Abstract
Bipolar disorder (BD) is a common disorder with high reoccurrence rate in general population. It is critical to have objective biomarkers to identify BD patients at an individual level. Neurocognitive signatures including affective Go/No-go task and Cambridge Gambling task showed the potential to distinguish BD patients from health controls as well as identify individual siblings of BD patients. Moreover, these neurocognitive signatures showed the ability to be replicated at two independent cohorts which indicates the possibility for generalization. Future studies will examine the possibility of combining neurocognitive data with other biological data to develop more accurate signatures.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s40345-017-0101-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Bipolar disorder, Neurocognition, Vulnerability, CANTAB, Machine learning
Correspondence
Bipolar disorder (BD) has a lifetime prevalence of 4–5% in the general population. It is frequently associated with high rates of morbidity, mortality, and completed suicides (Mathers et al. 2006; Merikangas 2007; Nordentoft et al. 2011). It has been reported that only 20% of BD patients experiencing a depressive episode are diagnosed with BD within the first year of seeking treatment. This greatly underscores the need for objective diagnostic and vulnerability markers of this debilitating illness (Goldberg et al. 2001). Noticeably, previous epidemiological studies have reported that first-degree relatives of BD patients have an increased tenfold risk of BD as compared to the general population—which strongly highlights the role of genetic factors to the etiology of BD (Kessler et al. 1994; Olvet et al. 2013). However, despite these facts, there are no clinically useful biomarkers of vulnerability to BD that guides the institution of prophylactic interventions. These timely interventions may delay the onset of BD and translate into better clinical outcomes such as decreased rates of recurrence, less severe episodes (Post et al. 2010), and reduced medical related costs due to less hospitalizations.
Multiple studies have reported neurocognitive abnormalities in BD patients as compared to demographically matched healthy controls (HCs). These abnormalities have primarily been shown in key cognitive domains such as: executive function, sustained attention, verbal learning, and working memory (Robinson and Ferrier 2006; Torres et al. 2007; Arts et al. 2008; Bora et al. 2009; Torres et al. 2010; Mann-Wrobel et al. 2011; Bourne et al. 2013; Bauer et al. 2015; Wu et al. 2016). Furthermore, studies examining neurocognitive measurements in first-degree relatives of BD patients have also reported deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives in similar neurocognitive domains. A recent meta-analysis summarized studies investigating neurocognitive endophenotypes in BD and reported abnormalities in first-degree relatives of BD patients in key domains such as: set-shifting, processing speed, verbal learning, and response inhibition (Bora et al. 2009). Similarly, in a recent review, Olvet et al. reported a consistent theme on memory-related deficits in unaffected twins and siblings of patients with BD as compared to HCs (Olvet et al. 2013). Specifically, verbal, declarative, and working memory deficits were shown in unaffected siblings (Gourovitch et al. 1999; Kéri et al. 2001; Kieseppä et al. 2005; Christensen et al. 2006). Moreover, several other studies have highlighted executive function and verbal memory abnormalities as candidate endophenotypes of BD following reported deficits in these domains in first-degree relatives of BD patients (Arts et al. 2008; Bora et al. 2009; Doyle et al. 2009). However, while these studies have undeniably advanced our understanding of vulnerability markers of BD, it remains unknown whether reported abnormalities can objectively identify unaffected individuals vulnerable to BD and at an individual level. Noticeably, being able to predict an individual participant’s probability of vulnerability to BD based on a hazard-free and easily accessible neurocognitive task could help in institution of individualized prophylactic interventions and translate into favorable clinical outcomes.
To achieve this objective, we recruited 21 euthymic BD patients (7 males, 14 females; age: 36.12 ± 16.55 years) and 21 demographically matched HCs (5 males, 16 females; age: 36.08 ± 12.66 years) at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill—a sample we refer to as the discovery cohort. A set of neurocognitive task scores were assessed for each individual using the Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB). The nine assessed CANTAB neurocognitive tasks include: Affective Go/No-Go, Big/Little Circle, Cambridge Gambling Task, Choice Reaction Time, Motor Screening, Match to Sample Visual Search, Rapid Visual Processing, Spatial Recognition Memory, and Spatial Span task. The essence and measurements of all nine tasks are summarized in Table 1. As a second step, a replication cohort of 15 BD patients (5 males, 10 females; age: 32.67 ± 9.26 years) and 16 demographically matched HCs (5 males, 11 females; age: 33.75 ± 10.95 years) were assessed at the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston. A set of CANTAB neurocognitive task measurements similar to the discovery cohort was also assessed. Notably, in the second center (replication cohort), an additional group of 15 age- and gender-matched siblings (SI) (4 males, 11 females; age: 32.20 ± 11.69 years) of BD patients (non-affected with BD) were also recruited and their CANTAB measurements were assessed. These data were first used to ‘train’ a least absolute shrinkage selection operator (LASSO) machine-learning algorithm in distinguishing patients from HCs. Second, the established predictive signature was further validated using an independent replication cohort of BD patients and HCs (Fig. 1). Lastly, the extent to which the validated predictive neurocognitive signature may differentiate the siblings (SIs) from HCs and BD patients was also examined.
Table 1.
No. | CANTAB task | Evaluation | Measurements |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Affective Go/No-Go | Inhibition control | Reaction time*, accuracy |
2 | Big/Little Circle | Comprehension, learning and reversal | Reaction time*, accuracy |
3 | Cambridge Gambling Task | Risk-taking behavior | Reaction time*, accuracy, proportion bets across trials with more/equally/less likely outcome |
4 | Choice Reaction Time | Simple (motor) processing speed | Reaction time*, accuracy |
5 | Motor Screening | Simple (motor) processing speed | Reaction time* |
6 | Match to Sample Visual Search | Ability to match motor and visual stimuli | Reaction time*, accuracy |
7 | Rapid Visual Processing | Sustained attention | Reaction time*, accuracy |
8 | Spatial Recognition Memory | Visual spatial recognition memory | Reaction time*, accuracy |
9 | Spatial Span task | Spatial working memory | Span length, number of attempts, reaction times* |
*Reaction time is in milliseconds
The LASSO algorithm identified individual BD patients from HCs in the discovery cohort with 69% accuracy, 76% sensitivity, 62% specificity, 67% of positive predictive values (PPV), 72% of negative predictive values (NPV), and an area under receiver operating characteristic curve (AUROC) of 0.6905 with Chi-square p = 0.0126 (Fig. 2 and Additional file 1: Table S1). In the discovery cohort, predictor variables identified by the LASSO algorithm as most relevant in distinguishing BD patients from HCs (non-zero coefficients) include: number of omission errors to negative stimuli on the Affective Go/No-Go task, delay aversion, and the risk adjustment on the Cambridge Gambling Task and the total number of hits on the Rapid Visual Processing (Fig. 3 and Additional file 1: Table S2). In the replication cohort, the LASSO model derived at the discovery stage identified individual BD patients from HCs in the replication cohort with 74% accuracy, 73% sensitivity, 75% specificity, 73% of PPV, 75% of NPV, and an AUROC of 0.7417 (Fig. 4 and Additional file 1: Table S3). These predictions were significant (Chi-square p = 0.007). Predicted probability scores of HCs differed significantly from SIs and BD patients with p = 0.027 and p = 0.008, respectively. On the other hand, SIs were largely indistinguishable from BD patients with p = 0.678. These tests were performed using a non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis statistical test.
From a cognitive viewpoint, compared to HCs, individuals with BD committed a greater number of errors when exposed to negative stimuli. This finding provides further support for the presence of a negative affective bias which is reflected by impaired cognitive processing resulting from exposure to negative stimuli in both adults with BD and offspring of BD patients (Pavuluri and Passarotti 2008; Abe et al. 2011; Passarotti et al. 2011, 2012; Bauer et al. 2015). Furthermore, HCs had a higher quality of risk adjustment on the CGT task compared with individuals with BD (Quraishi and Frangou 2002), which is a reliable estimate of impulsivity and risk taking (Swann et al. 2003). Therefore, our findings are consistent with previous evidence that patients with BD have a high reward-seeking response and are unable to delay gratification (Najt et al. 2007; Swann et al. 2009). Moreover, in spite of the absence of a diagnosis of BD, the at-risk individuals displayed the tendency to make poorer decisions compared with HCs. This finding is particularly relevant because, to date, few studies have focused on the cognitive functioning of siblings of BD patients. Previous studies of unaffected siblings found that they scored lower on tests of verbal learning, attention, and planning than healthy individuals (Kéri et al. 2001; Trivedi et al. 2008; Kulkarni et al. 2010; Nehra et al. 2014). Further, in line with our findings, the magnitude of these cognitive deficits of SIs has consistently been reported to be intermediate between that of HCs and BD patients. Another potential implication of our findings is that impulsivity, a trait typically associated with BD (Newman and Meyer 2014) and underlying decision making and reward tasks (Christodoulou et al. 2006) is a potential marker of vulnerability to BD in SIs.
The current study has some potential limitations. The overall sample size in both discovery and replication cohorts were small and therefore our results should be regarded as preliminary. The discovery cohort was relatively small as we only considered euthymic patients at the signature discovery stage to avoid potential confounders related to mood phase (e.g., depression, mania). Six SI participants were diagnosed with other mood disorders other than BD (e.g., major depression) and future studies should examine this research question using an SI cohort without any psychiatric diagnoses. BD patients included in the discovery cohort were taking psychotropic medications which may be a potential confounder but also a reflection of standard clinical practice.
In conclusion, we report a study showing neurocognitive signature able to distinguish individual BD patients from HCs. We suggest this signature could be combined with other biological features to potentially develop a BD prediction model. However, the current study serves as a proof-of-concept. Future studies will examine this hypothesis using other biological markers (e.g., neuroimaging) as well as attempt to integrate multi-scale biomarkers (e.g., neuroimaging and neurocognition) which may potentially improve the current prediction results.
Authors’ contributions
MW was in charge of data preprocessing, implementation of machine learning algorithms, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. BM participated in the implementation of machine learning algorithms, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. IP participated in data preprocessing, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. IB participated in data preprocessing, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. BC participated in data interpretation and manuscript preparation. TF participated in data acquisition, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. GZ participated in data acquisition, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. JS participated in data acquisition, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Competing interests
Prof. Soares has participated in research funded by Forest, Merck, BMS, GSK and has been a speaker for Pfizer and Abbott. Dr. Frazier has received federal funding or research support from, acted as a consultant to, received travel support from, and/or received a speaker’s honorarium from the Simons Foundation, the Ingalls Foundation, Forest Laboratories, Ecoeos, IntegraGen, Kugona LLC, Shire Development, Bristol-Myers Squibb, the National Institutes of Health, and the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. All other authors have no interests to declare.
Availability of data and materials
All data were stored at secured places at the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston and can only be accessed by authorized personnel under the supervision of IRB.
Consent for publication
All author gave their consent to publish this manuscript.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study was approved by the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC) and the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston (UTHealth) Institutional Review Boards. All participants signed informed consent before any study-related procedures were performed.
Funding
This research was funded by NIH R01 MH 085667, the Dunn Foundation and Pat Rutherford, Jr. Chair in Psychiatry at UTHealth to Jair C. Soares.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abbreviations
- BD
bipolar disorders
- HC
healthy control
- SI
sibling
- CANTAB
Cambridge Neurocognitive Test Automated Battery
- UNC
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
- UTHealth
University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston
- HAMD
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale
- MADRS
Montgomery–Åsberg Depression Rating Scale
- YMRS
Young Mania Rating Scale
- LASSO
Least Absolute Selection Shrinkage Algorithm
- PPV
positive predictive values
- NPV
negative predictive values
- AUROC
area under receiver operating characteristic curve
Additional file
Footnotes
Mon-Ju Wu and Benson Mwangi contributed equally to this work
In the original publication of this article there was an error in one of the author names. The original version of this article was revised.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s40345-017-0101-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
An erratum to this article is available at https://doi.org/10.1186/s40345-017-0105-5.
Contributor Information
Mon-Ju Wu, Phone: +17134862686, Email: mon-ju.wu@uth.tmc.edu.
Benson Mwangi, Email: Benson.Irungu@uth.tmc.edu.
Ives Cavalcante Passos, Email: IvesCP1@gmail.com.
Isabelle E. Bauer, Email: Isabelle.E.Bauer@uth.tmc.edu
Bo Cao, Email: Bo.Cao@uth.tmc.edu.
Thomas W. Frazier, Email: FRAZIET2@ccf.org
Giovana B. Zunta-Soares, Email: Giovana.B.ZuntaSoares@uth.tmc.edu
Jair C. Soares, Email: Jair.C.Soares@uth.tmc.edu
References
- Abe N, Uchida S, Otsuki K, Hobara T, Yamagata H, Higuchi F, Shibata T, Watanabe Y. Altered sirtuin deacetylase gene expression in patients with a mood disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45:1106–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, van Os J. Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol Med. 2008;38:771–785. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer IE, Ouyang A, Mwangi B, Sanches M, Zunta-Soares GB, Keefe RSE, Huang H, Soares JC. Reduced white matter integrity and verbal fluency impairment in young adults with bipolar disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;62:115–122. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord. 2009;113:1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourne C, Aydemir Ö, Balanzá-Martínez V, Bora E, Brissos S, Cavanagh JTO, Clark L, Cubukcuoglu Z, Dias VV, Dittmann S, et al. Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: an individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013;128:149–162. doi: 10.1111/acps.12133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen MV, Kyvik KO, Kessing LV. Cognitive function in unaffected twins discordant for affective disorder. Psychol Med. 2006;36:1119–1129. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christodoulou T, Lewis M, Ploubidis GB, Frangou S. The relationship of impulsivity to response inhibition and decision-making in remitted patients with bipolar disorder. Eur Psychiatr. 2006;21:270–273. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle AE, Wozniak J, Wilens TE, Henin A, Seidman LJ, Petty C, Fried R, Gross LM, Faraone SV, Biederman J. Neurocognitive impairment in unaffected siblings of youth with bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2009;39:1253–1263. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg JF, Harrow M, Whiteside JE. Risk for bipolar illness in patients initially hospitalized for unipolar depression. Am J Psychiatr. 2001;158:1265–1270. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gourovitch ML, Torrey EF, Gold JM, Randolph C, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE. Neuropsychological performance of monozygotic twins discordant for bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatr. 1999;45:639–646. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00148-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kéri S, Kelemen O, Benedek G, Janka Z. Different trait markers for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a neurocognitive approach. Psychol Med. 2001;31:915–922. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701004068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, Wittchen H-U, Kendler KS. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatr. 1994;51:8–19. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kieseppä T, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Haukka J, Van Erp T, Glahn D, Cannon TD, Partonen T, Kaprio J, Lönnqvist J. Memory and verbal learning functions in twins with bipolar-I disorder, and the role of information-processing speed. Psychol Med. 2005;35:205–215. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kulkarni S, Jain S, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T. Impairment of verbal learning and memory and executive function in unaffected siblings of probands with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2010;12:647–656. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00857.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mann-Wrobel MC, Carreno JT, Dickinson D. Meta-analysis of neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: an update and investigation of moderator variables. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13:334–342. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00935.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathers C, Iburg K, Begg S. Adjusting for dependent comorbidity in the calculation of healthy life expectancy. 2006. Popul Health Metr. http://pophealthmetrics.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1478-7954-4-4. Accessed 2 Feb 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Merikangas K. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of bipolar spectrum disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. JAMA Psychiatr. 2007;64(5):543–552. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Najt P, Perez J, Sanches M, Peluso MAM, Glahn D, Soares JC. Impulsivity and bipolar disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;17:313–320. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nehra R, Grover S, Sharma S, Sharma A, Sarkar S. Neuro-cognitive functioning in unaffected siblings of patients with bipolar disorder: comparison with bipolar patients and healthy controls. Indian J Psychiatr. 2014;56:283. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.140645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman AL, Meyer TD. Impulsivity: present during euthymia in bipolar disorder?—a systematic review. Int J Bipolar Disord. 2014;2:1–16. doi: 10.1186/2194-7511-2-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nordentoft M, Mortensen PB, Pedersen CB. Absolute risk of suicide after first hospital contact in mental disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatr. 2011;68:1058–1064. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olvet DM, Burdick KE, Cornblatt BA. Assessing the potential to use neurocognition to predict who is at risk for developing bipolar disorder: a review of the literature. Cogn Neuropsychiatr. 2013;18:129–145. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.724193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passarotti AM, Ellis J, Wegbreit E, Stevens MC, Pavuluri MN. Reduced functional connectivity of prefrontal regions and amygdala within affect and working memory networks in pediatric bipolar disorder. Brain Connect. 2012;2:320–334. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN. Fronto-limbic dysfunction in mania pre-treatment and persistent amygdala over-activity post-treatment in pediatric bipolar disorder. Psychopharmacology. 2011;216:485–499. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2243-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavuluri MN, Passarotti A. Neural bases of emotional processing in pediatric bipolar disorder. Expert Rev Neurother. 2008;8:1381–1387. doi: 10.1586/14737175.8.9.1381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Post RM, Leverich GS, Kupka RW, Keck PE, McElroy SL, Altshuler LL, Frye MA, Luckenbaugh DA, Rowe M, Grunze H, et al. Early-onset bipolar disorder and treatment delay are risk factors for poor outcome in adulthood. J Clin Psychiatr. 2010;71:864–872. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04994yel. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quraishi S, Frangou S. Neuropsychology of bipolar disorder: a review. J Affect Disord. 2002;72:209–226. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00091-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson LJ, Ferrier IN. Evolution of cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: a systematic review of cross-sectional evidence. Bipolar Disord. 2006;8:103–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00277.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swann AC, Lijffijt M, Lane SD, Steinberg JL, Moeller FG. Severity of bipolar disorder is associated with impairment of response inhibition. J Affect Disord. 2009;116:30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swann AC, Pazzaglia P, Nicholls A, Dougherty DM, Moeller FG. Impulsivity and phase of illness in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2003;73:105–111. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00328-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torres IJ, Boudreau VG, Yatham LN. Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;434:17–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01055.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torres IJ, DeFreitas CM, DeFreitas VG, Bond DJ, Kunz M, Honer WG, Lam RW, Yatham LN. Relationship between cognitive functioning and 6-month clinical and functional outcome in patients with first manic episode bipolar I disorder. Psychol Med. 2010;41:971–982. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trivedi JK, Goel D, Dhyani M, Sharma S, Singh AP, Sinha PK, Tandon R. Neurocognition in first-degree healthy relatives (siblings) of bipolar affective disorder patients. Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. 2008;62:190–196. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2008.01754.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu M-J, Passos IC, Bauer IE, Lavagnino L, Cao B, Zunta-Soares GB, Kapczinski F, Mwangi B, Soares JC. Individualized identification of euthymic bipolar disorder using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) and machine learning. J Affect Disord. 2016;192:219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All data were stored at secured places at the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston and can only be accessed by authorized personnel under the supervision of IRB.