Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2017 Aug 22;114(35):E7216–E7217. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712175114

Reply to Fattorini et al.: Children’s selected avoidance of wild greenspace is driven by more than cultural factors

Kathryn L Hand a,1, Claire Freeman b, Philip J Seddon a, Mariano R Recio a, Yolanda van Heezik a
PMCID: PMC5584465  PMID: 28831011

We welcome the response by Fattorini et al. (1), which opens up discussion on the many possible factors influencing how children use their local environments. Fattorini et al. argue that our study (2) fails to test the biophilia hypothesis by leaving untested a possible cultural influence for children to prefer formal gardens over wild and natural spaces. We believe that our conclusion that children’s selection of habitats does not conform to the biophilia hypothesis is valid; children did not behave as predicted by this hypothesis, as they avoided the most biodiverse spaces, typically the more natural and wild environments. That current human culture prefers formal greenspace does not detract from this finding. Our view is that cultural influences are one of many factors that have diverted children from innate biophilic preferences.

Under the biophilia hypothesis, we would expect people to preferentially select to be in more biodiverse habitats, which can include both formal, man-made greenspaces, or natural habitats. Gardens, in particular, can be highly variable in the type and quality of nature experience they provide. In our study (2), we test a selection of three types of gardens, ranging from type 1 (high biodiversity value and often “informal” in structure) to type 3 (low biodiversity in comparison with many other formal and natural habitat types) (3). Despite this approach, children selected any garden type above all other habitats. Children’s selection of poor-quality gardens over many more biodiverse habitats available clearly does not conform to the biophilia hypothesis.

In our study (2), we discuss various factors that might influence the decisions children make about where to spend time outdoors. Many people indeed show a preference for more formal greenspace, which may in part be a cultural preference for reasons described by Fattorini et al. (1), although children may be more influenced by the familiarity and recreational opportunities provided by these spaces (4, 5). We emphasize the role of parental restrictions in removing children’s freedom to access wilder greenspace, instead encouraging them to stay close to home in private gardens. This parental and societal influence may itself be driven by many factors, including culture, threat perception (6), and barriers to movement because of urban design (7). This argument is supported by the finding in our study (2) and in other studies that high numbers of children are actively restricted from visiting many biodiverse greenspaces, and strong selection exists to remain close to home (8).

While we conclude that children’s exhibited behavior does not conform to the biophilia hypothesis, this does not mean children are not biophilic, but rather that their ability to act in this way has been curtailed. The many possible reasons for this, including cultural influences, are important topics for further research. We agree with Fattorini et al. (1) that helping children to overcome these influences and improving opportunities to interact with the biodiversity present in urban areas is important to support their well-being and develop a conservation ethic (9, 10).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Fattorini S, et al. Children’s preferences for less diverse greenspaces do not disprove biophilia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114(35):E7215. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1711505114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hand KL, et al. The importance of urban gardens in supporting children’s biophilia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114:274–279. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1609588114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hand KL, Freeman C, Seddon PJ, Stein A, van Heezik Y. A novel method for fine-scale biodiversity assessment and prediction across diverse urban landscapes reveals social deprivation-related inequalities in private, not public spaces. Landsc Urban Plan. 2016;151:33–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Castonguay G, Jutras S. Children’s appreciation of outdoor places in a poor neighborhood. J Environ Psychol. 2009;29:101–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Samborski S. Biodiverse or barren school grounds: Their effects on children. Child Youth Environ. 2010;20:67–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Veitch J, Bagley S, Ball K, Salmon J. Where do children usually play? A qualitative study of parents’ perceptions of influences on children’s active free-play. Health Place. 2006;12:383–393. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2005.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Carver A, Timperio A, Crawford D. Playing it safe: The influence of neighbourhood safety on children’s physical activity. A review. Health Place. 2008;14:217–227. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Min B, Lee J. Children’s neighborhood place as a psychological and behavioral domain. J Environ Psychol. 2006;26:51–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gill T. The benefits of children’s engagement with nature: A systematic literature review. Child Youth Environ. 2014;24:10–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wells NM, Lekies KS. Nature and the life course: Pathways from childhood nature experiences to adult environmentalism. Child Youth Environ. 2006;16:1–24. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES