Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Aug 11;6:e25819. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25819

Sexual dimorphism in striatal dopaminergic responses promotes monogamy in social songbirds

Kirill Tokarev 1,2,, Julia Hyland Bruno 1,3, Iva Ljubičić 1,4, Paresh J Kothari 2, Santosh A Helekar 5, Ofer Tchernichovski 1,3,4, Henning U Voss 2
Editor: Naoshige Uchida6
PMCID: PMC5584986  PMID: 28826502

Abstract

In many songbird species, males sing to attract females and repel rivals. How can gregarious, non-territorial songbirds such as zebra finches, where females have access to numerous males, sustain monogamy? We found that the dopaminergic reward circuitry of zebra finches can simultaneously promote social cohesion and breeding boundaries. Surprisingly, in unmated males but not in females, striatal dopamine neurotransmission was elevated after hearing songs. Behaviorally too, unmated males but not females persistently exchanged mild punishments in return for songs. Song reinforcement diminished when dopamine receptors were blocked. In females, we observed song reinforcement exclusively to the mate’s song, although their striatal dopamine neurotransmission was only slightly elevated. These findings suggest that song-triggered dopaminergic activation serves a dual function in social songbirds: as low-threshold social reinforcement in males and as ultra-selective sexual reinforcement in females. Co-evolution of sexually dimorphic reinforcement systems can explain the coexistence of gregariousness and monogamy.

Research organism: Other

eLife digest

While monogamy is rare within the animal kingdom, some species – including humans and many birds – can be highly social and yet sustain monogamous relationships. Zebra finches, for example, are among a number of species of songbirds in which numerous males and females live closely together but maintain monogamous partnerships. Male songbirds use their songs to attract females, who do not themselves sing. But if female birds are attracted to any male song, how do they manage to remain monogamous when surrounded by potential suitors?

In songbirds, and in humans too, a region of the brain called the striatum regulates both social and sexual behaviors. It does this by modulating the release of a molecule called dopamine, which is the brain’s reward signal. Tokarev et al. show that hearing songs triggers dopamine release within the striatum of unattached male zebra finches, but has no such effect in single females. Unattached male songbirds will also put up with irritating puffs of air in exchange for being able to watch videos of singing birds, whereas unattached females will not. Female songbirds with partners will tolerate the air puffs, but only if the videos are accompanied with the songs of their own mate.

These findings suggest that song serves as a generic social stimulus for zebra finch males, helping large numbers of birds to live together. By contrast, for a female zebra finch, the song of her partner is a highly selective sexual stimulus. These sex-specific responses to the same socially-relevant stimuli may explain how gregarious animals are able to maintain monogamous pair bonds. More generally, these results are a step towards understanding how brain reward systems regulate social interactions. Studying these mechanisms in songbird species with different social and mating systems could ultimately provide insights into social and sexual behavior in people.

Introduction

Many species of highly gregarious and colonial birds form long-term monogamous pairs (Goodson et al., 2012; Goodson and Kingsbury, 2011; Griffith et al., 2010; Zann, 1994). Pair bonding and flocking behaviors are regulated by neuropeptides and dopaminergic reward system (Goodson et al., 2012; Goodson and Kingsbury, 2011). However, for an animal to be highly social and at the same time monogamous, it must possess two distinct reinforcement systems: one with low selectivity for social stimuli to promote aggregation, and another highly selective for sexual stimuli to promote monogamy. But many communicative stimuli, including birdsong, may serve both social and sexual functions. In such cases, reinforcement may depend on stimulus context: for example, in many solitary songbird males, producing the same song may either attract females or repel rival males (Kroodsma and Byers, 1991; Slater, 2003). In social songbirds, however, many females and males live in close proximity, which gives females immediate access to numerous males whose songs may sexually attract them. What is it, then, that allows gregariousness and monogamy to coexist? We investigated this question in zebra finches, which are highly social, yet monogamous songbirds (Griffith et al., 2010; Zann, 1994). Male zebra finches produce a single stereotyped song that can be female-directed or undirected (Jarvis et al., 1998; Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991; Sossinka and Böhner, 1980; ten Cate, 1985; Woolley and Doupe, 2008). Males typically tolerate the singing behavior of their neighbors even when housed in crowed cages, although the song is occasionally used in an aggressive context too (Ihle et al., 2015). Female zebra finches are attracted to male songs (Holveck and Riebel, 2007), but do not sing (Nottebohm and Arnold, 1976).

The zebra finch striatal dopaminergic reward circuitry is activated in both social and sexual context (Banerjee et al., 2013; Ihle et al., 2015; Iwasaki et al., 2014; Sasaki et al., 2006). In general, there are more dopamine-producing neurons in social than in territorial songbirds (Goodson et al., 2009). In zebra finches, gregariousness is correlated with the level of activity in dopaminergic neurons (Kelly and Goodson, 2015). Striatal dopamine increases in social situations, e.g., when adult males interact with females (Ihle et al., 2015; Sasaki et al., 2006), or juvenile males with adult male tutors, and importantly, even without singing in either of these contexts (Ihle et al., 2015). During pair formation striatal dopamine levels increase in both sexes (Banerjee et al., 2013; Iwasaki et al., 2014). In the context of song learning, striatal dopaminergic input is modulated during singing (Gadagkar et al., 2016; Hoffmann et al., 2016; Simonyan et al., 2012). However, although song is an important sexual stimulus in songbirds (Kroodsma and Byers, 1991; Slater, 2003), there is no direct evidence that hearing songs may affect striatal dopamine in either sexual or affiliative (Hausberger et al., 1995) context. Here we performed in vivo imaging and behavioral experiments that show the forebrain dopaminergic system response to song stimulation in zebra finches across sexes and breeding states, in order to distinguish between social and sexual components of song reinforcement in social songbirds.

We developed two complementary experimental approaches. First, we used a delayed positron emission tomography (PET) procedure (Patel et al., 2008) in order to measure dopamine neurotransmission (Laruelle, 2000) in awake and unrestrained birds. Zebra finches were injected with [11C]raclopride radiotracer, which binds to dopamine type 2 (D2) receptors. Instead of acquiring PET immediately, we first stimulated them with song playbacks for 20 min while awake and behaving and scanned them just after the stimulation under general anesthesia (delayed PET, Figure 1, see protocol in Materials and methods). Second, we developed an apparatus for assessing song reinforcement behaviorally. This approach is a variant on drug addiction experiments, which typically measure how much rodents are willing to work, or exchange mild punishment, in return for access to dopaminergic stimulants such as cocaine (Shaham et al., 2000) (Figure 2). We used a song stimulus instead of the drug and measured the extent to which birds were willing to receive mildly aversive air puffs (Tokarev and Tchernichovski, 2014) in exchange for hearing song playbacks. Finally, in order to test for causality between dopamine neurotransmission and song reinforcement behavior, we blocked dopamine neurotransmission with a selective antagonist of D2 receptors L-741,626 (Li et al., 2010; Watson et al., 2012). We used PET to determine the localization of dopaminergic blockage, and then tested behaviorally if blocking of dopamine D2 receptors was sufficient to diminish reinforcing effect of songs.

Figure 1. Delayed PET of dopamine neurotransmission in response to song stimuli.

Figure 1.

Adult zebra finches were injected with the D2 receptor tracer [11C]raclopride. Immediately after the injection, birds were either kept for 20 min in quiet conditions or exposed to novel conspecific songs. Each bird was tested in both conditions. PET scan was performed immediately afterwards, in groups of four.

Figure 2. Song reinforcement assay.

Figure 2.

An apparatus for testing the amount of aversive air puffs birds were willing to receive in exchange for hearing songs. Birds voluntarily perched next to a window through which they could see a video of a singing bird. Videos were presented either silently (control) or accompanied with song playbacks. When the infrared beam detected the bird perching next to the window, aversive air puffs were delivered in random (unpredictable) intervals (with a likelihood of 12.5% s).

Results

We first tested if our delayed PET technique could detect changes in striatal dopamine neurotransmission after hearing song playbacks. We scanned eight unmated female zebra finches, where we expected to find higher levels of dopamine neurotransmission after song playbacks (i.e., lower levels of [11C]raclopride binding), and eight unmated males, where we expected to find a weaker effect, if any. Each bird was scanned twice: after stimulation with a variety of unfamiliar songs (both female-directed and undirected) over 20 min, and after silence over the same duration (Figure 1). As expected from the distribution of dopamine receptors in the songbird brain (Kubikova et al., 2010), the averaged PET map showed that the striatum was the major site of [11C]raclopride binding in both conditions in males (Figure 3a) and in females (Figure 3b). However, against our expectations, lower level of [11C]raclopride binding after hearing songs (suggesting increased striatal dopamine neurotransmission) was detected only in the male group. In males, the song minus silence parametric difference map showed that song stimulation resulted in significantly lower level of [11C]raclopride binding in a part of the striatum (Figure 3c; cluster-level pcorrected = 0.024, paired t-test corrected for multiple comparisons). Exploratory analysis of individual changes (within the cluster of significant change) showed that [11C]raclopride binding was at lower levels in all males after hearing songs by 29 ± 8% (mean ± s.e.m. hereafter; Figure 3d; p=0.015, pair-wise t-test). These results, based on PET of D2 receptors, are comparable to the 26.5 ± 8.4% increase in dopamine detected with microdialysis in a study where male zebra finches were presented with females (Ihle et al., 2015), confirming that [11C]raclopride binding at D2 receptors is a robust indicator of the overall striatal dopamine neurotransmission.

Figure 3. Dopamine neurotransmission in response to song stimuli in unmated males and females.

Brain schemas in (a–c) show: cerebellum (Cb), auditory field L (L2), striatum (St), and song control nuclei Area X (X) and lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (LMAN). Section planes are shown as dashed orange lines. (a & b) Bright yellow areas represent the Statistical Parametric Map (SPM, intensity threshold at t ≥ 2) for averaged [11C]raclopride binding potential in males (a) and females (b) (n = 8 in both groups). SPM is shown over the brain template magnetic-resonance image. In both males and females [11C]raclopride binding was restricted to the striatum. (c) SPM of the difference in dopamine neurotransmission as detected by [11C]raclopride binding in song and silence conditions in males. SPM reveals significantly lower level of [11C]raclopride binding in response to hearing novel conspecific songs in males (pair-wise t statistic, cluster-level pcorrected = 0.024), which indicates higher dopamine neurotransmission in this condition. Significant difference was detected in one cluster within the dorsal striatum, mostly outside Area X. (d) Analysis of individual changes in [11C]raclopride binding in males, comparing song vs. silence. (e) Same for females. As no significant cluster was found in females, males’ cluster was used as a mask to produce individual values of [11C]raclopride binding within the same area.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Change in dopamine neurotransmission after song playbacks in males and females.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Comparison of [11C]raclopride binding change (song-silence) values between males and females confirmed significant difference in dopamine neurotransmission between sexes further (p=0.015, t-test). Box-and-whisker plot shows median values, first and third quartiles (box), lowest and highest data within 1.5 interquartile range (whiskers), and an outlier (dot) in the male group.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Body and head movements during song playbacks or silence in males and females.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Individual data of whole body (top) or head (bottom) movements in males and females (n = 8; see Table 1 and 2 for statistics). Average Euclidian distance every 0.3 s was measured in the video recordings.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Statistic parametric map (SPM) of differential striatal [11C]raclopride binding in male zebra finches at increased threshold.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

The cluster of voxels with significantly lower level of [11C]raclopride binding in males in response to hearing novel conspecific song is shown here at an increased threshold of significance compared to Figure 3 (t ≥ 5, pair-wise t statistic). The image is centered around the peak value of differential binding, dashed light-blue lines show section planes. SPM is laid over magnetic-resonance image of zebra finch brain template. Song system nuclei (shades of blue) were identified using the 3D atlas of the zebra finch brain (Poirier et al., 2008). Area X (X), the striatal component of the song system, did not overlap with the cluster of significant differential binding at this increased threshold, while the peak value appeared in the medial striatum. A frontal (left), a sagittal (middle) and a transverse (right) section are shown.

Surprisingly, females lacked any brain areas with significant change in [11C]raclopride binding in response to song playbacks. Nevertheless, we produced a mask image from the cluster of significant change in males (Figure 3c) and used it as a volume of interest to assess for a possible effect in females. Exploratory analysis of individual changes in females showed no apparent change in striatal [11C]raclopride binding in response to song playbacks (Figure 3e; 0.4 ± 6%, p=0.737, pair-wise t-test). A direct comparison between males and females showed statistically significant differences in striatal [11C]raclopride binding after hearing songs (Figure 3—figure supplement 1; p=0.015, t-test). Note, however, that the difference in the magnitude of change between males and females is, at least partially, driven by the low baseline (silence) [11C]raclopride binding in females (Figure 3e).

The sexually dimorphic striatal response to songs could reflect behavioral or anatomical differences between sexes not related to reinforcement. First, as striatal dopamine neurotransmission correlates with movement (Cousins and Salamone, 1996; Gadagkar et al., 2016; Howe and Dombeck, 2016), we tested if birds tended to move more when hearing song playbacks, in a manner that could explain our results. We analyzed movement in eight males and eight females, in similar conditions to those in our experiments before PET scan: injection of raclopride followed by 20 min of silence or song playbacks. We observed very little of such body movements as flying, hopping and wing-whirring, and also quantitatively tracked the whole body movement (analyzed every 0.3 s for the center of body mass), but there were no significant differences between conditions or sexes (Figure 3—figure supplement 2; Table 1). Tracking head movement, we observed a significant trend to move the head more during song playbacks in most birds (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). However, there was no significant difference between males and females in this respect (Table 2). Therefore, mere movement is unlikely to explain our finding of male-specific dopamine response to songs.

Table 1. Results of statistical tests to address the differences in body movement in zebra finch males and females in different conditions: in silence or during conspecific song playbacks.

Average Euclidian distance every 0.3 s was measured in the videos for the center of body mass. Bold-face numbers indicate significance levels p≤0.05.

Box's Test of Equality of Covariance Matrices Box's M F df1 df2 p-value
13.334 3.756 3 35280 0.01
Multivariate Tests (Pillai’s Trace) value F p-value
body movement 0.175 2.968 0.107
body movement * sex 0.02 0.21 0.886
Tests of Between-Subjects Effects df F p-value
sex 1 0.249 0.626

Table 2. Results of statistical tests to address the differences in head movement in zebra finch males and females in different conditions: in silence or during conspecific song playbacks.

Average Euclidian distance every 0.3 s was measured in the videos for the position of the beak. Bold-face numbers indicate significance levels p≤0.05.

Box's Test of Equality of Covariance Matrices Box's M F df1 df2 p-value
4.004 1.128 3 35280 0.336
Multivariate tests (Pillai’s Trace) Value F p-value
head movement 0.348 7.468 0.016
head movement * sex 0.016 0.225 0.643
Tests of Between-Subjects Effects df F p-value
sex 1 0.598 0.454

Another concern is that our results could simply reflect anatomical dimorphism in the basal ganglia pathway of the premotor song system: in particular, Area X, which has high density of dopamine D2 receptors (Kubikova et al., 2010) and receives dopamine during female-directed singing (Sasaki et al., 2006), exists only in zebra finch males. However, Area X was mostly excluded from the cluster of significant change (Figure 3c and Figure 3—figure supplement 3), suggesting that its contribution was small, if any. This is in line with the finding that Area X does not respond to auditory stimulation in awake songbirds, except for error signals during singing (Gadagkar et al., 2016).

Given that the expectation of reward is only one of several scenarios that could explain the unanticipated pattern of striatal dopamine neurotransmission that we observed (Cousins and Salamone, 1996; Gadagkar et al., 2016; Hoffmann et al., 2016; Howe and Dombeck, 2016; Kubikova and Kostál, 2010; Riters, 2011; Salimpoor et al., 2011; Schultz, 2002; Stuber et al., 2008), we developed an independent method for assessing the effect of song reinforcement in male and female zebra finches. In order to directly estimate song reinforcement we paired the song stimulus with a mild punishment. We presented the same birds that had been scanned earlier for dopamine with video of a perching male (Figure 2). Each bird was presented with two daily sessions of videos over ten days (20 sessions, 20 min each). In ten sessions the video was played in silence, and in the alternating ten sessions, it was accompanied by song playbacks (the same mix of initially unfamiliar songs as in the PET experiments). When a bird perched next to the window facing the video display, it would occasionally receive a mildly aversive air puff, in random intervals and without warning. We assessed reinforcement by measuring the number of air puffs the bird was willing to tolerate in return for the stimulus, comparing the silent sessions to the song playback sessions.

We found that males voluntarily received many more air puffs during song playback sessions compared to silent sessions (Figure 4; p=0.001, paired t-test); they appeared attentive during the sessions but did not show any aggressive behavior. Females, on the other hand, showed little motivation to hear song playbacks: their tendencies to receive air-puffs were moderate and did not differ significantly across song playback and silent sessions (Figure 4; p=0.267, paired t-test).

Figure 4. Song reinforcement in unmated males and females.

Figure 4.

Rate of air puffs (per hour) birds received during song playback and silent sessions: in males (left) and females (right) (n = 8 and n = 4, respectively; p-values for paired t-test shown).

To test whether the song reinforcement we observed in males was dependent on dopamine neurotransmission, we used the D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 to interfere with D2 receptors. First, we performed a whole brain PET after injections of L-741,626 in order to determine the localization of dopaminergic blockage: as expected, changes in [11C]raclopride binding were observed exclusively in the striatum (Figure 5). We found substantially lower levels of the striatal binding of [11C]raclopride after L-741,626 injection compared to saline (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Therefore, L-741,626 blocks D2 receptors in the songbird striatum as it does in rodents (Li et al., 2010; Watson et al., 2012) and primates (Achat-Mendes et al., 2010). We then tested song reinforcement in four males with our air-puff apparatus as described before, but after injections of either L-741,626 or saline on alternate sessions. On the days of L-741,626 injections, the animals were still active and approached the video, but stimulation with song playbacks no longer increased the number of air puffs they were willing to receive, while on the alternate days of saline injections, song reinforcement was similar to that of untreated males (Figure 6; see Table 3 for statistics).

Figure 5. Blockage of D2 receptor binding activity by L-741,626.

Statistical parametric map of average [11C]raclopride binding is shown over the zebra finch brain template magnetic-resonance image: after saline injection (top) and L-741,626 injection (bottom) (n = 2 in both conditions; t-values on the insert). Sagittal (left), frontal (middle) and transverse (right) sections are shown; dashed light-blue lines show section planes.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. L-741,626 activity at the striatal D2 receptors in zebra finches.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Comparison of the individual values of [11C]raclopride binding potential in the striatum in two males after L-741,626 injection and two males after sham injection (saline) demonstrates high levels of L-741,626 binding to D2 receptors in the striatum.

Figure 6. Song reinforcement after dopamine receptor blockage.

Figure 6.

Number of air puffs (per hour) birds received during silent and song playback sessions: after saline injection (left); after L-741,626 injection (right) (n = 4; significant p-values are shown for general linear model for repeated measurements; see Table 3 for statistics).

Table 3. Results of statistical tests to address the differences in tolerance to air puffs in zebra finch males in different conditions: in silence or during conspecific song playbacks after saline injections, or same after injection of dopamine receptor antagonist L-741,626.

Bold-face numbers indicate significance levels p≤0.05.

Mauchly's Test of Sphericity Mauchly's W df χ² p-value
# air puffs/h 0.022 5 6.604 0.318
Tests of Within-Subjects effects df F p-value
# air puffs/h 3 7.96 0.007
pair-wise post-hoc LSD tests p-value
song + saline vs silence + saline 0.041
song + saline vs silence + L-741,626 0.015
song + saline vs song + L-741,626 0.023
silence + saline vs silence + L-741,626 0.814
silence + saline vs song + L-741,626 0.394
song + L-741,626 vs silence + L-741,626 0.122

How is it that song stimuli are reinforcing in unmated males but not in unmated females? We hypothesized that the non-selective dopamine neurotransmission by unfamiliar songs in males might reflect a social function, but in females, song reinforcement might be exclusively sexually driven, as a part of the mate choice (Riebel, 2009). A possible explanation to those counterintuitive results is that reinforcement could be much more selective in females. We therefore measured song reinforcement in six mated females that were ready to breed (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). We compared song reinforcement in three conditions: video accompanied with the songs of their mates, video accompanied with songs of other, unfamiliar mated males, and video alone. The mated females showed little interest in the videos and minimal motivation to tolerate air puffs in return to hearing non-mate songs. However, they were willing to receive many air puffs in return for hearing their mates’ songs (Figure 7; see Table 4 for statistics).

Figure 7. Song reinforcement in mated females.

Number of air puffs (per hour) mated females received in exchange for silence, non-mate song (from male mated with another female), and mate’s song (n = 4; significant p-values are shown for general linear model for repeated measurements; see Table 4 for statistics).

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Experimental procedures for measuring dopamine neurotransmission in female zebra finches in response to their mates’ songs.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

Each adult female zebra finch was paired for at least several months with a male zebra finch, with whom they produced ≥ 4 clutches of offspring, throughout synchronized breeding cycles (one pair shown, but four females were scanned at a time). In the first week after their offspring hatched, the females were kept together with their mates; then they were moved together with offspring to the nursery room, in the absence of adult males. In a regular cycle, the females were returned to their mates at 30 days after offspring’s hatching. For the experiment, we used females shortly before their return to the mates. The PET procedure was similar to the one shown in Figure 1, with the exception that here the animals were exposed to song recordings of either their mates or unfamiliar males.

Table 4. Results of statistical tests to address the differences in tolerance to air puffs in mated zebra finch females in different conditions: in silence and during playbacks of songs of unfamiliar males or their mates.

Bold-face numbers indicate significance levels p≤0.05.

Mauchly's Test of Sphericity Mauchly's W df χ² p-value
# air puffs/h 0.484 2 1.453 0.484
Tests of Within-Subjects Effects df F p-value
# air puffs/h 2 13.139 0.006
pair-wise post-hoc LSD tests p-value
mate’s song vs silence 0.021
mate’s song vs non-mate song 0.049
non-mate song vs silence 0.259

Based on these behavioral results, we tested if the pattern of striatal dopamine neurotransmission would be also mate-selective in these females. Using delayed PET, we compared two sets of stimuli: playbacks of mates’ songs versus playbacks of songs produced by other mated males (in both conditions we played a mix of both female-directed and undirected songs). We detected a cluster of voxels with lower [11C]raclopride binding in response to mate song in a small part of the medial dorsal striatum (Figure 8a,b); however, the difference across those voxels did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (Figure 8b). An exploratory post-hoc analysis of individual differences in the same area found that [11C]raclopride binding was 12 ± 4% lower in response to mate song compared to non-mate song (Figure 8c; p=0.042, paired t-test). These differences suggested a weak trend for higher levels of dopamine transmission in response to mates’ songs in females, but this borderline effect should be treated with caution and validated in future studies.

Figure 8. Dopamine neurotransmission in response to song stimuli in mated females.

Figure 8.

(a) Brain schemas as in Figure 3a,b. Statistical parametric map (SPM, intensity threshold at t ≥ 2) for average [11C]raclopride binding is shown over the zebra finch brain template magnetic-resonance image. (b) SPM of the difference in [11C]raclopride binding in response to non-mate song and mate’s song in mated females (n = 6; pair-wise t statistic, p<0.05). This cluster, however, did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (pcorrected = 0.6, paired t-test corrected for multiple comparisons). (c) Individual changes in [11C]raclopride binding in this insignificant cluster in mated females, non-mate song vs. mate’s song. Supplementary information.

Discussion

We found in the zebra finch an unanticipated pattern of sexual dimorphism in dopaminergic responses to song. In males, stimulation with songs resulted in higher levels of striatal dopamine neurotransmission compared to silence condition. Behaviorally too, unfamiliar song playbacks were strongly reinforcing in males. Blocking striatal dopamine D2 receptors extinguished song reinforcement, suggesting involvement of the striatal dopaminergic reward system. In unmated females, hearing songs did not affect dopamine neurotransmission, and playbacks were not reinforcing behaviorally either. In mated females, mate song was strongly reinforcing, with high specificity, but we observed only slightly higher levels of dopamine neurotransmission in response to mate song compared to non-mate song. Thus, in males, both striatal dopamine neurotransmission and behavioral responses to song playbacks indicate low-threshold and non-specific positive reinforcement. This is consistent with a social, perhaps affiliative function of birdsong to promote aggregation (Hausberger et al., 1995). In females, both behavioral and dopaminergic responses to song were high-threshold and mate-selective, consistent with a sexual function to promote monogamy. However, even though behaviorally mated females showed strong reinforcement to mate song, their striatal dopaminergic responses to mate song were barely detectable. This discrepancy will require further assessment in future studies. Note that there are several open questions about the receptor mechanisms that could account for the sexual dimorphism we observed, including different receptors expression levels, different densities of dopaminergic cells, different reuptake mechanisms and different ratios of D1/D2 receptors. For example, it should be tested whether D1 receptors, which are known to be important for reinforcement (Robbins and Everitt, 1996), are also crucial in the reward mechanism of song in zebra finches.

A simple evolutionary scenario can explain the pattern of sexual dimorphism we observed. Territorial songbird males respond aggressively to intruders and are easy to irritate with conspecific song playbacks (Kroodsma and Byers, 1991; Slater, 2003). Females may show strong preference to certain male song features but are generally attracted to conspecific songs (Kroodsma and Byers, 1991; Slater, 2003). Monogamy could be sustained during an evolutionary transition from the territorial to gregarious behavior if male evolved high tolerance to song while female simultaneously co-evolved highly selective reinforcement threshold to songs. Our results are consistent with such a scenario. Future studies could test this hypothesis further by systematic examination of sexual dimorphism across territorial and social species of songbirds, and in species where both sexes sing. We would expect to see a lack of song reinforcement in non-social territorial songbirds, at least outside the breeding period. But possibly, aggressive reaction might also increase brain dopamine, and one should try to carefully dissect such effects. For example, it was shown that fighting cocks (Thompson, 1964) and Siamese fighting fish (Thompson, 1963) may perceive seeing a potential opponent as a reinforcing stimulus; so, either they may look forward to the fight, or it is an anticipation of reward after winning the fight. In Siamese fighting fish, it was shown that dominant males are more likely to use such stimuli than subordinate (Baenninger, 1970). Avian species demonstrate a wide range of social structures, so the reinforcement value of social clues may vary greatly among them. In sum, a sexually dimorphic activation of the dopaminergic reward circuitry that we observed in our study could provide a joint mechanism for aggregation and pair-bonding, two seemingly conflicting characteristics of the social structure of zebra finches and other gregarious yet monogamous species.

Materials and methods

Experimental design

This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the US National Institutes of Health and was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Hunter College of the City University of New York (protocol 'OT imaging 10/18–01') and Weill Cornell Medical College (protocol #2010–0003).

Eleven adult male and seventeen adult female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) bred at Hunter College (room temperature 19–24˚C, 12:12 hr light/dark cycle) were used in the neuroimaging experiments. The animals were raised by both parents until adulthood and spent their life, except for the time of experiments, in the colony room with possibility to engage in social interactions with other zebra finches. All males and nine of the females were non-mated, eight other females were mated in breeding pairs.

The concept of our work was similar to a human study, where favorite musical pieces were shown to increase striatal dopamine levels (Salimpoor et al., 2011), but we employed a modification in PET protocol that allowed to obtain measurements that reflected changes in dopamine release in awake songbirds. Before imaging, the non-mated animals were injected [11C]raclopride and then either exposed to recorded songs of unfamiliar male zebra finches or kept in quiet conditions for 20 min (Figure 1). This time interval was chosen according to the 11C half-life of 20 min and its detectability with the current PET technique. PET and anatomical X-ray computed tomography (CT) images were acquired immediately afterwards using an Inveon Research Workplace (Siemens). Delayed PET scans for dopamine are well established in several animal species (Marzluff et al., 2012; Patel et al., 2008), but since this is a novel method for measuring striatal responses to birdsong, we describe it in detail as a protocol in the next section.

Eight mated female zebra finches were tested in a similar experiment, but with songs of either their own mate or another mated male; they were also synchronized in their breeding cycle so that during stimulation and PET they would be in similar hormonal states (Figure 7—figure supplement 1. The females were kept together with their mates for the first week after hatching of the offspring but then were moved (together with offspring) to the nursery room in the absence of adult males until post-hatch day 30, after which they would reunite with their mates. This cycle is routinely performed in the laboratory to produce juvenile zebra finches not exposed to adult male song, which we use in other studies. For this experiment, we used females that had gone through this cycle several times, and stimulation/scanning took place shortly before their return to the mates (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Scanning procedures were the same as in the previous experiment and are described in more detail in the next section.

Eight of the males, four unmated females and four of the mated females were also tested in a behavioral paradigm for preferences to the auditory stimuli that had been used in the PET experiments (Figure 2). We modified our socially-reinforced auditory discrimination paradigm (Tokarev and Tchernichovski, 2014), so that after a period of isolation the zebra finches were attracted to a video of a male (Ljubičić et al., 2016). The video was played either in silence (20 min) or with the same auditory stimuli as in the PET experiments: a mix of songs of unfamiliar male zebra finches for the males and unmated females, and songs of unfamiliar males or mates for the mated females (20 min). The order of auditory accompaniment (silence/songs) in each session was random; each animal was tested in 10 sessions. In order to see the video and be closer to source of auditory stimulation, the animals had to sit on a perch that produced air puff in a random manner controlled by Bird Puffer software (http://soundanalysispro.com/bird-puffer). We previously determined that random air puffs with a probability of ~2/minute are well tolerated by the birds. Our software automatically registered the bird’s perching activity, delivered the air puffs, and kept continuous records of air puffs that each bird received. We then analyzed during which stimulation the animals were willing to receive more air puffs.

We also tested whether the movement might account for observed differences in striatal dopamine release. If dopamine level changes were due to movement, then movement should differ across treatments: higher in zebra finch males but not females when hearing songs compared to when they are kept in silence. To test if this were the case, we performed an additional control experiment with a new group of 8 males and eight females, where we simulated the song vs. silence pre-PET conditions (including transfer to the same room and raclopride injection), and also video tracked birds’ movement. We monitored for such body movements as flying, hopping and wing-whirring, as well as quantitatively analyzed videos for Euclidian distances every 0.3 s for the center of body mass and beak to continuously track changes in position of body and head, respectively.

Injections of L-741,626

To detect whether dopamine neurotransmission was necessary for the observed behavioral effects in males, four of them were injected with L-741,626 (Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO, USA), a very selective antagonist of D2-receptors, which had been used to study the function of D2-receptors in rodents (Dai et al., 2016; Li et al., 2010; Watson et al., 2012) and primates (Achat-Mendes et al., 2010). We injected L-741,626 intraperitoneally at 3.33 µg/g body weight, within the range described for rodents (Li et al., 2010; Watson et al., 2012), diluted in saline (acetic acid was added to increase solubility at first, then pH was neutralized by caustic soda solution). The L-741,626 injections were administered 30 min before each test with at least 48 hr between treatments, 5 times for each animal, with an intra-individual control of sham injections (saline) of the same volume.

Simultaneous PET on four zebra finches to measure dopamine released during auditory stimulation in awake unrestrained state

We established a minimally invasive method for in vivo imaging in zebra finches to measure dopamine neurotransmission in four awake unrestrained animals simultaneously; these measurements may be taken multiple times allowing for intra-subject comparisons (Figure 1). Due to their small size compared to the available imaging volume of our micro-PET, we were able to scan four birds simultaneously. Thus, the experiments were done in tetrads, with two animals in one condition, and two animals in another, and then the conditions were reversed for them in the subsequent PET scan. [11C]raclopride was delivered via intravenous (i.v.; ulnar vein) or intraperitoneal (i.p.) bolus injections that lasted around 1 min or less; radioactivity doses were ~300 µCi or less, in solutions of 150 µl for i.p. injections and 100 µl for i.v. injections with [11C]raclopride mass at ~0.3 nmol/g (body weight). Usage of [11C]raclopride to track changes in dopamine levels has been validated in studies with simultaneous microdialysis (Morris et al., 2008; Normandin et al., 2012).

When dopamine is released, decrease in radioactive [11C]raclopride signal is mediated through direct competition between these two molecules for D2 receptors (Fisher et al., 1995) and as a result of D2 receptors switching from low to high affinity for dopamine but not raclopride (Fisher et al., 1995; Seeman et al., 1994); also, the striatal [11C]raclopride signal does not rebound after its decline once dopamine is released (Endres et al., 1997). Therefore, differences in dopamine neurotransmission between zebra finches exposed to song playbacks and silence observed in our work were likely due to experimental conditions, even though imaging was performed after stimulation (Yoder et al., 2008). This method of delayed PET (aka ‘awake uptake’) was first used to detect changes in dopamine levels in freely moving rats (Patel et al., 2008). A similar protocol was also used in songbirds (crows), but with [18F]−2-fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose to detect general brain activation in response to visual stimuli (Marzluff et al., 2012).

The animals were let to recover after handling for 1–2 min and then were kept individually either in quiet conditions (20 min) or were presented with recordings of various zebra finch songs (one novel song every 15 s during 20 min), thus providing stimulation almost immediately after radioligand injection, similarly to previous studies (Marzluff et al., 2012; Patel et al., 2008). Food and water were provided ad libitum. None of the birds sang or attempted to sing during the 20 min of the experiment (in all conditions). Their behavioral activity was at minimum during the experiment with no drinking or feeding, and only occasional perching. This suggested that the difference in experimental conditions (song playbacks or silence) would be the sole factor in possible differences in dopamine neurotransmission. Immediately after the experiment, the animals were sedated ~2 min under 3% isoflurane in O2, 2 L/min, and transferred into a custom-made plexiglass chamber with 4 head holders made from vinyl tubes; their bodies were additionally fixed with a surgical tape to reduce spontaneous movements during scanning. Animal placement (2 in radial, 2 in axial direction; heads facing towards the center of the imaging volume) was chosen to maximize image quality (Siepel et al., 2010). The chamber was then placed in the micro-PET scanner, and anesthesia was reduced to 2% isoflurane. Acquisition of the radioactive signal lasted 60 min and was followed by an anatomical CT scan of 10 min duration. Differences in radioactive signal acquired during the PET scan were expected to reflect dopamine release during auditory stimulation, as after [11C]raclopride is displaced by dopamine its level does not rebound within this time frame despite clearance of dopamine and even with continuous infusion of [11C]raclopride (Endres et al., 1997), while we performed single bolus injection. We were able to inject a sufficient amount of radiotracer to obtain images of [11C]raclopride uptake, and all animals recovered quickly after the scan. We established that both i.v. and i.p. injections of [11C]raclopride produced a radioactive signal in striatum that was detectable by micro-PET, and the data from birds after i.v. and i.p. injections of [11C]raclopride overlapped and therefore were combined. Thus, both injection methods appeared to be effective for detection of dopamine level changes. We recommend i.p. injections for future research, as they are faster and easier to perform, require less handling and thus are less stressful for animals (and experimenters).

We also performed an additional PET scan on four males that had been tested with the D2 receptor antagonist, L-741,626, to confirm that it blocked binding at the receptor. Two of them were injected L-741,626 solution and two others saline 30 min before [11C]raclopride injection. The rest of the procedure was the same.

Radiochemistry

The radiotracer [11C]raclopride was synthesized on-site immediately before each experiment at the Citigroup Biomedical Imaging Center, Weill Cornell Medical College, following standard procedures (Broft et al., 2015; Mawlawi et al., 2001). The average specific activity of [11C]raclopride was 6046 mCi/μmol. [11C]raclopride was isolated and formulated into an isotonic solution containing 5–7% ethanol, with concentration of 0.13 µg/mL. Although alcohol could potentially influence behavioral state of the animal, the amount injected in our experiments (~0.3 g/kg) was substantially lower than that causing an intoxicated stupor in a previous study (2–3 g/kg) (Olson et al., 2014) and importantly was similar across all experimental conditions.

PET image preparation and statistical analysis

PET imaging data were first processed in PMOD software (http://www.pmod.com). As four animals were scanned simultaneously at each experiment, raw images were separated into four zones around each brain and cropped accordingly in PMOD software. PET data were summed across 6 evenly distributed time points for each scan. Further, PET data were processed and analyzed in SPM12 software (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm).

Anatomical CT images were transformed into standardized stereotaxic space and aligned with a 3D magnetic resonance imaging atlas of the zebra finch brain, which also references common brain areas (Poirier et al., 2008). All PET images were corrected for volume-to-volume motion by inter-frame realignment and then co-registered to the subject's anatomical CT image. All alignment transformations were visually inspected to ensure that there was no mismatch with the template brain image. Datasets of three males, one unmated and two mated females were discarded because of difficulties with alignment of the images due to motion during scans. Data from the remaining 22 animals were analyzed further.

[11C]raclopride binding potential for dopamine D2 receptors in each voxel was calculated using a simplified reference region method (Gunn et al., 1997; Lammertsma et al., 1996; Patel et al., 2008), with the cerebellum as the reference region, since it does not contain detectable D2 receptors and is traditionally used for determination of nonspecific binding and free radiotracer in the brain (Lammertsma et al., 1996; Litton et al., 1994): (CSt–CCb)/CCb, where CSt is radioactivity concentration in striatal (St) voxels (or anywhere else outside the reference region), and CCb is averaged radioactivity concentration in cerebellum (Cb). Therefore, [11C]raclopride binding potential was represented by a striatal-cerebellar ratio (SCR) of radioactive concentrations (Patel et al., 2008). As [11C]raclopride and dopamine compete for D2-receptors, decrease in [11C]raclopride binding potential indicates an increase of dopamine concentration (Endres et al., 1997; Fisher et al., 1995) and thus reflects increased dopamine neurotransmission (Laruelle, 2000; Martinez et al., 2003). Statistical parametric maps of [11C]raclopride binding potential change were produced by comparing the parametric SCR maps of the two scan sessions (song playbacks and quiet condition, or mate’s and unfamiliar songs); comparisons between two conditions were performed with paired t-tests, with two-tailed probability value of p<0.05 chosen as statistically significant (Urban et al., 2012). Clusters of significant change were identified in xjView (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview) at p<0.05; p values corrected for multiple comparisons were calculated for each cluster of contiguous voxels at a t threshold of 3.56 within a search volume equal to the whole brain and an effective spatial resolution of 1.4 mm full-width at half maximum (FWHM) (Salimpoor et al., 2011). Mean binding potential values were extracted from the significant cluster for each individual, and the normalized percent change in dopamine level was calculated as Δ = (SCRsilence–SCRsong)×100/SCRsilence.

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by National Science Foundation (grants #1261872, 0956306 and 1065678) and National Institutes of Health (grant # DC04722-17). We thank Michael Synan and Yeona Kang for help with PET image processing.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Kirill Tokarev, Email: kt66@hunter.cuny.edu.

Naoshige Uchida, Harvard University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Science Foundation 1261872 to Kirill Tokarev, Ofer Tchernichovski.

  • National Science Foundation 0956306 to Henning U Voss.

  • National Science Foundation 1065678 to Santosh A Helekar.

  • National Institutes of Health DC04722-17 to Kirill Tokarev, Ofer Tchernichovski.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Software, Validation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Validation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the US National Institutes of Health and was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Hunter College of the City University of New York (protocol 'OT imaging 10/18-01') and Weill Cornell Medical College (protocol #2010-0003).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25819.020

References

  1. Achat-Mendes C, Grundt P, Cao J, Platt DM, Newman AH, Spealman RD. Dopamine D3 and D2 receptor mechanisms in the abuse-related behavioral effects of cocaine: studies with preferential antagonists in squirrel monkeys. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 2010;334:556–565. doi: 10.1124/jpet.110.167619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baenninger R. Visual reinforcement, habituation, and prior social experience of Siamese fighting fish. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1970;71:1–5. doi: 10.1037/h0028975. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Banerjee SB, Dias BG, Crews D, Adkins-Regan E. Newly paired zebra finches have higher dopamine levels and immediate early gene Fos expression in dopaminergic neurons. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;38:3731–3739. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Broft A, Slifstein M, Osborne J, Kothari P, Morim S, Shingleton R, Kenney L, Vallabhajosula S, Attia E, Martinez D, Timothy Walsh B. Striatal dopamine type 2 receptor availability in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2015;233:380–387. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cate CT, ten Cate C. Directed song of male zebra finches as a predictor of subsequent intra- and interspecific social behaviour and pair formation. Behavioural Processes. 1985;10:369–374. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(85)90037-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cousins MS, Salamone JD. Involvement of ventrolateral striatal dopamine in movement initiation and execution: a microdialysis and behavioral investigation. Neuroscience. 1996;70:849–859. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00407-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dai WL, Xiong F, Yan B, Cao ZY, Liu WT, Liu JH, Yu BY. Blockade of neuronal dopamine D2 receptor attenuates morphine tolerance in mice spinal cord. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:38746. doi: 10.1038/srep38746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Endres CJ, Kolachana BS, Saunders RC, Su T, Weinberger D, Breier A, Eckelman WC, Carson RE. Kinetic modeling of [11C]raclopride: combined PET-microdialysis studies. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 1997;17:932–942. doi: 10.1097/00004647-199709000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Fisher RE, Morris ED, Alpert NM, Fischman AJ. In vivo imaging of neuromodulatory synaptic transmission using PET: A review of relevant neurophysiology. Human Brain Mapping. 1995;3:24–34. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460030104. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gadagkar V, Puzerey PA, Chen R, Baird-Daniel E, Farhang AR, Goldberg JH. Dopamine neurons encode performance error in singing birds. Science. 2016;354:1278–1282. doi: 10.1126/science.aah6837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Goodson JL, Kabelik D, Kelly AM, Rinaldi J, Klatt JD. Midbrain dopamine neurons reflect affiliation phenotypes in finches and are tightly coupled to courtship. PNAS. 2009;106:8737–8742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811821106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Goodson JL, Kingsbury MA. Nonapeptides and the evolution of social group sizes in birds. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy. 2011;5:13. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2011.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Goodson JL, Kelly AM, Kingsbury MA. Evolving nonapeptide mechanisms of gregariousness and social diversity in birds. Hormones and Behavior. 2012;61:239–250. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Griffith SC, Holleley CE, Mariette MM, Pryke SR, Svedin N. Low level of extrapair parentage in wild zebra finches. Animal Behaviour. 2010;79:261–264. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gunn RN, Lammertsma AA, Hume SP, Cunningham VJ. Parametric imaging of ligand-receptor binding in PET using a simplified reference region model. NeuroImage. 1997;6:279–287. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hausberger M, Richard-Yris M-A, Henry L, Lepage L, Schmidt I. Song sharing reflects the social organization in a captive group of European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1995;109:222–241. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.222. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hoffmann LA, Saravanan V, Wood AN, He L, Sober SJ. Dopaminergic contributions to vocal learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:2176–2189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3883-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Holveck M-J, Riebel K. Preferred songs predict preferred males: consistency and repeatability of zebra finch females across three test contexts. Animal Behaviour. 2007;74:297–309. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Howe MW, Dombeck DA. Rapid signalling in distinct dopaminergic axons during locomotion and reward. Nature. 2016;535:505–510. doi: 10.1038/nature18942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ihle EC, van der Hart M, Jongsma M, Tecott LH, Doupe AJ. Dopamine physiology in the basal ganglia of male zebra finches during social stimulation. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;41:1506–1514. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Iwasaki M, Poulsen TM, Oka K, Hessler NA. Sexually dimorphic activation of dopaminergic areas depends on affiliation during courtship and pair formation. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8:210. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F. For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression. Neuron. 1998;21:775–788. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80594-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kelly AM, Goodson JL. Functional interactions of dopamine cell groups reflect personality, sex, and social context in highly social finches. Behavioural Brain Research. 2015;280:101–112. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kroodsma DE, Byers BE. The function(s) of bird song. American Zoologist. 1991;31:318–328. doi: 10.1093/icb/31.2.318. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kubikova L, Kostál L. Dopaminergic system in birdsong learning and maintenance. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy. 2010;39:112–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kubikova L, Wada K, Jarvis ED. Dopamine receptors in a songbird brain. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2010;518:741–769. doi: 10.1002/cne.22255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lammertsma AA, Bench CJ, Hume SP, Osman S, Gunn K, Brooks DJ, Frackowiak RS. Comparison of methods for analysis of clinical [11C]raclopride studies. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 1996;16:42–52. doi: 10.1097/00004647-199601000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Laruelle M. Imaging synaptic neurotransmission with in vivo binding competition techniques: a critical review. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2000;20:423–451. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200003000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Li SM, Collins GT, Paul NM, Grundt P, Newman AH, Xu M, Grandy DK, Woods JH, Katz JL. Yawning and locomotor behavior induced by dopamine receptor agonists in mice and rats. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2010;21:171–181. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32833a5c68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Litton JE, Hall H, Pauli S. Saturation analysis in PET--analysis of errors due to imperfect reference regions. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 1994;14:358–361. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1994.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ljubičić I, Hyland Bruno J, Tchernichovski O. Social influences on song learning. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2016;7:101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.12.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Martinez D, Slifstein M, Broft A, Mawlawi O, Hwang DR, Huang Y, Cooper T, Kegeles L, Zarahn E, Abi-Dargham A, Haber SN, Laruelle M. Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography. Part II: amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the functional subdivisions of the striatum. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2003;23:285–300. doi: 10.1097/01.WCB.0000048520.34839.1A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Marzluff JM, Miyaoka R, Minoshima S, Cross DJ. Brain imaging reveals neuronal circuitry underlying the crow's perception of human faces. PNAS. 2012;109:15912–15917. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206109109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mawlawi O, Martinez D, Slifstein M, Broft A, Chatterjee R, Hwang DR, Huang Y, Simpson N, Ngo K, Van Heertum R, Laruelle M. Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography: I. Accuracy and precision of D(2) receptor parameter measurements in ventral striatum. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2001;21:1034–1057. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200109000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Morris ED, Normandin MD, Schiffer WK. Initial comparison of ntPET with microdialysis measurements of methamphetamine-induced dopamine release in rats: support for estimation of dopamine curves from PET data. Molecular Imaging and Biology. 2008;10:67–73. doi: 10.1007/s11307-007-0124-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Normandin MD, Schiffer WK, Morris ED. A linear model for estimation of neurotransmitter response profiles from dynamic PET data. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2689–2699. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nottebohm F, Arnold AP. Sexual dimorphism in vocal control areas of the songbird brain. Science. 1976;194:211–213. doi: 10.1126/science.959852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Olson CR, Owen DC, Ryabinin AE, Mello CV. Drinking songs: alcohol effects on learned song of zebra finches. PLoS One. 2014;9:e115427. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Patel VD, Lee DE, Alexoff DL, Dewey SL, Schiffer WK. Imaging dopamine release with Positron Emission Tomography (PET) and (11)C-raclopride in freely moving animals. NeuroImage. 2008;41:1051–1066. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Poirier C, Vellema M, Verhoye M, Van Meir V, Wild JM, Balthazart J, Van Der Linden A. A three-dimensional MRI atlas of the zebra finch brain in stereotaxic coordinates. NeuroImage. 2008;41:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Riebel K. Chapter 6 Song and female mate choice in zebra finches: a review. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 2009;40:197–238. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(09)40006-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Riters LV. Pleasure seeking and birdsong. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35:1837–1845. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1996;6:228–236. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80077-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Salimpoor VN, Benovoy M, Larcher K, Dagher A, Zatorre RJ. Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:257–262. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sasaki A, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Jarvis ED. Social context-dependent singing-regulated dopamine. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:9010–9014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1335-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Scharff C, Nottebohm F. A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 1991;11:2896–2913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02896.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schultz W. Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron. 2002;36:241–263. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Seeman P, Sunahara RK, Niznik HB. Receptor-receptor link in membranes revealed by ligand competition: example for dopamine D1 and D2 receptors. Synapse. 1994;17:62–64. doi: 10.1002/syn.890170107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Shaham Y, Erb S, Stewart J. Stress-induced relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking in rats: a review. Brain Research Reviews. 2000;33:13–33. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(00)00024-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Siepel FJ, van Lier MGJTB, Chen M, Disselhorst JA, Meeuwis APW, Oyen WJG, Boerman OC, Visser EP. Scanning multiple mice in a small-animal PET scanner: Influence on image quality. Nuclear Instruments and Methods in Physics Research Section A: Accelerators, Spectrometers, Detectors and Associated Equipment. 2010;621:605–610. doi: 10.1016/j.nima.2010.05.057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Simonyan K, Horwitz B, Jarvis ED. Dopamine regulation of human speech and bird song: a critical review. Brain and Language. 2012;122:142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Slater PJB. Fifty years of bird song research: a case study in animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour. 2003;65:633–639. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Sossinka R, Böhner J. Song types in the zebra finch (Poephila guttata castanotis) Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie. 1980;53:123–132. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1980.tb01044.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Stuber GD, Klanker M, de Ridder B, Bowers MS, Joosten RN, Feenstra MG, Bonci A. Reward-predictive cues enhance excitatory synaptic strength onto midbrain dopamine neurons. Science. 2008;321:1690–1692. doi: 10.1126/science.1160873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Thompson TI. Visual reinforcement in siamese fighting fish. Science. 1963;141:55–57. doi: 10.1126/science.141.3575.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Thompson TI. Visual reinforcement in fighting cocks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1964;7:45–49. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1964.7-45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Tokarev K, Tchernichovski O. A novel paradigm for auditory discrimination training with social reinforcement in songbirds. bioRxiv. 2014 doi: 10.1101/004176. [DOI]
  58. Urban NB, Slifstein M, Meda S, Xu X, Ayoub R, Medina O, Pearlson GD, Krystal JH, Abi-Dargham A. Imaging human reward processing with positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychopharmacology. 2012;221:67–77. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2543-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Watson DJ, Loiseau F, Ingallinesi M, Millan MJ, Marsden CA, Fone KC. Selective blockade of dopamine D3 receptors enhances while D2 receptor antagonism impairs social novelty discrimination and novel object recognition in rats: a key role for the prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:770–786. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Woolley SC, Doupe AJ. Social context-induced song variation affects female behavior and gene expression. PLoS Biology. 2008;6:e62. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Yoder KK, Kareken DA, Morris ED. What were they thinking? Cognitive states may influence [11C]raclopride binding potential in the striatum. Neuroscience letters. 2008;430:38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Zann RA. Reproduction in a zebra finch colony in south-eastern australia: The significance of monogamy, precocial breeding and multiple broods in a highly mobile species. Emu. 1994;94:285–299. doi: 10.1071/MU9940285. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Naoshige Uchida1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

[Editors’ note: this article was originally rejected after discussions between the reviewers, but the authors were invited to resubmit after an appeal against the decision.]

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Sexual dimorphism in striatal dopaminergic responses promotes monogamy in social songbirds" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom, Naoshige Uchida (Reviewer 3), is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by the Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor.

Our decision has been reached after consultation between the reviewers. Based on these discussions and the individual reviews below, we regret to inform you that your work will not be considered further for publication in eLife.

Tokarev and colleagues used a delayed PET method to test the role of striatal dopamine in social interactions in zebra finch. Specifically, the authors show that song playback increased the PET signal in the striatum only in males but not in females. Further experiments showed that the PET signal in the striatum increases in female birds only when mated females were exposed to the song of their mated males. Behaviorally, unmated males were willing to obtain song stimulation in exchange of mildly aversive air puffs whereas unmated females weren't. The authors found that mated females were willing to obtain the song of their mated males but not that of other males (unfamiliar song). Finally, the authors demonstrate that D2 dopamine receptor blockade impaired the reinforcing property of songs in males. These results are potentially interesting as it demonstrates positively reinforcing properties of song and the involvement of striatal dopamine in this process.

However, the reviewers found some substantive concerns. First, the authors do not provide evidence that the striatal dopamine signals are not related to animals' movement. Second, the interpretation of the signal obtained using the delayed-PET method is not well explained or is rather confusing. During discussion, we have additionally contacted an expert of PET techniques. This person largely agreed the Reviewer 3's concerns. His/her comments are summarized below:

Typically, a parametric pharmacokinetic model is used to derive a measure of non-displaceable [11C]raclopride binding potential for each voxel or region of interest. This is used as a proxy for "D2/D3 receptor availability." Because DA competes with [11C]raclopride for available D2/D3 receptors, a reduction in [11C]raclopride binding potential following a cognitive/behavioral or pharmacological challenge is taken as evidence for DA release. For example, administering amphetamine will cause striatal [11C]raclopride binding potential to decrease (compared to administration of placebo). In the few studies that have also obtained simultaneous direct measures of DA release (typically through microdialysis), the magnitude of stimulus-induced striatal DA release is strongly correlated with the size of the reduction in [11C]raclopride binding potential after stimulant administration.

With all that said, the pharmacokinetic models are not particularly robust. Practically, this places significant constraints on experimental design. I raise this point because it seems that the authors used a "delayed PET" approach that is highly nonstandard. In human PET imaging, investigators will typically inject [11C]raclopride, measure signal until the tracer reaches equilibrium to obtain a pre-challenge binding potential estimate, introduce the cognitive/behavioral/drug challenge, and then measure signal again to get a post-challenge binding potential estimate. It's this binding potential delta (reflecting the degree to which a challenge reduces binding potential) that's then compared between groups or correlated with some individual difference measure. In principle, it's correct to interpret a challenge-induced increase in [11C]raclopride binding potential as reflecting a challenge-induced decrease in DA. In practice, though, this is uncommon and such a finding would either have to be supported by corroborative measures or a compelling, a-priori mechanistic prediction. I should say that their design may be entirely appropriate. What concerning is that they don't provide a coherent justification for this non-standard design, nor do they offer any validation that their models fit the data well given this design.

The way the authors talk about PET measurements is incredibly confusing. [11C]raclopride PET does not measure "dopamine receptor activity;" it measures [11C]raclopride binding potential! A change in [11C]raclopride binding potential due to some intervention can be interpreted as reflecting an intervention-induced change in DA reIease (presuming the modeling has been done correctly, which I am uncertain about in the present case). It cannot be interpreted as reflecting a change in DA receptor 'activity' (whatever that might mean!). I assume that when they say that their manipulation "increases DA receptor activity" what they mean is that they found a reduction in [11C]raclopride binding potential (i.e. increased DA release). But how they talk about their findings is conceptually wrong and needlessly confusing to the reader.

In light of this feedback, the reviewers' concern regarding the interpretation and presentation of the PET signals has been further increased. Based on these comments, we concluded that further verification of the technique is required to properly interpret the PET signals, and this might significantly change the authors' interpretations and conclusions.

The reviewers' individual comments are appended below.

Reviewer #1:

Social songbirds, like zebra finches, have to balance tolerance to the surrounding neighbors and selectivity to keep monogamous pairs (social behavior vs sexual behavior). Both for social and sexual behavior, dopaminergic signals are recognized as representing rewards, however, how this neuromodulator acts on the brain circuits is still largely unknown. In this paper, experiments were well designed to answer how dopaminergic activity is regulated differently between sexes and contexts. The authors show there are sexual differences in the dopaminergic activity in the striatum in responses to the song stimulation.

I found this paper exciting and important, as it showed for the first time (as far as I know) that sexual dimorphic response in dopamine activity to the song presentation, depending on birds' mating history. It also provided the new method which can measure the dopamine activity in freely behaving birds, although detailed receptor mechanism, thresholding mechanism etc. have not yet demonstrated. That would explain the brain mechanism to code reward signal depending on the contexts and history of animals' behavior, as well as the possible sexual dimorphism in coding.

The paper is well written and can be read smoothly so that I have some minor concerns stated in the following section.

Reviewer #2:

This manuscript uses PET imaging and pharmacology to test the roles of dopamine-striatal circuits in mediating reinforcing responses to song in the zebra finch. They find that males and females exhibit different dopamine responses and different behavioral responses to male song. Males were willing to sustain mild airpuffs to hear a variety of songs. Females, on the other hand, only appeared to find the song of their mated male rewarding. Striatal dopamine responses in males and females were consistent with these behavioral responses. The authors suggest, but do not demonstrate, that these results could support social behavior among males consistent with a gregarious, but not territorial, society.

Strengths:

1) The assay to test song reinforcement behaviorally is a clever adaptation of addiction paradigms to measure reinforcement in birds. The PET imaging is highly complementary.

2) Figures 1 and 2 provide very clear representations of experimental design, making this a very easy paper to read and understand.

Weaknesses:

1) The main weakness in this study is that dopamine activity is that the authors do not consider or control for the plausible possibility that animal movement is a major contributor to dopamine activity, and a potential confound of their behavioral assays. Specifically, dopamine activity is strongly modulated by movement in mammals (e.g. Jin and Costa, 2010; Howe and Dombeck, 2016;) and in songbirds (Gadagkar et al., 2016). Thus, any stimuli that elicits increased animal movement will also increase striatal dopamine, even if the stimulus itself does not actually act on the dopamine system. This possibility would be relatively easy to control for. One route could be to compare PET dopamine levels in two groups of animals: one which recently underwent a high period of activity and one that did not. If movement does not influence PET DA, then their results will hold. But I find this outcome to be unlikely. Another possibility would be to measure animal movement in their behavioral assays and either modify those methods to ensure that movement across distinct groups is relatively equal, or to regress against a movement parameter to demonstrate that a song stimulus, and not movement, is primarily responsible for the PET signal.

Reviewer #3:

Tokarev and colleagues examined the role of dopamine in social interactions in zebra finch. The authors use "delayed-PET" to measure dopamine release in vivo while the birds were exposed to song playbacks. The authors injected [11C]raclopride radiotracer that binds to D2 dopamine receptors before experiment. The birds were then exposed to song playbacks. The authors found that song playback increased the PET signal in the striatum only in males but not in females. Further experiments showed that the PET signal in the striatum increases in female birds only when mated females were exposed to the song of their mated males. The authors also examined the reinforcing properties of songs behaviorally. Unmated males were willing to obtain song stimulation in exchange of mildly aversive air puffs whereas unmated females weren't. The authors found that mated females were willing to obtain the song of their mated males but not that of other males (unfamiliar song). Finally, the authors demonstrate that D2 dopamine receptor blockade impaired the reinforcing property of songs in males.

These results are potentially interesting as it demonstrates positively reinforcing properties of song and the involvement of striatal dopamine in this process. Additionally, in female birds, this effect was observed specifically when mated female listen to their mated males. Nonetheless, I do not fully understand the nature of the PET measurement (thus this requires more explanations).

1) The authors state that they used the delayed-PET to measure "dopaminergic activity" or "D2 receptor activity" but I do not fully understand how this technique works. The authors describe: "When dopamine is released, decrease in radioactive [11C]raclopride signal is mediated through direct competition between these two molecules for D2 receptors (Fisher et al., 1995) and as a result of D2 receptors switching from low to high affinity for dopamine but not raclopride (Seeman et al., 1994; Fisher et al., 1995)". My understanding is that there is a basal level of [11C]raclopride binding before experimental manipulations (song). This basal binding compete with dopamine released during experimental manipulations. This would mean that dopamine release should decrease [11C]raclopride binding to D2 receptors, this would in turn reduce radioactive signals. However, the authors discuss increased radioactive signals as increased dopaminergic activity or D2 receptor activity.

First, it seems confusing to use "dopaminergic activity" or "D2 receptor activity". Please use words that more directly relate to what were actually measured (e.g. radioactive signal).

Second, the interpretation of the PET signal appears to depend on many assumptions regarding what really happens at the receptors and extracelluar space. It is possible that this is well-established in the field, but it is important to more explicitly explain it explicitly. What does the increase in the PET signal really indicate? Does it indicate increased dopamine release or decrease?

[Editors’ note: what now follows is the decision letter after the authors submitted for further consideration.]

Thank you for submitting your article "Sexual dimorphism in striatal dopaminergic responses promotes monogamy in social songbirds" for consideration by eLife. Your revised article and letter of appeal have been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

Summary:

We have considered your appeal, and the revised manuscript was sent to the original reviewers and a PET expert (Please note that the previous Reviewer 3 was replaced by the PET expert). The individual comments are appended below. Overall, the reviewers found that, as in the previous review, the manuscript contains potentially interesting findings. The reviewers thought that the manuscript is improved. The authors now present their analysis on body movements that addresses the previous concern regarding the possibility that dopamine signals may be due to movements. However, the reviewers still raised concerns as to the author's analysis methods and their descriptions of signals. These issues need to be addressed before publication.

Essential revisions:

1) There are still many places where they refer to "dopaminergic activity", "dopamine receptor activity", "D2 receptor activity", "neuronal activity related to dopamine release" and "activation of dopamine receptors". For instance, the authors state that "First, we used a delayed positron emission tomography (PET) procedure (Patel et al., 2008) in order to measure the accumulation of dopaminergic activity (neuronal activity related to dopamine release and activation of dopamine receptors)." The reviewers found that these statements are very confusing. Although we appreciate that the authors reduced some of similar statements from the previous manuscript, we would like the authors to address thoroughly to avoid any confusion. Please refer to Reviewer 3's comment #1 for more detail.

2) The authors compare "silent" versus "song" conditions. Because the author does not compare different time points in the same animal, the authors cannot conclude whether dopamine binding was increased or decreased. What if "silent" condition caused a decrease in [11C]raclopride signals? Please avoid using "increase" or "decrease" unless the authors can justify it.

3) The authors stated that they injected D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 before injecting [11C]raclopride "to test whether the song reinforcement we observed in males was driven by striatal D2 receptor activity". However, [11C]raclopride radioactivity alone does not support this claim. Please revise it. Please refer to Reviewer #1's comment #2.

4) Reviewer 3 raised a concern regarding the distinction between "group" and "individual" analyses. Please address this point (his/her point #2).

5) Reviewer 3 raised a concern regarding the conversion from striatal-cerebellar ratios to the SOR (striatal occipital ratio) values. Please address this point (his/her point #3).

6) Please provide more information regarding the timing of stimulus presentation relative to [11C]raclopride injection time. (Reviewer 3's point #4).

Reviewer #1:

The revised manuscript looks better than the original one, especially in the points of describing detailed methods and additional data on D2 receptor antagonist injection.

However, their data presentation is still confusing and did not show the data directly. Especially, Reviewer 3 asked to use the better term to directly reflect their data, they still use 'D2 receptor activity' without justifying their use of this term. (Even though they stated that they removed the problematic term 'dopamine receptor activity' in their rebuttal letter). In more specific:

Their methods explained that they measure the [11C]raclopride radioactivity with PET. That means if DA releases happen [11C]raclopride radioactivity decreases by receptor binding competition. They use [11C]raclopride radioactivity in the cerebellum as a baseline as there is no D2 receptor expression in there.

In their delayed PET methods, if my understanding is correct, birds were injected with [11C]raclopride and exposed to song or silence for 20 min, then scanned [11C]raclopride radioactivity by PET. So, they measured the [11C]raclopride radioactivity only after song (silent) exposure and it is not possible to measure the [11C]raclopride radioactivity changes before and after song listening. However, they use the term 'increase' or 'decrease' of 'D2 receptor activity' and 'change in the dopamine level' which are really confusing about what they measured.

In the study which they provided as a reference for delayed-PET (Marzluff et al., 2012), they measured the radioactivity of [F-18]fluorodeoxyglucose over the time course of the presentation of different visual stimulus and compared them. I think they need more clear justification of their delayed-PET methods.

They stated that they injected D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 before injecting [11C]raclopride 'to test whether the song reinforcement we observed in males was driven by striatal D2 receptor activity' (l139). However, it can test only whether [11C]raclopride binding is on D2 receptor, and cannot test whether song reinforcement was driven by striatal D2 receptor (for that they should inject D2 receptor antagonist into the striatum and see the effect on song reinforcement behavior, the air puff experiment). Also, we can see DA binding in the striatum with smaller [11C]raclopride radioactivity comparing to the cerebellum. But if D2 receptor antagonist is there, we cannot see the [11C]raclopride radioactivity in any condition. What we can expect is only the [11C]raclopride radioactivity difference between with saline injection, which tells only that the delayed PET measure is D2 receptor specific (even for that it would be better to test D1 receptor antagonist also as D1A receptor expression is already reported). It should be clear that what this experiment is for.

Reviewer #2:

My main concern with the initial manuscript was that the potential influence of movement on striatal dopamine signals was not addressed. This revision uses 16 new birds to carefully assess movement patterns in response to song. They find that movement is highly unlikely to explain the differences in DA responses between males and females. I still think this is an interesting paper that provides a stong link between striatal dopamine and song-related social behavior.

The entire study depends on the validity of PET for measuring striatal DA. Regrettably I lack the specific expertise to weigh in on whether or not this revision adequately addresses the legitimate concerns of the PET expert. If it is determined that their measurements are valid, then I could support this paper. But if it is determined that the interpretations of the PET data are an overreach, then I would defer to the PET expert and support her/his decision.

Reviewer #3:

It is definitely an improvement. My original comments are not much changed. On balance, my sense is that the data are ok, but there are still a few things that bother me regarding the PET component of the study.

1) How they interpret and discuss condition-induced changes in [11C]raclopride binding. There are still many places where they refer to "dopaminergic activity" or dopamine receptor activity. Dopamine "transmission" is a more accurate way of phrasing this. In a few places, the way that their language just doesn't make sense to me. For example: "First, we used a delayed positron emission tomography (PET) procedure (Patel et al., 2008) in order to measure the accumulation of dopaminergic activity (neuronal activity related to dopamine release and activation of dopamine receptors)." PET doesn't measure the accumulation of "dopaminergic activity" and it definitely doesn't measure neuronal activity or the activation of dopamine receptors (though some of the PET signal may be due to agonist-induced internalization of DA receptors, I don't think that's what they're referring to or mean here). As another example: "confirming that D2 receptor activity is a robust indicator of the overall striatal dopamine release." They're not measuring D2 receptor activity; they're measuring condition-induced changes in D2 receptor availability. The discussion still has many references to receptor activity.

2) They quantify condition-induced changes in [11C] raclopride binding by extracting binding estimates from a region of interest for each subject from the group-averaged parameter map. This is not a problem. They make a distinction between "group" and "individual" analyses that is incorrect, because they subsequently submit those individual values to a group-level statistical contrast. This is fine as a means of quantifying the average change in [11C]raclopride binding. However, it is no more an "individual" analysis than the original group-level (imaging) contrast from which the individual values were derived. Here's an example of this:

“We detected a cluster of voxels with significantly lower [11C]raclopride binding in response to mate song in a small part of the medial dorsal striatum (Figure 8A,B). At the group level, the difference across those voxels did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (Figure 8B). Nevertheless, at the individual level, the same area did show a statistically significant 12{plus minus}4% decrease in [11C]raclopride binding to mate song compared to non-mate song (Figure 8C; p=0.042, paired t-test).”

This example is especially problematic because they're using the "individual level" analysis for inference. They didn't get a significant result from the whole-brain contrast, which is – appropriately – corrected for multiple comparisons, so they extracted signal from the non-significant cluster identified from that contrast and re-ran the analysis outside of imaging space. It is just under p<0.05, so they report it as significant. This practice is considered invalid in imaging because of what has been called circularity, non-independence, or double-dipping (see Vul 2008, Kriegskorte, 2009).

3) I am confused by their conversion from striatal-cerebellar ratios to the SOR (striatal occipital ratio) values displayed in the figures and apparently used for inference. They cite Patel, 2008, as justification for this, but Patel, 2008, never mention SOR values. I'm honestly not sure why they did this, but it is odd.

4) I would have liked more information regarding the timing of stimulus presentation relative to [11C]raclopride injection time. They mention [11C]raclopride half-life as their reason for selecting 20 minutes, but stimulus timing onset should be calibrated to an estimate of striatal D2 receptor saturation (i.e. the stimulus should be introduced once equilibrium is reached with [11C] raclopride). Again, this is one of those things that may very well be ok, but I was looking for more information/justification about the timing choice.

eLife. 2017 Aug 11;6:e25819. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25819.023

Author response


[Editors’ note: the author responses to the first round of peer review follow.]

We thank the three reviewers and the expert of PET techniques for their suggestions and criticism, all of which were addressed in this revised version provided with the rebuttal, which, we feel, has improved greatly in the process. In particular, we addressed the concern about movement artifacts by presenting new movement tracking data, and the PET technique is now explained in much more details, using standard terminology.

[…] Typically, a parametric pharmacokinetic model is used to derive a measure of non-displaceable [11C]raclopride binding potential for each voxel or region of interest. This is used as a proxy for "D2/D3 receptor availability." Because DA competes with [11C]raclopride for available D2/D3 receptors, a reduction in [11C]raclopride binding potential following a cognitive/behavioral or pharmacological challenge is taken as evidence for DA release. For example, administering amphetamine will cause striatal [11C]raclopride binding potential to decrease (compared to administration of placebo). In the few studies that have also obtained simultaneous direct measures of DA release (typically through microdialysis), the magnitude of stimulus-induced striatal DA release is strongly correlated with the size of the reduction in [11C]raclopride binding potential after stimulant administration.

With all that said, the pharmacokinetic models are not particularly robust. Practically, this places significant constraints on experimental design. I raise this point because it seems that the authors used a "delayed PET" approach that is highly nonstandard.

Indeed, delayed PET techniques are rarely used in human studies. However, this methodology is well established in freely moving non-human animals. This method is very useful as it prevents immobilization stress, which is known to interfere with the striatal dopaminergic system. Delayed PET technique was successfully implemented in rodents about a decade ago (Patel et al., 2008). Since then, it has been replicated in multiple species, including songbirds (Marzluff et al., 2012). Usage of [11C]raclopride to track changes in dopamine levels has been validated in studies with simultaneous microdialysis (Morris et al., 2008; Normandin et al., 2012). As we mentioned in the text, the changes observed in our work were in the same range as changes in dopamine levels measured by microdialysis in another study on zebra finches (Ihle et al., 2015). Finally, the idea of using PET to assess changes in striatal dopamine during rewarding auditory stimuli is not new either: a Nature Neuroscience paper from 2011 showed, in humans, that music-triggered changes in striatal dopamine (as measured by [11C]raclopride PET), strongly correlated with the amount of pleasure subjects reported (Salimpoor et al., 2011). Our study is very similar, except for the 20-minute delay in the scan (as per ‘delayed PET’ protocol). In the revised manuscript we have added these and other references in order to clarify that delayed PET scan methodology is well established in nonhuman animals.

In human PET imaging, investigators will typically inject [11C]raclopride, measure signal until the tracer reaches equilibrium to obtain a pre-challenge binding potential estimate, introduce the cognitive/behavioral/drug challenge, and then measure signal again to get a post-challenge binding potential estimate. It's this binding potential delta (reflecting the degree to which a challenge reduces binding potential) that's then compared between groups or correlated with some individual difference measure. In principle, it's correct to interpret a challenge-induced increase in [11C]raclopride binding potential as reflecting a challenge-induced decrease in DA. In practice, though, this is uncommon and such a finding would either have to be supported by corroborative measures or a compelling, a-priori mechanistic prediction. I should say that their design may be entirely appropriate. What concerning is that they don't provide a coherent justification for this non-standard design, nor do they offer any validation that their models fit the data well given this design.

We did offer validations. In fact, our work goes beyond other studies providing validations by combining three different methods: PET, behavioural reinforcement, and pharmacological D2-receptor blockage. All three methods point to the same phenomenon, e.g., the PET results suggest that dopamine level might increase during song playback, but only in males. The behavioural results show behavioural reinforcement only in males. Finally, blocking D2 receptors blocked the effect in males. It took us several years to establish those validations, and we feel that requiring us, in addition, to re-establish the validity on the delayed PET technique at the basic level should not be necessary, given previously published and our current validations.

The way the authors talk about PET measurements is incredibly confusing. [11C]raclopride PET does not measure "dopamine receptor activity;" it measures [11C]raclopride binding potential! A change in [11C]raclopride binding potential due to some intervention can be interpreted as reflecting an intervention-induced change in DA reIease (presuming the modeling has been done correctly, which I am uncertain about in the present case). It cannot be interpreted as reflecting a change in DA receptor 'activity' (whatever that might mean!). I assume that when they say that their manipulation "increases DA receptor activity" what they mean is that they found a reduction in [11C]raclopride binding potential (i.e. increased DA release). But how they talk about their findings is conceptually wrong and needlessly confusing to the reader.

In light of this feedback, the reviewers' concern regarding the interpretation and presentation of the PET signals has been further increased. Based on these comments, we concluded that further verification of the technique is required to properly interpret the PET signals, and this might significantly change the authors' interpretations and conclusions.

The reviewers' individual comments are appended below.

We sincerely apologize for the confusion caused by the terminology that we introduced in the paper. We have now reverted to using the standard terminology. We acknowledge that our documentation of the PET methodology was deficient. We have therefore revised the manuscript to thoroughly describe the method. We have also removed the problematic term ‘dopamine receptor activity’ and replaced it with the more appropriate term ‘[11C]raclopride binding’. We would like to explain here why we introduced a new terminology, and how it, unfortunately, ended up confusing the expert and Reviewer #3. We were worried that the technical term ‘[11C]raclopride binding’ could confuse readers that are unfamiliar with PET methodology, since one has to keep reversing the logic: less [11C]raclopride binding suggests an increase in dopamine level. Hence, we modified the formula to capture the negative relation between dopamine levels and the raclopride signal. But we ended up confusing readers who were familiar with PET methodology. We suspect that the PET expert did not notice the change we made to the formula (which we only presented in the Materials and methods section), as the claim "In principle, it's correct to interpret a challenge-induced increase in [11C]raclopride binding potential as reflecting a challenge-induced decrease in DA. In practice, though, this is uncommon…” does not apply to our study. As in all other papers on dopamine PET, we interpreted decrease in [11C]raclopride binding as reflecting an increase in DA, but we presented the data with the reversed formula. Given the inadvertent confusion this caused for readers familiar with PET, we are of course switching to the traditional terminology in the revised manuscript.

Reviewer #1:

Social songbirds, like zebra finches, have to balance tolerance to the surrounding neighbors and selectivity to keep monogamous pairs (social behavior vs sexual behavior). Both for social and sexual behavior, dopaminergic signals are recognized as representing rewards, however, how this neuromodulator acts on the brain circuits is still largely unknown. In this paper, experiments were well designed to answer how dopaminergic activity is regulated differently between sexes and contexts. The authors show there are sexual differences in the dopaminergic activity in the striatum in responses to the song stimulation. I found this paper exciting and important, as it showed for the first time (as far as I know) that sexual dimorphic response in dopamine activity to the song presentation, depending on birds' mating history. It also provided the new method which can measure the dopamine activity in freely behaving birds, although detailed receptor mechanism, thresholding mechanism etc. have not yet demonstrated. That would explain the brain mechanism to code reward signal depending on the contexts and history of animals' behavior, as well as the possible sexual dimorphism in coding.

We thank the reviewer for those kind words. We added a paragraph to the discussion, to highlight the limitations of our techniques and to present open questions about cellular/molecular mechanisms. There are several potential mechanisms that could account for the sexual dimorphism we observed, including different receptors expression levels, different densities of dopaminergic cells, different ratios of D1/D2 receptors and different reuptake mechanisms. Those are now mentioned in the revised discussion.

The paper is well written and can be read smoothly so that I have some minor concerns stated in the following section.

Reviewer #2:

This manuscript uses PET imaging and pharmacology to test the roles of dopamine-striatal circuits in mediating reinforcing responses to song in the zebra finch. They find that males and females exhibit different dopamine responses and different behavioral responses to male song. Males were willing to sustain mild airpuffs to hear a variety of songs. Females, on the other hand, only appeared to find the song of their mated male rewarding. Striatal dopamine responses in males and females were consistent with these behavioral responses. The authors suggest, but do not demonstrate, that these results could support social behavior among males consistent with a gregarious, but not territorial, society.

We thank the reviewer for clarifying this point: the sexual dimorphism that we found suggests a mechanism that can potentially explain the coexistence of gregariousness and monogamy, but we should be careful not to overstate this idea, as it should be tested further in future studies. In the revised version we added a discussion paragraph about how cross species PET studies could further test this hypothesis.

Strengths:

1) The assay to test song reinforcement behaviorally is a clever adaptation of addiction paradigms to measure reinforcement in birds. The PET imaging is highly complementary.

2) Figures 1 and 2 provide very clear representations of experimental design, making this a very easy paper to read and understand.

Weaknesses:

1) The main weakness in this study is that dopamine activity is that the authors do not consider or control for the plausible possibility that animal movement is a major contributor to dopamine activity, and a potential confound of their behavioral assays.

We were aware of this issue, and we did mention that in our experimental conditions, our birds did not move much. But we agree with the reviewer that even slight movements could potentially affect striatal dopamine. We added a new control group where we continuously tracked body and head movement to directly test if movement artifacts can explain our results (Figure 3—figure supplement 2; Tables 1 and 2).

Specifically, dopamine activity is strongly modulated by movement in mammals (e.g. Jin and Costa, 2010; Howe and Dombeck, 2016;) and in songbirds (Gadagkar et al., 2016). Thus, any stimuli that elicits increased animal movement will also increase striatal dopamine, even if the stimulus itself does not actually act on the dopamine system. This possibility would be relatively easy to control for.

We agree that if movement was elicited by presenting auditory stimuli we would need to single it out to see what actually caused the effect on the dopamine system.

One route could be to compare PET dopamine levels in two groups of animals: one which recently underwent a high period of activity and one that did not. If movement does not influence PET DA, then their results will hold. But I find this outcome to be unlikely. Another possibility would be to measure animal movement in their behavioral assays and either modify those methods to ensure that movement across distinct groups is relatively equal, or to regress against a movement parameter to demonstrate that a song stimulus, and not movement, is primarily responsible for the PET signal.

We are grateful for these suggestions, and we followed the second one in the revised manuscript. If dopamine level change were due to movement, then movement should differ across treatments and sexes: higher in male zebra finches when hearing songs compared to when they are kept in silence, but not so in females. To test if this were the case, we performed an additional control experiment with a new group of 8 males and 8 females, where we simulated the song vs. silence PET conditions (including transfer and non-radioactive ligand injection), and also video tracked birds’ movement. Even with a continuous account of all body movements (e.g., flying, hopping and wing-whirring) throughout the session, we failed to find any significant changes in the amount of movement across treatments. Although head movement was higher during song playbacks, there was no significant difference between males and females. Therefore, movement artifacts cannot explain the effect of songs on dopamine that we observed. We include these results in the revised manuscript (Figure 3—figure supplement 2; Tables 1 and 2).

Reviewer #3:

Tokarev and colleagues examined the role of dopamine in social interactions in zebra finch. The authors use "delayed-PET" to measure dopamine release in vivo while the birds were exposed to song playbacks. The authors injected [11C]raclopride radiotracer that binds to D2 dopamine receptors before experiment. The birds were then exposed to song playbacks. The authors found that song playback increased the PET signal in the striatum only in males but not in females. Further experiments showed that the PET signal in the striatum increases in female birds only when mated females were exposed to the song of their mated males. The authors also examined the reinforcing properties of songs behaviorally. Unmated males were willing to obtain song stimulation in exchange of mildly aversive air puffs whereas unmated females weren't. The authors found that mated females were willing to obtain the song of their mated males but not that of other males (unfamiliar song). Finally, the authors demonstrate that D2 dopamine receptor blockade impaired the reinforcing property of songs in males.

These results are potentially interesting as it demonstrates positively reinforcing properties of song and the involvement of striatal dopamine in this process. Additionally, in female birds, this effect was observed specifically when mated female listen to their mated males. Nonetheless, I do not fully understand the nature of the PET measurement (thus this requires more explanations).

1) The authors state that they used the delayed-PET to measure "dopaminergic activity" or "D2 receptor activity" but I do not fully understand how this technique works. The authors describe: "When dopamine is released, decrease in radioactive [11C]raclopride signal is mediated through direct competition between these two molecules for D2 receptors (Fisher et al., 1995) and as a result of D2 receptors switching from low to high affinity for dopamine but not raclopride (Seeman et al., 1994; Fisher et al., 1995)". My understanding is that there is a basal level of [11C]raclopride binding before experimental manipulations (song). This basal binding compete with dopamine released during experimental manipulations. This would mean that dopamine release should decrease [11C]raclopride binding to D2 receptors, this would in turn reduce radioactive signals.

Yes, this is correct. However, we were worried that the technical term ‘[11C]raclopride binding’ could confuse most readers (who are unfamiliar with PET methodology), since one has to keep reversing the logic: less [11C]raclopride binding suggests an increase in dopamine level. Hence, we modified the formula to capture the negative relation between dopamine levels and the raclopride signal. This simple modification in the formula was designed to show changes in dopamine levels directly rather than inversely. This modification was described in the last paragraph of the Materials and methods section, but neither this reviewer nor the PET expert noticed it. This unfortunate decision of ours is the source of the confusion, which we have fixed in the revised version of the MS, where the PET technique is describe with more details and without deviating from the standard terminology used in the PET literature.

However, the authors discuss increased radioactive signals as increased dopaminergic activity or D2 receptor activity.

Not at all! Like everyone else, we interpret decrease in the PET signal as an increase in dopamine. So, as the radioactive signal is inversely related to dopaminergic activity, we changed the formula in order to capture this notion, and came up with the term ‘dopaminergic activity’, to make it easer to interpret the figures. Unfortunately, this led to confusion, and therefore, we have now decided to abandon this idea and in the revised manuscript present the PET data without modifying the formula.

First, it seems confusing to use "dopaminergic activity" or "D2 receptor activity". Please use words that more directly relate to what were actually measured (e.g. radioactive signal).

We agree and have followed this advice in the revised manuscript. We now use the technical term ‘[11C]raclopride binding’ and have abandoned the change in the formula that we did in the original manuscript. We apologize for this inadvertent confusion and we are, of course, switching to the traditional terminology in the revised manuscript. We now define the term “dopaminergic activity” in the introduction, in order to facilitate the discussion of dopaminergic mechanisms in the abstract, introduction and discussion, as following: neuronal activity related to dopamine release and activation of dopamine receptors.

Second, the interpretation of the PET signal appears to depend on many assumptions regarding what really happens at the receptors and extracelluar space. It is possible that this is well-established in the field, but it is important to more explicitly explain it explicitly. What does the increase in the PET signal really indicate? Does it indicate increased dopamine release or decrease?

Yes, decrease in PET signal reflects increased dopamine release. Note our statement in the original manuscript (subsection “PET image preparation and statistical analysis”): “As [11C]raclopride and dopamine compete for D2-receptors, decrease in radioactive signal indicates an increase of dopamine concentration (Fisher et al., 1995; Endres et al., 1997).” As we explain in the replies to the previous comments, we now use standard PET terminology and formula. We followed the reviewer suggestion, and in the revised manuscript we have also included an additional description of our methodology.

[Editors’ note: the author responses to the re-review follow.]

Essential revisions:

1) There are still many places where they refer to "dopaminergic activity", "dopamine receptor activity", "D2 receptor activity", "neuronal activity related to dopamine release" and "activation of dopamine receptors". For instance, the authors state that "First, we used a delayed positron emission tomography (PET) procedure (Patel et al., 2008) in order to measure the accumulation of dopaminergic activity (neuronal activity related to dopamine release and activation of dopamine receptors)." The reviewers found that these statements are very confusing. Although we appreciate that the authors reduced some of similar statements from the previous manuscript, we would like the authors to address thoroughly to avoid any confusion. Please refer to Reviewer 3's comment #1 for more detail.

We have removed the term ‘dopaminergic activity’ and, following advice of Reviewer 3, replaced it by the term “dopamine neurotransmission”. Also, when referring to figures and data directly, we now consistently use the term [11C]raclopride binding. The revised manuscript uses the same terminology as in Salimpoor e al., 2011.

2) The authors compare "silent" versus "song" conditions. Because the author does not compare different time points in the same animal, the authors cannot conclude whether dopamine binding was increased or decreased. What if "silent" condition caused a decrease in [11C]raclopride signals? Please avoid using "increase" or "decrease" unless the authors can justify it.

We omitted these terms and refer to differences as “higher/lower levels of [11C]raclopride binding” when referring to figures and data directly, and “higher/lower levels of dopamine neurotransmission” elsewhere. We agree that our silent condition is not biologically ‘neutral’, it is just a reasonable baseline for assessing the effect of song playbacks on dopamine neurotransmission.

3) The authors stated that they injected D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 before injecting [11C]raclopride "to test whether the song reinforcement we observed in males was driven by striatal D2 receptor activity". However, [11C]raclopride radioactivity alone does not support this claim. Please revise it. Please refer to Reviewer #1's comment #2.

Indeed [11C]raclopride signal alone would not have been enough to prove our claim, but the experiment with D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 consisted of two parts, and in the second part we did provide evidence from the behavioral preference test while under the influence of D2 receptor antagonist. To make it clearer that these are parts of the same experiment, we have now combined those paragraphs into one, and rephrased the aim more accurately:

“To test whether the song reinforcement we observed in males was dependent on dopamine neurotransmission, we used the D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 to interfere with D2 receptors.”

4) Reviewer 3 raised a concern regarding the distinction between "group" and "individual" analyses. Please address this point (his/her point #2).

The reviewer is correct; these were not separate statistical analyses. We have now clarified it by changing the wording. We have replaced the term ‘individual analysis’ with ‘exploratory post-hoc analysis’. In the mated females group, where the cluster identified was not significant after adjustment but post-hoc analysis showed evidence to increase signal in that area (with p=0.04), we changed the wording to indicate that the p-value obtained should not be interpreted as statistically significant, but as evidence for a weak effect that should be validated in future studies.

5) Reviewer 3 raised a concern regarding the conversion from striatal-cerebellar ratios to the SOR (striatal occipital ratio) values. Please address this point (his/her point #3).

We are sorry for this error. The cerebellum was used as a reference area throughout our study. The correct term is “striatal-cerebellar ratio”.

6) Please provide more information regarding the timing of stimulus presentation relative to [11C]raclopride injection time. (Reviewer 3's point #4).

We provided the information on the onset of stimulation within 1–2 minutes of recovery after injection (subsection “Simultaneous PET on four zebra finches to measure dopamine released during auditory stimulation in awake unrestrained state”). We now have backed up this timing by citing two previous studies that used “delayed PET” method (subsection “Simultaneous PET on four zebra finches to measure dopamine released during auditory stimulation in awake unrestrained state”).).

Reviewer #1:

The revised manuscript looks better than the original ones, especially in the points of describing detailed methods and additional data on D2 receptor antagonist injection.

However, their data presentation is still confusing and did not show the data directly. Especially, Reviewer 3 asked to use the better term to directly reflect their data, they still use 'D2 receptor activity' without justifying their use of this term. (Even though they stated that they removed the problematic term 'dopamine receptor activity' in their rebuttal letter). In more specific:

This is now fixed. Just to clarify, in the previous submission we did remove the term “dopaminergic activity” from the Results, but defined it the Introduction, and used it only in the Discussion. We now have removed it entirely and following advice of Reviewer 3 have replaced it with “dopamine neurotransmission”. When referring to data directly we now use the term “[11C]raclopride binding”. This terminology is adapted from Salimpoor et al., (2011).

Their methods explained that they measure the [11C]raclopride radioactivity with PET. That means if DA releases happen [11C]raclopride radioactivity decreases by receptor binding competition. They use [11C]raclopride radioactivity in the cerebellum as a baseline as there is no D2 receptor expression in there.

Yes, we now refer to this measurement as the “striatal–cerebellar ratio”.

In their delayed PET methods, if my understanding is correct, birds were injected with [11C]raclopride and exposed to song or silence for 20 minutes, then scanned [11C]raclopride radioactivity by PET. So, they measured the [11C]raclopride radioactivity only after song (silent) exposure and it is not possible to measure the [11C]raclopride radioactivity changes before and after song listening. However, they use the term 'increase' or 'decrease' of 'D2 receptor activity' and 'change in the dopamine level' which are really confusing about what they measured.

This is a correct description, and we thank the reviewer for pointing out this poor choice of words when describing our results. We now refer to these differences as “higher/lower levels of [11C]raclopride binding” (or dopamine neurotransmission).

In the study which they provided as a reference for delayed-PET (Marzluff et al., 2012), they measured the radioactivity of [F-18]fluorodeoxyglucose over the time course of the presentation of different visual stimulus and compared them. I think they need more clear justification of their delayed-PET methods.

In Marzluff et al., the crows were presented with only one type of stimulus: either “caring” masks or “threatening” masks in 1-minuteon/off blocks for 14 min. We also presented stimulus of one type before the scan (one song every 15 sec during 20 min). In contrast to our work, each crow in Marzluff et al., study was assigned a treatment at random and scanned only once. We went further and did two scans on the same animals with different types of stimulation, which allowed us to do within–subject analyses. Other than that and a different ligand, we used a very similar protocol.

They stated that they injected D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 before injecting [11C]raclopride 'to test whether the song reinforcement we observed in males was driven by striatal D2 receptor activity' (Results section). However, it can test only whether [11C]raclopride binding is on D2 receptor, and cannot test whether song reinforcement was driven by striatal D2 receptor (for that they should inject D2 receptor antagonist into the striatum and see the effect on song reinforcement behavior, the air puff experiment).

There is some confusion here: The [11C]raclopride signal alone would not have been enough to support our claim, but the experiment with D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 actually consisted of two parts, and the second part provided evidence from the behavioral preference test. To clarify that these are two parts of the same experiment, we have now combined those paragraphs into one. We also now use a phrase that described the aim more accurately:

“To test whether the song reinforcement we observed in males was dependent on the striatal dopamine neurotransmission, we used the D2 receptor antagonist L-741,626 to interfere with D2 receptor activity.”

So, the first part of the experiment provided us with evidence that this blocker was indeed specific to D2 receptors (same type that we observed in our PET scans), and we saw the effect in the striatum. The second part confirmed that without activity of these receptors there was no rewarding effect by hearing songs in males.

Also, we can see DA binding in the striatum with smaller [11C]raclopride radioactivity comparing to the cerebellum. But if D2 receptor antagonist is there, we cannot see the [11C]raclopride radioactivity in any condition. What we can expect is only the [11C]raclopride radioactivity difference between with saline injection, which tells only that the delayed PET measure is D2 receptor specific (even for that it would be better to test D1 receptor antagonist also as D1A receptor expression is already reported). It should be clear that what this experiment is for.

We now clarify that this first part of the experiment is aimed only to test if the antagonist can block the PET signal in the striatum (which it does). The second experiment shows the behavioral effect of this blockage (diminishing the reinforcement effect of song playbacks).

Reviewer #2:

My main concern with the initial manuscript was that the potential influence of movement on striatal dopamine signals was not addressed. This revision uses 16 new birds to carefully assess movement patterns in response to song. They find that movement is highly unlikely to explain the differences in DA responses between males and females. I still think this is an interesting paper that provides a stong link between striatal dopamine and song-related social behavior.

We thank the reviewer for these kind words, and for the previous suggestions to corroborate our findings by testing the effect of movement directly.

The entire study depends on the validity of PET for measuring striatal DA. Regrettably I lack the specific expertise to weigh in on whether or not this revision adequately addresses the legitimate concerns of the PET expert. If it is determined that their measurements are valid, then I could support this paper. But if it is determined that the interpretations of the PET data are an overreach, then I would defer to the PET expert and support her/his decision.

We believe that we have now addressed all the remaining concerns regarding our PET methodology and data interpretation.

Reviewer #3:

It is definitely an improvement. My original comments are not much changed. On balance, my sense is that the data are ok, but there are still a few things that bother me regarding the PET component of the study.

We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment. We have thoroughly addressed all remaining concerns regarding our PET methodology and data interpretation as elaborated below.

1) How they interpret and discuss condition-induced changes in [11C]raclopride binding. There are still many places where they refer to "dopaminergic activity" or dopamine receptor activity. Dopamine "transmission" is a more accurate way of phrasing this. In a few places, the way that their language just doesn't make sense to me. For example: "First, we used a delayed positron emission tomography (PET) procedure (Patel et al., 2008) in order to measure the accumulation of dopaminergic activity (neuronal activity related to dopamine release and activation of dopamine receptors)." PET doesn't measure the accumulation of "dopaminergic activity" and it definitely doesn't measure neuronal activity or the activation of dopamine receptors (though some of the PET signal may be due to agonist-induced internalization of DA receptors, I don't think that's what they're referring to or mean here). As another example: "confirming that D2 receptor activity is a robust indicator of the overall striatal dopamine release." They're not measuring D2 receptor activity; they're measuring condition-induced changes in D2 receptor availability. The discussion still has many references to receptor activity.

Following the reviewer suggestion we replaced ‘dopaminergic activity with “dopamine neurotransmission”.

When referring directly to figures or data, we now use the term “[11C]raclopride binding” consistently.

2) They quantify condition-induced changes in [11C] raclopride binding by extracting binding estimates from a region of interest for each subject from the group-averaged parameter map. This is not a problem. They make a distinction between "group" and "individual" analyses that is incorrect, because they subsequently submit those individual values to a group-level statistical contrast. This is fine as a means of quantifying the average change in [11C]raclopride binding. However, it is no more an "individual" analysis than the original group-level (imaging) contrast from which the individual values were derived. Here's an example of this:

“We detected a cluster of voxels with significantly lower [11C]raclopride binding in response to mate song in a small part of the medial dorsal striatum (Figure 8A,B). At the group level, the difference across those voxels did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (Figure 8B). Nevertheless, at the individual level, the same area did show a statistically significant 12 +/- 4% decrease in [11C]raclopride binding to mate song compared to non-mate song (Figure 8C; p=0.042, paired t-test).”

This example is especially problematic because they're using the "individual level" analysis for inference. They didn't get a significant result from the whole-brain contrast, which is – appropriately – corrected for multiple comparisons, so they extracted signal from the non-significant cluster identified from that contrast and re-ran the analysis outside of imaging space. It is just under p<0.05, so they report it as significant. This practice is considered invalid in imaging because of what has been called circularity, non-independence, or double-dipping (see Vul 2008, Kriegskorte, 2009).

Yes, we completely agree, and we have changed the wording so it does not sound like those were separate statistical analyses. We no longer say that we look at the data either at the group or individual level, and simply provide individual data points as exploratory post–hoc analysis. In this particular example, we now say that it is only “a trend for higher levels of dopamine transmission in response to mates’ songs in females” and we clarify that the p value of 0.04 in the should be interpreted as statistically significant, but as suggesting a weak effect in those females that should be further tested in future studies (Results section, Discussion section).

3) I am confused by their conversion from striatal-cerebellar ratios to the SOR (striatal occipital ratio) values displayed in the figures and apparently used for inference. They cite Patel, 2008, as justification for this, but Patel, 2008, never mention SOR values. I'm honestly not sure why they did this, but it is odd..

We are sorry for this mistake. We use the cerebellum as a reference area throughout the study. The correct term is, of course, “striatal–cerebellar ratio”.

4) I would have liked more information regarding the timing of stimulus presentation relative to [11C]raclopride injection time. They mention [11C]raclopride half-life as their reason for selecting 20 minutes, but stimulus timing onset should be calibrated to an estimate of striatal D2 receptor saturation (i.e. the stimulus should be introduced once equilibrium is reached with [11C] raclopride). Again, this is one of those things that may very well be ok, but I was looking for more information/justification about the timing choice.

Animals were presented with stimulation within 1–2 minutes after injection for 20 minutes (subsection “Simultaneous PET on four zebra finches to measure dopamine released during auditory stimulation in awake unrestrained state”).

We now have backed up this timing by citing two previous studies that used “delayed PET” method (subsection “Simultaneous PET on four zebra finches to measure dopamine released during auditory stimulation in awake unrestrained state”), as our approach is similar to that of Patel el al., 2008 (immediately after injection for 30 minutes) and Marzluff et al., 2012 (immediately after injection for 14 minutes).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25819.020

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES