predictive of outcome at above chance levels (56). When we compared models, we found that the Maternal behavior-only model was significantly different from the young adult-only model (the latter was better: Z = −1.99, P = 0.05) and the combined Maternal behavior and young adult model (the combination model was better: Z = −2.24, P = 0.03). We therefore concluded that both maternal style and young adult behavior are important, but the young adult measures were more powerful from a predictive standpoint.

Table S10.

AUCs and 95% confidence intervals for three separate models

Predictors AUC 95% CI
Maternal behavior PC 0.664 (0.548, 0.774)
Young adult performance 0.750 (0.640, 0.849)
Maternal behavior PC and young adult combined 0.754 (0.649, 0.851)

The 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated using a bootstrapping method with 2,000 iterations.

For illustrative purposes, we summarize in Fig. 1 the main effects of Maternal behavior and young adult performance on outcome and illustrate the likelihood of success when dogs were ranked according to their performance on these measures.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Relation between dogs’ scores on three behavioral measures (A) and their success in the program (B). (A) Dogs were ranked according to their scores on the three behavioral measures that most strongly predicted outcome (low levels of maternal behavior, good performance on young adult multistep problem solving, and slow latency to vocalize to a novel object as a young adult) and then divided into thirds (top third, n = 34; middle third, n = 32; and bottom third, n = 32) based on the sum of their ranks. (B) Same dogs’ mean percentage of success in The Seeing Eye program, calculated by group, is depicted. Error bars represent the SEMs.

SI Materials and Methods

Breed Classifications.

Puppies from cross-litters (n = 31) were categorized as the breed that contributed over 50% of their genes. If they were 50% Labrador Retriever and 50% Golden Retriever, they were assigned their mother’s breed (Table S1). In total, we tested 51 German Shepherds, 60 Labrador Retrievers, and 22 Golden Retrievers. In the final analysis of program outcome (n = 98), the sample consisted of 39 German Shepherds, 44 Labrador Retrievers, and 15 Golden Retrievers (Table S3).

Material Behavior.

Mothers were housed singly in indoor pens with access to an outdoor area through a guillotine door, and puppies were contained in towel-lined kiddie pools (“nursing boxes”) over the first 3 wk. We coded distinct behaviors by mothers: time spent in the nursing box with puppies, contact, licking/grooming, lateral nursing (mother lying on side), vertical nursing (mother sitting/standing), ventral nursing (mother lying on stomach), and orienting away from puppies. These behaviors all loaded strongly onto one PC, Maternal behavior. This component explained a significant portion of the variance, remained stable over time, and had predictive validity because it was correlated with independent experimental and hormonal measures of mothering (36). Mothers that scored high on Maternal behavior were vigilant; often in proximity to their litter; and regularly contacted, licked, groomed, and nursed their puppies.

Maternal Behavior as a Predictor Variable.

Because Maternal behavior scores were significantly positively correlated across weeks 1, 2, and 3 (36), we use week 2 Maternal behavior scores in all analyses as our predictor variable. We were unable to observe two litters on week 2, so these puppies were given the average of their mothers’ week 1 and week 3 Maternal behavior scores. Because we could not identify puppies individually on our videotapes, all littermates received the same score.

Maternal Behavior Association with Outcome.

We conducted a GEE-GLM with outcome as the dependent variable, Maternal behavior as the predictor variable, and litter ID as a random effect. Breed, maternal parity, sex of puppy, and age at return were included as covariates. Results (Table S5) revealed a significant main effect of Maternal behavior (Wald = 12.98, P < 0.001): Puppies raised by mothers exhibiting more maternal behavior were more likely to be released from the program (OR = 3.39). The odds of program release were 3.39-fold higher with each SD increase in Maternal behavior. None of the covariates were significant predictors.

Testing After Return to The Seeing Eye and After Surgery.

After returning to headquarters for training, males were housed individually, whereas females were often housed with a same-sex kennelmate. All dogs received food twice a day (7:00 hours and 16:00 hours), and water was always available. The lights were switched on around 6:30 hours in the morning and turned off at 18:00 hours at night.

Young adult testing took place from May through October 2015. Most dogs completed postarrival (PA) testing 2 d (n = 107) or 3 d (n = 22) after they were returned to headquarters from their puppy-raising families. One dog was tested 1 d PA, two were tested 5 d PA, and one was tested 6 d PA. The majority then completed a second round of postsurgery (PS) testing 2 d (n = 100) or 3 d (n = 24) after undergoing anesthesia for alteration surgery and/or hip X-rays. One dog each was tested 8, 16, 21, 22, and 23 d PS, whereas four dogs did not undergo anesthesia at all before PS testing. PS testing was identical to PA testing.

All testing occurred in an 11 × 7-ft examination room located within an unoccupied kennel wing. Testing occurred between 7:30 hours and 17:30 hours. On a testing day, each dog was tested twice and tasks were always presented in the same order. Within a given dog’s testing session, tasks occurred one after the other with only brief breaks for setup. Food rewards consisted of Zuke’s Mini Naturals treats. Testing sessions were videotaped using Sony video cameras (HDR-PJ230, HDR-CX405) mounted on tripods.

The following variables were coded from videotape either by one of the authors (E.E.B.) or by a research assistant who had participated in data collection: all variables from isolation, perseveration during multistep problem solving, all variables from the novel object task, and initial response during the umbrella-opening task. To assess the reliability of the video-coded variables, an additional coder coded 20% of randomly selected trials. The interrater reliability was assessed by calculating Spearman’s rho for continuous variables and Cohen’s kappa for categorical variables (Table S2). All other performance measures were coded live.

Data Reduction Applied to Young Adult Cognitive and Temperament Tasks.

To determine which variables to include in the young adult analysis, we compared the dogs’ rankings on the 29 scores (derived from 11 tasks; Table S2) at PA testing with their rankings on these same scores at PS testing by computing Kendall rank correlation coefficients. Results revealed significant correlations (P < 0.05) for 21 of 29 scores and a marginally significant correlation (P = 0.09) for one of 29 scores. Seven of 29 scores were not significantly correlated (P > 0.10). Thus, the dogs’ rank order of performance was significantly correlated in 21 of 29 (72%) task variables. Moreover, the lack of correlation in three scores (cylinder task and two detour problem-solving performance measures) was most likely due to a ceiling effect on the task at the time of the second testing. Finally, the conditions surrounding PA testing were much more consistent across dogs than the conditions surrounding PS testing. PA testing happened 2 to 3 d PA in 97% of subjects. PS testing happened 2 to 3 d PS in 92% of subjects, but time of surgery varied by dog and ranged from 1 to 30 d PA. In addition, some dogs never had surgery [e.g., dogs earmarked for the breeding program at the time of testing (n = 15), dogs that had previously been spayed or neutered (n = 4), dogs that had medical issues (n = 2)], and three of the four dogs that were previously altered did not undergo anesthesia before their second testing. We therefore elected to use only results from PA (the initial) testing in subsequent analyses. Only seven dogs (five males and two females) were altered before young adult testing, so data for altered and intact dogs were not considered separately.

Using PA data only, we looked at each of the 11 tasks to determine how and/or if each of the task variables could be summarized in one or two PCs. Results revealed that the 29 variables from all tasks could be summarized by 15 measures: 13 PCs using a varimax rotation and two z-scored variables that were not appropriate for PCA due to unacceptable Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin values below 0.50 (Table S7).

Given the modest size of our dataset, it was necessary to reduce the total number of young adult behaviors that could be considered in a multivariate model (33). To screen the young adult behaviors most associated with outcome in the program, we first evaluated each task score component in a separate GEE-GLM that clustered young adult dogs by litter as the unit of observation (Table S8). These models allowed us to evaluate the association between individual task score and program outcome after adjustment for important confounders (breed, maternal parity, sex of puppy, and age at return). We did not adjust for multiple testing because we expected that our task performance measures were correlated with one another, especially measures that were derived from the same task. Furthermore, because our goal was prediction, we needed to look at each measure’s individual association with outcome to select the best smaller subset to be considered jointly (53).

Young Adult Testing Association with Outcome.

The following scores were associated with release from the program: Dogs with slow solve times and high levels of perseveration on the multistep problem-solving task were more likely to be released (Table S9, task 5: OR = 1.75, Wald = 5.63, P = 0.02, odds of program release 1.75-fold higher; Movie S1), whereas dogs displaying a long latency to vocalize during the novel object task were less likely to be released (task 10b: OR = 0.43, Wald = 6.51, P = 0.01, odds of program release 57% lower; Movie S2). There was also a significant interaction between breed and reactivity in the umbrella-opening task (task 11a: OR = 0.23, Wald = 10.30, P = 0.001): Golden Retrievers that visibly reacted to the surprising event were less likely to be released from the program (Wald = 6.22, OR = 0.40, P = 0.01, odds of program release 60% lower), whereas Labrador Retrievers that reacted strongly were more likely to be released (Wald = 4.15, OR = 1.70, P = 0.04, odds of program release 1.70-fold higher). Age at return and sex were also significant independent predictors of outcome: Dogs that returned to headquarters at older ages had a lower probability of being released (Wald = 4.41, P = 0.04, OR = 0.59, odds of program release 41% lower), and the risk of program release was 69% lower for males than for females (Wald = 4.03, P = 0.045, OR = 0.31).

Discrimination of Models.

To assess the discrimination of each model, we computed the areas under a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve using the R package “pROC” (35). Ninety-five percent confidence intervals were computed using the bootstrapping method, also in pROC (36). We then used paired-design “roc.tests” to compare the area under the ROC curve (AUC) of the different models with one another.

Associations Between Maternal Behavior and Measures of Young Adult Test Performance in Which There Was an Interaction Between Maternal Behavior and Breed.

There was an interaction between breed and Maternal behavior on superior performance during memory problem solving (Table S4, task 4a: Wald = 4.68, P = 0.03), as well as between breed and Maternal behavior on accuracy during this task (task 4b; Wald = 8.81, P = 0.003): Golden Retrievers that experienced high levels of maternal care were persistent and quick to solve the memory problem-solving task (Wald = 6.25, P = 0.01), but they were also less accurate (Wald = 10.10, P < 0.001). We also found a significant interaction between breed and Maternal behavior on wariness during the novel object task (task 10a: Wald = 8.25, P = 0.004): Labrador Retrievers that experienced higher levels of maternal care oriented toward the novel objects at high levels but were slow to approach them (Wald = 9.16, P < 0.01). Finally, we found an interaction between breed and Maternal behavior on recovery during umbrella opening (task 11b; Wald = 7.57, P < 0.01): Labrador Retrievers that experienced higher levels of maternal behavior were slower to approach the umbrella postopening and spent less time in contact with it (Wald = 8.51, P < 0.01).

Discussion

Like Foyer et al. (21) and Guardini et al. (22), we found an association between maternal behavior and young adult behavior in tests of temperament. However, contrary to their results, we found that increased maternal behavior was positively associated with undesirable anxiety-related behaviors and performance in young adult dogs, including high activity when isolated, a short latency to vocalize when presented with a novel object, and perseverative errors and poor performance during a problem-solving task.

In our final model, the only nonbehavioral covariate related to outcome was the age at which dogs were returned for training. Dogs that returned from their puppy-raising families at a younger age (14 mo) were less likely to succeed than older dogs (17 mo). These results are consistent with the results obtained in one previous study (57), where dogs entering training at 17 mo were more likely to succeed than older individuals (up to 27 mo). Given that personality traits in dogs, such as calmness and boldness, have been linked to age (58), it seems possible that returning for training at a specific age leads to better acclimation to a kennel setting. Additionally, the accrual of more “real-world” experiences before training may lead to better outcomes, but only up to a point.

Even when controlling for the effect of age on outcome, however, behavioral differences in mothers and puppies had significant consequences for success in the program.

First, variation in Maternal behavior was significantly associated with dogs’ later success in guide dog training. Contrary to our expectations, however, puppies that received higher levels of maternal behavior were less likely to succeed in the program. This finding may not be an isolated result. Parker and Maestripieri (59) point out that the influence of stress on outcome has long focused on extreme disruptions of the parent–offspring relationship, and thus been treated in the literature as a linear function, in which the more early life stress an individual faces, the worse the outcome. They argue, however, that the relationship is actually quadratic: Too much stress is certainly a bad thing, but so is too little, because young animals then lack the experience of learning to deal independently with stress. Rather, facing an intermediate amount of stress in early life can have an inoculating effect on subsequent behavior (60). Several studies support this view. Although long maternal separations are universally acknowledged to have deleterious consequences (e.g., refs. 6164), studies in squirrel monkeys show that repeated short-term separations give young animals a chance to respond to temporary aversive events, which is adaptive over the long term (65). These benefits may also extend to cognitive performance and response inhibition.

Consistent with this view, we found that high levels of ventral nursing were associated with program release, whereas high levels of vertical nursing were related to program success. These differences in nursing styles may provide different opportunities for puppies to “prevail over small challenges” (65). When mothers nursed ventrally, while lying on their stomachs, they were relatively immobile and their nipples were at the puppies’ face level, making it easy for puppies to stay attached. In contrast, when mothers nursed vertically, while sitting or standing, nursing was a more difficult, active, and effortful endeavor for puppies. Vertical nursing in dogs is most similar to arched-back nursing in rodents, which has been linked to positive outcomes in adulthood, including better spatial memory (19) and lower anxiety (66). Some of these effects in rodents might be explained by the nipple switching facilitated by arched-back nursing, which results in increased tactile stimulation (19). Interestingly, in our population, as in rats (e.g., Fig. 1 in ref. 67), vertical nursing was the rarest of the nursing styles. Therefore, one possible explanation for our results is that a moderate amount of maternal care is beneficial but higher levels of maternal care are not challenging enough, and thereby have a negative effect on later performance. Perhaps in this population of dogs, where all puppies obtain sufficient maternal care and nutrition, receiving comparatively less (or an average amount of) maternal attention fosters resilience, whereas more maternal attention increases vulnerability.

One limitation of the current study is the potential confound of genetic effects. Because all mothers were related to the puppies they were rearing, we are unable to determine a precise causal link between maternal style and later puppy behavior and outcome. The mother’s behavior could potentially be an artifact of the dog’s genetic makeup contributing to a specific temperament, which is then inherited by the puppy. Cross-fostering studies similar to the studies conducted on rodents (e.g., refs. 19, 68) might help to disentangle the genetic vs. behavioral effects of maternal style on program outcome.

Some measures of temperament and problem-solving abilities were also linked to dogs’ later success in the guide dog program. In a multistep problem-solving task, dogs that perseverated less and were quickest to solve the problem were more likely to succeed. This result supports our prediction that problem solving and impulse control are central to success. Similarly, dogs with shorter latencies to vocalize during the novel object task, a likely sign of higher anxiety (69), were more likely to be released from the program. This result is consistent with the findings of Harvey et al. (26), who found that the guide dogs predicted to be successful had lower scores on a fear/anxiety PC at 5 mo of age. The component was partially based on vocalizing during tasks.

Both the combined model and the young adult-only model had significantly higher AUCs than the Maternal behavior-only model. The fact that young adult temperament and cognitive measures had more predictive power is not entirely surprising, because they were collected much closer to the time of actual outcome. Importantly, however, the AUCs of the maternal style, young adult performance, and combination models were all greater than chance, indicating that data from both the maternal environment and young adult time period can be useful in predicting program outcome.

Additionally, we now know that maternal style affects both young adult behavior and outcome. We also know from our combination model that maternal style has a significant effect on outcome, even when controlling for young adult behavior. Thus, it remains for future research to examine whether the association between maternal behavior and program outcome is partially mediated by the association between maternal behavior and young adult performance.

In sum, what predicts a successful guide dog? Our results support previous studies on other animals in reaffirming the enduring benefits of maternal care—in moderation. Furthermore, they suggest that a few targeted tests associated with temperament, perseveration, and cognition may capture individual differences in ability that continue throughout adulthood.

Materials and Methods

Subjects (Table S1) were 21 mothers (nine German Shepherds, eight Labrador Retrievers, and four Golden Retrievers) and their 21 litters (n = 138 puppies) belonging to The Seeing Eye, Inc. (Morristown, NJ), a philanthropic organization that breeds, raises, and trains guide dogs for the blind and visually impaired. The Seeing Eye granted informed consent to the study. All mothers lived at the breeding station, where the puppies were whelped and weaned. The young adult testing took place at headquarters, where puppies returned for training and placement. All testing procedures adhered to regulations set forth by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol 805210).

Mothers and litters were videotaped (n = 328 min per litter on average) over the puppies’ first 3 wk of life (36). Puppies were weaned at 5 wk and then sent at 7 wk to “puppy-raising” families who fostered, trained basic obedience, and exposed the puppies to a variety of experiences. Subjects returned to The Seeing Eye at the age of 13–17 mo (n = 133) for training. Before their formal entrance into the training program, we tested subjects individually on 11 cognitive and temperament tasks (Table S2).

Before 3 y of age, all dogs were either successfully placed as a guide or breeder or released from the program. Breeders completed 2 mo of guide dog training and then were selected for the breeding program based on health and behavior. Dogs could be released at any point, although only 4% of our sample was released before returning for training. The primary reasons that dogs were released were behavioral, such as lack of confidence, excitability, and inability to focus. Because we were only interested in program release for behavioral reasons, dogs released for medical reasons were excluded from analyses (e.g., ref. 29). Of the original 138 observed dogs, 29% (n = 40) were excluded from analysis due to release for medical concerns (n = 27), transfer to another organization (n = 1), or missing data on the young adult tasks (n = 12) (Table S3).

Maternal Style.

Complete methods used to study maternal style can be found in a study by Bray et al. (36) (SI Materials and Methods).

Young Adult Performance.

All testing took place at The Seeing Eye headquarters (SI Materials and Methods). Each dog first completed an hour-long session involving seven tasks (Table S2, tasks 1–7), was given at least an hour-long break, and then completed a 30-min second session (Table S2, tasks 8–11) (70). The main experimenter and dog handler were present at each session. These roles were always filled by two of five females of similar age, with the first author (E.E.B.) as the main experimenter in 87% of sessions.

Data Processing and Statistical Analysis.

All statistical analyses were carried out in R version 3.3.0 (71). To test for associations between maternal behavior, young adult test performance, and outcome, we built logistic regression models. Variance estimates for the statistical tests on the regression coefficients were adjusted for clustering due to litter effects using GEE-GLM (72). Models were fit using “geepack” in R (73). To assess the calibration of each model, we performed Hosmer–Lemeshow goodness-of-fit tests (74). Nonsignificant P values (P > 0.05) for all models indicated there was no evidence of poor fit and all models were therefore correctly specified. Following previous studies (e.g., refs. 21, 36, 37), breed, maternal parity (1–5), sex of puppy (1/0, male vs. female), litter size (2–10 puppies), and age in months (14–17) when the dog returned for training were included as covariates in all models. Birth season (1/0, winter vs. spring) was not included because it was highly correlated with age when dogs returned for training. Covariates were then removed using a backward-selection strategy, with the final model retaining confounders that influenced any association of interest by greater than 15%.

We first built models to examine the effect of the PC, Maternal behavior, on young adult performance. These GEE-GLMs used a Gaussian error distribution with litter as the unit of analysis.

We next built models to examine how outcome was affected by Maternal behavior, the variables that comprised Maternal behavior, performance on cognitive and temperament tasks as a young adult, and Maternal behavior combined with young adult performance. These GEE-GLMs were conducted with the “logit” link and a binomial error distribution. In these models, litter size could be excluded as a covariate due to lack of confounding.

Finally, we evaluated the predictive ability of these models to discriminate correctly between dogs that were successful and dogs that were released from the program (SI Materials and Methods).

Supplementary Material

Supplementary File
Download video file (82.7MB, mp4)
Supplementary File
Download video file (60.4MB, mp4)

Acknowledgments

We thank S. Bartner, L. Cohen, S. Frommer, N. Gay, A. Gersick, M. Ream, R. Schwartz, A. Seely, and M. Torres for assistance with data collection, dog testing, and coding. We also thank S. Hasan for his enormous help with Datavyu export code. We are grateful to Dr. Dolores Holle for coordinating our work at The Seeing Eye, as well as the leadership team of The Seeing Eye, Breeding Station Manager Maria Hevner, and Director of Canine Development Peggy Gibbon. We also thank the breeding station and training kennel staff for giving us access to the kennels at both the breeding station and headquarters, and allowing us to work with their dogs. This work was supported, in part, by the University of Pennsylvania Department of Psychology’s Norman Anderson Graduate Student Fund, a University of Pennsylvania University Research Fund award, the Class of 1971 Robert J. Holtz Endowed Fund for Undergraduate Research, the University of Pennsylvania’s University Scholars program, and a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (DGE-1321851 to E.E.B.).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Data deposition: The data reported in this paper have been deposited on Dryad Digital Repository, www.datadryad.org (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.50fj0).

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1704303114/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Spearman C. “General intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. Am J Psychol. 1904;15:201–292. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Arden R, Bensky MK, Adams MJ. A review of cognitive abilities in dogs, 1911 through 2016: More individual differences, please! Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25:307–312. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Deary IJ. Looking Down on Human Intelligence: From Psychometrics to the Brain. Oxford Univ Press; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Overington SE, Cauchard L, Côté KA, Lefebvre L. Innovative foraging behaviour in birds: What characterizes an innovator? Behav Processes. 2011;87:274–285. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Benson-Amram S, Holekamp KE. Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas. Proc Biol Sci. 2012;279:4087–4095. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Marshall-Pescini S, Virányi Z, Kubinyi E, Range F. Motivational factors underlying problem solving: Comparing wolf and dog puppies’ explorative and neophobic behaviors at 5, 6, and 8 weeks of age. Front Psychol. 2017;8:180. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rao H, et al. Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: Evidence from brain morphology in humans. Neuroimage. 2010;49:1144–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tomalski P, Johnson MH. The effects of early adversity on the adult and developing brain. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2010;23:233–238. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283387a8c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Diamond MC. Response of the brain to enrichment. An Acad Bras Cienc. 2001;73:211–220. doi: 10.1590/s0001-37652001000200006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Levine S. Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the neonatal rat: The role of maternal behavior. Neurotox Res. 2002;4:557–564. doi: 10.1080/10298420290030569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Suomi S. Long-term effects of different early rearing experiences on social, emotional, and physiological development in nonhuman primates. In: Keshavan MS, Murray RM, editors. Neurodevelopment and Adult Psychopathology. Cambridge Univ Press; Cambridge, UK: 1997. pp. 104–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. 2007;87:873–904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Maestripieri D, Hoffman CL, Anderson GM, Carter CS, Higley JD. Mother-infant interactions in free-ranging rhesus macaques: Relationships between physiological and behavioral variables. Physiol Behav. 2009;96:613–619. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.12.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Maestripieri D, McCormack K, Lindell SG, Higley JD, Sanchez MM. Influence of parenting style on the offspring’s behaviour and CSF monoamine metabolite levels in crossfostered and noncrossfostered female rhesus macaques. Behav Brain Res. 2006;175:90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Onyango PO, Gesquiere LR, Wango EO, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Persistence of maternal effects in baboons: Mother’s dominance rank at son’s conception predicts stress hormone levels in subadult males. Horm Behav. 2008;54:319–324. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Davis EP, Parker SW, Tottenham N, Gunnar MR. Emotion, cognition, and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A developmental perspective. In: de Haan M, Johnson MH, editors. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Development. Psychology Press; New York: 2003. pp. 181–206. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Meaney MJ, Aitken DH, van Berkel C, Bhatnagar S, Sapolsky RM. Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science. 1988;239:766–768. doi: 10.1126/science.3340858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Liu D, Diorio J, Day JC, Francis DD, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, hippocampal synaptogenesis and cognitive development in rats. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:799–806. doi: 10.1038/77702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Duckworth AL, Kim B, Tsukayama E. Life stress impairs self-control in early adolescences. Front Psychol. 2013;3:608. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Foyer P, Wilsson E, Jensen P. Levels of maternal care in dogs affect adult offspring temperament. Sci Rep. 2016;6:19253. doi: 10.1038/srep19253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Guardini G, et al. Influence of morning maternal care on the behavioural responses of 8-week-old Beagle puppies to new environmental and social stimuli. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2016;181:137–144. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tiira K, Lohi H. Early life experiences and exercise associate with canine anxieties. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0141907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Batt LS, Batt MS, Baguley JA, McGreevy PD. Factors associated with success in guide dog training. J Vet Behav. 2008;3:143–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sinn DL, Gosling SD, Hilliard S. Personality and performance in military working dogs: Reliability and predictive validity of behavioral tests. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2010;127:51–65. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Harvey ND, et al. Test-retest reliability and predictive validity of a juvenile guide dog behavior test. J Vet Behav. 2016;11:65–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Slabbert J, Odendaal JSJ. Early prediction of adult police dog efficiency—A longitudinal study. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1999;64:269–288. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Batt LS, Batt MS, Baguley JA, McGreevy PD. The value of puppy raisers’ assessments of potential guide dogs’ behavioral tendencies and ability to graduate. Anthrozoos. 2009;22:71–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Duffy DL, Serpell JA. Predictive validity of a method for evaluating temperament in young guide and service dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;138:99–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tomkins LM, Thomson PC, McGreevy PD. Associations between motor, sensory and structural lateralisation and guide dog success. Vet J. 2012;192:359–367. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2011.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Svartberg K. Shyness–boldness predicts performance in working dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79:157–174. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Tomkins LM, Thomson PC, McGreevy PD. Behavioral and physiological predictors of guide dog success. J Vet Behav. 2011;6:178–187. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Maejima M, et al. Traits and genotypes may predict the successful training of drug detection dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2007;107:287–298. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wilsson E, Sundgren P-E. The use of a behaviour test for the selection of dogs for service and breeding, I: Method of testing and evaluating test results in the adult dog, demands on different kinds of service dogs, sex and breed differences. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1997;53:279–295. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. Genetics of traits which determine the suitability of dogs as guide-dogs for the blind. Appl Anim Ethol. 1983;9:299–315. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bray EE, Sammel MD, Cheney DL, Serpell JA, Seyfarth RM. Characterizing early maternal style in a population of guide dogs. Front Psychol. 2017;8:175. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wilsson E, Sundgren P-E. Behaviour test for eight-week old puppies—Heritabilities of tested behaviour traits and its correspondence to later behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1998;58:151–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gazzano A, Mariti C, Notari L, Sighieri C, McBride EA. Effects of early gentling and early environment on emotional development of puppies. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;110:294–304. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. The relationship of fearfulness to, and the effects of, sex, age and experience on exploration and activity in dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1984;12:267–278. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Barrera G, Fagnani J, Carballo F, Giamal Y, Bentosela M. Effects of learning on social and nonsocial behaviors during a problem-solving task in shelter and pet dogs. J Vet Behav. 2015;10:307–314. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bray EE, MacLean EL, Hare BA. Context specificity of inhibitory control in dogs. Anim Cogn. 2014;17:15–31. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0633-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.MacLean EL, et al. The evolution of self-control. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:E2140–E2148. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323533111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Fox MW, Stelzner D. Behavioural effects of differential early experience in the dog. Anim Behav. 1966;14:273–281. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(66)80083-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Osthaus B, Marlow D, Ducat P. Minding the gap: Spatial perseveration error in dogs. Anim Cogn. 2010;13:881–885. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0331-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pfaffenberger CJ, Scott JP, Fuller JL, Ginsburg BE, Bielfelt SW. Guide Dogs for the Blind: Their Selection, Development, and Training. Elsevier; Amsterdam: 1976. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. Early prediction of adult behaviour in potential guide dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1986;15:247–260. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. A factor analysis of fearfulness in potential guide dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1984;12:253–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.King T, Hemsworth PH, Coleman GJ. Fear of novel and startling stimuli in domestic dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003;82:45–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.van der Borg JAM, Netto WJ, Planta DJU. Behavioural testing of dogs in animal shelters to predict problem behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1991;32:237–251. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Sherman BL, et al. A test for the evaluation of emotional reactivity in Labrador retrievers used for explosives detection. J Vet Behav. 2015;10:94–102. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Marshall-Pescini S, Valsecchi P, Petak I, Accorsi PA, Previde EP. Does training make you smarter? The effects of training on dogs’ performance (Canis familiaris) in a problem solving task. Behav Processes. 2008;78:449–454. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Peduzzi P, Concato J, Kemper E, Holford TR, Feinstein AR. A simulation study of the number of events per variable in logistic regression analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49:1373–1379. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(96)00236-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Greenland S. Modeling and variable selection in epidemiologic analysis. Am J Public Health. 1989;79:340–349. doi: 10.2105/ajph.79.3.340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Robin X, et al. pROC: An open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics. 2011;12:77. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-12-77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Carpenter J, Bithell J. Bootstrap confidence intervals: When, which, what? A practical guide for medical statisticians. Stat Med. 2000;19:1141–1164. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0258(20000515)19:9<1141::aid-sim479>3.0.co;2-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Streiner DL, Cairney J. What’s under the ROC? An introduction to receiver operating characteristics curves. Can J Psychiatry. 2007;52:121–128. doi: 10.1177/070674370705200210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ennik I, Liinamo A-E, Leighton E, van Arendonk J. Suitability for field service in 4 breeds of guide dogs. J Vet Behav. 2006;1:67–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kubinyi E, Turcsán B, Miklósi A. Dog and owner demographic characteristics and dog personality trait associations. Behav Processes. 2009;81:392–401. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Parker KJ, Maestripieri D. Identifying key features of early stressful experiences that produce stress vulnerability and resilience in primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:1466–1483. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lyons DM, Parker KJ, Katz M, Schatzberg AF. Developmental cascades linking stress inoculation, arousal regulation, and resilience. Front Behav Neurosci. 2009;3:32. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.032.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Pryce CR, Dettling A, Spengler M, Spaete C, Feldon J. Evidence for altered monoamine activity and emotional and cognitive disturbance in marmoset monkeys exposed to early life stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1032:245–249. doi: 10.1196/annals.1314.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kosten TA, et al. Memory impairments and hippocampal modifications in adult rats with neonatal isolation stress experience. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;88:167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Grissom EM, et al. Learning strategy is influenced by trait anxiety and early rearing conditions in prepubertal male, but not prepubertal female rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2012;98:174–181. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Aisa B, Tordera R, Lasheras B, Del Río J, Ramírez MJ. Effects of maternal separation on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses, cognition and vulnerability to stress in adult female rats. Neuroscience. 2008;154:1218–1226. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lyons DM, Parker KJ. Stress inoculation-induced indications of resilience in monkeys. J Trauma Stress. 2007;20:423–433. doi: 10.1002/jts.20265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Caldji C, et al. Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:5335–5340. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Myers MM, Brunelli SA, Squire JM, Shindeldecker RD, Hofer MA. Maternal behavior of SHR rats and its relationship to offspring blood pressures. Dev Psychobiol. 1989;22:29–53. doi: 10.1002/dev.420220104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Weaver IC, et al. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:847–854. doi: 10.1038/nn1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Beerda B, Schilder MBH, van Hooff JARAM, de Vries HW. Manifestations of chronic and acute stress in dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1997;52:307–319. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Bray EE, Sammel MD, Seyfarth RM, Serpell JA, Cheney DL. Temperament and problem solving in a population of adolescent guide dogs. Anim Cogn. July 10, 2017 doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1112-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.R Development Core Team . R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna: 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Liang K-Y, Zeger SL. Regression analysis for correlated data. Annu Rev Public Health. 1993;14:43–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pu.14.050193.000355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Halekoh U, Højsgaard S, Yan J. The R package geepack for generalized estimating equations. J Stat Softw. 2006;15:1–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Hosmer DW, Jr, Lemeshow S. Applied Logistic Regression. 2nd Ed Wiley; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary File
Download video file (82.7MB, mp4)
Supplementary File
Download video file (60.4MB, mp4)

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES