Abstract
We study the influence of seasonally and geographically related daily dynamics of daylight and ambient temperature on human resting or sleeping patterns using mobile phone data of a large number of individuals. We observe two daily inactivity periods in the people’s aggregated mobile phone calling patterns and infer these to represent the resting times of the population. We find that the nocturnal resting period is strongly influenced by the length of daylight, and that its seasonal variation depends on the latitude, such that for people living in two different cities separated by eight latitudinal degrees, the difference in the resting periods of people between the summer and winter in southern cities is almost twice that in the northern cities. We also observe that the duration of the afternoon resting period is influenced by the temperature, and that there is a threshold from which this influence sets in. Finally, we observe that the yearly dynamics of the afternoon and nocturnal resting periods appear to be counterbalancing each other. This also lends support to the notion that the total daily resting time of people is more or less conserved across the year.
Introduction
In modern societies, daily activities are marked by the entrainment of the human circadian clock, which is strongly influenced by two different cues, each one following a 24-hour period but with a different offset. The first cue is related to environmental and sun-based events, and therefore being subjected to seasonal variations due to the yearly movement of earth around the sun. The second cue follows a local civil time, where social and economical factors impose restrictions on the timing and routine of human activities1. Regardless of the nature of the external cue participating in the entrainment of the human circadian clock, any change in it has a direct influence on various aspects of human life2, 3.
For human circadian rhythms, particularly for the sleep wake cycle (SWC), there are various biological4, sociological5 and environmental6 cues affecting the entrainment of the circadian clock, sometimes with undesirable effects on the mental and physical health of individuals2, 7–12. Due to its importance, the SWC has been studied in recent years from different perspectives, trying to understand and identify what are the processes and pacemakers governing its dynamics13, 14. Broadly speaking the current understanding of the generation and maintenance of SWC is that sleep is governed by two main mechanisms consisting of the 24-hour circadian system and awake-dependent homeostatic build-up of sleep pressure. Then sleep itself feeds back to regulate the circadian system and homeostatic sleep pressure build-up. Light plays a key role in synchronising and pace-making the circadian cycle or rhythm, gated by the SWC in suppressing the melatonin production in the pineal gland, regulating alertness. Apart from these, the daily social activities also play a role in driving SWC.
In general, the current research on the human SWC has focused on experiments on small groups under controlled situations8, 11, and on studies based on questionnaires6, 15, 16, mainly the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ). The use of questionnaires for studying SWC are proved to be efficient when assessing sleep wake cycle, but have some limit to the domain of its applicability17. Also, it has been suggested that humans living in modern societies are subjected to new environments, like electrical lighting18, 19, and exposure to these has disrupted and changed people’s natural sleeping habits, although there is no agreement in the field about these possible changes19–23. Studies of the sleeping patterns of people living in pre-industrial societies20 have shown that their sleeping times are similar to those of modern societies, and that temperature could play an important role in the dynamics of sleep. In the recent past the presence of new communication technologies as well as the accessibility to large-scale techno-social datasets (‘Big data’) have allowed the study of human behavior from diverse perspectives applying reality mining techniques. In particular, mobile phone call detail records (CDRs) have been analysed to study mental health24, social networks25–28, sociobiology29–33, behavior of cities34, 35, as well as mobility36–41.
In the recent past, data collected from sensors and logs from mobile phone have been used to study circadian rhythms29, 32, 38, 42–44. Aledavood et al.29 studying mobile phone calling patterns together with data collected from questionnaires, described the characteristic time at which the peak of alertness of a small cohort of 24 students occurs. In another study, comparing various medium size (around 100, 1200, 700 and 2400 participants) datasets of mobile phone records and emails, Aledavood et al.45 showed each individual level has a distinctive, persistent daily communication pattern that strongly differs from the average pattern.
Users of the mobile phone network have specific time intervals when their calling activity ceases and it is expected that these periods are characterised by users taking rest. Each day shows two periods of low calling activity, the first after lunch time around 4:00 p.m., and the second coinciding with the night period, centered around 4:00 a.m. In turn, these two periods delimit two regions of high activity, one peaking around noon and the second one around 8:00 p.m. (see Fig. 1B). The daily calling activity described by these four periods follows a complex dynamics across the year and along different geographical zones, allowing it to be used to provide insight into the dynamics of human resting or sleeping pattern.
Results
We analyse a dataset containing anonymised CDRs corresponding to a 12 month period in 2007 from a mobile phone service provider having subscribers in a number of cities in a country located in the Southern Europe subregion of the United Nations geoscheme46. The dataset contains more than 3 billion calls between 50 million unique identifiers, from which 10 million were associated with individuals having a contract with the company in question. The remaining identifiers belong to the subscribers of other companies or land-lines. Each call in the dataset involves at least one subscriber. For the majority of the subscribers, the age, gender, postal code, and location of the most accessed cell tower (MACT) are available, and we include into the analysis only those subscribers (termed as ‘users’ from here on) whose demographic information is complete. A user is considered to “live in a city” if the following three geographical locations – the associated city centre, the location of the MACT and the centre of the postal code are sufficiently close (details in SI). We choose cities having more than a hundred thousand inhabitants in the year 2007, such that our final analysis takes into account a set of 36 cities with around 1 million users in total.
The calling activity of an entire city depends on a number of factors, but it can be described in terms of two variables: the time of the day, and the date of the year. From the dataset, we calculate the probability distribution p all(t, d) of finding an outgoing call at time t of a day d = 1,…, 365 by a caller living in a particular city. We define a ‘day’ starting from 4:00 a.m. of a calendar day and running to 3:59 a.m. of the next calendar day. The reason of this choice is to clearly distinguish between different daily calling activity periods, separating them into two parts: those events occurring before the observed termination of the calling activity during the night and those occurring after that. The time when the calling activity falls to its minimal observed value in general occurs around 4:00 a.m., and we choose this natural bound as the beginning/end point of the calling activity ‘day’, delimiting the calling distributions p all of different days.
In Fig. 1A we show p all(t, d) (green curve) during the two different pairs of consecutive days, d = 46–47 (marking mid-February) and d = 214–215 (the beginning of August) of 2007, for a city with around six hundred thousand inhabitants. The distribution p all(t, d) turns out to be bimodal with the first mode corresponding to the calls made during the morning, peaking around noon, and the second mode being related to calls during the evening, reaching its maximum around 8:00 p.m. Such bimodal patterns are found for all the days around the year and for all the cities included in this study. These two peaks are naturally delimited by two regions when the activity falls to a minimum, one between 4:00 p.m. and 5:00 p.m., associated mainly with the time after lunch, and a second at the end/beginning of the day, between 4:00 a.m. and 5:00 a.m., which lies inside the normal sleeping period. We will refer to the former as the afternoon calling activity minimum g aft and to the latter as the nocturnal calling activity minimum g noc.
In order to study the periods of low activity, we split the day into two non-overlapping periods – ‘morning’ and ‘night’, each 11 hours long, delimited by g noc and g aft. Here we define the ‘morning’ as the time period between 5:00 a.m. and 3:59 p.m., and ‘night’ the time period between 5:00 p.m. and 3:59 a.m. on the following calendar day. Each ‘morning’, a user can make a number of calls but to focus on the time when the calling activity starts we consider only the first call made during the ‘morning’ for each user and construct the associated probability distribution of the time of the first call P F(t, d). Similarly, we find the last call made by every user during the ‘night’ and construct the corresponding probability distribution for the time of the last call P L(t, d). It should be emphasized that in this study we include only the calls made by the users (outgoing calls), excluding all the incoming calls, because these may not depend on the activity pattern of the users. In Fig. 1A we compare the probability distribution for all the calls P all(t, d) with the corresponding distributions P L(t, d) and P F(t, d) for the times of the last call (blue) and first call (red), respectively, for two different pairs of consecutive days (during winter and summer) for the particular city with a population over six hundred thousand. The shapes of the distributions P L(t, d) and P F(t, d) depicted in Fig. 1A appear to be preserved for all the days and cities we have studied. For some particular days, mainly related to holidays and festivities, the corresponding data were filtered out from the analysis due to their atypical behavior (see Fig. A1 in SI).
From here on our assumption is that the two periods of low calling activity, characterise two commonly observed daily periods of low human activity (or resting periods). We identify the period of low calling activity occurring in the night, with the nightly sleep. There are results reported in the literature showing that analyses based on smart-phone usage describe the nocturnal sleeping period, thus giving support to our approach42, 44. In these studies as well as in ours, the underlying methods rely on estimating the sleep periods from the inactivity of the type of device from which the data is collected. Nonetheless, both methods have limitations on accurately estimating the sleep periods which vary depending on the nature of collected data.
For the case of the afternoon period of low calling activity, it has been shown in different studies47–51 that there is decay in the performance and intensity level of certain behavioral/physiological human activities, coinciding with the rising of the sleep propensity in the sleep/wake regulation. This period occurring during the afternoon has been associated with napping and/or resting49–52, and in general it is reported to occur between 3:00 p.m. and 6:00 p.m.48, 49, 51, which coincides with the afternoon period of low calling activity. As these periods overlap, we assume that the period of low calling activity is a good indicator of the resting time.
Influence of latitude in seasonal variability of low-activity period
As regards to the calling activity presented in Fig. 1A, we expect the sleeping hours to be mainly concentrated in the night when the calling activity falls to a minimum. This nocturnal period of resting (NPR) is quantified in the following fashion. From distributions P L and P F for each city, we calculate the two means and to determine the times when the last call of the day and first call of the following morning were made, respectively. Thus the NPR is defined to be the region bounded by and . In order to estimate the duration of the period when the calling activity has practically ceased, we determine the width of NPR, T night, by taking into account the widths of the distributions P F and P L, determined by the standard deviations σ F and σ L, respectively. Hence, we can write (see Fig. 1A). This definition for the width of NPR does not require any introduction of arbitrary cut-off parameters.
We have calculated P L and P F for 12 cities lying within one of three latitudinal bands, centered at 37°N, 40°N, and 42.5°N, in such a way that each band contains 4 cities. In Fig. 2 we show the yearly variation of T night for these 12 cities. The plots show that the width T night changes across the year, being longest near the winter solstice and shortest six months later, near the summer solstice. Comparing the different plots corresponding to T night, the following two characteristics should be noted. First, cities lying in the same latitude band show similar T night curves, differing only by a vertical offset. This implies that although each city has a characteristic period of low activity, its yearly variation is very similar to that in other cities at the same latitude. Secondly, the difference between the highest (near winter solstice) and lowest values (near summer solstice) of T night changes from latitude to latitude, being larger in southern cities and showing that there is an external factor that influences T night with different intensity at different latitudes.
As a matter of fact, the shape of T night(d) throughout the year resembles the yearly behavior of the length of the night N length(d), defined here as the time between the sunset and the sunrise for each city. The value of N length peaks around the winter solstice (d = 356) and decreases monotonically until it reaches a minimum value around the summer solstice (d = 172), coinciding with the behavior of T night. In the scatter plots of vs. (Fig. 3A) we can see a noticeably linear dependence between these quantities, with the southern cities (≈37°N) showing the strongest effect. We use a linear regression, , to quantify the yearly variation of as a function of . Here is defined as the average value of T night from Monday to Thursday each week, to characterize typical weekdays and to reduce the fluctuations of T night. We carry out the regression for 36 different cities (including the aforementioned 12 cities) located between the 36°N and 44°N, for weekdays. Similarly, to characterize typical weekend days, we have calculated averaged across Fridays, Saturdays and Sundays. In order to study the effect of latitude on the seasonal variation of the NPR, we have calculated the net change δ season between for the winter solstice (maximum) and for the summer solstice (minimum), given by . This simple definition provides a way to calculate how the latitude affects the range where the period of low activity varies. For the 12 cities studied, we find that N length and T night are highly correlated, as can be seen in the inset in Fig. 3B, with the correlation r in the intervals [0.54, 062], [0.40, 048], and [0.31, 0.47], for the cities lying on the 37°N, 40°N, and 42.5°N latitudinal bands, respectively, with a confidence level greater than 99% in all cases. Besides this high correlation between N length and T night, we explore if there is a time lag between their corresponding time-series. For this, we calculate the normalized cross-correlation ρ between the time-series for the same 12 cities as above (see Methods). In Fig. 3B it can be seen that ρ reaches its maximum when the time lag τ is zero, meaning that these time-series vary in the same phase. In Fig. 3C we show δ season for each of the 36 cities at their own latitudes. Remarkably, the time difference δ season between winter and summer is always at least 15 minutes larger for the southern cities compared to the northern cities. For weekends we observed a difference of 30 minutes, with almost a ratio 2:1 (30 minutes in the northernmost cities against one hour in the southernmost ones). The slopes of the linear regression obtained for each one of the 36 cities can be seen in the SI, Fig. A4. Cities around the same latitude have similar slopes (βs), and these slopes are larger for cities at the southern latitudes. In studies based on MCTQ, Allebrandt et al.53 and Kantermann et al.54 have reported that sleep duration variation across the year is around 20 minutes, for a sample of ≈55,000 people from Central Europe and a sample ≈58,00 people from Estonia, respectively. In contrast, in studies using wrist band activity sensors applied on subjects still living in hunter-gatherer societies (San and Tsimane), Yetish et al.20 reported that sleep duration of these tribes living in urban-free environments changes around 50 minutes across the year.
Ambient temperature effects on the duration of diurnal resting period
For all the 36 cities included in this study and for almost every day, P all shows apart from the nocturnal period of resting or predominantly night-time sleeping period, another low activity period centered around the diurnal gap g aft, ranging from 4:00 p.m. to 5:00 p.m. (Fig. 1C). The dynamics of this diurnal afternoon period of resting (DPR), associated with a decrease in human activity, can also be traced from the P all dynamics though here the decrease is less dramatic than in the nocturnal case. We find that P all can be described by the sum of two normal distributions, with one of the activity peaks centered around the noon, and the other in the evening around 8:00 p.m. as seen in Fig. 1C. Unlike in the NPR case where almost all the activity of people ceases due to them sleeping, the DPR shows a more moderate decrease in activity between these two peaks. This is associated with only a fraction of people having naps or taking rest. The length of this low activity period DPR can be measured with the fitting function given by . Here and σ M are the mean and the standard deviation of the noon-centered distribution and and σ N the corresponding values for the evening-centered one. Hence we define the duration of the DPR or the afternoon break period to be T break as (Fig. 1C). The yearly change in T break for 12 cities located at one of three different latitudinal bands 37°N, 40°N, or 42.5°N is depicted in Fig. 2.
In order to study the influence of the daily temperature on the dynamics of T break, we present it as a scatter plot with the maximum daily temperature θ for the 12 above mentioned cities, as depicted in Fig. 4. It can be seen that T break follows two different dynamics, distinguishable by a threshold temperature θ*, below which T break seems not to be strongly influenced by the temperature and above which there is a clear linear trend (see Fig. A5 in the SI for a comparison between the time-series). To make this distinction more visible and to estimate the threshold, we apply a clustering algorithm to split the data-points into two sets, each one containing those points representing the dynamics below or above the threshold temperature. As the scatter of the data points is not compact and they are not spread along two separate regions with a well defined border, the density-based clustering algorithms57, 58 are not well-suited for determining a turning point in the dynamics. Instead we chose to use a spectral clustering technique based on the Shi-Malik algorithm55, 56, where a graph is built from the data points and mapped into a low-dimensional eigen-space, where the graph is partitioned based on the similarities of the eigenvectors. We use an implementation of this algorithm found in a standard machine learning library59. This spectral clustering technique requires to set the number of clusters that data points must be split into. In order to search for the above-mentioned turning-point in the dynamics we set the number of clusters to two, which corresponds to the visual evidence (see Fig. 4) of the presence of two different behaviors of the afternoon resting period. An evaluation of different choice of parameters is shown in the SI (Figs. A8–A9). The results for each city can be seen in Fig. 4, in the form of two sets (red and blue points), with a threshold temperature ranging from 18° to 25° (Fig. 4). Once each set is split into two subsets, we calculate the correlation between T break and θ for the set of cities (Fig. 4, right panels). We observe a weak correlation when the temperature is below the threshold, i.e. for almost all of the 12 cities r ≤ 0.25 and p ≈ 0.05. On the other hand, when the temperature is above the threshold, we observe the correlations to be strong, i.e. r ≥ 0.5 and p ≈ 0.0001, which indicates a strong influence of ambient temperature on the afternoon break period T break.
Here we have shown that both, the daily NPR and DPR (i.e. the nocturnal and diurnal resting periods), as well as the characteristic times T night and T break follow specific dynamics showing seasonal and geographical variations. As depicted in Fig. 2 these resting periods seem to follow opposite dynamics across the year such that when added up, the resulting total daily resting period shows only a small variation over the year (see Fig. A6 in the SI). This lends support to the notion that the total daily resting time of people is more or less conserved.
Discussion
At the individual level the SWC is known to depend on various physiological and social factors that make it difficult to identify the precise causes influencing its dynamics. However, studying the yearly variation of the NPR averaged over a big population gives us quite unique insight into its complex behavior and helps us to identify some of the key influencing factors. The SWC is bounded inside the NPR and we expect that both entities follow similar dynamics but with somewhat different onset and termination times. We have shown that the duration of the NPR, characterized by the width T night, closely follows the seasonal variation in the duration of the night. Moreover, we have observed that the duration of NPR is influenced by the latitude of the city in question. The difference between T night in winter and in summer for the people in cities located at 36°N was found to be almost twice as much as that for the people in cities located 8° further north. It seems that the activity of people in southern cities are most affected by the length of the night (or conversely length of the daylight), despite the fact that in northern locations the length of the night changes faster and spans a bigger time interval than more southern latitudes.
Human physiology and its hormonal regulation is expected to govern the dynamics of the SWC at the individual level. In particular, the melatonin hormone is known to follow a circadian rhythm60 and has been linked to the SWC. For individuals, melatonin secretion commences during the evening between 8 p.m. and 10 p.m., peaking between 2:00 a.m. and 4:00 a.m. However, the onset and the duration of melatonin secretion during the circadian cycle is in turn related with the exposure to light and darkness, and considerable research has been done61–64 showing how changes in the onset and length of the light exposure disrupts the secretion of melatonin, as well as the SWC65. For people living at latitudes far away from the equator, where the seasonal changes in the onset and length of daylight are large, it has been shown that their melatonin cycle is seasonally altered and this perturbation gives rise to disruptions in their SWC66. Thus it could be possible that the melatonin cycle is participating in the entrainment of the SWC (and the NPR) with the duration of the daylight across the year. On the other hand, the diurnal period of resting DPR is found to depend rather strongly on the temperature, making its duration during hot days longer, but showing no noticeable variation during cold or cool days. Surprisingly, however, this increase in the daily diurnal resting time is found to be counterbalanced by the decrease of the daily nocturnal resting time. Hence it seems that on warm or hot days a bigger fraction of the total resting period is taken by the afternoon break, thereby reducing the homeostatic pressure for sleeping and consequently the need for nocturnal resting period.
The evidence of the trade-off between the afternoon and nighttime rest or sleep at latitudes closer to the tropics has implications for our understanding of the time budgets of tropical hunter-gatherers who have also been reported to take afternoon ‘naps’ for about a quarter of a day20. Our results suggest that, in high temperatures tropical habitats, humans find it difficult to remain active during the afternoon when ambient temperatures are at their highest, much as in the case of for most monkeys and apes67, 68. Figure 4 suggests that there is a critical threshold at which rest becomes necessary and that this occurs at ambient temperatures somewhere between 20–25 °C (though ‘naps’ of significant length are probably not common until ambient temperatures exceed ≈30°). This has important implications in that it removes a significant amount of time from the active working day, thereby reducing the time that can be devoted to ecologically or economically more important activities. Such constraints have imposed significant limitations on our species’ ability to evolve into complex societies69.
Methods
Time binning and smoothing of distributions
To generate each probability distributions, we divide the temporal axis in 5 minutes bins, in such a way that each distribution P all contains 268 points, whilst P L and P F contain 132 points each one. In some cases, mainly for cities with small population, their associated probability distributions show fluctuations due to small sample size, then we apply a smoothing process to each distribution in order to reduce the noise, using the Savitsky-Golay70 algorithm (7 points and degree 4).
Data filtering
From our dataset, two locations could be associated to each user. First one is the location of the most accessed cell tower (MACT) by the mobile device. From the zip code of the domicile of the user we get the second one, namely the location of the centre of the postal zone (postal location). These two locations were used when determining which users should be included in the analysis. We discarded those users with at least one of these two locations missing.
To reduce the noise due to poor sampling, we chose from the whole set of towns included in the mobile phone network only those cities with more than hundred thousand inhabitants in the studied year, and lying between latitudes 36°N and 44°N, which accounts for 36 cities. For each selected city, we encircled it with a 30 km diameter circle, and use the center of the circle as the associated geographical location of the city. All the studied cities fit inside a 30 km diameter circle.
Finally, we choose only those users who live in one of these cities. To ensure this into some extent, we impose the next two restrictions:
The distance between the city location and at least one of the associated user locations (MACT and postal) should be less that 15 km, and
The distance between the MACT and postal locations of the user must be less than 30 km.
There is no way to verify that the user actually lives in the associated city, but one could expect that almost every user lives in the same city as the one specified by the domicile, and that the MACT is indicative of their usual location. Thus the imposed restrictions seem to be a good way to ensure that a big fraction of the included users are correctly assigned to their actual city.
From the demographic information available, one can find the age of the users ranges from eighteen to more than hundred years. In this work we choose only users with age between 30 and 75 years old, because the calling pattern in younger people (mainly in the 18 to 25 years range) is more erratic than for older people. Older people (75+) were excluded the analysis due to the sparse calling activity that many of them had in the CDRs. After the previous filtering, the included users into the analysis were 925, 135 individuals.
Cross-Correlation
To measure the possible time delay between the time-series and we calculate the cross-correlation between them, finding the time delay at which the cross-correlation reaches its maximum, meaning that the time-series are in the best possible alignment (in phase).
The (normalized) cross-correlation for a time delay τ between two time-series , , is given by
1 |
where E[] denotes the expectation value, and , and σ X, σ Y, are the average values and the standard deviations of X and Y, respectively.
Using the discrete form of , , with n the number of samples, and time delay τ as an integer between −n/2 and n/2, then the cross-correlation reads as follows
2 |
where , , , and , the average and standard deviation of X and Y, calculated accordingly the usual definitions. For a given τ, the lower and upper bound of the summation as well as the denominator (n-τ) are chosen in such a way that only the overlapping part of the time-series is contributing to calculation of the cross-correlation.
Data availability
The datasets analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to a signed non-disclosure agreement. The dataset contains sensitive information of the subscribers, particularly age, gender, postal address and the location of the most accessed cell tower. Calling time distributions from which all the results shown in this work were generated are available from the last author on reasonable request.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
AG and KK acknowledge project COSDYN, Academy of Finland (Project No. 276439) for financial support. DM, KB, and KK acknowledge EU HORIZON 2020 FET Open RIA project (IBSEN) No. 662725. DM also acknowledges CONACYT (Mexico), grant No. 383907. RD acknowledges European Research Council for the Advanced Investigator Grant No. 295663. Authors thank AL Barabási for the dataset used in this research.
Author Contributions
K.B., A.G. and D.M. carried out the analysis of the data. All the authors were involved in designing the project and the preparation of the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-11125-z
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Aschoff J, Wever R. Human circadian rhythms: a multioscillatory system. Federation proceedings. 1976;35:2326–2332. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lange T, Dimitrov S, Born J. Effects of sleep and circadian rhythm on the human immune system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1193:48–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05300.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Souêtre E, et al. Circadian rhythms in depression and recovery: evidence for blunted amplitude as the main chronobiological abnormality. Psychiatry research. 1989;28:263–278. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90207-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Czeisler CA, et al. Bright light resets the human circadian pacemaker independent of the timing of the sleep-wake cycle. Science. 1986;233:667–671. doi: 10.1126/science.3726555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Grandin LD, Alloy LB, Abramson LY. The social zeitgeber theory, circadian rhythms, and mood disorders: review and evaluation. Clinical psychology review. 2006;26:679–694. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Roenneberg T, Kumar CJ, Merrow M. The human circadian clock entrains to sun time. Current Biology. 2007;17:R44–R45. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lewy AJ, Lefler BJ, Emens JS, Bauer VK. The circadian basis of winter depression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2006;103:7414–7419. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602425103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.OrzeƗ-Gryglewska J. Consequences of sleep deprivation. International journal of occupational medicine and environmental health. 2010;23:95–114. doi: 10.2478/v10001-010-0004-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Evans JA, Davidson AJ. Health consequences of circadian disruption in humans and animal models. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci. 2013;119:283–323. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00010-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Möller-Levet CS, et al. Effects of insufficient sleep on circadian rhythmicity and expression amplitude of the human blood transcriptome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:E1132–E1141. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1217154110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Davies SK, et al. Effect of sleep deprivation on the human metabolome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111:10761–10766. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402663111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Santhi N, et al. Sex differences in the circadian regulation of sleep and waking cognition in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016;113:E2730–E2739. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521637113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wright KP, Hughes RJ, Kronauer RE, Dijk D-J, Czeisler CA. Intrinsic near-24-h pacemaker period determines limits of circadian entrainment to a weak synchronizer in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2001;98:14027–14032. doi: 10.1073/pnas.201530198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hofstra WA, de Weerd AW. How to assess circadian rhythm in humans: a review of literature. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2008;13:438–444. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International journal of chronobiology. 1975;4:97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. Journal of biological rhythms. 2003;18:80–90. doi: 10.1177/0748730402239679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Levandovski R, Sasso E, Hidalgo MP. Chronotype: a review of the advances, limits and applicability of the main instruments used in the literature to assess human phenotype. Trends in psychiatry and psychotherapy. 2013;35:3–11. doi: 10.1590/S2237-60892013000100002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wright KP, et al. Entrainment of the human circadian clock to the natural light-dark cycle. Current Biology. 2013;23:1554–1558. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.de la Iglesia HO, et al. Ancestral sleep. Current biology: CB. 2016;26:R271–2. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Yetish G, et al. Natural sleep and its seasonal variations in three pre-industrial societies. Current Biology. 2015;25:2862–2868. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Peever J, Horner RL. Human behavior: Sleep in hunter–gatherer societies. Current Biology. 2015;25:R1133–R1135. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Yetish G, et al. Response to de la iglesia et al. Current Biology. 2016;26:R273–R274. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ekirch AR. Segmented sleep in preindustrial societies. Sleep. 2016;39:715–716. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Torous J, Staples P, Onnela J-P. Realizing the potential of mobile mental health: new methods for new data in psychiatry. Current psychiatry reports. 2015;17:1–7. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0602-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kovanen L, Kaski K, Kertész J, Saramäki J. Temporal motifs reveal homophily, gender-specific patterns, and group talk in call sequences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:18070–18075. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307941110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Eagle N, Pentland AS, Lazer D. Inferring friendship network structure by using mobile phone data. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2009;106:15274–15278. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900282106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jiang Z-Q, et al. Calling patterns in human communication dynamics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:1600–1605. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220433110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Blondel VD, Decuyper A, Krings G. A survey of results on mobile phone datasets analysis. EPJ Data Science. 2015;4:1–55. doi: 10.1140/epjds/s13688-015-0046-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Aledavood T, et al. Daily rhythms in mobile telephone communication. PloS one. 2015;10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.David-Barrett, T. et al. Communication with family and friends across the life course. arXiv preprint arXiv:1512.09114 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 31.Bhattacharya, K., Ghosh, A., Monsivais, D., Dunbar, R. I. M. & Kaski, K. Sex differences in social focus across the life cycle in humans. Royal Society Open Science3, doi:10.1098/rsos.160097 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 32.Aledavood, T. et al. Channel-specific daily patterns in mobile phone communication. In Proceedings of ECCS 2014, 209–218 (Springer, 2016).
- 33.Wang Q, Gao J, Zhou T, Hu Z, Tian H. Critical size of ego communication networks. EPL (Europhysics Letters) 2016;114 doi: 10.1209/0295-5075/114/58004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Louail, T. et al. From mobile phone data to the spatial structure of cities. Scientific reports4 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 35.Sun L, Axhausen KW, Lee D-H, Huang X. Understanding metropolitan patterns of daily encounters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:13774–13779. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306440110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Song C, Qu Z, Blumm N, Barabási A-L. Limits of predictability in human mobility. Science. 2010;327:1018–1021. doi: 10.1126/science.1177170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sevtsuk A, Ratti C. Does urban mobility have a daily routine? learning from the aggregate data of mobile networks. Journal of Urban Technology. 2010;17:41–60. doi: 10.1080/10630731003597322. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Stopczynski A, et al. Measuring large-scale social networks with high resolution. PloS one. 2014;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Pappalardo, L. et al. Returners and explorers dichotomy in human mobility. Nature communications6 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 40.Jiang, S. et al. The timegeo modeling framework for urban motility without travel surveys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 201524261 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 41.Pappalardo, L. & Simini, F. Modelling individual routines and spatio-temporal trajectories in human mobility. arXiv preprint arXiv:1607.05952 (2016).
- 42.Abdullah, S., Matthews, M., Murnane, E. L., Gay, G. & Choudhury, T. Towards circadian computing: early to bed and early to rise makes some of us unhealthy and sleep deprived. In Proceedings of the 2014 ACM international joint conference on pervasive and ubiquitous computing, 673–684 (ACM, 2014).
- 43.Christensen MA, et al. Direct measurements of smartphone screen-time: Relationships with demographics and sleep. PloS one. 2016;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cuttone A, et al. Sensiblesleep: A bayesian model for learning sleep patterns from smartphone events. PloS one. 2017;12 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Aledavood T, Lehmann S, Saramäki J. Digital daily cycles of individuals. Frontiers in Physics. 2015;3 doi: 10.3389/fphy.2015.00073. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Standard country or area codes for statistical use. https://unstats.un.org/unsd/methodology/m49/. Accessed: 2017-08-01.
- 47.Mitler MM, et al. Catastrophes, sleep, and public policy: consensus report. Sleep. 1988;11 doi: 10.1093/sleep/11.1.100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Broughton R, Mullington J. Circasemidian sleep propensity and the phase-amplitude maintenance model of human sleep/wake regulation. Journal of sleep research. 1992;1:93–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1992.tb00017.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Broughton RJ. Scn controlled circadian arousal and the afternoon ‘nap zone’. Sleep Res Online. 1998;1:166–178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bes F, Jobert M, Schulz H. Modeling napping, post-lunch dip, and other variations in human sleep propensity. Sleep. 2009;32:392–398. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.3.392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Strogatz SH, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA. Circadian pacemaker interferes with sleep onset at specific times each day: role in insomnia. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 1987;253:R172–R178. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1987.253.1.R172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mitler, M. Two-peak 24-hour patterns in sleep, mortality, and error. In Sleep and health risk, 65–77 (Springer, 1991).
- 53.Allebrandt KV, et al. Chronotype and sleep duration: the influence of season of assessment. Chronobiology international. 2014;31:731–740. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2014.901347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kantermann T, Juda M, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. The human circadian clock’s seasonal adjustment is disrupted by daylight saving time. Current Biology. 2007;17:1996–2000. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Shi J, Malik J. Normalized cuts and image segmentation. IEEE Transactions on pattern analysis and machine intelligence. 2000;22:888–905. doi: 10.1109/34.868688. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Von Luxburg U. A tutorial on spectral clustering. Statistics and computing. 2007;17:395–416. doi: 10.1007/s11222-007-9033-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.MacKay, D. J. Information theory, inference and learning algorithms (Cambridge university press, 2003).
- 58.Ester M, Kriegel H-P, Sander J, Xu X, et al. A density-based algorithm for discovering clusters in large spatial databases with noise. Kdd. 1996;96:226–231. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Pedregosa F, et al. Scikit-learn: Machine learning in Python. Journal of Machine Learning Research. 2011;12:2825–2830. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Arendt J. Melatonin, circadian rhythms, and sleep. New England Journal of Medicine. 2000;343:1114–1116. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200010123431510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Brown GM. Light, melatonin and the sleep-wake cycle. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience. 1994;19 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Dawson D, Encel N. Melatonin and sleep in humans. Journal of pineal research. 1993;15:1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-079X.1993.tb00503.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Broadway J, Arendt J, Folkard S. Bright light phase shifts the human melatonin rhythm during the antarctic winter. Neuroscience letters. 1987;79:185–189. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(87)90694-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Vondrašová D, Hájek I, Illnerová H. Exposure to long summer days affects the human melatonin and cortisol rhythms. Brain research. 1997;759:166–170. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00358-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wehr TA. Effect of seasonal changes in daylength on human neuroendocrine function. Hormone Research in Paediatrics. 1998;49:118–124. doi: 10.1159/000023157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wehr TA. The durations of human melatonin secretion and sleep respond to changes in daylength (photoperiod) The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 1991;73:1276–1280. doi: 10.1210/jcem-73-6-1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Korstjens AH, Lehmann J, Dunbar R. Resting time as an ecological constraint on primate biogeography. Animal Behaviour. 2010;79:361–374. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Dávid-Barrett T, Dunbar RI. Bipedality and hair loss in human evolution revisited: The impact of altitude and activity scheduling. Journal of human evolution. 2016;94:72–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Dunbar, R. Human evolution: A Pelican introduction (Penguin UK, 2014).
- 70.Savitzky A, Golay MJ. Smoothing and differentiation of data by simplified least squares procedures. Analytical chemistry. 1964;36:1627–1639. doi: 10.1021/ac60214a047. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to a signed non-disclosure agreement. The dataset contains sensitive information of the subscribers, particularly age, gender, postal address and the location of the most accessed cell tower. Calling time distributions from which all the results shown in this work were generated are available from the last author on reasonable request.