Skip to main content
Howard Hughes Medical Institute Author Manuscripts logoLink to Howard Hughes Medical Institute Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Aug 24.
Published in final edited form as: Cell. 2017 Aug 17;170(5):1000–1012.e19. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.07.013

Distinct neural circuits for the formation and retrieval of episodic memories

Dheeraj S Roy 1,5, Takashi Kitamura 1,5, Teruhiro Okuyama 1, Sachie K Ogawa 1, Chen Sun 1, Yuichi Obata 2, Atsushi Yoshiki 2, Susumu Tonegawa 1,3,4,6,*
PMCID: PMC5586038  NIHMSID: NIHMS901328  PMID: 28823555

SUMMARY

The formation and retrieval of a memory is thought to be accomplished by activation and reactivation, respectively, of the memory-holding cells (engram cells) by a common set of neural circuits, but this hypothesis has not been established. The medial temporal-lobe system is essential for the formation and retrieval of episodic memory for which individual hippocampal subfields and entorhinal cortex layers contribute by carrying out specific functions. One subfield whose function is poorly known is the subiculum. Here, we show that dorsal subiculum and the circuit, CA1 to dorsal subiculum to medial entorhinal cortex layer 5, plays a crucial role selectively in the retrieval of episodic memories. Conversely, the direct CA1 to medial entorhinal cortex layer 5 circuit is essential specifically for memory formation. Our data suggest that the subiculum-containing detour loop is dedicated to meet the requirements associated with recall such as rapid memory updating and retrieval-driven instinctive fear responses.

INTRODUCTION

It is generally thought that formation and retrieval of a memory are accomplished by activation and reactivation of memory-holding cells (engram cells), respectively, by a largely common set of neural circuits that convey relevant sensory and/or processed information. However, this hypothesis has not been well studied. One of the best neural systems to prove this issue is the medial temporal lobe (MTL), including the hippocampus (HPC) and entorhinal cortex (EC), which plays crucial roles in episodic memory (Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Squire, 1992). Numerous studies have identified specific and crucial roles of individual HPC subfields and EC layers to the overall mnemonic function (Deng et al., 2010; Hasselmo and McClelland, 1999; Hitti and Siegelbaum, 2014; Moser et al., 2014; Nakazawa et al., 2004). However, the essential function of one HPC subfield, subiculum (Sub), is poorly known. The mammalian HPC formation is organized primarily as a unidirectional circuit, where information transferred from the EC’s superficial layers to the dentate gyrus (DG) is processed successively in CA subfields: CA3, CA2, and CA1. Dorsal CA1 (dCA1) sends its primary projections directly to medial EC layer 5 (EC5) or indirectly via dorsal subiculum (dSub) (a detour circuit). One of the interesting differences between the direct and indirect HPC output pathways is that in the latter, dSub projects not only to EC5, but also to many cortical and subcortical brain regions (Ding, 2013; Kishi et al., 2000).

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging of human subjects, several studies have suggested that the DG and CA subfields are selectively activated during episodic memory formation, whereas subiculum (Sub) is active during the recollection of an episode (Eldridge et al., 2005; Gabrieli et al., 1997). In rodents, ibotenic acid lesions of the CA1 subfield or Sub caused impairments in the acquisition of place navigation (Morris et al., 1990). However, since human imaging studies provide only correlative, rather than causal, evidence and rodent lesions are not well targeted to a specific hippocampal subregion, especially given the close proximity of CA1 and dSub, it has not been possible to identify the essential function of Sub in episodic memory. Furthermore, previous studies did not address the potential purpose of the parallel diverging and converging dCA1 to medial EC5 and dCA1 to dSub to medial EC5 circuits in memory formation versus retrieval.

In the present study, we addressed these issues by creating a mouse line expressing Cre recombinase specifically in dSub neurons. Combined with circuit tracing and optogenetic manipulations during behavioral paradigms, we found differential roles of dSub projections in hippocampal memory retrieval and retrieval-induced stress hormone responses. We demonstrate that dSub and the circuit, CA1→dSub→EC5, is selectively required for memory retrieval, while the dSub to mammillary bodies (MB) circuit regulates stress hormones following memory retrieval. In contrast, the direct CA1→EC5 circuit is essential for hippocampal memory formation, but not recall. Our study reveals a functional double-dissociation between parallel hippocampal output circuits that are important for memory formation versus memory retrieval.

RESULTS

Generation of FN1-Cre Mice

We took advantage of the finding that fibronectin-1 (FN1) gene expression is restricted to dSub neurons (Lein et al., 2004) and created a transgenic mouse line (FN1-Cre) that expresses Cre recombinase under the FN1 promoter (Figure 1A, and see Methods). When infected with a Cre-dependent adeno-associated virus containing an eYFP gene, eYFP expression was highly restricted to dSub neurons and was completely absent in neighboring dCA1 excitatory neurons identified by WFS1 (Figure 1B). The expression of eYFP was restricted to CaMKII+ excitatory neurons in both the deep and superficial layers of dSub (Figures 1C–1D, and see Figure S1A). This eYFP expression accounted for over 85% of all excitatory neurons in this brain region, and was dSub-specific along the entire medial-lateral axis (Figures 1E–1K). Further, Cre expression was absent in ventral subiculum (vSub) and medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) in this mouse line (Figures S1B–S1K). Using in situ hybridization, we confirmed that Cre expression in this mouse line is highly restricted to dSub, and the dorsal tegmental nucleus (DTg) in the brain stem (Figure S1L). Thus, FN1-Cre mice allows for the genetic manipulation of dSub excitatory neurons with unprecedented specificity.

Figure 1. Genetic targeting of dSub neurons using FN1-Cre mice.

Figure 1

(A) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent virus expressing eYFP into dSub.

(B) Cre+ dSub neurons (eYFP, green) do not overlap with dCA1 excitatory neurons (labeled with WFS1, red). Sagittal image (left), higher magnification image of boxed region (right). Dashed white line denotes CA1/dSub border (right).

(C, D) Cre+ dSub neurons (eYFP, green), in sagittal sections, express the excitatory neuronal marker CaMKII (red; C). Over 85% of all CaMKII+ neurons in the dSub region also expressed eYFP (n = 3 mice). Images are taken with a 20× objective. Cre+ dSub neurons do not express the inhibitory marker GAD67 (red; D). White arrows indicate GAD67+ cell bodies (D). Images are taken with a 40× objective. See also Figure S1A. DAPI staining in blue.

(E–K) Medial to lateral (ML, in millimeters relative to Bregma) sagittal sections show that eYFP signal is restricted to dSub neurons. DAPI staining (blue). No eYFP signal was observed in ventral subiculum (vSub) or medial entorhinal cortex (MEC). Dashed white line denotes perirhinal cortex/MEC border (J, K).

Input-Output Organization of dSub Neurons

We next examined the inputs to dSub excitatory neurons as well as their anterograde brain-wide projection pattern. Monosynaptic retrograde tracing experiments using a Cre-dependent helper virus combined with rabies virus (RV) expressing mCherry (Wickersham et al., 2007), labeled 78% of dSub cells relative to all cells (i.e., DAPI+ cells). The results confirmed that dCA1 provides the major input to dSub excitatory neurons (Figures 2A–2C) (Ding, 2013; Kishi et al., 2000). Other brain areas that provide inputs to dSub include parasubiculum (PaS), retrosplenial agranular cortex (RSA), superficial layers of EC (MEC II/III), nucleus of the diagonal band (NDB), nucleus accumbens shell (Acb Sh), and several thalamic nuclei (Thal Nucl) (Figure 2D, and see Figure S1M).

Figure 2. Input-output organization of dSub excitatory neurons.

Figure 2

(A) Monosynaptic retrograde tracing of dSub inputs used a Cre-dependent helper virus (tagged with eGFP) combined with a rabies virus (RV, mCherry) injected into dSub of FN1-Cre mice. Avian leukosis and sarcoma virus subgroup A receptor (TVA) and rabies glycoprotein (G).

(B, C) Representative ipsilateral sections confirmed efficient overlap of helper and RV-infected dSub neurons. Sagittal image (left; B), higher magnification images of boxed region (right; B). Quantification revealed that 78% of dSub cells, relative to DAPI+ neurons, were RV-positive (n = 4 mice), which is the starting population for retrograde tracing. Dashed white lines denote dSub Cre+ neuron target region. Both ipsilateral and contralateral sagittal sections revealed that dorsal CA1 provides the major input to dSub Cre+ neurons (C).

(D) Inputs to dSub Cre+ neurons were quantified based on percentage of neurons in the target brain region relative to DAPI+ neurons (n = 4 mice). Ipsilateral (Ipsi) and contralateral (Contra) counts. Parasubiculum (PaS), retrosplenial agranular cortex (RSA), MEC layers II/III (MEC II/III), nucleus of the diagonal band (NDB), nucleus accumbens shell (Acb Sh), and thalamic nuclei (Thal Nucl).

(E) FN1-Cre mice expressing ChR2-eYFP (Cre-dependent virus) in dSub neurons were used for CLARITY followed by light sheet microscopy (top). 2.5 mm optical section in sagittal view shows projections to RSA and mammillary bodies (MB, bottom).

(F) Whole-brain, stitched z-stack (horizontal view) shows all major projections from dSub Cre+ neurons including RSA, MB, EC5, and postrhinal cortex (Pos).

(G, H) Standard sagittal brain sections of FN1-Cre mice expressing ChR2-eYFP (Cre-dependent virus) in dSub neurons showing dSub projections to EC5 and Pos (G), as well as medial and lateral MB (H).

(I–M) Representative standard sagittal brain sections showing dSub neuronal populations projecting to MB (red, CTB555; I) or EC5 (green, CTB488; J). The respective CTB was injected into MB or EC5. Overlap image (K). Quantification, including weakly labeled CTB+ neurons, revealed that 81% of dSub cells were double positive (n = 4 mice). Scale bar in panels I–J applies to panel K. Dashed white line denotes CA1/dSub border. Higher magnification images of boxed regions indicated in Figure 2K (L–M).

A Cre-dependent channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2)-eYFP virus combined with light sheet microscopy of CLARITY (Chung et al., 2013, see Methods)-processed brain samples revealed that major efferents of dSub neurons were directed to RSA, mammillary bodies (MB), medial EC5, and postrhinal cortex (Pos) (Figures 2E–2F). No projections from dSub were observed in the superficial layers (II/III) of MEC (Figure 2G). These dSub neurons converged on both medial and lateral regions of MB (Figure 2H). Using a Cre-dependent synaptophysin virus to label dSub axonal terminals, we found that these Cre+ neurons express vesicular glutamate transporters 1 and 2 (Kaneko et al., 2002), reflecting their excitatory nature (Figures S1N–S1P). Injection of a retrograde tracer, cholera toxin subunit B (CTB), into the MB revealed a gradient of CTB555 with higher intensity labeling in the proximal part of dSub (i.e., closer to CA1), whereas injection into medial EC5 showed a gradient of CTB488 with higher intensity labeling in the distal part of dSub (i.e., away from CA1) (Figures 2I–2M, and see Figure S1Q) (Witter et al., 1990). However, neurons in both proximal and distal parts of dSub were weakly labeled by CTB injected into MB or EC5. Together, these results indicate that dCA1 serves as the main input structure to dSub, and that the majority of dSub neurons send projections to multiple downstream target structures.

The dSub→EC5 Circuit Bidirectionally Regulates Episodic Memory Retrieval

To examine the functional role of dSub neurons and their circuits, we performed optogenetic inhibition experiments using a Cre-dependent eArch3.0-eYFP virus. During the contextual fear-conditioning (CFC) paradigm, we confirmed that green light inhibition of dSub decreased behavior-induced immediate early gene cFos-positive neurons (Figures S2A–S2L). Inhibition of dSub neurons during CFC training had no effect on footshock-induced freezing behavior or long-term memory formation (Figure 3A). In contrast, dSub inhibition during CFC recall tests decreased behavioral performance (Figure 3B). Inhibition of dSub neurons had no effect on motor behaviors in an open-field assay (Figure S2M). Inhibition of dSub terminals in medial EC5, but not in MB, also revealed a memory retrieval deficit (Figure 3C). Since the behavioral effect of dSub inhibition in this mouse line is based on eArch expression in approximately 85% of excitatory neurons in this brain region, we examined the effect of a more complete inhibition of dSub neurons. Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals in wild type mice using an EF1α-eArch3.0-eYFP virus revealed a greater memory retrieval deficit (Figure S3 vs. Figure 3C). Further, inhibition of vSub→EC5 terminals showed normal levels of memory recall (Figure S3).

Figure 3. Differential roles of dSub projections in hippocampal memory retrieval and retrieval-induced stress hormone responses.

Figure 3

(A, B) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent virus expressing eArch3.0-eYFP into dSub. Optogenetic inhibition of dSub neurons during contextual fear conditioning (CFC) training had no effect on long-term memory (n = 12 mice per group; A). Inhibition of dSub neurons during CFC recall impaired behavioral performance (n = 12 mice per group; B). A two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed a behavioral epoch-by-eArch interaction and significant eArch-mediated attenuation of freezing (A–B: F1,44 = 5.70, P < 0.05, recall). For dSub optogenetic manipulation experiments, injections were targeted to dSub cell bodies and the extent of virus expression is shown in Figures 1E–1K.

(C) Terminal inhibition of dSub projections to EC5 (bottom left), but not MB (bottom right), disrupted CFC memory recall (n = 11 mice per group). A two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed a dSub terminal-by-eArch interaction and significant eArch-mediated attenuation of freezing (F1,40 = 7.63, P < 0.01, dSub→EC5 terminals).

(D) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent virus expressing ChR2-eYFP into dSub. Optogenetic activation of dSub→EC5 terminals during CFC memory recall increased freezing levels (left), which was not observed in a neutral context (middle) or using no shock mice (right, n = 10 mice per group).

(E) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during trace fear conditioning (TFC) recall decreased tone (Tn)-induced freezing levels (n = 12 mice). A two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed a behavioral epoch-by-eArch interaction and significant eArch-mediated attenuation of freezing (E and Figure S4A: F1,44 = 7.11, P < 0.05, recall). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panel), averaged freezing levels during the two light-off tones and the two light-on tones (right panel).

(F) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during cocaine-induced conditioned place preference (CPP) recall impaired behavioral performance (n = 14 mice per group). Behavioral schedule (left, top part). Average heat maps showing exploration time during pre-exposure and recall trials (left, bottom part). Dashed white lines demarcate individual zones in the CPP apparatus. Pre-exposure preference duration (right, top graph) and recall preference duration (right, bottom graph). Saline (S or Sal), cocaine (C or Coc). A two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed a drug group-by-eArch interaction and significant eArch-mediated attenuation of preference duration (F1,52 = 5.16, P < 0.05, cocaine). For CPP training inhibition, see Figure S4F. NS, not significant.

(G) Stress hormone: Terminal inhibition of dSub projections to MB, but not EC5, following CFC memory recall tests decreased stress responses as measured by corticosterone (CORT) levels. Optogenetic activation of dSub→MB terminals following CFC memory recall increased CORT levels (n = 10 mice per group). Context (ctx). CORT levels in CPP paradigm are shown in Figure S4H.

Unless specified, statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Conversely, optogenetic activation of ChR2-eYFP-expressing dSub projections to medial EC5 during CFC recall tests increased recall-induced freezing behavior in the training context, but not in a neutral context (Figure 3D, and see Figure S4A). This result indicates that dSub is involved in hippocampal memory retrieval in a context-specific manner. Activation of dSub→EC5 in mice that did not receive footshocks during training lacked freezing behavior during the recall test, supporting the specificity of increased memory retrieval in CFC-trained animals. Our interpretation of these optogenetic activation experiments is that in the training context natural recall cues reactivate engram cells in all subfields of the hippocampus, like DG, CA3, and CA1, but also in dSub. When the activity of dSub projections to EC5 is further increased by ChR2 this leads to enhanced freezing due to increased activation of dSub engram cells. On the other hand, in a neutral context lacking the specific natural recall cues to reactivate dSub engram cells, the ChR2 activation without engram labeling is not sufficient to induce memory recall. In another hippocampus-dependent memory paradigm, trace fear-conditioning, dSub→EC5 inhibition impaired memory recall (Figure 3E, and see Figures S4B–S4C). In contrast, inhibition of dSub→EC5 had no effect on the recall of a hippocampus-independent memory formed during delay fear-conditioning (Figures S4D–S4E). Together, these experiments indicate that the dSub→EC5 circuit regulates episodic memory retrieval bidirectionally. We confirmed that the dSub→EC5 projection is also necessary for the retrieval of a positive-valence, hippocampus-dependent (Raybuck and Lattal, 2014) memory formed in a conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm (Figure 3F, and see Figures S2N, S4F–S4G).

The dSub→MB Circuit Regulates Retrieval-Induced Stress Hormone Responses

During both CFC training and recall, levels of the stress hormone corticosterone (CORT) increases in the blood (Figure 3G), which is believed to be important to prepare the animal for a predicted immediate danger (Kelley et al., 2009). Given our finding that dSub neurons are required for memory retrieval, but not memory formation, we investigated whether the dSub→MB circuit is involved in retrieval-induced stress hormone responses. Optogenetic inhibition of dSub→MB projections following CFC recall, but not following CFC training, prevented the CORT increase (Figure 3G, and see Figure S2N). This deficit was specific to dSub→MB terminal inhibition, since dSub→EC5 terminal inhibition had no effect. In addition, optogenetic activation of ChR2-expressing dSub→MB projections following CFC recall increased CORT levels, revealing a bidirectional regulation of blood stress hormone levels by the dSub→MB circuit following fear memory retrieval. Interestingly, we did not observe increased CORT levels following CPP memory retrieval (Figure S4H). From our finding that the dSub→EC5 circuit is crucial for CPP memory retrieval (Figure 3F), it is clear that dSub neurons are activated and therefore both downstream EC5 and MB circuits would be activated. The lack of increased CORT levels following CPP memory retrieval suggests that the dSub→MB circuit is necessary but not sufficient to induce CORT. These experiments uncovered a neural circuit originating from dSub that regulates stress hormone responses to conditioned cues.

Heterogeneity of dCA1 Neurons that Project to dSub and EC5

The dCA1 neurons send primary projections directly to medial EC5, or indirectly via dSub (Ding, 2013). We examined whether the same dCA1 neurons send divergent projections to both dSub and EC5, or whether these two circuits involve distinct subpopulations of dCA1 neurons. To test these possibilities, we conducted monosynaptic retrograde tracing by injecting a Cre-dependent helper virus combined with rabies virus expressing mCherry (Wickersham et al., 2007) into dSub of FN1-Cre mice combined with CTB488 injected into medial EC5 (Figures 4A–4C, and see Figure S1Q). The dCA1 cells revealed a gradient of RV-mCherry with higher intensity labeling in the distal part of dCA1 (i.e., closer to dSub) (Figure 4B) and a gradient of CTB488 with higher intensity labeling in the proximal part of dCA1 (i.e., away from dSub) (Figure 4C). The higher intensity labeling of distal dCA1 by RV-mCherry and proximal dCA1 by CTB488 are consistent with earlier observations (Knierim et al., 2013), and suggests their stronger projections to dSub and EC5, respectively. However, in counting the total number of labeled cells regardless of labeling intensity, we did not find any significant difference in the number of RV-mCherry-positive dCA1 cells nor in the number of CTB488-positive dCA1 cells along the proximal-distal axis (Figure S5J). This suggests that the strength of the projections from dCA1 to dSub and from dCA1 to EC5 are not reflected in the total number of projecting dCA1 cells, but in their labeling intensity. Such differences in projection strength and targets of the distal vs. proximal dCA1 cells may provide the basis for their differential roles in behaviors. It has been suggested that proximal and distal dCA1 may play differential roles in memory formation (Nakazawa et al., 2016).

Figure 4. Projection from CA1 to EC5 is crucial for encoding, but not for retrieval, of hippocampal memories.

Figure 4

(A–C) Retrograde monosynaptic identification of dCA1 neurons projecting to dSub (in FN1-Cre mice) using a Cre-dependent helper virus combined with a rabies virus (RV). The extent of RV-positive dSub cells, which is the starting population for retrograde tracing, is shown in Figure 2B. Simultaneous retrograde monosynaptic identification of dCA1 neurons projecting to EC5 using CTB. DAPI (blue; A), RV-mCherry (red; B), CTB488 (green; C). Representative sagittal sections, dashed white line denotes CA1/CA2 border.

(D–F) Higher magnification images of boxed regions indicated in Figure 4C.

(G) Percentage of dCA1 neurons labeled with mCherry (dSub only), CTB488 (EC5 only), or mCherry and CTB double positive (dSub+EC5, n = 4 mice). Dashed line indicates chance level (6%), calculated from a control experiment (Figures S5A–S5H, and see Methods). One-sample t tests against chance level were performed.

(H) Representative sagittal sections of hippocampus from TRPC4-Cre mice showing dCA1 neurons labeled with a Cre-dependent histone H2B-GFP virus (green, bottom) and stained with DAPI (blue, top).

(I, J) TRPC4-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent virus expressing eArch3.0-eYFP into dCA1. Terminal inhibition of CA1→EC5 during CFC training impaired long-term memory (n = 10 mice per group; I). Inhibition of CA1→EC5 terminals during CFC recall had no effect on behavioral performance (n = 10 mice per group; J). A two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed a behavioral epoch-by-eArch interaction and significant eArch-mediated attenuation of freezing (I–J: F1,36 = 9.19, P < 0.01, training).

(K, L) Terminal inhibition of CA1→dSub during CFC training had no effect on long-term memory (n = 13 mice per group; K). Inhibition of CA1→dSub terminals during CFC recall disrupted behavioral performance (n = 13 mice per group; L). A two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed a behavioral epoch-by-eArch interaction and significant eArch-mediated attenuation of freezing (K–L: F1,48 = 5.16, P < 0.05, recall).

(M, N) Memory updating. Experimental schedule (top) for pre-exposure mediated contextual fear conditioning (PECFC) with optogenetic terminal inhibition of CA1→EC5 (using TRPC4-Cre mice; M) and dSub→EC5 (using FN1-Cre mice; N) during the pre-footshock period (left panels) or footshock period alone (right panels) on Day 2. Freezing levels during recall tests (Day 3) to the conditioned context (bottom). eYFP and eArch conditions (n = 12 mice per group). NS, not significant. Immediate shock (Imm. shk). A two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed a behavioral epoch-by-eArch interaction and significant eArch-mediated attenuation of freezing (M: F1,44 = 9.81, P < 0.01, recall in right panel; N: F1,44 = 4.75, P < 0.05, recall in left panel).

Unless specified, statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Importantly, we observed three neuronal populations distributed throughout the proximal-distal axis of dCA1, namely RV-mCherry-positive dCA1 cells, CTB488-positive dCA1 cells, and double-positive dCA1 cells (Figures 4D–4F), indicating that dCA1 neurons project collaterally to both dSub and medial EC5 (22%), project to dSub alone (18%), or to medial EC5 alone (23%) (Figure 4G, and see Figures S5A–S5H). A significant proportion of the remaining dCA1 neurons most likely send primary projections to the deep layers of the lateral EC (LEC5) (Knierim et al., 2013), which we confirmed using CTB retrograde tracing (Figure S5I). Thus, these data demonstrate that, although there are distinct dCA1 subpopulations that project to either dSub or EC5, a significant proportion of dCA1 neurons projecting to dSub and EC5 are shared between these two circuits.

The dCA1→EC5 Circuit is Crucial for Episodic Memory Encoding

Given the selective role of the dSub→EC5 circuit in memory retrieval and our finding that heterogeneous subpopulations of dCA1 neurons project to dSub and medial EC5, respectively, we next investigated the behavioral contributions of the direct dCA1→EC5 circuit. The injection of a Cre-dependent H2B-GFP virus into dCA1 of CA1 pyramidal cell-specific Cre transgenic mice, TRPC4-Cre (Okuyama et al., 2016), resulted in GFP expression restricted to dCA1 pyramidal cells without any expression in dSub (Figure 4H). Terminal inhibition of CA1 axons at medial EC5 during CFC training impaired long-term memory formation (Figure 4I), whereas the same manipulation during CFC recall had no effect on behavioral performance (Figure 4J). Further, consistent with the role of dSub in CFC recall, terminal inhibition of dCA1→dSub during CFC recall, but not during CFC training, decreased behavioral performance (Figures 4K–4L). Therefore, the direct dCA1→EC5 circuit plays a crucial role in the encoding, but not recall, of CFC long-term memory, whereas the indirect dCA1→dSub→EC5 circuit is crucial for memory recall, but not encoding.

Distinct Roles for the Direct and Indirect Circuits in Memory Updating

A potential purpose of the parallel diverging and converging direct dCA1→EC5 and indirect dCA1→dSub→EC5 circuits could be to support rapid memory updating (Lee, 2010). To test this possibility, we performed the pre-exposure mediated contextual fear-conditioning (PECFC) paradigm with optogenetic terminal inhibition of CA1→EC5 (Figure 4M) or dSub→EC5 (Figure 4N) during the pre-footshock period (context retrieval) only or footshock period (fear association) only, on Day 2. CA1→EC5 inhibition specifically during the footshock period of Day 2 impaired the context-shock association evidenced by decreased freezing on Day 3, whereas inhibition during the pre-footshock period had no effect (Figure 4M). In contrast, dSub→EC5 inhibition during the pre-footshock period of Day 2 impaired the context-shock association on Day 3, while inhibition restricted to the footshock period had no effect (Figure 4N). Together, these data indicate that the dSub→EC5 circuit is crucial for the rapid recall in order to perform memory updating, while the CA1→EC5 circuit is crucial for encoding new information into a long-term memory.

cFos Activation in dCA1 and dSub during Memory Encoding and Retrieval

Expression of immediate-early genes (IEGs) has been used to map specific functions onto neuronal activity in a given brain region (Kubik et al., 2007). In order to acquire cellular level evidence supporting the dedicated role of dSub in recall rather than encoding of CFC memory, we monitored IEG cFos activation in dCA1 and dSub during CFC behavior. To measure cFos activation by training or recall, we took advantage of a virus-mediated strategy (Roy et al., 2016) using a cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-H2B-GFP viruses (Figures 5A–5B). Wild type mice raised on a doxycycline (DOX) diet to prevent activity-dependent labeling by the injected virus cocktail were taken off DOX 24 hr before CFC encoding or recall to visualize H2B-GFP labeling in CA1 and dSub (Figures 5C–5E). There was significant cFos activation in both CA1 and dSub following encoding or recall epochs as compared to the home cage group (Figure 5F). Interestingly, in dSub, memory recall epochs enhanced cFos activation more compared with encoding epochs, whereas there was no difference of cFos activation in CA1 neurons elicited by these epochs (Figure 5G).

Figure 5. Distinct cFos activation patterns in CA1 and dSub neurons.

Figure 5

(A) Virus-mediated cFos+ neuronal labeling strategy using a cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-H2B-GFP (left). Wild-type mice raised on doxycycline (DOX) food were injected with the two viruses bilaterally into CA1 and dSub (right).

(B) Behavioral schedule and H2B-GFP labeling (see Methods). Beige shading indicates animals are maintained on DOX food.

(C–E) Representative sagittal section of hippocampus showing H2B-GFP-labeled cell bodies (green) in CA1 and dSub counterstained with DAPI (blue), following CFC training (C). Boxed regions in C are shown in higher magnification for CA1 (D) and dSub (E). Dashed white line denotes CA1/dSub border (E).

(F) H2B-GFP+ (cFos+) cell counts in CA1 (left) and dSub (right) from home cage, CFC training (encoding), and CFC recall groups (n = 6 mice per group). NS, not significant.

(G) Ratio of recall to training H2B-GFP+ neurons in CA1 and dSub (cell counts from Figure 5F). A ratio of 1.0 indicates comparable H2B-GFP+ counts during training and recall epochs. Statistical comparison used a Fisher’s exact test.

(H) Overlap between CFC-induced cFos and CA1 neurons projecting (labeled by CTB555) to dSub (left) or EC5 (right). Representative overlap images are shown in Figures S5K–S5N. Dashed lines indicate chance levels (n = 5 mice per group, see Methods). One-sample t tests against chance level were performed (#P < 0.05).

(I–M) Wild-type mice raised on DOX were used for these experiments. Representative coronal section of CA1 showing DAPI staining (I), CFC training-induced cFos-positive engram cells labeled with a cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-ChR2-eYFP (J), cFos antibody staining following CFC recall tests performed one day after training and engram labeling (K), and CA1 neurons projecting to either dSub or EC5 visualized by retrograde CTB555 labeling (L). The circled region with a single asterisk (*) shows an engram cell that is cFos but CTB555+ and the region with two asterisks (**) shows an engram cell that is cFos+ and CTB555+. White arrows show additional examples of CA1 engram cells that are both cFos+ and CTB555+. Overlap of recall-induced cFos, CA1 engram cells labeled during training, and circuit specific CA1 projection neurons (n = 6 mice per group; M).

(N) Representative coronal section of basolateral amygdala (BLA) showing cFos activation following memory recall (left). cFos+ cell counts (n = 6 mice per group) in BLA following natural recall, and recall with eArch inhibition of the CA1→EC5 or dSub→EC5 circuits (right). TRPC4-Cre mice were used for CA1 circuit manipulations and FN1-Cre mice were used for dSub circuit manipulations.

Unless specified, statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Further, we examined the overlap between behavior-induced cFos in CA1 and CA1 cells that were retrogradely labeled by injection of CTB555 into dSub or medial EC5 (Figure S1Q). Consistent with the optogenetic manipulation experiments, CA1 neurons projecting to EC5 showed higher levels of cFos activation during CFC encoding rather than retrieval, whereas CA1 neurons projecting to dSub showed higher levels of cFos activation during retrieval (Figure 5H, and see Figures S5K–S5N). To examine CA1 memory engram cell reactivation following recall, among dSub and EC5 projecting subpopulations, we tagged CA1 engram cells formed during CFC encoding using a virus cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-ChR2-eYFP (Liu et al., 2012), while simultaneously labeling CA1 cells projecting to dSub or medial EC5 with CTB555. One day after training, we quantified the overlap between recall-induced cFos in CA1 and CA1 engram cells that were retrogradely labeled (Figures 5I–5L). Strikingly, dSub-projecting CA1 engram cells showed higher cFos reactivation following memory retrieval compared to EC5-projecting CA1 engram cells (Figure 5M). Next, following CFC recall, we measured cFos activation levels in the basolateral amygdala (BLA), which plays crucial roles in fear memory encoding and recall (Hall et al., 2001). Terminal inhibition of the dSub→EC5 circuit, but not the dCA1→EC5 circuit, decreased cFos levels in the BLA (Figure 5N), further indicating that the direct and indirect dCA1 output circuits have differential functional roles in memory retrieval.

In Vivo Calcium Imaging of dCA1 and dSub Neurons

We also investigated activation of dSub and dCA1 pyramidal cells in response to training and recall by monitoring in vivo calcium (Ca2+) transients using a miniaturized microendoscope (Kitamura et al., 2015; Sun et al., 2015; Ziv et al., 2013). For this purpose, Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus was injected into the dSub of FN1-Cre mice to specifically express the Ca2+ indicator in dSub cells (Figure 6A, and see Figures S6A–S6B). As expected, expression of GCaMP6f was restricted to dSub, with no expression in CA1 neurons in these mice (Figures 6B, 6D). Similarly for dCA1 neurons, GCaMP6f virus was injected into the dCA1 of dCA1-specific WFS1-Cre mice (Kitamura et al., 2014, and see Methods) (Figures 6C, 6E). With the open field paradigm (Figures S6C–S6G, and see Movies S1S2), CA1 neurons showed homogeneous activation profiles, whereas dSub neurons displayed two types of activation profiles (Geva-Sagiv et al., 2016; Sharp and Green, 1994; Staff et al., 2000; Taube, 1993): short-tail cells whose profiles were similar to those of CA1 cells, and long-tail cells in which the post-stimulation activity persisted as long as 15 s (Figures 6F–6G). Consistent with a previous study (Sharp and Green, 1994), dSub neurons exhibited place fields, which were larger in both types of dSub cells compared to CA1 pyramidal cells (Figure 6H, and see Figure S6H).

Figure 6. dSub neurons exhibit enhanced neuronal activity during hippocampal memory retrieval.

Figure 6

(A–C) Implantation of a microendoscope right above dSub of FN1-Cre mice (A) or dCA1 of WFS1-Cre mice. For dCA1, the medial region along the proximodistal axis was targeted (see also Figures S6A–S6B). Calcium (Ca2+). Representative sagittal sections of hippocampus from FN1-Cre (B) and WFS1-Cre (C) mice showing GCaMP6f-labeled cells (green) and DAPI staining (blue).

(D, E) Representative maximum intensity projection images, as seen through the microendoscope camera, of dSub neurons expressing GCaMP6f (D) or CA1 neurons expressing GCaMP6f (E) acquired during a 30 min recording session in an open field arena (see Methods).

(F, G) Representative Ca2+ traces of CA1 cells (left, labeled in E) and two types of dSub cells (middle and right, labeled in D) from the open field paradigm (F), and cell type quantification (n = 759 CA1 cells, n = 428 dSub short tail cells, n = 371 dSub long tail cells, n = 4 mice per group; G). See also Figure S6G.

(H) Representative place field Ca2+ events (red dots, left panels) and heat maps (right panels) for CA1 and dSub cells (cell counts in Figure 6G), along with quantification. See also Figure S6H and Methods. ND, not detected.

(I) Ca2+ activity during CFC. Pre-footshock levels (Pre). Percentage of active cells (see Methods) during Pre, training, and recall tests (top), including non-freezing (NF) and freezing (F) epochs (bottom), in CA1 and dSub (n = 550 CA1 cells, n = 429 dSub short tail cells, n = 203 dSub long tail cells, n = 3 CA1 mice, n = 4 dSub mice). Within session NF and F comparisons used paired t tests. Comparisons across sessions used a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests. See also Figures S6I–S6K.

For statistical comparisons, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Next, we investigated Ca2+ activity patterns as mice went through the CFC paradigm (Figure 6I, and see Figures S6I–S6K). CA1 showed an increased percentage of active cells during both training and recall periods compared to the pre-footshock period in the context in which a footshock was subsequently delivered. The dSub neurons showed an increased percentage of active cells during recall compared to the pre-footshock or training periods, and no significant difference of active cell percentages was observed between the latter two periods (top row, Figure 6I). We then divided the training and recall periods into two epochs—non-freezing (NF) and freezing (F)—in order to differentiate an effect of the animal’s movement state (Ranck, 1973) on the proportion of active cells. During training, the proportion of active CA1 cells was greater during F epochs compared to the NF epochs, whereas these proportions were similar during recall. In contrast, the proportions of active dSub cells were greater during recall compared to training regardless whether the mice were in F or NF epochs. We then subdivided active dSub cells into short-tail and long-tail cells, and found that the proportion of active short-tail cells were greater during recall compared to training regardless whether mice were in F or NF epochs. In contrast, the proportion of active long-tail dSub cells was greater specifically during recall-induced F epochs, compared to the other three types of epochs (bottom row, Figure 6I). Together, and consistent with the behavior and cFos activation experiments, these data demonstrate distinct contributions of dCA1 and dSub cells to memory encoding and memory recall, respectively.

DISCUSSION

It has been established that CA1 and Sub serve as the major output structures of the hippocampus (O’Mara, 2006); however, the functional role of Sub in hippocampus-dependent episodic memory has remained elusive. Here, we have shown that optogenetic inhibition of dSub during recall, but not during encoding, impairs behavioral performance in three hippocampal-dependent memory paradigms: CFC, trace fear-conditioning, and conditioned place preference. The activity of dSub neurons is capable of regulating memory recall bidirectionally: its inhibition impairs recall and its activation enhances recall. To our knowledge, this is the first identification of the specific causal role of dSub neurons in episodic memory recall.

Previously, lesions (Morris et al., 1990) as well as optogenetic inhibition (Goshen et al., 2011) showed that in rodents, neuronal activity in the CA1 subfield is necessary for both the encoding and retrieval of long-term memories. In this study, we employed optogenetic inhibition of specific terminals of CA1 cell projections and found that the CA1→dSub circuit is crucial for memory recall but not for encoding, whereas the CA1→EC5 circuit is crucial for memory encoding but not for recall. Supporting this role of the CA1→dSub circuit is the finding that inhibition of the downstream dSub terminals in medial EC5 also impairs memory recall selectively. Together, these data indicate that the hippocampal output pathways are functionally segregated: episodic memory encoding uses primarily the direct dCA1→EC5 circuit, while episodic memory retrieval uses primarily the indirect dCA1→dSub→EC5 circuit. The functional dissociation between these two dCA1 output circuits is especially striking given that a significant proportion of dCA1 neurons projecting to dSub and EC5 are overlapping, and that the overall difference in cFos activation levels between dCA1 neurons projecting to dSub versus EC5 during either training or recall epochs is approximately 2% of all dCA1 cells. Further, it is intriguing that we found that about 20% of dCA1 engram cells, those projecting to EC5, are not reactivated by memory recall and thus do not contribute to this behavioral epoch. What could be the purpose of these dCA1 engram cells? We speculate that these engram cells are the stable holder of the original memory, which are undisturbed by a retrieval process, and contribute to the generation of engrams in downstream regions, such as remote memory engram cells in the prefrontal cortex.

The dSub neurons displayed two types of activation profiles—short-tail cells and long-tail cells. These cells may not correspond to the previously reported non-bursting cells and bursting cells (Geva-Sagiv et al., 2016; Sharp and Green, 1994; Staff et al., 2000; Taube, 1993), because the temporal dynamics of bursting activity measured by previous electrophysiological studies was 10–20 msec, which is different from the long-tail cells identified by calcium transients lasting as long as 6–15 sec. Interestingly, the proportion of active long-tail dSub neurons is greater specifically during recall-induced freezing epochs compared to training-induced freezing epochs. This may be because activation of long-tail cells requires more powerful drive than short-tail cells, and because such a potent drive may be provided only by reactivation of previously formed CA1 engram cells by recall cues (Tonegawa et al., 2015), and not by activation of naïve CA1 cells which occurs during training.

What advantages would the distinct circuits for memory encoding and recall provide? One possible merit may be related to episodic memories with negative valence. Fear memory retrieval by conditioned cues induces not only an instinctive fear response (anxiety, avoidance, freezing, etc.), but also an increase in blood stress hormones that organizes multiple body systems to prepare the animal for a predicted immediate danger (Kelley et al., 2009). While a recent study showed that an area of the rodent’s olfactory cortex plays a key role in the hormonal component of the instinctive fear response to volatile predator odors (Kondoh et al., 2016), neural circuits responsible for triggering both episodic memory retrieval and retrieval-induced stress hormone responses have remained unknown. In this study, we have identified two neural circuits originating from dSub that independently regulate freezing behavior and stress hormone responses to conditioned cues: the dSub→EC5 circuit mediates appropriate freezing behavior during memory retrieval, while the dSub→MB circuit is crucial for memory retrieval-induced stress responses via bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) and the hypothalamic corticotropin hormone-releasing neurons (Herman et al., 1998). The preferential activation of long-tail dSub neurons by recall cues may contribute to a sustained enhancement of hormonal release from these downstream areas (Bourque et al., 1993). It has been known that glucocorticoid hormone synthesis is enhanced during memory consolidation (Roozendaal, 2002). Similarly, the retrieval-induced enhancement of CORT may promote memory reconsolidation triggered by recall. Therefore, the Sub→MB pathway may regulate memory retrieval-based emotions and together with the Sub→EC5 pathway that controls the retrieval-based instinctive fear response, would allow for more flexible actions that improve the animal’s survival during challenging events in nature.

Another possible merit of distinct circuits for encoding and retrieval of memory may be to perform rapid memory updating. When a new salient stimulus (such as footshock) is delivered while a subject is recalling a previously acquired memory, the original memory is known to be modified (or updated) by incorporating the concurrently delivered salient stimulus. The diverging followed by converging CA1→dSub→EC5 and CA1→EC5 circuits seem to be ideal for this mnemonic processing: the content of the previously formed memory is retrieved by dSub and a stimulus transmitted directly from CA1 will be co-delivered to EC5 to make a new association resulting in memory updating. It has previously been suggested that such memory updating takes place in the PECFC paradigm by converting the previously acquired contextual memory to a context-dependent fear memory (Lee, 2010). Our findings, that in the PECFC paradigm, conversion of a contextual memory to a context-dependent fear memory is impaired by either the inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals targeted to the short (8 sec) context recall period, or inhibition of CA1→EC5 terminals targeted to the period when an association of the recalled context memory with footshock takes place, supports the crucial role of dSub in memory updating.

Our study is on hippocampus-dependent episodic (or episodic-like) memories, which involves information processing by the hippocampus and other medial temporal lobe structures. Additional work is required to examine whether distinct circuits for encoding and retrieval is a property shared by brain regions responsible for the formation of non-episodic memories, which would involve structures other than the temporal lobe. In this context, it is interesting that a recent study with a worm (Caenorhabditis elegans) showed that aversive long-term memory formation and retrieval are carried out by distinct neural circuits (Jin et al., 2016). Therefore, it is possible that distinct circuits for long-term memory formation versus retrieval may be an evolutionarily conserved feature in many species that are capable of learning. With regards to cognitive disorders, it is widely believed that subiculum is among the earliest brain regions affected in Alzheimer’s disease patients (Hyman et al., 1984). Our findings contribute to a better understanding of neural mechanisms underlying episodic memory formation and may provide insights into pathological conditions affecting memory retrieval.

CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING

Further information and requests for reagents may be directed and will be fulfilled by the corresponding author Susumu Tonegawa (tonegawamit.edu).

EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS

Animals

The C57BL/6J wild type male mice were obtained from Jackson Laboratory. For optogenetic behavioral manipulations of dCA1 neuronal terminals, we used the previously described CA1-specific TRPC4-Cre transgenic mouse line (Okuyama et al., 2016). For in vivo Ca2+ imaging of dCA1 neurons, we used the previously described dCA1-specific WFS1-Cre transgenic mouse line (Kitamura et al., 2014). WFS1-Cre mice were used for dCA1-specific Ca2+ imaging experiments due to their lower levels of transgene expression compared to TRPC4-Cre mice, which was crucial for stable long-term recordings. All transgenic mouse lines were maintained as hemizygotes. Mice had access to food and water ad libitum and were socially housed in numbers of two to five littermates until surgery. Following surgery, mice were singly housed. For behavioral experiments, all mice were male and 3–5 months old. For virus-mediated activity-dependent labeling experiments (Roy et al., 2016), male mice had been raised on food containing 40 mg kg−1 doxycycline (DOX) for at least one week before surgery, and remained on DOX for the remainder of the experiments except for 24 hr preceding the target labeling day. For CLARITY and in vivo Ca2+ imaging experiments, male mice were 4–6 months old at the time of surgery. All experiments were conducted in accordance with U.S. National Institutes of Health (NIH) guidelines and the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Department of Comparative Medicine and Committee of Animal Care.

Generation of FN1-Cre mice

Bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) recombineering was carried out using the EL250 bacterial strain, provided by Dr. Neal Copeland at the Houston Methodist Research Institute. BAC clone RP24-211L16 containing the whole FN1 (NM_001276408.1, Mus musculus fibronectin 1) was obtained from Invitrogen and transferred into EL250. To introduce the Cre sequence in-frame following the first exon of FN1, a BAC modifying cassette was prepared: a 5’ homology arm, Cre, the kanamycin resistance gene flanked by FRTs, and a 3’ homology arm. The modifying cassette was electroporated into EL250 carrying the BAC clone. Resistant clones were selected and confirmed for appropriate homologous recombination, after which kanamycin was removed using the site-specific recombinase FLP. Purified DNA from the selected clone containing the modified BAC was digested with NotI, which cut both ends of the insert. Insert DNA was purified (1 ng/µl) and microinjected into C57BL/6JCrlj male pronuclei of fertilized eggs. Two-cell stage embryos were transferred to pseudopregnant recipient female mice. The C57BL/6J-Tg(FN1-Cre)41(RBRC03020) transgenic mice were established and maintained in the same background. Cre mRNA expression was visualized by in situ hybridization as previously described (Okuyama et al., 2016).

METHOD DETAILS

Viral constructs

The AAV9-EF1α-DIO-eYFP, AAV9-EF1α-DIO-ChR2-eYFP, and AAV9-EF1α-DIO-eArch3.0-eYFP viruses were acquired from the University of North Carolina (UNC) at Chapel Hill Vector Core. The AAV9-EF1α-DIO-mCherry, AAV9-EF1α-DIO-eArch3.0-mCherry, and AAV9-EF1α-eArch3.0-eYFP viruses were acquired from Vector BioLabs. The AAV2-Syn-DIO-GCaMP6f and AAV5-Syn-DIO-GCaMP6f viruses were acquired from the University of Pennsylvania Vector Core. The AAV8-CMV-DIO-Synaptophysin-mCherry construct was provided by Dr. Rachael Neve at the MIT Viral Gene Transfer Core and packaged at the University of Massachusetts Medical School Gene Therapy Center and Vector Core. The pAAV-EF1α-DIO-H2B-GFP plasmid was constructed by cloning the histone H2B gene into a pAAV-EF1α-DIO-GFP backbone, which was serotyped with AAV9 coat proteins and packaged at Vigene Biosciences. The c-Fos-tTA (Roy et al., 2016) and TRE-H2B-GFP (Okuyama et al., 2016) vectors were serotyped with AAV9 and AAV5 coat proteins respectively, and packaged at the University of Massachusetts Medical School Gene Therapy Center and Vector Core. The TRE-ChR2-eYFP (Liu et al., 2012) vector was serotyped with AAV9 coat proteins and packaged at Vigene Biosciences. We used our previously established method (Roy et al., 2016) for labeling memory engram cells using a virus cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-H2B-GFP or c-Fos-tTA and TRE-ChR2-eYFP (Figure 5J). Viral titers were 1.2 × 1013 genome copy (GC) ml−1 for AAV9-EF1α-DIO-eYFP, 4 × 1012 GC ml−1 for AAV9-EF1α-DIO-ChR2-eYFP, 1.6 × 1013 GC ml−1 for AAV9-EF1α-DIO-eArch3.0-eYFP, 3 × 1013 GC ml−1 for AAV9-EF1α-DIO-mCherry and AAV9-EF1α-DIO-eArch3.0-mCherry, 1 × 1011 GC ml−1 for AAV9-EF1α-eArch3.0-eYFP, 4 × 1012 GC ml−1 for AAV2-Syn-DIO-GCaMP6f and AAV5-Syn-DIO-GCaMP6f, 1.1 × 1013 GC ml−1 for AAV8-CMV-DIO-Synaptophysin-mCherry, 2 × 1014 GC ml−1 for AAV9-EF1α-DIO-H2B-GFP, 4 × 1013 GC ml−1 for AAV9-c-Fos-tTA, 1.4 × 1013 GC ml−1 for AAV5-TRE-H2B-GFP, and 1.5 × 1013 GC ml−1 for AAV9-TRE-ChR2-eYFP.

Surgery and optic fiber implants

Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane or 500 mg kg−1 avertin for stereotaxic injections. Injections were targeted bilaterally to the dCA1 (−2.1 mm AP, +/− 1.5 mm ML, −1.4 mm DV), dSub (−3.08 mm AP, +/− 1.5 mm ML, −1.5 mm DV), vSub (−4.2 mm AP, +/− 3.25 ML, −4.0 mm DV), medial EC5 (−4.63 mm AP, +/− 3.36 mm ML, −2.55 mm DV), MB (−2.8 mm AP, +/− 0.35 mm ML, −4.9 mm DV), and lateral EC5 (−3.40 mm AP, +/− 4.0 mm ML, −4.30 mm DV). Injection volumes were 400 nl for dCA1, 200 nl for dSub and vSub, 300 nl for medial EC5, 300 nl for MB, and 400 nl for lateral EC5. Viruses were injected at 70 nl min−1 using a glass micropipette attached to a 10 ml Hamilton microsyringe. The needle was lowered to the target site and remained for 5 min before beginning the injection. After the injection, the needle stayed for 10 min before it was withdrawn. Custom dSub and MB implants containing two optic fibers (200 mm core diameter; Doric Lenses) was lowered above the injection site (dSub: −3.08 mm AP, +/− 1.5 mm ML, −1.2 mm DV; MB: −2.8 mm AP, +/− 0.35 mm ML, −4.8 mm DV). Single optic fiber implants (200 mm core diameter; Doric Lenses) were lowered above the EC5 injection sites (−4.8 mm AP, +/− 3.36 mm ML, −2.20 mm DV). The implant was secured to the skull with two jewelry screws, adhesive cement (C&B Metabond) and dental cement. An opaque cap derived from the top part of an Eppendorf tube protected the implant. Mice were given 1.5 mg kg−1 metacam as analgesic and allowed to recover for 2 weeks before behavioral experiments. All injection sites were verified histologically. As criteria, we only included mice with virus expression limited to the targeted regions.

Retrograde neuronal tracing

Cholera toxin subunit B

To characterize neuronal populations in dSub and dCA1 based on downstream projection targets, we used cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) conjugated to Alexa-488 or Alexa-555 diluted in phosphate buffered saline (PBS) solution at a final concentration of 1% wt vol−1. Diluted CTB was aliquoted and stored at −20°C. For tracing experiments, 50–200 nl CTB was unilaterally injected into target sites. Six days after injections, mice were perfused for histology and imaging.

Rabies virus

To identify major inputs to dSub Cre+ neurons, we used a monosynaptic retrograde tracing approach via a Cre-dependent helper virus combined with rabies virus (RV) technology. The first component was an AAV vector that allowed simultaneous expression of three genes: TVA, eGFP, and RV glycoprotein (G). Briefly, this vector was constructed by deleting the sequence between the inverse terminal repeats of pAAV-MCS (Stratagene), and replacing it with a cassette containing the following: human synapsin-1 promoter (Syn, Genbank NG_008437); the Kozak sequence; a FLEX cassette containing the transmembrane isoform of TVA (lacking a start codon), eGFP, and G separated by the highly efficient porcine teschovirus self-cleaving 2A element; the woodchuck post-transcriptional regulatory element (WPRE) and a bovine growth hormone polyadenylation site. This vector was termed pAAV-synP-FLEX-sTpEpB (i.e., the helper virus), serotyped with AAVrh8 coat proteins, and packaged at the University of Pennsylvania Vector Core. The second component was a deletion-mutant RV produced by replacing the eGFP gene in cSPBN-4GFP with the mCherry gene (i.e., the RVΔG-mCherry virus, also known as the rabies virus), which was packaged with the ASLV-A envelope protein. For tracing experiments, 50 nl of the Cre-dependent helper virus was unilaterally injected into dSub of FN1-Cre mice. One week later, 50 nl of RVΔG-mCherry virus was unilaterally injected into the same dSub. Six days after the second viral injection, mice were perfused for histology and imaging.

Immunohistochemistry

Mice were dispatched using 750–1000 mg kg−1 avertin and transcardially perfused with PBS, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains were extracted and incubated in 4% PFA at room temperature overnight. Brains were transferred to PBS and 50 µm coronal slices were prepared using a vibratome. For immunostaining, each slice was placed in PBS + 0.2% Triton X-100 (PBS-T), with 5% normal goat serum for 1 hr and then incubated with primary antibody at 4°C for 24 hr. Slices then underwent three wash steps for 10 min each in PBS-T, followed by a 2 hr incubation with secondary antibody. After three more wash steps of 10 min each in PBS-T, slices were mounted on microscope slides. All analyses were performed blind to the experimental conditions. Antibodies used for staining were as follows: CA1-specific excitatory neurons were stained with rabbit anti-WFS1 (1:400, Proteintech) and anti-rabbit Alexa-555 (1:500), excitatory neurons were stained with mouse anti-CaMKII (1:200, Abcam) and anti-mouse Alexa-555 (1:300), inhibitory neurons were stained with mouse anti-GAD67 (1:500, Millipore) and anti-mouse Alexa-555 (1:300), nuclei were stained with DAPI (1:3000, Sigma), neuronal nuclei were stained with mouse anti-NeuN (1:200, Millipore) and anti-mouse Alexa-555 (1:300), parvalbumin inhibitory neurons were stained with mouse anti-PV (1:500, Swant) and anti-mouse Alexa-555 (1:300), Wisteria floribunda lectin was stained with biotinylated WFA lectin (1:3000, Vector Labs) and streptavidin Alexa-555 (1:200), calbindin was stained with mouse anti-CALB (1:500, Swant) and anti-mouse Alexa-555 (1:300), vesicular glutamate transporter 1 was stained with rabbit anti-VGLUT1 (1:1000, Synaptic Systems) and anti-rabbit Alexa-488 (1:300), vesicular glutamate transporter 2 was stained with rabbit anti-VGLUT2 (1:500, Synaptic Systems) and anti-rabbit Alexa-488 (1:300), vesicular glutamate transporter 3 was stained with guinea pig anti-VGLUT3 (1:1000, Millipore) and anti-guinea pig Alexa-488 (1:500), myelin basic protein (MBP) was stained with rabbit anti-MBP (1:1000, Abcam) and anti-rabbit Alexa-546, cFos was stained with rabbit anti-cFos (1:400, Santa Cruz Biotechnology) and anti-rabbit Alexa-488, and TRE-ChR2-eYFP (Figure 5J) was stained with chicken anti-GFP (1:1000, Life Technologies) and anti-chicken Alexa-633.

CLARITY

Mice were anesthetized with 750–1000 mg kg−1 avertin and transcardially perfused with a hydrogel monomer solution containing 4% acrylamide, 0.05% bis-acrylamide, 0.25% VA-044 initiator, and 4% PFA in PBS. Brains were incubated in the monomer solution at 4°C for 48–72 hr. For hydrogel tissue embedding, the brains were degassed in a desiccation chamber replacing all the gas with nitrogen, after which polymerization was initiated by raising the solution temperature to 37°C for 1 hr. Brains were extracted from the hydrogel, and washed in clearing solution containing 200 mM boric acid, 4% sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS), and sodium hydroxide (pH 8.5) at 37°C for 24 hr. Brains were placed in a custom-built electrophoretic tissue-clearing (ETC) chamber, as previously described (Chung et al., 2013). Clearing solution was circulated through the ETC chamber at 37°C for 3 days along with 15V application across the brain sample. After clearing, brains were washed twice for 24 hr each in PBS-T at room temperature. Before imaging, brains were incubated in FocusClear solution for 2 days at room temperature to achieve the optimal refractive index of 1.45. Whole brain fluorescence z-stacks were acquired using a light sheet fluorescence microscope (5×). Stitching and high resolution rendering of z-stacks was performed using an Arivis Vision4D software package at the Harvard Center for Biological Imaging (HCBI).

Cell counting

To quantify the number of neurons in each brain region projecting to dSub Cre+ neurons, rabies virus (RV)-mCherry+ neurons in each target structure were counted from 4–5 sagittal slices per mouse (n = 4 mice). To quantify the number of dCA1 neurons projecting to lateral EC5, CTB555+ neurons were counted from 5–6 coronal slices per mouse (n = 3 mice). Fluorescence images were acquired using a Zeiss AxioImager.Z1/ApoTome microscope (10, 20, or 40×). Automated cell counting analysis was performed using ImageJ software. DAPI+ counts were approximated from 5 sagittal slices using ImageJ. Percentage of neurons in each brain region projecting to dSub was calculated as ((mCherry+) / (Total DAPI+)) × 100. To characterize dSub Cre+ eYFP neurons, the overlap between eYFP and several molecular markers (labeled with mCherry-tagged antibodies) were examined. The number of eYFP+, mCherry+, and eYFP+ mCherry+ neurons were counted from 4–5 sagittal slices per mouse (n = 3 mice per group). Percentage of dSub Cre+ neurons expressing the different molecular markers was calculated as ((eYFP+ mCherry+) / (Total eYFP+)) × 100. A similar quantification strategy was used to examine the overlap of cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) 488 and 555 from different downstream brain regions in dSub and dCA1, as well as the overlap of RV-mCherry and CTB488 from different downstream brain regions in dCA1. For these retrograde tracing experiments, the percentage of overlap was calculated as ((eYFP+ mCherry+) / (Total DAPI+)) × 100. Chance overlap calculated as ((eYFP+ / Total DAPI+) × (mCherry+ / Total DAPI+)) × 100, where eYFP+ and mCherry+ represents the total population of cells labeled by eYFP and mCherry, respectively. For activity-dependent labeling experiments using cFos (as TRE-H2B-GFP signal) or cFos staining, cFos+ neurons were counted from 5–7 sagittal or coronal slices per mouse (n = 5–6 mice). The cell body layers of dCA1, dSub, EC5, MB, or basolateral amygdala (BLA) were outlined as regions of interest (ROIs) and the percentage of cFos+ neurons were calculated as ((cFos+) / (Total DAPI+)) × 100. For cFos+ CTB555+ neurons in dCA1, percentage overlap was calculated as ((cFos+ CTB555+) / (Total DAPI+)) × 100. Chance overlap calculated as ((cFos+ / Total DAPI+) × (CTB555+ / Total DAPI+)) × 100, where cFos+ and CTB555+ represents the total population of cells labeled by cFos and CTB555, respectively. For cFos+ ChR2+ CTB555+ neurons in dCA1 (Figure 5M), percentage overlap was calculated as ((cFos+ ChR2+ CTB555+) / (Total DAPI+)) × 100. The numbers of fluorophore-positive cells per section were quantified after applying a threshold above background fluorescence. Data were analyzed using Microsoft Excel with the Statplus plug-in, or Prism 6 software. All counting experiments were conducted blind to experimental group. Researcher 1 trained the animals, prepared slices and randomized images, while Researcher 2 performed semi-automated cell counting. Statistical comparisons were performed using unpaired t tests, one-sample t tests, and Fisher’s exact tests: *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

In vivo calcium imaging

Microendoscope surgery

We used our previously established method (Kitamura et al., 2015; Sun et al., 2015) for microendoscope surgeries. Briefly, for Ca2+ imaging experiments, unilateral injections were targeted to the right dSub of FN1-Cre mice or the right dCA1 of WFS1-Cre mice. Mice were injected with AAV2-Syn-DIO-GCaMP6f (FN1-Cre) or AAV5-Syn-DIO-GCaMP6f (WFS1-Cre). One month after AAV injection, we implanted a microendoscope lens (1 mm diameter) above the dorsal region of CA1 (−2.0 mm AP, +/− 1.5 mm ML, −1.2 mm DV), specifically targeting the medial region along the proximodistal axis, or the dorsal region of Sub (−3.1 mm AP, +/− 1.5 mm ML, −1.0 mm DV). These microendoscope lenses have a working distance of 0.3 mm (Inscopix, Inc.). One month later, the baseplate for a miniaturized microscope camera (Ziv et al., 2013) was attached above the implanted microendoscope. Following baseplate surgeries, mice were habituated to investigator handling and the attachment of a microscope camera for 2 weeks.

Imaging during open field and CFC

Mice were housed in a reverse light cycle room (dark period: 9 am to 9 pm). This is different from the light cycle room for the optogenetic behavior experiments (see below). This was necessary in order to maximize the animal’s movement during the open field experiments for best coverage of the arena, which was crucial to examine spatial information and place field properties. All Ca2+ imaging experiments were performed during the dark cycle. Open field tests were conducted using a 50 × 50 cm white plastic platform, which lacked walls and was raised by 15 cm above the table. Under dim light conditions, Ca2+ activity in the open field arena was collected for 30 min per mouse in order to obtain sufficient numbers of transients for adequately powered statistical analyses. Contextual fear conditioning (CFC) tests were conducted using a 29 × 25 × 22 cm chamber with grid floors, dim white lighting, and scented with 1% acetic acid. Mice were conditioned (300 s exploration, one 0.75 mA shock of 2 s duration at 300 s, 120 s post-shock period). One day later, mice were returned to the conditioned chamber (7 min) to assess memory recall-induced freezing behavior. Before and between runs in the open field and CFC paradigms, the experimental apparatus was cleaned with quatricide. Mouse behavior, specifically position tracking and freezing epochs, was recorded using an automated infrared (IR) detection system (EthoVision XT, Noldus). As we previously described (Kitamura et al., 2015; Sun et al., 2015), Ca2+ events were captured at 20 Hz on an Inscopix miniature microscope.

Image processing and cell identification

We used our previously established method (Sun et al., 2015) for image processing and single cell identification analyses. Briefly, Ca2+ imaging movies were motion corrected using Inscopix Mosaic software: translation and rotation; reference region with spatial mean (r = 20 pixels) subtracted, inverted, and spatial mean (r = 5 pixels) applied. Using ImageJ software, each image was divided one pixel at a time by a low pass (r = 20 pixels) filtered version, after which the ΔF/F signal was calculated. Two hundred cell regions of interest (ROIs) were carefully selected from the resulting movie by PCA-ICA method (300 output PCs, 200 ICs, 0.1 weight of temporal information in spatio-temporal ICA, 750 iterations maximum, 1 × 10−5 fractional change to end iterations) in Inscopix Mosaic software, and the independent components (ICs) were binarized using a threshold of 0.5 of the maximum intensity. Non-circular ROIs (i.e., if its length exceeded its width by greater than 2 times) were not included in the analysis. Ca2+ events were detected by applying a threshold (greater than 2 standard deviations from the ΔF/F signal) at the local maxima of the ΔF/F signal. Since we employed GCaMP6f, our analysis used a threshold of >=5 frames (~250 ms). Events that occurred within 250 ms of a previous Ca2+ event were excluded from the analysis. For ICs that satisfied all criteria, Ca2+ traces during behavior were computed. For open field, using this high resolution spatial map, the distribution of Ca2+ events were calculated, while event rate heat maps were calculated by binning the behavioral arena into 50 × 50 array bins, each of which covered a 2 × 2 cm area. Smoothed rate heat maps were constructed with each pixel boxcar averaged over the surrounding 5 × 5 pixel area using a Gaussian smoothing kernel (σ = 2 pixels). For CFC, 8–15 s epochs during pre-footshock periods (Pre), and non-freezing (NF) and freezing (F) periods of training and recall were examined. The total duration examined within each test session was held constant across mice. For the NF and F epoch analysis, the total time was held constant across each of the test sessions, which was necessary in order to make meaningful comparisons between these epochs. On average, 10–20 epochs per mouse were analyzed. Active cells (Figure 6I) were defined as those exhibiting at least 15 significant Ca2+ events during a given recording session. We confirmed that these active cell results show robustness to change around this value (15) in the range of 10–18 significant Ca2+ events.

Spatial information, place field size, and sparsity

We used our previously established method (Sun et al., 2015) for these analyses. Briefly, the behavior position tracking data was sorted into 5 × 5 cm spatial bins. Ca2+ event rate per spatial bin was calculated for all dCA1 and dSub cells. Individual spatial bins were accepted if their event rate exceeded 0.2 Hz. Bins that had mouse occupancy duration less than 100 ms were not included in the analysis. Without smoothing, the spatial information rate in bits per second was calculated for each cell according to (Skaggs et al., 1992):

information rate=ipiλi log2λiλ

where pi is the probability of the mouse occupying the i-th bin for all i, λi is the mean Ca2+ event rate in the i-th bin, and λ is the overall Ca2+ event rate. Cells with significant spatial information were identified as those above the 95th percentile of all shuffles (all cell event times were shuffled 100 times, for a total of approximately 20,000 shuffles per mouse). To identify a place field, the criterion was at least 4 contiguous spatial bins (16 cm2). For each place cell, only the largest place field was considered for the place field size analysis. Single cell sparsity is defined as <R>2 / <R>2, where R is the calcium activity rate in a particular spatial bin, and **< > denotes the average value over all spatial bins. A sparsity of 0.15 indicates that the cell is active in 15% of the open field arena (Treves and Rolls, 1992).

Classification of short and long tail cells

For each dCA1 and dSub cell, Ca2+ events that were greater than 4 standard deviations were included for this analysis. For each cell and each Ca2+ event, the total time to decay to 0.33 the maximum value was defined as the event width. Cells that had an average decay time of less than 3.5 s were categorized as short tail cells, while cells that had an average decay time of greater than 3.5 s were categorized as long tail cells. We confirmed that these cell type classification results show robustness to change around this value (3.5 s) in the range of 3.0–4.0 s. The distributions of individual calcium transient durations for the entire population of dCA1 and dSub cells were plotted (the x-axes used a logarithmic scale for optimal visualization of the data). A line fit was included in each population distribution (black lines). The cell type classification results are supported by the fact that dSub calcium transient durations showed a bimodal distribution (Figure S6C).

Corticosterone assay

To measure stress responses following behavior, we used a CORT enzyme-linked immunosorbant assay (ELISA; Enzo Life Sciences). One hour after training or testing sessions, we collected trunk whole blood. For sample collection following optogenetic manipulations (Figure 3G), ChR2 activation (40 min blue light on) and eArch inhibition (10 min green light on, 2 min green light off, repeated for 40 min) was carried out immediately after CFC training or recall. Specifically, once mice were removed from the CFC training context, they were returned to their home cages, optic fibers were attached, and optogenetic manipulations were initiated. Details of the laser light sources and power are provided in the optogenetic manipulations section (see below). Samples remained on ice until centrifugation (2000 × g, for 10 min) to isolate blood plasma. Plasma samples were aliquoted and stored at −80°C. 1–3 days after plasma collection, ELISA assays were performed.

Behavior assays

Experiments were conducted during the light cycle (7 am to 7 pm). Mice were randomly assigned to experimental groups for specific behavioral assays immediately after surgery. Mice were habituated to investigator handling for 1–2 minutes on three consecutive days. Handling took place in the holding room where the mice were housed. Prior to each handling session, mice were transported by wheeled cart to and from the vicinity of the behavior rooms to habituate them to the journey. For natural memory recall sessions, data were quantified using FreezeFrame software. Optogenetic manipulations interfered with motion detection, and therefore freezing behavior in these experiments were manually quantified. All behavior experiments were analyzed blind to experimental group. Unpaired student’s t-tests were used for independent group comparisons, with Welch’s correction when group variances were significantly different, or two-way ANOVAs followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests were used. Given behavioral variability, initial assays were performed using a minimum of 10 mice per group to ensure adequate power for any observed differences. Following behavioral protocols, brain sections were prepared to confirm efficient viral labeling in target areas. Animals lacking adequate labeling were excluded prior to behavior quantification.

Contextual fear-conditioning

Two distinct contexts were employed (Roy et al., 2016). The conditioning context were 29 × 25 × 22 cm chambers with grid floors, dim white lighting, and scented with 0.25% benzaldehyde. The neutral context consisted of 30 × 25 × 33 cm chambers with white perspex floors, red lighting, and scented with 1% acetic acid. All mice were conditioned (180 s exploration, one 0.75 mA shock of 2 s duration at 180 s, 120 s post-shock period), and tested (3 min) one day later. Experiments showed no generalization in the neutral context. Floors of chambers were cleaned with quatricide before and between runs. Mice were transported to and from the experimental room in their home cages using a wheeled cart. The cart and cages remained in an anteroom to the experimental rooms during all behavioral experiments. For activity-dependent labeling using cFos (as H2B-GFP signal), mice were kept on regular food without DOX for 24 hours prior to training or recall. When training or recall was complete, mice were switched back to food containing 40 mg kg−1 DOX.

Trace and delay fear-conditioning

The conditioning context were 29 × 25 × 22 cm chambers with grid floors, bright white lighting, and scented with 1% acetic acid. The recall test context consisted of 30 × 25 × 33 cm chambers with white perspex floors, red lighting, and scented with 0.25% benzaldehyde. For trace fear-conditioning, mice were conditioned (240 s exploration, 20 s tone, 20 s trace period, a 0.75 mA shock of 2 s duration at 280 s, 60 s post-shock period, repeated 3 more times). For delay fear-conditioning, mice were conditioned (240 s exploration, 20 s tone co-terminating with a 0.75 mA shock of 2 s duration, 60 s post-shock period, repeated 3 more time). For both paradigms, memory recall was tested (14 min; 2 min exploration, 60 s tone, 120 s post-tone period, repeated 3 more times) one day later. The tone was calibrated to 75 dB SPL, with a frequency of 2 kHz. Experiments showed no generalization in the recall test context.

Conditioned place preference

The conditioned place preference (CPP) behavior chamber was a rectangular arena (40 × 15 cm), divided into three quadrants (left, middle, right). The left and right quadrants were 15 cm long, while the middle quadrant was 10 cm long. The left quadrant had grid floors and a pattern (series of parallel lines) on the wall. The right quadrant had smooth polypropylene floors and a pattern (series of circles) on the wall. On day 1 (pre-exposure), mice were allowed to explore the entire arena for 30 min. Experiments showed no preference to any one quadrant. On day 2 (training), mice were confined to the left or right quadrants for 20 min following cocaine (20 mg kg−1) or saline intraperitoneal administration. On days 3–7 (training continued), mice were conditioned in opposite quadrants in an alternating manner (i.e., cocaine left-saline right-cocaine left, etc.) until every mouse received 3 cocaine and 3 saline pairings. For every behavioral cohort, half the mice were conditioned with cocaine in the left quadrant, while the remaining mice received cocaine in the right quadrant. On day 8, memory recall was measured by preference to the left or right quadrant (10 min). All sessions were performed with dim white lighting. Mouse behavior, specifically position tracking and duration, was recorded using an automated infrared (IR) detection system (EthoVision XT, Noldus). The tracking software plotted heat maps for each mouse, which was averaged to create representative heat maps for each group. Raw data was extracted and analyzed using Microsoft Excel.

Memory updating

We employed the pre-exposure mediated contextual fear-conditioning (PECFC) paradigm to examine memory updating. The behavior context were 29 × 25 × 22 cm chambers with grid floors, dim white lighting, and scented with 0.25% benzaldehyde. On day 1, mice were allowed to explore the context for 6 min. On day 2, mice were conditioned (8 s exploration, one 0.75 mA shock of 2 s duration at 8 s, no post-shock period), and tested for 3 min one day later (day 3).

Open field assay

Spontaneous motor activity was measured in an open field arena (52 × 26 cm) for 10 min. Mice were transferred to the testing room and acclimated for 30 min before the test session. During the testing period, lighting in the room was turned off. The apparatus was cleaned with quatricide before and between runs. Total movement (distance traveled and velocity) in the arena was quantified using an automated infrared (IR) detection system (EthoVision XT, Noldus). The tracking software plotted heat maps for each mouse, which was averaged to create representative heat maps for each group. Raw data were extracted and analyzed using Microsoft Excel.

Optogenetic manipulations

All behavioral paradigms were performed as described above. For experiments that included optogenetic manipulations, the behavior chamber ceilings were customized to hold a rotary joint (Doric Lenses) connected to two 0.32 m optic fibers. All mice had optic fibers attached to their optic fiber implants prior to training and recall tests. For ChR2 experiments, dSub terminals were stimulated at 20 Hz (15 ms pulse width) using a 473 nm laser (10–15 mW, blue light), for the entire duration (3 min) of CFC recall tests or neutral context tests (Figures 3D, S4A). For eArch experiments, dSub cell bodies/terminals, vSub terminals, and dCA1 terminals were inhibited using a 561 nm laser (10 mW, constant green light). For CFC experiments (Figures 3A, 3B, 3C, 4I, 4J, 4K, 4L; Figures S2, S3), green light inhibition was performed during the entire duration (3 min) of training or recall tests. For TFC and DFC training experiments (Figures S4B, S4D), green light inhibition was performed during the entire duration from tone initiation through shock delivery (for all tone-shock pairings). The following day, tone-induced memory recall was tested without green light. For TFC and DFC recall experiments (Figures 3E, S4C, S4E), green light inhibition was performed during the first and third tones (each tone is 60 s) in half the mice, while the remaining mice received inhibition during the second and fourth tones. This reflects a counter-balanced experimental design. For CPP training experiments (Figure S4F), green light inhibition was performed during days 2–7, for the entire 20 min session per day (9 min green light on, 1 min green light off, 10 min green light on). For CPP recall experiments (Figures 3F, S4G), green light inhibition was performed during the entire duration (10 min) of recall tests on day 8. For memory updating experiments (Figures 4M, 4N), green light inhibition was performed during the entire pre-footshock periods (first 8 s on day 2, left panels) or during the footshock periods alone (last 2 s on day 2, right panels). For open field experiments (Figure S2M), green light inhibition was performed during the entire duration (10 min).

QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

Data are presented as mean values accompanied by SEM. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample sizes. Data analysis was performed blind to the conditions of the experiments. Data were analyzed using Microsoft Excel with the Statplus plug-in and Prism 6 software. Two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post-hoc test, unpaired t test, Fisher’s exact test, one-sample t test, and paired t test were used to test for statistical significance when appropriate. Statistical parameters including the exact value of n, precision measures (mean ± SEM), and statistical significance are reported in the figure legends. The significance threshold was placed at α = 0.05 (NS, P > 0.05; *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; ***, P < 0.001).

Supplementary Material

Figure S1. Figure S1. Characterization of FN1-Cre mice, Related to Figures 12.

(A) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent virus expressing eYFP. Percentage of eYFP+ cells in dSub expressing various molecular markers (n = 3 mice per group). Cre+ dSub neurons expressed the neuronal nuclei (NeuN) protein and the excitatory neuronal marker CaMKII (see Figure 1C). Cre+ dSub neurons did not overlap with CA1 excitatory neurons labeled with WFS1 (see Figure 1B), parvalbumin (PV) inhibitory neurons, the inhibitory neuronal marker GAD67 (see Figure 1D), Wisteria floribunda (WFA) lectin, or the granule cell marker calbindin. ND, not detected.

(B–D) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent histone H2B-GFP virus (see Methods viral constructs section; B). Representative medial sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue, C) and H2B-GFP labeling in dSub (green, D). Dashed white line (C, D) denotes CA1/dSub border.

(E–H) Representative lateral sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue, E) and H2B-GFP labeling (green, F) in dSub, but not vSub. Higher magnification images of boxed regions in F (G–H).

(I–K) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a cocktail of Cre-dependent eYFP virus and CTB into EC5. Representative lateral sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue, I), CTB555 (red, J), and eYFP labeling (green, K). CTB555 signal reflects the injection site. Cre-dependent eYFP labeling was observed in dSub, but not MEC.

(L) Cre mRNA expression in FN1-Cre mice by in situ hybridization (ISH) showing clear signals in dSub, and the dorsal tegmental nucleus (DTg) in the brain stem. Anterior to posterior (AP, in millimeters relative to Bregma) coronal sections.

(M) Representative sagittal sections showing brain regions projecting to dSub Cre+ neurons (see Figure 2D), including thalamic nuclei (Thal Nucl), nucleus accumbens shell (Acb Sh), and retrosplenial agranular cortex (RSA). Rabies virus-positive neurons (red), DAPI staining (blue). White arrows indicate multiple thalamic nuclei containing rabies virus-positive neurons. Higher magnification image of boxed thalamic nuclei region (second image from left). Medial to lateral (ML, in millimeters relative to Bregma).

(N–P) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent synaptophysin (SYP) virus to label dSub axonal terminals. Reflecting the excitatory nature of these dSub Cre+ neurons, SYP labeling (red) overlapped with vesicular glutamate transporters 1 (VGLUT1 in green; N) and 2 (VGLUT2 in green; O). dSub neurons do not express VGLUT3 (green; P), which mainly occurs in non-glutamatergic neurons. White arrows indicate axonal terminals originating from dSub Cre+ neurons that express VGLUT1 (N) or VGLUT2 (O). Representative 40× sagittal confocal images.

(Q) CTB injection sites. Representative sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue) and CTB555 labeling (red). Small volume (50 nl) injections targeting MB (left panel), EC5 (middle panel), or dSub (right panel). Dashed white line (right panel) denotes CA1/dSub border.

Medial to lateral (ML, in millimeters relative to Bregma) coordinates. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S2. Figure S2. Optogenetic inhibition using eArch decreased memory recall-induced cFos expression in dSub cell bodies as well as terminals, Related to Figure 3.

(A–D) FN1-Cre mice were injected in dSub with a Cre-dependent eArch3.0-mCherry or mCherry alone virus (A–B). Dashed white line (A, B) denotes CA1/dSub border. To measure cFos levels, a virus cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-H2B-GFP viruses were injected into dSub (see Methods; C). Representative cFos expression in dSub cell bodies from mCherry mice (C, left panel) and eArch3.0-mCherry mice (C, right panel). During CFC memory recall, optogenetic inhibition of dSub neurons decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons (n = 5 mice per group; D).

(E–H) Axonal terminals originating from dSub Cre+ neurons observed in MB (outlined by the white dashed line; E–F). Representative cFos expression in dSub→MB terminals from mCherry mice (G, left panel) and eArch3.0-mCherry mice (G, right panel). Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in MB during CFC memory recall decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons (n = 6 mice per group; H).

(I–L) Axonal terminals originating from dSub Cre+ neurons observed in EC5 (outlined by the white dashed line; I–J). Representative cFos expression in dSub→EC5 terminals from mCherry mice (K, left panel) and eArch3.0-mCherry mice (K, right panel). Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in EC5 during CFC memory recall decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons (n = 6 mice per group; L).

DAPI staining in representative sagittal sections (A, E, I), eArch3.0-mCherry labeling (B, F, J), and H2B-GFP labeling (C, G, K).

(M) Open field assay. FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent eArch3.0-eYFP or eYFP alone virus into dSub. Optogenetic inhibition of dSub cell bodies during an open field test. Average heat maps (n = 10 mice per group) showing exploration time of eYFP light ON and eArch light ON groups (left). Distance traveled (centimeters, cm) and velocity (cm/second) (right). NS, not significant.

(N) Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in EC5 during CPP memory recall decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons in EC5 (n = 5 mice per group, left panel). Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in MB following CFC memory recall (for CORT, Figure 3G) decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons in MB (n = 5 mice per group, right panel).

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S3. Figure S3. Optogenetic inhibition of dSub and/or vSub terminals in EC5 during CFC memory recall, using wild type mice, Related to Figure 3.

(A–C) Wild type C57BL/6J mice were injected with an EF1α-eArch3.0-eYFP virus. Behavioral schedule (A). Representative sagittal section showing eArch3.0-eYFP expression (green, B). DAPI staining (blue). Inhibition of dSub terminals in EC5 by green light during CFC memory recall impaired behavioral performance (n = 12 mice; C), while inhibition of vSub terminals in EC5 by green light showed normal levels of CFC memory recall (n = 14 mice). Consistent with these results, inhibition of both dSub and vSub (d+v) terminals in EC5 revealed a memory retrieval deficit (n = 12 mice).

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S4. Figure S4. Control experiments for optogenetic behavior manipulations, Related to Figure 3.

(A) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent ChR2-eYFP virus into dSub. Optogenetic activation of dSub→MB terminals during contextual fear conditioning (CFC) recall had no effect on freezing levels (n = 8 mice per group). NS, not significant.

(B) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent eArch3.0-eYFP virus into dSub. Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during trace fear conditioning (TFC) training had no effect on tone (Tn)-induced freezing levels during recall tests (n = 11 mice per group). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panels), averaged freezing levels (right panels). NS, not significant.

(C) Inhibition of dSub→MB terminals during trace fear conditioning (TFC) recall had no effect on freezing levels (n = 10 mice). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panel), averaged freezing levels during the two light-off tones and the two light-on tones (right panel). NS, not significant.

(D) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during delay fear conditioning (DFC) training had no effect on tone (Tn)-induced freezing levels during recall tests (n = 10 mice per group). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panels), averaged freezing levels (right panels). NS, not significant.

(E) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during delay fear conditioning (DFC) recall had no effect on tone (Tn)-induced freezing levels (n = 11 mice). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panels), averaged freezing levels during the two light-off tones and the two light-on tones (right panels). NS, not significant.

(F) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during cocaine-induced conditioned place preference (CPP) training had no effect on recall tests (n = 13 mice per group). Behavioral schedule (top part). Pre-exposure preference duration (bottom, left graph) and recall preference duration (bottom, right graph). Saline (S or Sal), cocaine (C or Coc).

(G) Inhibition of dSub→MB terminals during CPP recall had no effect (n = 11 mice per group). Behavioral schedule (top part). Pre-exposure preference duration (bottom, left graph) and recall preference duration (bottom, right graph). Saline (S or Sal), cocaine (C or Coc).

(H) CORT levels following CPP memory recall tests were not significantly different between the saline (Sal) and cocaine (Coc) treated animals (n = 7 mice per group). NS, not significant.

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S5. Figure S5. Retrograde tracing and cFos-CTB overlap experiments, Related to Figures 45.

(A–H) To quantify the chance level at which retrograde tracing using CTB results in non-overlapping neuronal populations in the upstream target structure, CTB555 and CTB488 were co-injected into either EC5 or dSub. Most EC5-projecting CA1 cells (n = 3 mice; A–D) as well as dSub-projecting CA1 cells (n = 3 mice; E–H) showed high levels of overlap between CTB555 (red) and CTB488 (green). The maximum error between the expected perfect overlap and these experimental data was observed at 6% (CTB555 in CA1 from EC5; D), which is used as the chance level for obtaining non-overlapping neuronal populations in a target upstream structure using CTB dyes (in Figure 4G). Representative coronal sections are shown.

(I) Wild type C57BL/6J mice were injected with CTB555 into deep layers of the lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC5), to retrogradely label CA1 projection neurons. DAPI (blue, left) and CTB (red, right). Representative sagittal section showing hippocampal CA1 (left). Higher magnification image of boxed region showing CTB+ neurons (right). Quantification revealed that 32% of dCA1 cells were CTB+ (n = 3 mice).

(J) Percentage of proximal, intermediate, and distal dCA1 neurons retrogradely labeled with rabies virus (RV) from dSub or CTB488 from EC5 (in Figures 4A–4G). NS, not significant.

(K–N) Wild type C57BL/6J mice were injected in dSub or EC5 with CTB555 to retrogradely label dCA1 neurons projecting to either brain region. DAPI (K) and CTB (L). To measure cFos levels, a virus cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-H2B-GFP viruses were injected into dCA1 (see Methods; M). The overlap of behavior-induced cFos and CTB555 (N) was quantified in Figure 5H. Representative coronal images are taken with a 10× objective. White arrows indicate dCA1 neurons that are both cFos+ and CTB+.

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S6. Figure S6. Physiological characteristics of CA1 and dSub cells during open field and CFC tests, Related to Figure 6.

(A–B) Representative histological sections showing the damage induced by the microendoscope. Coronal image from a CA1-GCaMP6f mouse (A), see also Figure 6C. For CA1, the medial region along the proximodistal axis was targeted. Sagittal image from a dSub-GCaMP6f mouse (B), see also Figure 6B. Approximate microendoscope location outlined by the dashed white lines. DAPI staining (blue) and myelin basic protein (MBP) staining (red).

(C) Distribution of individual Ca2+ transient durations for the entire population of dCA1 (left) and dSub (right) cells. Y-axes indicate frequency of events (0.01 corresponds to 1%). X-axes indicate Ca2+ event width (s) plotted in logarithmic (log) scale for optimal visualization. Line fit for each distribution (black lines). Cell-type classification cutoff (log(3.5 s) = 0.54) depicted by red dashed lines. To the right of the red dashed line (right graph) represents dSub long-tail cells, which are not found in the CA1 population (left graph). Over 90% of the entire population of dSub calcium events are represented by the two major peaks (right graph), but only one peak for CA1 cells (left graph), showing that the dSub cell population is bimodal.

(D–E) Representative Ca2+ traces from a dSub short tail (D) and a dSub long tail (E) cell during an open field imaging session of 30 minutes.

(F) Ca2+ traces from 10 dSub short tail cells during an imaging session of 10 minutes (left two columns). Ca2+ traces from 10 dSub long tail cells during an imaging session of 10 minutes (right two columns).

(G) Ca2+ activity during the open field paradigm. Ca2+ event width (s, left) and rate (Hz, right) are quantified (n = 759 CA1 cells, n = 428 dSub short tail cells, n = 371 dSub long tail cells, n = 4 mice per group).

(H) Place field size (cm2), spatial information (bits event−1), and sparsity quantification (cell counts in Figure S6G). A sparsity value of 0.15 indicates that the cell is active in 15% of the open field arena. ND, not detected.

(I–K) Ca2+ event rate during the pre-footshock period of CFC training (I) and non-freezing (NF) and freezing (F) periods of CFC training and recall (n = 550 CA1 cells, n = 429 dSub short tail cells, n = 203 dSub long tail cells, n = 3 CA1 mice, n = 4 dSub mice; J). Proportion of time in each session quantified as NF or F epochs for pre-footshock periods (Pre), training, and recall (K).

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Movie S1. Movie S1. In vivo calcium imaging of CA1 Cre+ neurons, Related to Figure 6.

Representative movie from a dCA1-specific WFS1-Cre mouse injected with a Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus into dCA1. Raw movie acquired using the microendoscope during behavior, followed by motion correction (left panel). The motion-corrected movie is further processed by subtraction of the spatial mean (middle panel). The final movie showing ΔF/F signals from single CA1 neurons (right panel).

Download video file (14.1MB, avi)
Movie S2. Movie S2. In vivo calcium imaging of dSub Cre+ neurons, Related to Figure 6.

Representative movie from a dSub-specific FN1-Cre mouse injected with a Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus into dSub. Raw movie acquired using the microendoscope during behavior, followed by motion correction (left panel). The motion-corrected movie is further processed by subtraction of the spatial mean (middle panel). The final movie showing ΔF/F signals from single dSub neurons (right panel).

Download video file (8.5MB, avi)

Acknowledgments

We thank S. LeBlanc, S. Huang, A. Arons, J. Martin, L. Smith, F. Bushard, A. Hamalian, W. Yu, and C. Lovett for help with experiments; N. Mise and Y. Koga for help with generating the dorsal subiculum mouse line; M. Morrissey for comments; L. Brenner for proofreading; and all members of the Tonegawa laboratory for their support. We thank Ian Wickersham for providing rabies virus, Ki Goosens for sharing a microplate reader, and the Harvard Center for Biological Imaging for access to a light sheet fluorescence microscope. This work was supported by the RIKEN Brain Science Institute, the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, and the JPB Foundation (to S.T.).

Footnotes

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

D.S.R., T.K., and S.T. contributed to the study design. D.S.R., T.K., T.O., S.K.O., and C.S. contributed to the data collection and interpretation. D.S.R. and T.K. conducted the surgeries, behavior experiments, and histological analyses. Y.O. and A.Y. generated the dorsal subiculum mouse line. D.S.R. and S.T. wrote the paper. All authors discussed and commented on the manuscript.

References

  1. Bourque CW, Oliet SH, Kirkpatrick K, Richard D, Fisher TE. Extrinsic and intrinsic modulatory mechanisms involved in regulating the electrical activity of supraoptic neurons. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1993;689:512–519. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1993.tb55581.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Chung K, Wallace J, Kim SY, Kalyanasundaram S, Andalman AS, Davidson TJ, Mirzabekov JJ, Zalocusky KA, Mattis J, Denisin AK, et al. Structural and molecular interrogation of intact biological systems. Nature. 2013;497:332–337. doi: 10.1038/nature12107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Deng W, Aimone JB, Gage FH. New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:339–350. doi: 10.1038/nrn2822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ding SL. Comparative anatomy of the prosubiculum, subiculum, presubiculum, postsubiculum, and parasubiculum in human, monkey, and rodent. J. Comp. Neurol. 2013;521:4145–4162. doi: 10.1002/cne.23416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Eichenbaum E, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;30:123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Eldridge LL, Engel S, Zeineh MM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ. A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:3280–3286. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Gabrieli JD, Brewer JB, Desmond JE, Glover GH. Separate neural bases of two fundamental memory processes in the human medial temporal lobe. Science. 1997;276:264–266. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5310.264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Geva-Sagiv M, Romani S, Las L, Ulanovsky N. Hippocampal global remapping for different sensory modalities in flying bats. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:952–958. doi: 10.1038/nn.4310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Goshen I, Brodsky M, Prakash R, Wallace J, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K. Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories. Cell. 2011;147:678–689. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Hall J, Thomas KL, Everitt BJ. Fear memory retrieval induces CREB phosphorylation and Fos expression within the amygdala. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;13:1453–1458. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01531.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hasselmo ME, McClelland JL. Neural models of memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1999;9:184–188. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)80025-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Herman JP, Dolgas CM, Carlson SL. Ventral subiculum regulates hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical and behavioural responses to cognitive stressors. Neuroscience. 1998;86:449–459. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00055-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hitti FL, Siegelbaum SA. The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memory. Nature. 2014;508:88–92. doi: 10.1038/nature13028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hyman BT, Van Hoesen GW, Damasio AR, Barnes CL. Alzheimer’s disease: cell-specific pathology isolates the hippocampal formation. Science. 1984;225:1168–1170. doi: 10.1126/science.6474172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Jin X, Pokala N, Bargmann CI. Distinct circuits for the formation and retrieval of an imprinted olfactory memory. Cell. 2016;164:632–643. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kaneko T, Fujiyama F, Hioki H. Immunohistochemical localization of candidates for vesicular glutamate transporters in the rat brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 2002;444:39–62. doi: 10.1002/cne.10129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kelley JB, Balda MA, Anderson KL, Itzhak Y. Impairments in fear conditioning in mice lacking the nNOS gene. Learn. Mem. 2009;16:371–378. doi: 10.1101/lm.1329209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kishi T, Tsumori T, Ono K, Yokota S, Ishino H, Yasui Y. Topographical organization of projections from the subiculum to the hypothalamus in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;419:205–222. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000403)419:2<205::aid-cne5>3.0.co;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kitamura T, Pignatelli M, Suh J, Kohara K, Yoshiki A, Abe K, Tonegawa S. Island cells control temporal association memory. Science. 2014;343:896–901. doi: 10.1126/science.1244634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kitamura T, Sun C, Martin J, Kitch LJ, Schnitzer MJ, Tonegawa S. Entorhinal cortical ocean cells encode specific contexts and drive context-specific fear memory. Neuron. 2015;87:1317–1331. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Knierim JJ, Neunuebel JP, Deshmukh SS. Functional correlates of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex: objects, path integration and local-global reference frames. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2013;369:20130369. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kondoh K, Lu Z, Ye X, Olson DP, Lowell BB, Buck LB. A specific area of olfactory cortex involved in stress hormone responses to predator odors. Nature. 2016;532:103–106. doi: 10.1038/nature17156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kubik S, Miyashita T, Guzowski JF. Using immediate-early genes to map hippocampal subregional functions. Learn. Mem. 2007;14:758–770. doi: 10.1101/lm.698107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lee JL. Memory reconsolidation mediates the updating of hippocampal memory content. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2010;4:168. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lein ES, Zhao X, Gage FH. Defining a molecular atlas of the hippocampus using DNA microarrays and high-throughput in situ hybridization. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:3879–3889. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4710-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajan A, Deisseroth K, Tonegawa S. Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature. 2012;484:381–385. doi: 10.1038/nature11028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Morris RG, Schenk F, Tweedie F, Jarrard LE. Ibotenate lesions of hippocampus and/or subiculum: dissociating components of allocentric spatial learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1990;2:1016–1028. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1990.tb00014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Moser EI, Roudi Y, Witter MP, Kentros C, Bonhoeffer T, Moser MB. Grid cells and cortical representation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;15:466–481. doi: 10.1038/nrn3766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Nakazawa K, McHugh TJ, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. NMDA receptors, place cells and hippocampal spatial memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;5:361–372. doi: 10.1038/nrn1385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Nakazawa Y, Pevzner A, Tanaka KZ, Wiltgen BJ. Memory retrieval along the proximodistal axis of CA1. Hippocampus. 2016;26:1140–1148. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. O'Mara S. Controlling hippocampal output: the central role of subiculum in hippocampal information processing. Beh. Brain Res. 2006;174:304–312. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, Itohara S, Tonegawa S. Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory. Science. 2016;353:1536–1541. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ranck JB. Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. I. Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. Exp. Neurol. 1973;41:461–531. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(73)90290-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Raybuck JD, Lattal KM. Differential effects of dorsal hippocampal inactivation on expression of recent and remote drug and fear memory. Neurosci. Lett. 2014;569:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Roozendaal B. Stress and memory: opposing effects of glucocorticoids on memory consolidation and memory retrieval. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2002;78:578–595. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2002.4080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Roy DS, Arons A, Mitchell TI, Pignatelli M, Ryan TJ, Tonegawa S. Memory retrieval by activating engram cells in mouse models of early Alzheimer's disease. Nature. 2016;531:508–512. doi: 10.1038/nature17172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sharp PE, Green C. Spatial correlates of firing patterns of single cells in the subiculum of the freely moving rat. J. Neurosci. 1994;14:2339–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-04-02339.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Gothard KM. An information-theoretic approach to deciphering the hippocampal code. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. 1992;5:1030–1037. [Google Scholar]
  39. Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol. Rev. 1992;99:195–231. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Staff NP, Jung HY, Thiagarajan T, Yao M, Spruston N. Resting and active properties of pyramidal neurons in subiculum and CA1 of rat hippocampus. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;84:2398–2408. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.5.2398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Sun C, Kitamura T, Yamamoto J, Martin J, Pignatelli M, Kitch LJ, Schnitzer MJ, Tonegawa S. Distinct speed dependence of entorhinal island and ocean cells, including respective grid cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2015;112:9466–9471. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1511668112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Taube JS. Electrophysiological properties of neurons in the rat subiculum in vitro. Exp. Brain Res. 1993;96:304–318. doi: 10.1007/BF00227110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Tonegawa S, Liu X, Ramirez S, Redondo R. Memory engram cells have come of age. Neuron. 2015;87:918–931. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Treves A, Rolls ET. Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus. 1992;2:189–199. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wickersham IR, Lyon DC, Barnard RJ, Mori T, Finke S, Conzelmann KK, Young JA, Callaway EM. Monosynaptic restriction of transsynaptic tracing from single, genetically targeted neurons. Neuron. 2007;53:639–647. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Witter MP, Ostendorf RH, Groenewegen HJ. Heterogeneity in the dorsal subiculum of the rat. Distinct neuronal zones project to different cortical and subcortical targets. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1990;2:718–725. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1990.tb00462.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Ziv Y, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Hamel EO, Ghosh KK, Kitch LJ, El Gamal A, Schnitzer MJ. Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:264–266. doi: 10.1038/nn.3329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure S1. Figure S1. Characterization of FN1-Cre mice, Related to Figures 12.

(A) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent virus expressing eYFP. Percentage of eYFP+ cells in dSub expressing various molecular markers (n = 3 mice per group). Cre+ dSub neurons expressed the neuronal nuclei (NeuN) protein and the excitatory neuronal marker CaMKII (see Figure 1C). Cre+ dSub neurons did not overlap with CA1 excitatory neurons labeled with WFS1 (see Figure 1B), parvalbumin (PV) inhibitory neurons, the inhibitory neuronal marker GAD67 (see Figure 1D), Wisteria floribunda (WFA) lectin, or the granule cell marker calbindin. ND, not detected.

(B–D) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent histone H2B-GFP virus (see Methods viral constructs section; B). Representative medial sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue, C) and H2B-GFP labeling in dSub (green, D). Dashed white line (C, D) denotes CA1/dSub border.

(E–H) Representative lateral sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue, E) and H2B-GFP labeling (green, F) in dSub, but not vSub. Higher magnification images of boxed regions in F (G–H).

(I–K) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a cocktail of Cre-dependent eYFP virus and CTB into EC5. Representative lateral sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue, I), CTB555 (red, J), and eYFP labeling (green, K). CTB555 signal reflects the injection site. Cre-dependent eYFP labeling was observed in dSub, but not MEC.

(L) Cre mRNA expression in FN1-Cre mice by in situ hybridization (ISH) showing clear signals in dSub, and the dorsal tegmental nucleus (DTg) in the brain stem. Anterior to posterior (AP, in millimeters relative to Bregma) coronal sections.

(M) Representative sagittal sections showing brain regions projecting to dSub Cre+ neurons (see Figure 2D), including thalamic nuclei (Thal Nucl), nucleus accumbens shell (Acb Sh), and retrosplenial agranular cortex (RSA). Rabies virus-positive neurons (red), DAPI staining (blue). White arrows indicate multiple thalamic nuclei containing rabies virus-positive neurons. Higher magnification image of boxed thalamic nuclei region (second image from left). Medial to lateral (ML, in millimeters relative to Bregma).

(N–P) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent synaptophysin (SYP) virus to label dSub axonal terminals. Reflecting the excitatory nature of these dSub Cre+ neurons, SYP labeling (red) overlapped with vesicular glutamate transporters 1 (VGLUT1 in green; N) and 2 (VGLUT2 in green; O). dSub neurons do not express VGLUT3 (green; P), which mainly occurs in non-glutamatergic neurons. White arrows indicate axonal terminals originating from dSub Cre+ neurons that express VGLUT1 (N) or VGLUT2 (O). Representative 40× sagittal confocal images.

(Q) CTB injection sites. Representative sagittal sections showing DAPI staining (blue) and CTB555 labeling (red). Small volume (50 nl) injections targeting MB (left panel), EC5 (middle panel), or dSub (right panel). Dashed white line (right panel) denotes CA1/dSub border.

Medial to lateral (ML, in millimeters relative to Bregma) coordinates. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S2. Figure S2. Optogenetic inhibition using eArch decreased memory recall-induced cFos expression in dSub cell bodies as well as terminals, Related to Figure 3.

(A–D) FN1-Cre mice were injected in dSub with a Cre-dependent eArch3.0-mCherry or mCherry alone virus (A–B). Dashed white line (A, B) denotes CA1/dSub border. To measure cFos levels, a virus cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-H2B-GFP viruses were injected into dSub (see Methods; C). Representative cFos expression in dSub cell bodies from mCherry mice (C, left panel) and eArch3.0-mCherry mice (C, right panel). During CFC memory recall, optogenetic inhibition of dSub neurons decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons (n = 5 mice per group; D).

(E–H) Axonal terminals originating from dSub Cre+ neurons observed in MB (outlined by the white dashed line; E–F). Representative cFos expression in dSub→MB terminals from mCherry mice (G, left panel) and eArch3.0-mCherry mice (G, right panel). Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in MB during CFC memory recall decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons (n = 6 mice per group; H).

(I–L) Axonal terminals originating from dSub Cre+ neurons observed in EC5 (outlined by the white dashed line; I–J). Representative cFos expression in dSub→EC5 terminals from mCherry mice (K, left panel) and eArch3.0-mCherry mice (K, right panel). Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in EC5 during CFC memory recall decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons (n = 6 mice per group; L).

DAPI staining in representative sagittal sections (A, E, I), eArch3.0-mCherry labeling (B, F, J), and H2B-GFP labeling (C, G, K).

(M) Open field assay. FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent eArch3.0-eYFP or eYFP alone virus into dSub. Optogenetic inhibition of dSub cell bodies during an open field test. Average heat maps (n = 10 mice per group) showing exploration time of eYFP light ON and eArch light ON groups (left). Distance traveled (centimeters, cm) and velocity (cm/second) (right). NS, not significant.

(N) Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in EC5 during CPP memory recall decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons in EC5 (n = 5 mice per group, left panel). Optogenetic inhibition of dSub terminals in MB following CFC memory recall (for CORT, Figure 3G) decreased the percentage of cFos-positive neurons in MB (n = 5 mice per group, right panel).

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S3. Figure S3. Optogenetic inhibition of dSub and/or vSub terminals in EC5 during CFC memory recall, using wild type mice, Related to Figure 3.

(A–C) Wild type C57BL/6J mice were injected with an EF1α-eArch3.0-eYFP virus. Behavioral schedule (A). Representative sagittal section showing eArch3.0-eYFP expression (green, B). DAPI staining (blue). Inhibition of dSub terminals in EC5 by green light during CFC memory recall impaired behavioral performance (n = 12 mice; C), while inhibition of vSub terminals in EC5 by green light showed normal levels of CFC memory recall (n = 14 mice). Consistent with these results, inhibition of both dSub and vSub (d+v) terminals in EC5 revealed a memory retrieval deficit (n = 12 mice).

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S4. Figure S4. Control experiments for optogenetic behavior manipulations, Related to Figure 3.

(A) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent ChR2-eYFP virus into dSub. Optogenetic activation of dSub→MB terminals during contextual fear conditioning (CFC) recall had no effect on freezing levels (n = 8 mice per group). NS, not significant.

(B) FN1-Cre mice were injected with a Cre-dependent eArch3.0-eYFP virus into dSub. Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during trace fear conditioning (TFC) training had no effect on tone (Tn)-induced freezing levels during recall tests (n = 11 mice per group). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panels), averaged freezing levels (right panels). NS, not significant.

(C) Inhibition of dSub→MB terminals during trace fear conditioning (TFC) recall had no effect on freezing levels (n = 10 mice). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panel), averaged freezing levels during the two light-off tones and the two light-on tones (right panel). NS, not significant.

(D) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during delay fear conditioning (DFC) training had no effect on tone (Tn)-induced freezing levels during recall tests (n = 10 mice per group). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panels), averaged freezing levels (right panels). NS, not significant.

(E) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during delay fear conditioning (DFC) recall had no effect on tone (Tn)-induced freezing levels (n = 11 mice). Pre-tone baseline freezing (Pre). Recall-induced freezing levels during individual tone presentations (left panels), averaged freezing levels during the two light-off tones and the two light-on tones (right panels). NS, not significant.

(F) Inhibition of dSub→EC5 terminals during cocaine-induced conditioned place preference (CPP) training had no effect on recall tests (n = 13 mice per group). Behavioral schedule (top part). Pre-exposure preference duration (bottom, left graph) and recall preference duration (bottom, right graph). Saline (S or Sal), cocaine (C or Coc).

(G) Inhibition of dSub→MB terminals during CPP recall had no effect (n = 11 mice per group). Behavioral schedule (top part). Pre-exposure preference duration (bottom, left graph) and recall preference duration (bottom, right graph). Saline (S or Sal), cocaine (C or Coc).

(H) CORT levels following CPP memory recall tests were not significantly different between the saline (Sal) and cocaine (Coc) treated animals (n = 7 mice per group). NS, not significant.

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S5. Figure S5. Retrograde tracing and cFos-CTB overlap experiments, Related to Figures 45.

(A–H) To quantify the chance level at which retrograde tracing using CTB results in non-overlapping neuronal populations in the upstream target structure, CTB555 and CTB488 were co-injected into either EC5 or dSub. Most EC5-projecting CA1 cells (n = 3 mice; A–D) as well as dSub-projecting CA1 cells (n = 3 mice; E–H) showed high levels of overlap between CTB555 (red) and CTB488 (green). The maximum error between the expected perfect overlap and these experimental data was observed at 6% (CTB555 in CA1 from EC5; D), which is used as the chance level for obtaining non-overlapping neuronal populations in a target upstream structure using CTB dyes (in Figure 4G). Representative coronal sections are shown.

(I) Wild type C57BL/6J mice were injected with CTB555 into deep layers of the lateral entorhinal cortex (LEC5), to retrogradely label CA1 projection neurons. DAPI (blue, left) and CTB (red, right). Representative sagittal section showing hippocampal CA1 (left). Higher magnification image of boxed region showing CTB+ neurons (right). Quantification revealed that 32% of dCA1 cells were CTB+ (n = 3 mice).

(J) Percentage of proximal, intermediate, and distal dCA1 neurons retrogradely labeled with rabies virus (RV) from dSub or CTB488 from EC5 (in Figures 4A–4G). NS, not significant.

(K–N) Wild type C57BL/6J mice were injected in dSub or EC5 with CTB555 to retrogradely label dCA1 neurons projecting to either brain region. DAPI (K) and CTB (L). To measure cFos levels, a virus cocktail of c-Fos-tTA and TRE-H2B-GFP viruses were injected into dCA1 (see Methods; M). The overlap of behavior-induced cFos and CTB555 (N) was quantified in Figure 5H. Representative coronal images are taken with a 10× objective. White arrows indicate dCA1 neurons that are both cFos+ and CTB+.

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Figure S6. Figure S6. Physiological characteristics of CA1 and dSub cells during open field and CFC tests, Related to Figure 6.

(A–B) Representative histological sections showing the damage induced by the microendoscope. Coronal image from a CA1-GCaMP6f mouse (A), see also Figure 6C. For CA1, the medial region along the proximodistal axis was targeted. Sagittal image from a dSub-GCaMP6f mouse (B), see also Figure 6B. Approximate microendoscope location outlined by the dashed white lines. DAPI staining (blue) and myelin basic protein (MBP) staining (red).

(C) Distribution of individual Ca2+ transient durations for the entire population of dCA1 (left) and dSub (right) cells. Y-axes indicate frequency of events (0.01 corresponds to 1%). X-axes indicate Ca2+ event width (s) plotted in logarithmic (log) scale for optimal visualization. Line fit for each distribution (black lines). Cell-type classification cutoff (log(3.5 s) = 0.54) depicted by red dashed lines. To the right of the red dashed line (right graph) represents dSub long-tail cells, which are not found in the CA1 population (left graph). Over 90% of the entire population of dSub calcium events are represented by the two major peaks (right graph), but only one peak for CA1 cells (left graph), showing that the dSub cell population is bimodal.

(D–E) Representative Ca2+ traces from a dSub short tail (D) and a dSub long tail (E) cell during an open field imaging session of 30 minutes.

(F) Ca2+ traces from 10 dSub short tail cells during an imaging session of 10 minutes (left two columns). Ca2+ traces from 10 dSub long tail cells during an imaging session of 10 minutes (right two columns).

(G) Ca2+ activity during the open field paradigm. Ca2+ event width (s, left) and rate (Hz, right) are quantified (n = 759 CA1 cells, n = 428 dSub short tail cells, n = 371 dSub long tail cells, n = 4 mice per group).

(H) Place field size (cm2), spatial information (bits event−1), and sparsity quantification (cell counts in Figure S6G). A sparsity value of 0.15 indicates that the cell is active in 15% of the open field arena. ND, not detected.

(I–K) Ca2+ event rate during the pre-footshock period of CFC training (I) and non-freezing (NF) and freezing (F) periods of CFC training and recall (n = 550 CA1 cells, n = 429 dSub short tail cells, n = 203 dSub long tail cells, n = 3 CA1 mice, n = 4 dSub mice; J). Proportion of time in each session quantified as NF or F epochs for pre-footshock periods (Pre), training, and recall (K).

Statistical comparisons are performed using unpaired t tests; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Movie S1. Movie S1. In vivo calcium imaging of CA1 Cre+ neurons, Related to Figure 6.

Representative movie from a dCA1-specific WFS1-Cre mouse injected with a Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus into dCA1. Raw movie acquired using the microendoscope during behavior, followed by motion correction (left panel). The motion-corrected movie is further processed by subtraction of the spatial mean (middle panel). The final movie showing ΔF/F signals from single CA1 neurons (right panel).

Download video file (14.1MB, avi)
Movie S2. Movie S2. In vivo calcium imaging of dSub Cre+ neurons, Related to Figure 6.

Representative movie from a dSub-specific FN1-Cre mouse injected with a Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus into dSub. Raw movie acquired using the microendoscope during behavior, followed by motion correction (left panel). The motion-corrected movie is further processed by subtraction of the spatial mean (middle panel). The final movie showing ΔF/F signals from single dSub neurons (right panel).

Download video file (8.5MB, avi)

RESOURCES