Abstract
Collective circular motion is a common yet spectacular behavior of pigeon flocks. Efficient and robust inter-individual communication is required for flock coordination during this widely-spreaded movement pattern. When a flock hovers near the home loft, the rotational direction undergoes regular spontaneous variations. Based on intensive analyses of high spatial-temporal resolution GPS data, we quantified the intensity of velocity alignment among different individuals in terms of their velocity fluctuations. It is found that pigeon flocks employ an intermittent interaction (alignment) mechanism, where intra-group information transmission is not required at every instant. However, the union of the topologies of several consecutive interaction networks always keeps connected. This biological observation strongly suggests the presence of a jointly connected topology in pigeon flocks, which helps substantially reduce the communication and/or information processing requirements while retaining the agility and stability of the group. Finally, we conducted extensive investigation on the interaction mechanism as well as the spontaneous changes in rotational direction of pigeon flocks. These results shed some light onto the coordination mechanism exploration of bird flocks’ collective motions.
Introduction
In nature, cooperative behavior is common in biological systems ranging from microscopic to macroscopic levels, such as bacterial colonies1, migrating cells2, insect swarms3, fish schools4, bird flocks5 and mammal herds6–8. To understand the often encountered spectacular behavior of social animals, collective motion analysis has been widely carried out in recent years, and it is continuing to attract more and more attention from biologists, as well as physicists, life scientists, and computer scientists9. As a milestone study, Vicsek et al. proposed a well-known flock model, where each agent’s direction of movement is determined by the average direction of its neighbors. The so-called Vicsek model (VM)10 captures the behavior of highly ordered structures that emerge in bird flocks. Another well-known study11 of Couzin et al. proposed a three-sphere model that yields three typical patterns of universal collective motions in fish schools, i.e., swarm, torus, and migration states.
Recently, Ballerini et al.12 proposed a new alternative possibility for the interaction rule in a huge flock comprising about 2600 starlings, where it was observed that each bird interacted with only a fixed number of topological neighbors, instead of individuals within a specific metric distance. This model provided a better explanation of the interaction mechanism among starlings, which was later reinforced by both theoretical analysis13 and inter-species experiments in mosquitofish schools14. By further considering visual sensory limitation, Strandburg-Peshkin et al.15 predicted the propagation of behavioral change in fish schools during leadership events, and found that structural properties of visual interaction networks differ remarkably from previous relative spatial position-based models, including metric10 and topological12 counterparts, which expanded the understanding of collective flocking behaviors. Using a GPS tracking device, Dell’ Ariccia et al.16 studied homing pigeons (Columba livia) and found that the homing performance of birds flying in a flock was significantly better than that of birds released individually. Still, using high-resolution GPS data obtained from pigeon flocks, a hierarchical leadership network was revealed by Nagy et al.17, where each pigeon acts as a leader or a follower, or plays a dual role when situating on middle layers. To investigate whether pigeon flocks obey a hierarchical or egalitarian interaction pattern, Zhang et al.18 explored free flights of pigeon flocks and indicated that each pigeon tends to follow the average of its neighbors while moving along a smooth trajectory, whereas it switches to follow the leaders upon sudden turns or zigzags occur. Later, Chen et al.19 reanalyzed the same homing flight datasets17 and indicated that a pigeon flock has a fixed long-term leader for smooth moving trajectories in homing flights, whereas the leadership passes to a temporary one on sudden turns or zigzags. To investigate the principle governing self-organized patterns, Ferrante et al.20 developed an active-elastic-sheet, which well explains the emergence of the ordered state of both natural and artificial swarms. Based on stereo imaging techniques, Attanasi et al.21 collected a high-resolution spatial dataset composed of thousands of starlings, which they used to formulate a realistic flocking dynamics model concerning spontaneous symmetry breaking and conservation rules. This model suggests that turning information propagates across the flock according to a linear dispersion law with negligible attenuation. Aforementioned studies have provided specific insights into industrial applications of a huge volume of networked processes or multi-agent systems, such as unmanned air vehicles22, attitude alignment of satellite clusters23, and multi-robot formation control24, 25. Still, from mathematical analytical point of view, a general theoretical framework describing the dynamics of biological group behaviors was presented by Olfati-Saber et al.26, which provides deep insights into the emergence of highly-coordinated group behavior by simple inter-individual interactions. Jadbabaie et al.27 suggested a connectivity condition to guarantee that no agent escapes from the influence of the entire group.
Previous studies28–31 have shown that group decision-making strategies can not be elucidated without deeply understanding the interactions among individuals. Thus, an increasing number of investigations have been devoted to the intra-group interactions32, communication networks33, and information transmission mechanisms15. However, the influence of inter-agent connection on bird flock dynamics is still far from being fully understood. For instance, do pigeons interact with others all the time? Are the interaction intensities among pigeons flocks strong or weak? To answer these fascinating questions, we focused on circular movements and conducted a detailed analysis on combined datasets consisting of 41 releases of four pigeon flocks, each of which has ten individuals. We used the data of three pigeon flocks (labeled here as in the original paper: flocks A, B, and C) with 30 releases from34 (sampling period 0.1 s), as well as one flock (labeled here flock D) with 11 releases (labeled ff1-ff11 free flights) from ref. 17 (sampling period 0.2 s). We observed that, during the flight process, a pigeon did not align itself with others frequently, but only occasionally. This biological observation strongly suggests that pigeon flocks adopt an intermittent alignment mechanism. More significantly, it implies that, although the interaction (i.e., alignment) network of each instant is not necessarily connected, their union always keeps connected. It should be noted that throughout the study, we only focus on the ordered flock state during free flights, interaction is thus deemed as the widely-used index of pairwise velocity alignment17, 35, 43. Therefore, the present study gives an biological clue of the existence of jointly connected interaction networks in pigeon flocks.
Results
Intermittent interaction mechanism
We focus on the emergence of a highly ordered state during the free flights of pigeons. The degree of ordering in a flock is measured by the so-called order parameter , which is employed as a standard index of order during the study of collective animal behavior10. Here, denotes the velocity of bird i in the horizontal plane, and N is the total number of birds in the flock. Apparently, the index ϕ is zero for totally disordered and one for synchronized flocks (the evolution of the index ϕ for flock D in the ff2 release is shown in Figure S1).
In order to quantify the intensity of velocity alignment between each pair of individuals, we formulate a quantity related to the instantaneous correlation with time delay of two pigeons (see Methods) in terms of their velocity fluctuations (Fig. 1A–C). Since the magnitude of velocity fluctuation is much smaller than that of full velocity, it provides a sharper index for measuring the pairwise correlations, especially for highly synchronous collective motions. A previous study35 proposed a minimally structured (maximum entropy) model to investigate the interaction mechanism in large flocks of starlings, where the pairwise strength of interaction is defined as the intensity that the pair tend to align their moving directions. However, in small-sized pigeon flocks, we use the velocity fluctuation vector (both magnitude and direction are considered) to figure out the specific interactions in circular motions. In detail, the two individuals are considered to interact with each other when the instantaneous correlation (Eq. (1)) quantifying the velocity alignment intensity has a sufficient large value. Noted that individuals interact with each other by previously perceived information17, therefore, the index τ representing the time delay has been considered in the definition. After analyzing the combined datasets17, 34, the degree (number of neighbors) distributions are shown in Fig. 2A. It is observed that the mostly encountered case is that individuals merely interact with one or two neighbors. In addition, no pigeon interacts with all the other members in the flock at each time instant. This also suggests that the information achieved by one pigeon only propagates to a few neighbors at one instant.
According to Fig. 2A, we can predict that only the local diffusive transmission of information propagates among pigeon flocks. To investigate the dynamic characteristics of each individual, we also show the evolutionary interaction of each pigeon in Fig. 2B. In order to demonstrate the pairwise interactions more vividly, we show slots with different colors which indicate different numbers of communicating pigeons (i.e., neighbors) at a specific instant. Clearly, white and colorful regions appear intermittently, which suggests that each pigeon adopt an intermittent interaction protocol during the whole flight. In other words, some independent individuals may not interact with others, but spontaneously join the interaction now and then. We show the average durations of both interaction and non-interaction situations corresponding to each pigeon of flock D in Fig. 2C (other flocks, Figure S2). It is observed that each pigeon maintains the interaction with neighbors during around 3.5 s, and flies independently for some subsequent instants. Furthermore, the average durations of both interaction and non-interaction situations remain steady. Such observation indicates the reduced energy of communication and/or information processing defined as the proportion of the time that a pigeon does not interact with others. To examine the generality of the observation for different sampling periods, we also show the results of 5 Hz frequency case for flocks A, B and C in Figure S3, respectively. The results keep consistent with those of sampling period 0.1 s, which validates that sampling time does not influence the results. With a time delay, the instantaneous correlation (Eq. (1)) includes both non-interaction and interaction conditions. More interestingly, four distinct interaction cases can be formulated as well, such as Case 1: the pigeon only interacts with its neighbors without time delay; Case 2: the pigeon has directed interactions (with time delay) and plays the dual role as a leader and a follower simultaneously; Case 3: the pigeon has directed interactions and only act as a follower; Case 4: the pigeon has directed interactions and only plays the leading role. The proportions of the four cases in flock D are shown in Fig. 2D. It is observed that directed interactions with time delay are more frequently encountered than undirected interactions (other flocks, Figure S4).
Jointly connected network
The instantaneous correlation (Eq. (1)) defines a directed relationship for non-zero time delays for each pair of pigeons. A directed graph is called weakly connected if replacing all the directed edges with undirected edges produces a connected undirected graph. The term strongly connected is used if the network contains both a directed path from u to v and the reverse for every pair of nodes u and v. In this paper, both the weakly and strongly connected conditions of interaction networks are investigated. As shown in Fig. 3A,B, a small possibility of connectivity is associated to the union of interaction networks at some consecutive instants. Evidently, the possibility grows larger with increasing durations. If the flock is more hierarchical, transitive relationships are more directed with fewer feedbacks from the lower level to the higher level, which results in a smaller possibility for the flock to achieve a strongly connected topology. Thus, under both the weakly and the strongly connected conditions of the directed networks, flock D (trained racing pigeons, more hierarchical, flying above urban area in Budapest) has lower connectivity probability compared with those of flocks A, B, C (free-ranging domestic pigeons, flying above country area near Oxford). It indicates that pigeon flocks employ a jointly connected interaction network in free flight where the instantaneous interaction network does not keep connected, but the union of several continuous instants becomes connected. If we consider a sufficiently long period, a path will always exist in the union of the sequential instantaneous interaction networks from one individual to any other in the flock. It should be noted that the network represents a whole landscape of pairwise velocity alignments, but not the channels of information transmission. Although in small bird flocks, information may independently transfer extremely swiftly21, individuals do not prefer to interact at every instant. To give a clearer illustration of the evolution of connectivity, we pick the networks of five consecutive instants in flock D, which are shown in Fig. 3C–G. It is weakly connected when mergeing two consecutive networks into a union. However, only the network corresponding to the union of the five instants shown in Fig. 3G is strongly connected. Compared to weakly connected conditions, a strongly connected scenario is rarer to be achieved. Now a question is naturally inspired: to achieve coordination, whether pigeon flocks employ a strongly or weakly connected mechanism? In rotational movements, we suggest that a jointly and weakly connected condition is essential, whereas strong connections improve both extensiveness of information transmission and coordination among individuals.
To investigate the pairwise interaction among individuals, we show the heat map of interaction occurrence rate of each pairwise members in Figure S5 (four releases of flock D). During the entire flights, almost all members may have pairwise interaction among the flock. However, for a same flock, the interaction occurrence rate of each pairwise members is not fixed in different releases. In addition, we show the probability distribution of pairwise metric distances of both interaction and non-interaction situations in Fig. 4A. It is observed that the probability distribution of non-interaction decays strictly with increasing pairwise metric distances, whereas for interaction situation, the maximum occurs within the range of 2–4 m. Substantially, pigeons interact more frequently to their neighbors with smaller metric distance. Meanwhile, we show the scatter diagram with linear fitting lines for all pairs of ratios of interaction and the reciprocals of average pairwise metric distance in Fig. 4B. A significant positive correlation is observed by using permutation tests on each individual (nine pairs, sampling size: n 1 = n 2 = 9) in four flocks A, B, C, and D (mean ± SD: p A = 0.0052 ± 0.0062, p B = 0.0052 ± 0.0062, p C = 0.0052 ± 0.0062, p D = 0.0044 ± 0.0060).
Theoretical model
To quantitatively verify the understanding of circular motions with spontaneous changes in rotational directions, we propose a self-propelled particle model based on an intermittent interaction mechanism. Therein, three types of forces act on each particle: centripetal, alignment, and homing forces (Figure S6 and Methods). The centripetal force is designed to drive each individual to rotate. Meanwhile, we use the self-dynamics Eq. (3) (Methods) to generate the independent circular motion for each individual. The alignment force is defined as the combination of neighboring forces to achieve the convergence of the circular centers. The homing force is used to yield attraction from the loft to each pigeon, which is designed as a convex function. With respect to homing force, a previous study36 has reported the tendency of pigeons towards the roosting area and introduced three types of forces to simulate the free flights, i.e., homing force, velocity regulation, and interaction/alighment force. The motions of individuals are mediated by the positions, velocities, and directions of their neighbors, as assumed in the VM10. In addition, we employ a time-varying connectivity topology and apply a restriction on the communication capacity of each individual. More precisely, for each individual, we randomly pick five individuals as its neighbors if there are more than five ones within its interaction range. Note that if the connections are stronger with larger neighborhood size, then collective motion is more rapidly achieved37. But this implies greater communication cost. Therefore, the proposed model with limited communication capacity enables the verification of the feasibility of the inferred rules for reproducing coordinated circular motions. Additionally, the anisotropy of interaction based on our correlation method has been considered (Figure S7). Comparing with the results in starling flocks12, we observe that pigeons tend to interact with neighbors located right or left, but not along their velocity directions. To focus on the more crucial mechanism of intermittent interaction, we have not introduced the non-linear function related to the anisotropic visual apparatus of birds into the alignment force.
To simulate and explain how a heterogeneous flock of pigeons can achieve spontaneous changes in rotational direction, we assume that every individual has a unique depletion time38 which follows a Gaussian distribution (see Table 1 for the parameters) without explicit interaction of inner variables (see Figure S8 and Methods). More precisely, each pigeon is assigned a priori value of the length of flight time t i, at which the pigeon will start to feel the depletion of its energy reserve, and would be increasingly wishing to change rotational direction. When a pigeon feels “tired” or wants to change the rotational direction but the others not, it must follow them unwillingly but with increasingly greater intension to change39. Thus, when a sufficient number of members have been accumulated willing to change their rotational direction, they drive the whole flock to switch. More precisely, when individual i reaches its depletion time t i, the noise bias of angular speed increases from 10% to 20% to facilitate the rotational change. When the directional difference between the biased individual and the mean of the flock reaches 45°, it will drive the entire flock to switch.
Table 1.
Parameter | ν 0 | ω i | U A | U B | ρ ƒ | c 0 | w α | w β | L | μ | σ | ξ |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Value | 20 | 0.5 | 100 | 20 | 5 | 2.5e −6 | 0.2 | 2.2 | 400 | 250 | 30 | 2 |
Dimension | R/Δt | D/Δt | R | R | R/Δt | — | — | — | R | Δt | Δt | R/Δt |
The main characteristics of circular motions by pigeon flocks are a highly synchronous state with unpredictable changes in rotational direction (Fig. 5A). As shown in Fig. 5B–D, another unique feature is that pigeons cyclically rotate around their loft during the flight. When a collective decision is made to change rotational direction, the average positions corresponding to three continuous instants lie on a relatively straight line. Thus, the radius of the curvature suddenly increases and hence the distance increases between the average circular center and the loft. To investigate more deeply into the mechanism underneath these behaviors, as shown in Fig. 5E–H, where we compare the results of the numerical simulations with the experimental data based on the trajectory containing both counter-clockwise and clockwise circular motion patterns. The numerical results agree well with the experimental data. More precisely, in the natural situation, pigeons fly in a three-dimensional space, so staggered overlaps are often observed along the two-dimensional projected trajectory. Therefore, we do not consider repulsion forces in the simulation. Analogously, when driven by the three forces in the present model, the individuals move collectively in circles with different radii. In addition, all of the pigeons change their rotational direction spontaneously, but never stray far from their loft.
Discussion
Intermittent interaction exists in many species, such as locust40, sheep41, and humans42, among which Bazazi et al.40 reported that in the long movements which account for faster, and more energetic circling motions in the arena, intermittence can allow for some energy recovery. In this study, based on investigation of the circular motions, we suggest that pigeon flocks employ an intermittent interaction mechanism with low communication costs. In particular, each pigeon communicates with others only occasionally rather than continuously. The present study focuses on highly ordered circular motions, because turning information is propagated through the entire flock on almost every occasion, where the alignment of velocity fluctuations may carry significant directional information of interactions35, 43. Clearly, coordinated flock spatial structure with a high correlation of pairwise velocities is the foremost fascinating result, yielded by inter-individual interactions, or conversely, interactions are ciphered in the spatial structure12. Such interactions may refer to Newtonian forces, visual/auditory signal guidances, and coordinated velocity alignment, etc44.
In a previous empirical study35, interaction was found consistent with the directional correlations quantifying the intensity of velocity alignment, i.e., , where s i denotes the normalized velocity. Further, a unique index J was introduced as the strength in the interaction . Still, in another work43, to quantify the size of the 3d-domains, the correlation function of velocity fluctuations was defined as , where δ(r − r ij) is a smoothed Dirac δ–function characterizing the pairs of birds at mutual distance r, c 0 is a normalized factor, and u i and u j are velocity fluctuations of birds i and j, respectively. Clearly, a large value of C(r) implies that the vectors are nearly parallel and thus strongly correlated. They further suggested that each bird with an effective perception has a much larger correlation range than the direct inter-individual interaction range. Therefore, it is reasonable to believe that with a sufficiently high threshold of correlation, the alignment mechanism can be captured. In this study, it should be noted that the so-called interactions only correspond to the mutual alignment of velocity fluctuations for the entire flock to keep synchronous. Accordingly, pairwise alignment can result in the velocity correlation, and in turn, such correlation may imply the causal velocity alignment as well. We conduct a numerical study based on the standard Vicsek model, which indicates that the instantaneous pairwise velocity alignment can be effective to reveal the underlying interaction among coordinated clustered entities (Figure S9). Limited to the current available data, it still remains a mystery what the real interaction is in natural bird flocks. It is hard to answer whether the time delay between two correlated individuals may imply the real leader-follower relationship, or it is just caused by the response to a third focal individual or even more complicated situations. These questions leave the door open to the future research on collective animal behaviors.
Furthermore, synchronous organization based on a jointly connected communication protocol substantially reduces the intra-group communication cost. Unlike human social networks, pigeons are not likely to establish connections with fixed neighbors45. Moreover, the interaction network of a pigeon flock is even almost always unconnected at each instant, whereas it becomes connected when all the networks are merged over sufficiently long consecutive time intervals. Thus, pigeon flocks employ a jointly connected principle instead to achieve coordination, thereby substantially reducing the communication costs, which better explains actual intermittent communication situations in biological groups5, 46. Significantly, the study provides biological evidence of the existence of jointly connected communication network in bird flocks.
The jointly connected condition guarantees that each individual can communicate with others directly or indirectly after a sufficiently long time. However, what will happen if an agent escapes the influence of the others for a long period? In the present numerical simulation, by using a weaker jointly connected condition with a faster decay rate of ρ, an individual far from the others becomes an outlier after a change in the current movement pattern, so it whirls constantly and spontaneously as observed in nature17. Although Ferdinandy et al.47 suggested that a centrifugal or centripetal force is not needed to induce spontaneous rotation (a scattered distribution of individuals along the circle), if the animal group has incentive to stay in one place, we still introduce a centripetal force for each individual to achieve the cohesively circular movements, and the independent rotation. We suggest that to achieve collective coordination, the influence of alignment should be sufficiently strong, especially under some specific sensitive conditions, such as the rotational direction switching time and the occurrence of conflicting decisions. With respect to the way for pigeon flocks to achieve cohesion and synchronization, the revealed connectivity probability trend here provides an explanation. Note that unlike the previous study21 on the way of information transmission in starling flocks, the connections in the network correspond to the channels of pairwise interaction defined as the alignment of behaviors, but not the extremely swift information propagation. However, we suggest that to maintain cooperation, small-sized pigeon flocks employ a rapidly-increasing connectivity mechanism, where the decays of interaction possibility with the increasing of inter-individual distance is similar to the scale-free decays of correlation in huge bird flocks43. In addition, we suggest that instantaneous correlation method helps discover the interaction mechanism from a dynamical perspective in collective behaviors. Interaction occurrence rate also suggests another way to identify reciprocal pairwise relationship in hierarchical leadership networks.
In terms of energy savings in biological groups, previous studies on V-shaped flocks of geese48, 49 have been conducted from an aerodynamics perspective and claimed that individuals favor energy savings. However, Usherwood et al.50 stated that unlike V-formation pelicans, pigeons with increased flap frequency do not gain any aerodynamic advantage from flying in a flock. Still, other studies5, 46 have focused mainly on self-assembly shapes and formations, rather than the pairwise interaction protocols considered in the present study. In the scenario where a pigeon has no neighbors occupies a large proportion, we suggest that communication energy is substantially saved. In future investigations, it will be necessary to scale up from the current small flocks to larger ones, and to check the inter-specie issue to other kinds of animal groups like fish schools and insect colonies. A previous study12 of huge starling flocks indicated that each individual only interacts with a fixed limited number of topological neighbors, which supports the communication energy saving result. However, do larger groups of coordinated animals employ intermittent or frequent communication mechanisms? Whether their interaction manners obey strongly or weakly connected network structures? These appealing questions merit further investigation.
Methods
Velocity fluctuation
To investigate the influences of the velocity fluctuations of one pigeon on others, we calculate the derivation from the mean velocity for each pigeon i, which is defined as The spatial mean of the velocity fluctuations is zero by construction, thereby implying zero net motion at the center of mass43.
Instantaneous correlation
Considering that the maximum of time delay in flock D is around one second17, we accordingly formulate the quantity related to the instantaneous correlation of two pigeons with time delay −1s ≤ τ ≤ 1s as
1 |
Therein, the instantaneous correlation of velocity fluctuations is defined as
2 |
Thus, , and if . This instantaneous correlation (Eq. (1)) integrates the correlation of velocity fluctuations. A sufficiently high value of corresponds to the situation where the velocity fluctuation correlation is strong. In this case, the two pigeons are considered to align with each other, and hence pigeon i has the neighbor of pigeon j. (Setting the threshold of P ij is shown in Figure S10). It should be noted that due to the difficulty to detect the real interaction in natural animal groups, we adopt the well-accepted assumption12, 35, 43 that the high correlation of the velocity fluctuations from each pair of birds quantifying the alignment intensity implies the occurrence of interaction. The results based on different values of P * are shown in Figure S10, where the intermittent interaction can still be observed.
Definitions of the forces
In the model, we consider a group of n = 10 units moving in a planar space, where each has a velocity in two-dimensional real space, i.e., . Furthermore, , , where ν i and θ i are the linear speed and direction of individual i, respectively. For conciseness, we omit the time variable t (e.g. v i = v i(t)), which denotes the step in the simulation. Let be the particle’s Cartesian coordinates. The centripetal force F cen is a virtual force which will drive particle i to rotate independently. It is ciphered in the following dynamics of each individual i
3 |
where ω i is the angular speed which is fixed in the simulation, and η ν and η ω denote the random noise with values of ± 10 percentage of the linear and angular speeds, respectively.
Subsequently, the three types of forces are given through the following stochastic differential equation
4 |
with ξ denoting a Poisson process of delta-correlated white noise with zero mean, among which based on the empirical results that individuals interact with others by aligning their positions and moving angles, the alignment force F align is derived from its neighbors in order to achieve the convergence of the motion centers. Thus, the alignment rule is defined as follows
5 |
with i, j ∈ {1, 2, …, n} and denoting the Euclidean distance between individual i and j. Analogously, x ij = x ij − x j and y ij = y i − y j denote the Euclidean distance between individuals i and j in x- and y-axes, respectively. It should be noted that the system dynamics is handled by discretization in the simulation. For counter-clockwise circular motion of the center of mass, the unit alignment forces f(x ij) and f(y ij) are defined as
6 |
with r i = v i/ω i denoting the radius of curvature, which contains the convergence rule of both individual positions and circle centers. By contrast, for clockwise circular motion of the center of mass, f(x ij) and f(y ij) are defined as
7 |
In the simulation, we pick
8 |
where w α > 0 is the weight of alignment, and ρ denotes the range (Euclidean distance) of alignment to induce the intermittent interaction, which is defined as a triangular-wave function with constant positive and negative amplitudes U A, U B, and frequency ρ f. In particular, a larger value of w α suggests a high rate of coordination.
The homing force denotes the attraction from the loft to each particle, i.e.,
9 |
where c 0 is a constant, w β denotes the strength of attraction, is the Cartesian coordinates of the loft, and L is the side length for the simulation space. The parameters in the simulation are shown in Table 1.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We would express our sincere gratitude to Professor Tamás Vicsek and Professor Wen-Xu Wang for useful discussions and guidance. We thank the anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments. The work of H.-T. Zhang was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under Grant 61673189 and Grant 51535004, the Guangdong Innovative and Entrepreneurial Research Team Program under Grant 2014ZT05G304, and the HUST key Innovative Interdisciplinary Team Program under Grant 2016JCTD103.
Author Contributions
D.C., H.-T.Z. wrote the main manuscript and designed the experiments; D.C., X.L. and B.X. performed the experiments; H.-T.Z. and D.C. designed the data evaluation; D.C., X.L. and B.X. performed the analysis and the data visualization. All of the authors reviewed the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-09986-5
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Sokolov A, Aranson IS, Kessler JO, Goldstein RE. Concentration dependence of the collective dynamics of swimming bacteria. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2007;98 doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.98.158102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Szabo B, et al. Phase transition in the collective migration of tissue cells: experiment and model. Phys. Rev. E. 2006;74 doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.74.061908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Buhl J, et al. From Disorder to Order in Marching Locusts. Science. 2006;312:1402–1406. doi: 10.1126/science.1125142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hoare DJ, Couzin ID, Godin JG, Krause J. Context-dependent group size choice in fish. Anim. Behav. 2004;67:155–164. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.04.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bajec IL, Heppner FH. Organized flight in birds. Anim. Behav. 2009;78:777–789. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gueron S, Levin SA, Rubenstein DI. The dynamics of herds: from individuals to aggregations. J. Theor. Biol. 1996;182:85–98. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1996.0144. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ákos Z, et al. Leadership and path characteristics during walks are linked to dominance order and individual traits in dogs. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2014;10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Giuggioli L, McKetterick TJ. Holderied, M. Delayed response and biosonar perception explain movement coordination in trawling bats. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2015;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Vicsek T, Zafeiris A. Collective motion. Phys. Rep. 2012;517:71–140. doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2012.03.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Vicsek T, Czirók A, Ben-Jacob E, Cohen I, Shochet O. Novel type of phase transition in a system of self-driven particles. Phys. Rev. Lett. 1995;75 doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.75.1226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Couzin ID, Krause J, James R, Ruxton GD, Franks NR. Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups. J. Theor. Biol. 2002;218:1–11. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.2002.3065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ballerini M, et al. Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: Evidence from a field study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:1232–1237. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711437105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bode NWF, Franks DW, Wood AJ. Limited interactions in flocks: relating model simulations to empirical data. J. R. Soc. Interface. 2010;8:301–304. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2010.0397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Herbert-Read JE, et al. Inferring the rules of interaction of shoaling fish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:18726–18731. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1109355108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Strandburg-Peshkin A, et al. Visual sensory networks and effective information transfer in animal groups. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:R709–R711. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Dell’Ariccia G, Dell’Omo G, Wolfer DP, Lipp HP. Flock flying improves pigeons’ homing: GPS track analysis of individual flyers versus small groups. Anim. Behav. 2008;76:1165–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nagy M, Ákos Z, Biro D, Vicsek T. Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks. Nature. 2010;464:890–893. doi: 10.1038/nature08891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zhang H-T, et al. Route-dependent switch between hierarchical and egalitarian strategies in pigeon flocks. Sci. rep. 2014;4 doi: 10.1038/srep05805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chen D, et al. Switching hierarchical leadership mechanism in homing flight of pigeon flocks. EPL. 2016;114 doi: 10.1209/0295-5075/114/60008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ferrante E, Turgut AE, Dorigo M, Huepe C. Elasticity-based mechanism for the collective motion of self-propelled particles with springlike interactions: a model system for natural and artificial swarms. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2013;111 doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.111.268302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Attanasi A, et al. Information transfer and behavioural inertia in starling flocks. Nat. Phys. 2014;10:691–696. doi: 10.1038/nphys3035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Beard RW, McLain TW, Goodrich MA, Anderson EP. Coordinated target assignment and intercept for unmanned air vehicles. IEEE Trans. Robot. Automat. 2002;18:911–922. doi: 10.1109/TRA.2002.805653. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Campbell ME. Planning algorithm for multiple satellite clusters. J. Guid. Contr. Dynam. 2003;26:770–780. doi: 10.2514/2.5111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Zhang H-T, Chen Z, Yan L, Yu W. Applications of collective circular motion control to multirobot systems. IEEE Trans. Contr. Sys. Tech. 2013;21:1416–1422. doi: 10.1109/TCST.2012.2200679. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Vásárhelyi, G. et al. Outdoor flocking and formation flight with autonomous aerial robots. IROS Proc. IEEE 3866–3873 (2014).
- 26.Olfati-Saber R. Flocking for multi-agent dynamic systems: Algorithms and theory. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 2006;51:401–420. doi: 10.1109/TAC.2005.864190. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jadbabaie A, Lin J, Morse AS. Coordination of groups of mobile autonomous agents using nearest neighbor rules. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 2003;48:988–1001. doi: 10.1109/TAC.2003.812781. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Couzin ID, Krause J, Franks NR, Levin SA. Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature. 2005;433:513–516. doi: 10.1038/nature03236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Petit O, Bon R. Decision-making processes: the case of collective movements. Behav. Processes. 2010;84:635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Pettit B, Perna A, Biro D, Sumpter DJT. Interaction rules underlying group decisions in homing pigeons. J. R. Soc Interface. 2013;10 doi: 10.1098/rsif.2013.0529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Conradt L. Models in animal collective decision-making: information uncertainty and conflicting preferences. Interface focus. 2012;2:226–240. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2011.0090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Eftimie R, De VG, Lewis MA. Complex spatial group patterns result from different animal communication mechanisms. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:6974–6979. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611483104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McGregor, P. K. Animal communication networks. Cambridge University Press (2005).
- 34.Nagy M, et al. Context-dependent hierarchies in pigeons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:13049–13054. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305552110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bialek W, et al. Statistical mechanics for natural flocks of birds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:4786–4791. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1118633109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Heppner, F. & Grenander, U. A stochastic nonlinear model for coordinated bird flocks. The ubiquity of chaos, 233–238 (1990).
- 37.Chen Z, Zhang H-T. No-beacon collective circular motion of jointly connected multi-agents. Automatica. 2011;47:1929–1937. doi: 10.1016/j.automatica.2011.03.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ferdinandy B, et al. Landing together: How flocks arrive at a coherent action in time and space in the presence of perturbations. Physica A. 2012;391:1207–1215. doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2011.10.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bhattacharya K, Vicsek T. Collective decision making in cohesive flocks. New J. Phys. 2010;12 doi: 10.1088/1367-2630/12/9/093019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bazazi S, Bartumeus F, Hale JJ, Couzin ID. Intermittent motion in desert locusts: behavioural complexity in simple environments. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2012;8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ginelli F, et al. Intermittent collective dynamics emerge from conflicting imperatives in sheep herds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:12729–12734. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1503749112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Helbing D, Johansson A, Mathiesen J, Jensen MH, Hansen A. Analytical approach to continuous and intermittent bottleneck flows. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2006;97 doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.97.168001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cavagna A, et al. Scale-free correlations in starling flocks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:11865–11870. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005766107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Moussaid M, Helbing D, Theraulaz G. How simple rules determine pedestrian behavior and crowd disasters. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:6884–6888. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016507108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bode NWF, Wood AJ, Franks DW. Social networks and models for collective motion in animals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011;65:117–130. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1111-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hemelrijk CK, Hildenbrandt H. Schools of fish and flocks of birds: their shape and internal structure by self-organization. Interface focus. 2012;2:726–737. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2012.0025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ferdinandy B, Ozogány K, Vicsek T. Collective motion of groups of self-propelled particles following interacting leaders. Physica A. 2017;479:467–477. doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2017.03.025. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cutts C, Speakman J. Energy savings in formation flight of pink-footed geese. J. Exp. Biol. 1994;189:251–261. doi: 10.1242/jeb.189.1.251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Weimerskirch H, Martin J, Clerquin Y, Alexandre P, Jiraskova S. Energy saving in flight formation. Nature. 2001;413:697–698. doi: 10.1038/35099670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Usherwood JR, Stavrou M, Lowe JC, Roskilly K, Wilson AM. Flying in a flock comes at a cost in pigeons. Nature. 2011;474:494–497. doi: 10.1038/nature10164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.