Abstract
In human societies, ritualized greeting behavior includes gestural and vocal displays to signal the social acceptance of an encountering person. These displays are universal across cultures suggesting a pre-human origin. Vocal greeting displays are only reported for monkeys and apes with complex social systems, but none of these studies confirmed that greeting signals fulfill all criteria characterizing human greeting behavior. In this study, we analyzed for the first time whether vocal exchanges between mother and infants in a non-human primate fulfill the criteria of human greeting behavior and whether vocal greeting behavior is present in a basal primate with a less complex social system, the gray mouse lemur. By comparing spontaneous leave-takings and reunions, we found that vocal exchanges during mother-infant reunions fulfilled all six criteria characterizing human greeting behavior. Thus, predictable reciprocal vocal exchanges occurred at the start of the reunion (but not during leave-taking), when mother and infant had visual contact to each other. Thus, we argued that mother-infant vocal exchanges governing reunions are essential to establish social bonds and to ritualize the greeting function. Our findings suggest that ritualized vocal greeting has its origins deeply rooted in mammalian phylogeny and derives from vocal exchanges during parent-infant reunions.
Introduction
Human “greeting” rituals include complex and individual patterns made up of several gestures often combined with linguistic and non-linguistic vocal displays (e.g., “hello” or laughter) to signal the social acceptance of an encountering person1–5. According to Duranti2 the human “greeting” is defined by six criteria: (1) context of encounter – greetings have to occur at an encounter and have to be different from behavioral displays during leave-taking, (2) shared perceptual field – the greeting occurs when both interaction partners visually or acoustically (e.g., telephone) recognize each other, (3) reciprocal exchange – a greeting is an exchange between both interactors testing the relationship to each other, (4) predictability – greeting displays are relatively predictable in form and content, (5) temporal unit of interactions – the greeting occurs at the beginning of an interaction and (6) identification of interlocutor – the greeting allows an identification of a group membership (e.g., greeting pattern differs depending on the relationships between the two persons e.g., friend or stranger, mother or infant6, 7). Human greeting rituals are often multi-modal communicative displays involving acoustic, visual and tactile sensory modalities2–5, however, each modality can also be used independently. Thus, for example telephone openings8 rely only on the acoustic modality. Such vocal greetings occur across different cultures and languages2, 3 and are the first words children acquire (English/Italian children9; Danish children10) or we learn in a foreign language2. This universality suggests a pre-human origin2, 3 and leads to the question whether vocal greeting rituals following the same behavioral patterns and functions as in humans can be found in non-human primates11, our closest living relatives.
In nonhuman mammals reciprocal greeting displays are reported for the visual and tactile modality including facial expressions, mounting, embracing, and erected penis displays (e.g., baboons12–14, spider monkey15, 16, hyaena17, 18). In contrast, our knowledge on the existence of reciprocal vocal greeting displays is very limited (e.g. chimpanzees11, 19, 20, howler monkeys21, macaques22). None of these studies confirmed that the so-called vocal greeting signals fulfill the criteria of human greeting behavior as suggested by Duranti2. Moreover, in chimpanzees vocal reciprocal displays were observed in only 12% of the interactions11 concluding that these signals do not fulfill the criteria of reciprocity. Thus, it is unclear whether vocal greeting displays are present in non-human mammals.
The aim of this study was to explore whether vocal exchanges between mother and infant gray mouse lemurs at the sleeping site fulfill the criteria of vocal greeting according to Duranti2. Mouse lemurs are ideal primate models to investigate the evolutionary roots of vocal greeting behavior: (1) Due to their nocturnal life style the vocal modality was found to play an important role for communication23. Thus, vocalizations convey kin-, group- and individual-specific signatures24, 25 and are used in a broad variety of contexts26. (2) Mouse lemurs show an infant-parking system27–29 (i.e. while the mother forages solitarily, she parks her infants in either tree holes or dense vegetation), have a rapid life history30, 31 and can be bred successfully in captivity30. This allows to model natural situations by analyzing leave-takings and mother-infant reunions at the sleeping site using a standardized captive setting. (3) They are socially disperse living primates (i.e. solitary foragers where females form kin-related sleeping groups during the daytime30). This allows investigating whether vocal greeting already occurs in a transition state between solitary ranging and gregariousness or is limited to taxa with highly complex social systems such as chimpanzees. (4) Mouse lemurs are described as basal primates representing an ancestral primate model28. Thus, this allows investigating whether vocal greeting already evolved at the basis of the primate stock.
We analyzed vocal exchanges between mothers and their infants during observation intervals (OI) of one minute for two conditions: Leave-taking (=mother and infants were together at sleeping site before mother left the nest; Fig. 1B) and Reunion (=mother returned to the infant at sleeping sites; Fig. 1B) to test the criteria suggested by Duranti2.
Results
Call rate of mothers and infants
Mothers produced trill calls (Fig. 2A) whereas infants produced vocal streams (Fig. 2B–D) including vocalizations similar in acoustic shape to parts of the greeting call in the mother (proto-trill; Fig. 3D). All females (N = 11) produced trill calls in the Reunion condition, but four females produced trill calls in the Leave-taking condition. Thereby, general calling probability of mothers and infants was significantly higher in the Reunion compared to the Leave-taking condition (Mother: T = 0.00, N = 11, p = 0.003; Infants: T = 5.00, N = 11, p = 0.013; see Supplementary 1 – Table 1). This was also supported by a binomial GLMM analysis using subject and littersize as random factors (Mother: p < 0.001; Infants: p < 0.001).
Reciprocal vocal exchange
Reciprocal sequences (percentage of OIs, where mother and infant were calling; see Supplementary 1 – Table 2) were significantly increased in the Reunion (median: 57.14%, interquartile range (=IR): 47.37–74.07%) compared to the Leave-taking condition (median: 0.00%; IR: 0.00–0.00%; T = 0.00, N = 11, p = 0.003; binomial GLMM: p < 0.001; Fig. 3A). The mother initiated 66.67% (=median; IR: 44.44–95.45%; see Supplementary 1 – Table 3) of these reciprocal exchanges which was not significant different from 50% chance level (T = 40, N = 11, p = 0.202). Sequential sequences (i.e. an alternate exchange of at least two infant and one mother calls or vice versa (e.g., mother-infant-mother or infant-mother-infant)) could be observed in 33.33% (=median, IR: 4.55–50.00) of the cases whereas antiphonal call sequences (i.e. an alternate exchange of at least two infant and two mother calls (e.g., mother-infant-mother-infant or infant-mother-infant-mother)) were rare (median: 11.11%, IR: 0.00–16.67%).
Coordination of calling during Reunion in the nest
During Reunion, mothers started to vocalize significantly earlier than infants (T = 1, N = 11, p = 0.004; Fig. 3B; see Supplementary 1 – Table 3). The temporal distribution shows that mother trill calls occurred at the beginning of the reunion when the mother entered the sleeping box (medianstart: 13 s; medianend: 19 s) whereas infants started calling after the mother had vocalized (medianstart: 25 s; medianend: 47 s).
Discussion
Mother-infant vocal exchanges in gray mouse lemurs fulfill all criteria of human greeting behavior established by Duranti2, therefore suggesting that patterns of human greeting behavior can be found in non-human mammals even with less complex social systems. Thus, observed vocal exchanges occur during Reunion but not during Leave-taking fulfilling the criteria of context of encounter. Thereby, vocal exchanges occurred at the start of the encounter fulfilling the criteria of temporal unit of interaction. During vocalizing the mother and the infant mouse lemurs had visual, olfactory and acoustic contact to each other fulfilling the criteria of shared perceptual field. The constant use of the same call types by the mother and infants fulfilled the criteria of predictability. Thereby mothers and infants used different call types (trill versus infant calls) which enabled information about the interlocutors. Mother and infants uttered vocalizations in the same time unit, showing that greeting displays fulfill the criteria of reciprocal exchange. Thus, our findings show that all criteria of human vocal greeting proposed by Duranti2 were fulfilled in mother-infant vocal exchanges in gray mouse lemurs.
Thereby, a striking difference between mouse lemurs, chimpanzees and macaques was the higher proportion of reciprocal exchanges in mouse lemurs (57% in comparison to 12% in chimpanzees11, 15.5% in Japanese macaques22). The reciprocal display is one criterion for human greeting behavior, thus unidirectional vocal displays of chimpanzees and macaques are not vocal greeting signals according to the criteria of Duranti2. Moreover, these vocal displays were uttered mainly by approaching higher-ranking group members11, 20. This suggests that in chimpanzees and macaques these signals might be more related to submissive displays to inhibit aggression. In contrast, howler monkeys use vocal reciprocal exchanges also in affiliative contexts21. Thereby, these vocal exchanges were the most common affiliative interaction between males and suggest that they have an important function in strengthening the social bonds among males32. The importance of social affiliation was also shown for chimpanzees, where the occurrence of reciprocal vocal exchanges depends on the strength of the social bond between both interaction partners11. Thus, for chimpanzees which had a strong affiliation to each other the probability for reciprocal vocal exchanges increased. Therefore, it might be argued that unidirectional vocal displays may function to inhibit aggression whereas reciprocal vocal exchanges during greeting are important to strengthen the social bonds between two interaction partners.
The strongest social bond across mammals is the social bond between the parents and their offspring33, 34. Vocal communication thereby plays a crucial role since it is used for vocal recognition35, 36, to induce infant- or maternal behavior (e.g.37, 38), or to release hormones (e.g.39, 40). Mother and infant contact calls have been described in a variety of mammalian and colony-breeding bird species (e.g.35, 36, 41–43). Thereby, Farmer44 argued that parental care might be the key innovation which accounts for many convergent behavioral displays of birds and mammals such as the contact calls between parents and offspring. Thus, in species where parents and infant are temporarily separated, vocal displays to find, recognize and socially accept each other would be important for the survival of the infant. Indeed in our studied primate species such greeting patterns do occur between mother and infants, but they do also seem to persist into adulthood. Braune et al.45 showed that adult mouse lemur females utter trill calls during sleeping group formation but not during dispersal, thereby suggesting that the trill call persists as a greeting signal also outside the mother-infant context. In our study, infants responded to mother calls by producing vocal streams with high vocal plasticity, including vocalizations similar in acoustic shape to parts of the greeting call in the mother (proto-trill; Fig. 3D). This suggests that the adult trill call develops during infancy and that parental interactions may be important for infant vocal development as shown in marmosets46–50 and gibbons51. Thus, during parent-infant reunions parental calls might be used as auditory templates to develop adult structure but also to ritualize the vocal communicative “greeting” function as suggested for human infants52, 53.
Our findings in a basal solitarily living primate show that the pre-human origin of the vocal greeting is evolutionarily deeply rooted in mammalian evolution and is independent from the degree of species sociality. In human greeting rituals vocal and gestural greeting displays can be used independently from each other (e.g., vocal: telephone opening8, gestural: waving from a distance7, facial: smiling3), but are generally combined2–5. Thereby, it is argued that the multimodal use is advantageous for transmitting more complex information which is especially important for species living in a complex and cohesive social system (Multiple Message hypothesis 54, 55). Thus, especially in group-living primates multimodal greeting displays evolved to predict more efficiently the behavior and intention of group-members (e.g., friendly approach, aggressive approach). Indeed complex systems of gestural communication also involved in greeting behavior are mainly observed in group- or pair-living monkeys and apes56. In contrast, mouse lemurs, like most non-primate mammals, are constrained in displaying complex gestural and facial displays54 limiting the use of the visual and tactile modality as greeting signal in comparison to monkeys and apes. Olfactory communication may also play an important role in recognition, but chemical cues are long-lasting57 and thereby not flexible enough to be used as a greeting signal. Thus, the evolutionary origin of human greeting behavior may be located in the unimodal use of vocal exchanges whereas multimodal greeting displays integrating gestural displays and facial expressions58 evolved later in primate evolution (when social systems became more complex). Thus, group-living combined with a longer life history (longer gestation period, a longer dependency from their parents and a longer life span31) in monkeys and apes compared to solitary ranging basal primates with a rapid life history may favor the development of complex multimodal greeting signals to challenge the social complexity of group-living. Thereby, sequentially organized behavioral patterns during parent-infant interaction will become ontogenetically ritualized to coordinate the social interactions of adult conspecifics. Thus, for both vocal and gestural displays parent-infant interactions play an essential role to acquire these communicative exchanges which are proposed to follow similar patterns of human conservation19, 46–49, 59, 60.
Methods
Subjects and data recording
We recorded the vocalizations of eleven family groups of gray mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus; females with their offspring) at their sleeping sites for 24 hours at day 10 and 11 after birth. One female had one infant, seven females had twins and three females had triplets. Animals were housed in the breeding colony at the Institute of Zoology, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover (housing details see30, 61). Females and infants were kept and observed in their home cages made of wire mesh (mesh was 1 × 1 cm; width × depth × height: 0.5 × 0.8 × 1.5 m; Bioscape GmbH, Castrop-Rauxel, Germany) including at least one sleeping box (20 × 12 × 15 cm). Each sleeping box was equipped with an ultrasonic microphone (SMX-US Weatherproof; Wildlife Acoustic, Inc., Concord, MA, USA), an infrared-emitting LED fixed to the ceiling and an infrared video camera (Sony Model 2005 XA B/W Ex view, Sony Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) filming through a clear plastic wall at the long side of the box. The microphones of two neighboring sleeping boxes were linked to a Song Meter SM2 Bat + recorder (Wildlife Acoustic, sampling frequency: 192 kHz per channel) resulting in a stereo-wave file. Videos were recorded with 25 frames per second using 4CH Full D1 DVR Network recording devices (RF Concepts Ltd., Dundonald, United Kingdom). An external acoustic signal that was recorded by the microphones of both the Song Meter and the infrared video camera was used to synchronize the audio files with the respective video files.
Video and Audio analysis
To define the observation intervals for the Reunion and Leave-taking condition (Fig. 1A), we first analyzed the video material using The Player Lite HJ (AVTECH PlayerLiteHJ, AVTECH) for two behavioral events: (1) the time at which the mother enters the nest box (=mother is with all four limbs in the nest box) and (2) the time at which the mother leaves the nest box (mother is with all four limbs outside the nest box). In a pre-analysis, we noticed that mothers sit in front of the nest and call inside before entering the nest with all four limbs. To consider this approaching phase of the mother, the observation intervals (OIs) of the Reunion condition started the last 10 seconds before the mother entered the nest box and finished 50 seconds after entering. Further, to be sure that the absence of the mother was long enough to induce reunion behavior, mothers had to be at least 3 minutes absent from the nest box. The Leave-taking condition was defined as the 60 seconds before a mother leaves the nest. To be sure that mothers and infants stayed long enough together to observe mother-infant interactions, the mother had to be in the nest box for at least 2 minutes. For both conditions observation intervals (OIs) of one minute were defined and at least one infant had to be inside the nest box. In total, we analyzed 219 OIs for the Reunion condition and 429 OIs for the Leave-taking condition which met the aforementioned criteria.
To quantify calling behavior within the nest box, we inspected the spectrograms (Window size: 512; window type: Hanning) of the audio files for each OI visually using the audio-editor Audacity (2.0.1, 2.0.3. freeware, http://audacity.sourceforge.net). Vocalizations in the nest box were identified based on descriptions of the gray mouse lemur adult and infant vocal repertoire by Zimmermann23 and Scheumann et al.27. For each OI it was counted whether a mother call and/or an infant call occurred. Additionally, the time points of these calls were noted.
Statistical analysis
To compare the calling probabilities between the Reunion and the Leave-taking condition, we calculated for each family group and each condition the percentage of OIs which contained at least one (1) mother call, (2) infant call and (3) both, mother and infant calls ( = reciprocal calling; see Supplementary 1 – Table 1–3). Since data were not normally distributed, we used the non-parametric Wilcoxon test for pairwise comparison and calculated the median and the interquartile range (25–75% quartile). To account for differences in littersize as well as to a different number of OIs per family, we conducted additionally a binomial GLMM using family and littersize as random factors (R version 3.1.1 (2014–07–10); R Core Team, 2014 “lme4” package; see Supplementary 1 – Table 4).
To investigate the initiator of the reciprocal vocal interactions in the Reunion condition, we calculated for each family group the percentage of OIs where the mother vocalized first followed by the infant. To test whether the mother initiated significant more interactions than expected by chance, we performed an one sample Wilcoxon test). Additionally, we calculated the percentage of OIs showing sequential or antiphonal calling. Sequential calling was defined as an alternate exchange of at least two infant and one mother calls or vice versa (e.g., mother-infant-mother or infant-mother-infant). Antiphonal calling was defined as an alternate exchange of at least two infant and two mother calls (e.g., mother-infant-mother-infant or infant-mother-infant-mother).
To analyze the timing pattern, we calculated for each family group the average time point of the first and last mother call and the first and last infant call, including all reciprocal OIs (see Supplementary 1 – Table 5).
All statistics were performed using SPSS 24 or R. The level of significance was set to p ≤ 0.05.
Ethical approval and informed consent
The animal husbandry fulfilled all recommendations and was approved by the local veterinary authority (Lower Saxony State Office for Consumer Protection, Food Safety, and Animal Welfare Service, 42500/1 H). All observations were performed in accordance with the animal care guidelines of the European Directive 2010/63/EU and the applicable national laws in Germany. Since observations were performed in the housing cages no specific permission was required.
Data availability
Audio and video files are stored in the data archive of the Institute of Zoology, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover. The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. A supplementary file includes all raw data used in this manuscript.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank Sebastian Demtroeder for his help in analysing the video- and audio data. Furthermore, we would like to acknowledge Sönke von den Berg for his support in preparing the figures and F. Sherwood-Brock for professionally proofreading the English. This publication was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation within the funding programme Open Access Publishing.
Author Contributions
M.S. designed the study, performed the monitoring and the statistical analysis and wrote the manuscript; S.L. performed the audio- and video analysis; E.Z. initiated and supervised the study and wrote the manuscript. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-10417-8
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Firth, R. In The interpretation of ritual (ed J.S.LA. Fontaine) 1–38 (Routledge, 1972).
- 2.Duranti A. Universal and culture‐specific properties of greetings. J Linguistic Anthropology. 1997;7:63–97. doi: 10.1525/jlin.1997.7.1.63. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. Grundriß der vergleichenden Verhaltensforschung. (Piper Verlag GmbH, 1999).
- 4.Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. Zur Ethologie des menschlichen Grussverhaltens. I. Beobachtungen an Balinesen, Papuas und Samoanern nebst vergleichenden Bemerkungen. Z Tierpsychol. 1968;25:727–744. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1968.tb00041.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Eibl-Eibesfeldt I. Zur Ethologie menschlichen Grußverhaltens: II. Das Grußverhalten und einige andere Muster freundlicher Kontaktaufnahme der Waika (Yanoáma) Z Tierpsychol. 1971;29:196–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1971.tb01733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Heslin R, Boss D. Nonverbal intimacy in airport arrival and departure. Pers Soc Psychol B. 1980;6:248–252. doi: 10.1177/014616728062010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Blurton Jones, N. & Leach, G. M. In Ethological studies of child behaviour (ed N. Blurton Jones) 217–247 (Cambridge University Press, 1972).
- 8.Hopper R, Chen CH. Languages, cultures, relationships: Telephone openings in Taiwan. Res Lang Soc Interac. 1996;29:291–313. doi: 10.1207/s15327973rlsi2904_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Caselli MC, et al. A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cogn Develop. 1995;10:159–199. doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(95)90008-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wehberg S, et al. Danish children’s first words: Analysing longitudinal data based on monthly CDI parental reports. First Lang. 2007;27:361–383. doi: 10.1177/0142723707081723. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Luef, E. M. & Pika, S. Reciprocal greeting in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at the Ngogo community. J Neurolinguistics, doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.11.002 (in press).
- 12.Colmenares F. Greeting behavior between male baboons - estrous females, rivalry and negotiation. Anim Behav. 1991;41:49–60. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80502-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Colmenares F. Greeting behavior in male baboons, I: communication, reciprocity and symmetry. Behaviour. 1990;113:81–116. doi: 10.1163/156853990X00446. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Smuts BB, Watanabe JM. Social relationships and ritualized greetings in adult male baboons (Papio-Cynocephalus-Anubis) Int J Primatol. 1990;11:147–172. doi: 10.1007/BF02192786. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schaffner CM, Aureli F. Embraces and grooming in captive spider monkeys. Int J Primatol. 2005;26:1093–1106. doi: 10.1007/s10764-005-6460-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Aureli F, Schaffner CM. Aggression and conflict management at fusion in spider monkeys. Biol Lett. 2007;3:147–149. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.East ML, Hofer H, Wickler W. The Erect Penis Is a Flag of Submission in a Female-Dominated Society - Greetings in Serengeti Spotted Hyenas. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1993;33:355–370. doi: 10.1007/BF00170251. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Smith JE, et al. Greetings promote cooperation and reinforce social bonds among spotted hyaenas. Anim Behav. 2011;81:401–415. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.11.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Laporte MNC, Zuberbühler K. The development of a greeting signal in wild chimpanzees. Developmental Sci. 2011;14:1220–1234. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01069.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Laporte MNC, Zuberbühler K. Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Anim Behav. 2010;80:467–473. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.06.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dias PAD, Luna ER, Espinosa DC. The functions of the “greeting ceremony” among male mantled howlers (Alouatta palliata) on Agaltepec Island, Mexico. Am J Primatol. 2008;70:621–628. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Katsu N, Yamada K, Nakamichi M. Development in the usage and comprehension of greeting calls in a free-ranging group of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) Ethol. 2014;120:1024–1034. doi: 10.1111/eth.12275. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Zimmermann, E. In Handbook of mammalian vocalization: An integrative neuroscience approach. (ed. S. M. Brudzynski) 215–225 (Academic Press, 2010).
- 24.Leliveld LMC, Scheumann M, Zimmermann E. Acoustic correlates of individuality in the vocal repertoire of a nocturnal primate (Microcebus murinus) J Acoust Soc Am. 2011;129:2278–2288. doi: 10.1121/1.3559680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kessler SE, Scheumann M, Nash LT, Zimmermann E. Paternal kin recognition in the high frequency/ultrasonic range in a solitary foraging mammal. Bmc Ecol. 2012;12 doi: 10.1186/1472-6785-12-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zimmermann, E. In Handbook of mammalian vocalization: an integrative neuroscience approach (ed. S. M. Brudzynski) Ch. 6.3, 215–226 (Academic Press, 2010).
- 27.Scheumann M, Zimmermann E, Deichsel G. Context-specific calls signal infants’ needs in a strepsirrhine primate, the gray mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus) Dev Psychobiol. 2007;49:708–718. doi: 10.1002/dev.20234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Martin RD. A preliminary field-study of the lesser mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus J. F. Miller 1777) Fortschritte der Verhaltensforschung. 1972;9:43–89. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Eberle M, Kappeler PM. Family insurance: kin selection and cooperative breeding in a solitary primate (Microcebus murinus) Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60:582–588. doi: 10.1007/s00265-006-0203-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zimmermann, E., Radespiel, U., Mestre-Francés, N. & Verdier, J. M. In The Dwarf and Mouse Lemurs of Madagascar: Biology, Behavior and Conservation Biogeography of the Cheirogaleidae Cambridge Studies in Biological and Evolutionary Anthropology (No. 73) (eds S. M. Lehmann, U. Radespiel, & E. Zimmermann) Ch. 8, 174–194 (Cambridge University Press, 2016).
- 31.Zimmermann, E. & Radespiel, U. In Handbook of paleoanthropology. Vol. 2: Primate evolution and human origins (eds W. Henke & I. Tattersall) Ch. 10, 1163–1205 (Springer, 2007).
- 32.Wang E, Milton K. Intragroup social relationships of male Alouatta palliata on Barro Colorado Island, Republic of Panama. Int J Primatol. 2003;24:1227–1243. doi: 10.1023/B:IJOP.0000005989.29238.ce. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Broad KD, Curley JP, Keverne EB. Mother-infant bonding and the evolution of mammalian social relationships. Philos T R Soc B. 2006;361:2199–2214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Okabe S, Nagasawa M, Mogi K, Kikusui T. The importance of mother-infant communication for social bond formation in mammals. Anim Sci J. 2012;83:446–452. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-0929.2012.01014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Insley SJ. Mother-offspring vocal recognition in northern fur seals is mutual but asymmetrical. Anim Behav. 2001;61:129–137. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Torriani MV, Vannoni E, McElligott AG. Mother-young recognition in an ungulate hider species: a unidirectional process. Am Nat. 2006;168:412–420. doi: 10.1086/506971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Algers B. Nursing in pigs - communicating needs and distributing resources. J Anim Sci. 1993;71:2826–2831. doi: 10.2527/1993.71102826x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Haskins R. Effect of kitten vocalizations on maternal behavior. J Comp Physiol Psych. 1977;91:830–838. doi: 10.1037/h0077362. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Seltzer LJ, Ziegler TE, Pollak SD. Social vocalizations can release oxytocin in humans. P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci. 2010;277:2661–2666. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Delgadillo JA, et al. Male goat vocalizations stimulate the estrous behavior and LH secretion in anestrous goats that have been previously exposed to bucks. Horm Behav. 2012;62:525–530. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Esser KH. Audio-vocal learning in a nonhuman mammal - the lesser spear-nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor. Neuroreport. 1994;5:1718–1720. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199409080-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Insley SJ, Paredes R, Jones IL. Sex differences in razorbill Alca torda parent-offspring vocal recognition. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2003;206:25–31. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jouventin P, Aubin T. Acoustic systems are adapted to breeding ecologies: individual recognition in nesting penguins. Anim Behav. 2002;64:747–757. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.4002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Farmer CG. Parental care: The key to understanding endothermy and other convergent features in birds and mammals. Am.Nat. 2000;155:326–334. doi: 10.1086/303323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Braune P, Schmidt S, Zimmermann E. Spacing and group coordination in a nocturnal primate, the golden brown mouse lemur (Microcebus ravelobensis): the role of olfactory and acoustic signals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005;58:587–596. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0944-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Takahashi, D. Y., Fenley, A. R. & Ghazanfar, A. A. Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys. Philos T R Soc B371, doi:10.1098/Rstb.2015.0370 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 47.Takahashi DY, et al. The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal production. Science. 2015;349:734–738. doi: 10.1126/science.aab1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Elowson AM, Snowdon CT, Lazaro-Perea C. Infant ‘babbling’ in a nonhuman primate: Complex vocal sequences with repeated call types. Behaviour. 1998;135:643–664. doi: 10.1163/156853998792897905. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gultekin YB, Hage SR. Limiting parental feedback disrupts vocal development in marmoset monkeys. Nat Commun. 2017;8 doi: 10.1038/ncomms14046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Snowdon CT. Learning from monkey “talk” Nonhuman primates are capable of several key elements of human language. Science. 2017;355:1120–1122. doi: 10.1126/science.aam7443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Koda H, et al. Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS One. 2013;8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.McCune, L., RougHellichius, L., Vihman, M. M., Delery, D. B. & Gogate, L. Grunt communication in human infants (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol110, 27–37, doi:10.1037//0735-7036.110.1.27 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 53.Lipkind D, et al. Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature. 2013;498:104–108. doi: 10.1038/nature12173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Liebal, K., Waller, B. M., Burrows, A. M. & Slocombe, K. E. Primate communication. A multimodal approach. (Cambridge University Press, 2014).
- 55.Ghazanfar AA. Multisensory vocal communication in primates and the evolution of rhythmic speech. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2013;67:1441–1448. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1491-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Call, J. & Tomasello, M. The gestural communication of apes and monkeys (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2007).
- 57.Kappeler, P. Verhaltensbiologie. (Springer Verlag, 2006).
- 58.Fitch, W. T. The evolution of language. (Cambridge University Press, 2010).
- 59.Tomasello M, et al. The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. J Hum Evol. 1985;14:175–186. doi: 10.1016/S0047-2484(85)80005-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Fröhlich M, et al. Unpeeling the layers of language: Bonobos and chimpanzees engage in cooperative turn-taking sequences. Sci Rep-Uk. 2016;6 doi: 10.1038/srep25887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Wrogemann D, Radespiel U, Zimmermann E. Comparison of reproductive characteristics and changes in body weight between captive populations of rufous and gray mouse lemurs. Int J Primatol. 2001;22:91–108. doi: 10.1023/A:1026418132281. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Audio and video files are stored in the data archive of the Institute of Zoology, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover. The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. A supplementary file includes all raw data used in this manuscript.