Abstract
Functional status is an important component of quality of life for older adults and for their caregivers. Factors associated with level of functional status include age, comorbidity, cognitive status, depression, social support, and activity. Of the types of activity linked with functional status, the strongest evidence is for physical exercise, with weaker evidence for social and productive activity. Engagement is a construct including motivation, commitment, and participation.
Objective:
The objective of this study was to determine whether or not engagement is associated with level of functional status after controlling for age, comorbidity, and depression among older adults.
Methods:
In all, 92 older adults were recruited from senior living centers and a university-based senior exercise group. The main independent variable was engagement. Other independent variables were age, comorbidity, and depression. The dependent variable was level of functional status. Two different measures of level of functional status were used: the Katz Activities of Daily Living Index and the Lawton–Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale.
Results:
Engagement was a significant predictor of Lawton–Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale level of function in the Lawton–Brody model (odds ratio: 1.147 (1.011–1.302)) and age was a significant negative predictor (odds ratio: 0.838 (0.769–0.914)). There were no significant predictors in the Katz model.
Conclusion:
Engagement is a significant predictor of perfect Lawton–Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale score after controlling for age, comorbidity, and depression. Health-care professionals should consider including engagement in their assessment of older clients, whether using formal instruments such as the Engagement with Meaningful Activity Survey or assessing informally.
Keywords: Engagement, older adults, activity theory, functional status
Introduction
Wang defines functional status as the level of ability to do “activities performed by an individual to realize the needs of daily living in many aspects of life including physical, psychological, spiritual, intellectual, and roles.”1 It is typically measured by assessing ability to do activities of daily living (ADLs) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) independently.2,3 Engagement is a construct consisting of three components: motivation, commitment, and participation in a given activity.4 In this article, literature concerning the relationship between activity and the functional status of older adults is briefly reviewed and a study of the relationship between engagement and level of functional status is reported.
Functional status of older adults
The portion of the US population over 60 years of age is expected to be 27% of 400 million by the year 2050.5 Currently, only 2.3% of the US adult population requires assistance with ADLs, but this number rises to 3.4% among adults aged 65–74 years and 12% for those over 75 years.6 In 2014, there were an estimated 46.2 million older adults in the United States,7 about 680,000 of whom are in residential care communities and 1.2 million in nursing homes.8
Functional status is one of the most important determinants of quality of life for older adults.9–11 Functional status is usually operationalized as the degree of dependence a person experiences in performing ADLs and IADLs.2,3 The ADLs typically included in functional status assessment are eating, transfers, continence, toileting, bathing, and dressing.12 More sensitive indicators of functional status include mobility and IADLs such as shopping, meal preparation, telephone use, housekeeping, laundry, arranging transportation, and managing medications and finances.2
In addition to being related to quality of life, functional status also has an important relationship to physical health and longevity.13 Functional decline is defined as a need for assistance in at least one ADL.13 Functional decline is thus a level of functional status, as is intact functional status. Decourcelle et al. did a hospital-based study of 272 Canadians with an acute coronary event and found that those with functional decline were more likely to die than those without functional decline. They found hazard ratios for mortality of 3.63 at 6 months and 2.69 at greater than 6 months from the coronary event.
Functional decline is also a financial burden. Current annual long-term care costs are estimated at 147.4 billion dollars for the United States.14 A study of US Alzheimer’s patients reported an estimated average increase of US$1406 annually in direct medical care costs and US$3333 in total costs per patient for each one point increase in the Blessed Dementia Rating Scale.15 Investigators in Ireland estimated a 6-month increase of 796 euros for each one point decline in the Katz ADL Scale.16
Functional decline affects family members, who frequently become caregivers as a result. In a primary practice-based study of 165 informal caregivers, Nichols et al.17 found that new caregivers tended to request information about the technical aspects of caregiving, but as time went on caregivers tended to focus more on the emotional aspects. As a group, the caregivers had high rates of depression and they tended to neglect their own health. Findings of a matched control study of 37 caregivers were that the caregivers had greater rates of depression and anxiety and lower sense of belonging, positive affect, and sleep efficiency than their controls.18
Engagement
Lequerica and Kortte4 define engagement as the level of motivation, commitment, and participation that a person has toward an activity. The important things to measure in assessments of engagement are the individuals expressed attitude toward the activity as well as acknowledgment of need and actual participation in the activity.19 Some investigators measure engagement with direct observations,20 while others use questionnaires.21–23
Factors which are known to be related to level of functional status include age,24 comorbidity,25 cognitive status, depression,26 social support,27 and activity.28 There has been little study of the relationship between engagement and level of functional status. One study of orthopedic rehabilitation patients reported better level of functional status for more engaged patients.19 Cohen-Mansfield et al.29 found a negative correlation between level of functional status and engagement among nursing home residents with dementia. The purpose of the study reported here is to investigate the relationship between engagement and level of functional status in older adults. The research question is as follows: Controlling for age, comorbidity, and depression, is engagement related to level of functional status among cognitively intact older adults?
Framework
The framework for this study is based on activity theory. Activity theory holds that older adults generally desire to maintain their accustomed activities and that doing so is beneficial for them both physically and mentally.30 The framework includes the concepts of engagement, role function, meaningful activity, and functional status. According to the framework, roles are activated by meaningful activity. This activation results in engagement, which leads to a self-reinforcing cycle. Improved or maintained functional status is a byproduct of this cycle. The relationships between the concepts of the framework are illustrated in Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Activity study framework.
Age
One of the factors known to be associated with level of functional status is age.24 A multi-year study of the prevalence of functional decline in Sweden reported rates of 10%–12% for participants in the 81–84 year range compared with 32%–40% for those between 90 and 99 years.31 Cumulative life-time adversity and depression are moderator variables for age in the trajectory of functional decline.32
Comorbidity
Comorbidities are also associated with functional decline.25 Comorbidities of particular interest include arthritis, hypertension, heart disease,33 obesity,34 liver disease,35 stroke, and chronic obstructive lung disease.36
Cognitive function
Impaired cognitive ability correlates with functional decline.26 Executive function is especially important for maintaining level of functional status,37 as are attention and verbal memory.38 In this study, cognitive function was not measured, but it was controlled for by only admitting participants whose cognitive function was sufficient to understand and verbalize the contents of the consent document.
Depression
The relationship between depression and level of functional status is complex. Some studies suggest that depression leads to functional decline,32,39,40 but others suggest that functional decline leads to depression.41–43
Social support
Higher social support is associated with increased level of physical activity44 as well as with higher level of functional status.27 Social support may also be a moderator between functional status and depression.45,46 McLaughlin et al.27 found that the participant’s satisfaction with social support is more predictive of level of functional status than the size of the social support network itself.
Activity
Of the various types of activity, physical exercise is the activity most strongly linked to better level of functional status.28,47 Self-efficacy is a mediator between physical exercise and level of functional status.48 The benefit of physical exercise for level of functional status is dose related.49
Solitary cognitive activity, such as reading or doing crossword puzzles, is associated with a slower trajectory of cognitive decline in older adults with Alzheimer’s disease.50 Social activity is associated with higher level of functional status.51,52 Soubelet found that social activity is a mediating variable between age and cognitive function.53
Research on productive activity, such as paid employment, volunteer work, or informal childcare, is mixed. Hsu54 found that although paid work was protective against mortality, for women, unpaid productive activity such as childcare was associated with increased mortality, but unpaid productive activity for men was associated with lower rates of cognitive decline.
Engagement
Engagement is a construct with three components: the degree of motivation, participation, and commitment which a person has toward an activity.4 Meaningful activity is a closely related concept which consists of actions which the individual considers important.55 Activity which is congruent with a person’s accustomed roles is more likely to be meaningful.56
Method
A power analysis determined that a sample of 100 would give 83% power for R2 = 0.08. This was considered adequate to detect any clinically significant relationship. This value for R2 translates to a value of F2 = 0.087. Although the intention was to recruit 100 participants, in fact, only 92 older adults were recruited from three comprehensive senior living communities in a large southern metropolitan area and from a university-based senior exercise group. The study was approved by the university institutional review board (IRB). Participants had to be 65 years or older, have intact decisional capacity, and be able to understand the written consent. Decisional capacity was assessed by having the potential participant read the consent or have it read to her/him and then asking the participant to describe her/his understanding of the purpose and activities of the study. Study procedures included written survey forms and oral interviews. No participants used a proxy to respond to the survey items, but two participants did ask the investigator to read the items to them so that they could respond verbally. All study procedures were conducted in one session at the recruitment site. Variables of interest were, age, engagement, comorbidity, depression, and level of functional status. Participants were compensated for their time and trouble with a small gift card.
Instruments
The instruments used were the Engagement with Meaningful Activity Survey (EMAS), the Functional Comorbidity Index (FCI), the Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form (GDS-SF), the Katz Index of Independence in ADLs (Katz), and the Lawton–Brody IADL Scale (Lawton–Brody). The independent variable of interest was engagement. Age, comorbidity, and depression were control variables.
The engagement variable was measured by the Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey (EMAS).57 This test had a Cronbach’s alpha score of 0.84 and a test–re-test reliability of 0.69 in the work of the original researchers. Concurrent validity was supported by EMAS scores correlating positively with measures of life satisfaction and purpose in life.55 The scores correlated negatively with depression. Possible EMAS scores range from 12 to 48 with higher scores indicating greater engagement.
The FCI is a measure of comorbidities specifically designed with functional status as a primary outcome variable.58 It consists of yes/no questions to a list of 18 different diagnoses. The instrument was developed using data from the Canadian Multi-Centre Osteoporosis Study and the National Spine Network database. FCI scores can range from 0 to 18. Higher scores indicate more comorbidities.
The GDS-SF is a 15-question version of the original 30-question Geriatric Depression Scale.59,60 Cronbach’s alpha over various studies has ranged from 0.76 to 0.83, and test–retest reliability was an average of 0.85. A comparison of GDS scores with diagnosed depression resulted in a sensitivity rate of 78.7% and specificity of 93.9%.61 The range for GDS-SF scores is 0–15 with higher scores indicating greater depression.
The Katz is an assessment of basic functional areas such as dressing, transferring, and continence.62 The Katz is predictive of discharge to home, and it correlates well with measurements of home confinement and mobility.63 In contrast, the Lawton–Brody focuses on higher cognitive level self-care areas such as telephone use and managing transportation needs.64 Reported measures of reliability and validity for the Lawton–Brody include inter-rater reliability of 0.85 and concurrent validity demonstrated by high correlation with other tools for measuring IADLs, emotional status, social adjustment, and health.65 Katz scores can range from 0 to 6, and Lawton–Brody scores have a range of 0–8. Higher scores on both of these instruments indicate greater functional independence.
Analyses
Descriptive statistics were compiled for age, gender, Hispanic identity, race, and marital status. Missing data were imputed using multiple imputation. Two regression models were fit, one with the Katz as the dependent variable and the other using the Lawton–Brody. Statistical packages used were R core software66 along with the lawstat67 and mice68 packages for R.
The research variables were tested for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test and for homoscedasticity using the Levene test. Age was found to be normally distributed, as were the squared values of the EMAS scores, but the rest of the variables were skewed. The Lawton–Brody was very heteroscedastic. Rethinking the situation, the researchers decided that anything less than a perfect score on the functional status measures indicated some degree of dependence. This fits well with Decourcelle et al.’s13 definition of functional decline as being the need for assistance on one or more ADLs. The researchers in this study expanded this definition to include need for assistance in one or more IADLs.
Katz and Lawton–Brody scores are not truly interval/ratio data even though they are often treated as such. One could go down a point in the Katz for incontinence or one could go down a point for needing assistance with eating. These two very different situations could result in the same Katz score. The researchers decided that the broader concepts of dependence and independence were a better way to handle this situation. For these reasons, the decision was made to dichotomize the outcome variables (Katz score and Lawton–Brody score) and to use logistic regression in the models.
The resulting dichotomized variables were either a perfect score or a less-than perfect score for each instrument. Dichotomization can be justified when the underlying structure of the data is itself categorical.69 MacCullum et al. give the example of a study where data are collected on numbers of cigarettes smoked per day. There would be a large cluster of data at the zero point with the rest of the data distributed over a range from 1 to 20 or more. The variable in question could be dichotomized as “smokers” and “nonsmokers.” The distinction between being functionally independent and having some degree of dependence is similar to the distinction between being a smoker and being a nonsmoker.
Results
Sample description
Participants were recruited from three different senior living centers and from one university-based senior exercise group. A total of 95 participants were recruited, but 3 were excluded for failure to meet the age criterion. Data were incomplete on one or more instruments for eight participants. Sample demographics are given in Table 1. Means for the main variables are given in Table 2.
Table 1.
Sample demographics.
Characteristic | n | % | Characteristic | n | % |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Female | 68 | 74% | Assisted living | 2 | 2% |
Male | 24 | 26% | Independent living (senior center) | 57 | 62% |
White | 88 | 96% | Community dweller | 33 | 36% |
Black | 2 | 2% | Divorced | 17 | 18% |
American Indian | 1 | 1% | Married | 42 | 46% |
Did not answer race question | 1 | 1% | Single | 4 | 4% |
Hispanic | 1 | 1% | Widowed | 37 | 40% |
Not Hispanic | 69 | 75% | No answer to marital question | 2 | 2% |
No answer to Hispanic question | 22 | 24% |
Table 2.
Means and ranges for main variables.
Variable | Mean (SD), n | Range |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | 80.18 (8.01), n = 92 | 65–101 |
EMAS | 39.57 (4.54), n = 88 | 27–48 |
FCI | 4.07 (2.23), n = 92 | 0–10 |
GDS-SF | 1.61 (1.76), n = 88 | 0–10 |
Katz | 5.67 (0.56), n = 91 | 3–6 |
Lawton–Brody | 7.47 (1.06), n = 92 | 3–8 |
SD: standard deviation; EMAS: Engagement with Meaningful Activity Survey; FCI: Functional Comorbidity Index; GDS-SF: Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form; Katz: Katz Activities of Daily Living Index; Lawton–Brody: Lawton–Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale.
Models
In all, 64 participants scored 1 for the dichotomized Katz, and 65 scored 1 for the dichotomized Lawton–Brody. Two models were fit. Independent variables for both models were age, EMAS score, FCI score, and GDS-SF score. The logistic regression model using the Katz as the outcome variable did not have any significant predictors, but age and EMAS score were significant in the model for the dichotomized Lawton–Brody. Both models are summarized in Tables 3 and 4.
Table 3.
Logistic regression model with dichotomized Katz score as dependent variable.
Crude OR | Adjusted OR | p | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 1.002 (0.947–1.062) | 0.985 (0.923–1.053) | 0.660 |
EMAS score | 0.926 (0.826–1.030) | 0.922 (0.814–1.035) | 0.180 |
FCI score | 0.850 (0.685–1.043) | 0.837 (0.664–1.040) | 0.115 |
GDS-SF score | 1.063 (0.818–1.439) | 1.204 (0.883–1.766) | 0.286 |
Katz: Katz Activities of Daily Living Index; OR: odds ratio; EMAS: Engagement with Meaningful Activity Survey; FCI: Functional Comorbidity Index; GDS-SF: Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form.
Table 4.
Logistic regression model with dichotomized Lawton–Brody score as dependent variable.
Crude OR | Adjusted OR | p | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 0.860 (0.793–0.922) | 0.847 (0.767–0.919) | <0.001 |
EMAS score | 1.087 (0.980–1.212) | 1.183 (1.037–1.376) | 0.018 |
FCI score | 0.927 (0.755–1.134) | 0.889 (0.681–1.147) | 0.370 |
GDS score | 0.902 (0.696–1.172) | 1.090 (0.794–1.521) | 0.587 |
Lawton–Brody: Lawton–Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale; OR: odds ratio; EMAS: Engagement with Meaningful Activity Survey; FCI: Functional Comorbidity Index; GDS-SF: Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form.
Because eight of the participants had incomplete data, multiple imputation was used to estimate the missing values. The models using multiple imputation are summarized in Tables 5 and 6. As can be seen from tables, the odds ratio estimates for independent variables were fairly close whether or not multiple imputations were used. The lambda value in these tables represents the proportion of each estimate which is attributable to missing data.
Table 5.
Dichotomized Katz model with multiple imputation.
p | Nmis | Fmi | Lambda | Odds ratio | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 0.825 | 0 | 0.032 | 0.009 | 1.007 (0.947–1.070) |
EMAS score | 0.316 | 4 | 0.073 | 0.049 | 0.947 (0.850–1.054) |
FCI score | 0.162 | 1 | 0.051 | 0.028 | 0.860 (0.695–1.064) |
GDS score | 0.683 | 4 | 0.101 | 0.077 | 1.064 (0.788–1.437) |
Katz: Katz Activities of Daily Living Index; EMAS: Engagement with Meaningful Activity Survey; FCI: Functional Comorbidity Index; GDS-SF: Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form; Nmis: number missing; Fmi: fraction of missing information.
Table 6.
Dichotomized Lawton–Brody model with multiple imputation.
p | Nmis | Fmi | Lambda | Odds ratio | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 0.001 | 0 | 0.028 | 0.006 | 0.838 (0.769–0.914) |
EMAS score | 0.003 | 4 | 0.028 | 0.005 | 1.147 (1.011–1.302) |
FCI score | 0.287 | 1 | 0.024 | 0.001 | 0.871 (0.674–1.126) |
GDS score | 0.620 | 4 | 0.105 | 0.079 | 1.082 (0.789–1.484) |
Lawton–Brody: Lawton–Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale; EMAS: Engagement with Meaningful Activity Survey; FCI: Functional Comorbidity Index; GDS-SF: Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form; Nmis: number missing; Fmi: fraction of missing information.
Goodness of fit for the models was tested by comparison of residuals with null models. Chi square for the dichotomized Katz model was χ2 (4, N = 86) = 5.549, p = 0.235. For the dichotomized Lawton–Brody, it was χ2 (4, N = 86) = 22.471, p < 0.001.
Two of the instruments are relatively new: the EMAS and the FCI. For the EMAS, Cronbach’s alpha for this study was 0.83, which is close to the 0.84 reported by the instrument developers.57 In contrast to Eakman et al.s’55 study that reported negative correlation between the EMAS and depression, there was no significant correlation between EMAS score and GDS-SF score in this study. FCI score was not significantly correlated with any study variable. The closest relationships to significance were with the GDS-SF (r = 0.158, p = 0.133) and with the Katz (r = –0.123, p = 0.243).
Effect of engagement
The odds ratio for age in the Lawton–Brody model was 0.838. The inverse can be taken, which translates to an odds ratio of 1.193 for getting a less-than perfect Lawton–Brody score, meaning that for each additional year of age, the odds of getting an imperfect score increased by 19.3%. The odds ratio for the EMAS score is 1.147. For each one point increase in EMAS score, a participant’s odds of getting a perfect score on the Lawton–Brody increase by 14.7%.
Lack of correlation for depression and comorbidity
One of the unexpected findings of this study was the lack of significant influence of the comorbidity and depression variables. FCI score was the variable which came closest to significance in the Katz model. This contrasts with the study by Marengoni et al.,70 which found that one comorbidity multiplied the odds by 1.4 and four or more comorbidities multiplied them by 6.2. The Marengoni study had a much larger sample than this study and was longitudinal.
GDS-SF score was not a significant predictor in either model. Correlations between GDS-SF and the Katz ADL and Lawton Brody IADL scores were r = 0.012 (p = 0.914) and r = –0.082 (p = 0.448), respectively. Mehta et al.71 found that depression was a risk factor for functional decline among functionally intact community-dwelling older adults but not for those who already had some functional decline. Since this study was cross-sectional, the more depressed participants could be at risk for future functional decline which is not yet apparent.
Discussion
This study identified engagement as a factor associated with better IADL level of function, but it is unclear whether the participants were more engaged because they had better level of function or whether they had better level of function because they were more engaged. Future studies should be done using more objective and more sensitive tools to measure level of functional status. There is also a need for longitudinal studies concerning this relationship.
There were significant predictors in the Lawton–Brody model, but not in the Katz model. Both of these instruments had ceiling effects in this study. The Lawton–Brody measures more complex skills such as doing light housework or arranging for transportation. This suggests that some of the older adults in this study were in the earliest stages of functional decline which was captured by the Lawton–Brody scores but not by the Katz scores.
Glass et al.72 studied the relationship between social engagement and depression among community-dwelling older adult participants in a population-based study. They found that social engagement was protective against the development of depressive symptoms and hypothesized three possible mechanisms: that social engagement may stimulate body functions such as cardiovascular or endocrine functions, that social engagement may slow cognitive decline, and that social engagement may be a proxy for coping ability. These three mechanisms postulated by Glass et al. could also play a role in the relationship between engagement and IADL level of function.
Hall et al.73 developed a theory which postulates both engagement strategies and disengagement strategies in response to health challenges. Engagement strategies involve striving to maintain active control. Disengagement strategies involve giving up control in areas that one can no longer control in the interests of self-protection. Although engagement strategies are associated with greater longevity following acute events such as myocardial infarction or stroke, they are maladaptive when directed to the burden of chronic illness. Disengagement strategies are protective in the presence of chronic illness but maladaptive in the case of acute events. This could mean that when prized goals and activities are unattainable, it is more adaptive to revise expectations than to persist in futile striving. In relation to this study, engagement could be helpful in the case of a patient with mobility limitations who needs motivation to exercise regularly, but it would not be helpful in the case of a patient with no realistic possibility of recovery.
Kratz et al.74 studied chronic pain in patients with neurological disorders such as multiple sclerosis and found that activity engagement predicted better adjustment to living with chronic pain than did pain willingness. Pain willingness is the amount of pain a person will accept. Although both of these variables are associated with positive adjustment to life with chronic pain, activity engagement is a more robust predictor. These findings echo those of Hall et al. mentioned above. Pain acceptance represents a disengagement strategy and activity engagement represents an engagement strategy.
Limitations
Limitations of this study include its reliance on convenience sampling and self-report data. Since the sample was skewed toward women and toward White/Caucasian participants, the findings may not be applicable to other groups. The participants were recruited from different sites, and there may have been site-dependent variation in their functional ability. The Lequerica and Kortte’s4 model included environment as one of its concepts, which is a further argument for having controlled the environment variable. The construct of interest was engagement, but the participation component of engagement was not measured by the tool which was used. There were pronounced ceiling effects for both measures of functional status. Some variables of interest such as cognitive status, socioeconomic status, educational level, and social support were not measured.
This study was limited to participants with sufficient cognitive function to verbalize understanding of the consent document. Although some participants may have had mild dementia, it was not noticeable. Further study is needed to determine whether the relationship between engagement and IADL level of function holds true for older adults with dementia. The study procedures would also have to be modified to accommodate participants with dementia.
Implications for research and practice
Nurses working with older adults might consider facilitating engaged activity for their patients. One way of doing this is to consider a patient’s former accustomed roles and to offer a patient role-congruent activities. Activity which activates the accustomed roles of the patient is more likely to be meaningful and thus to elicit higher levels of engagement. For example, a former executive might balk when asked to do arts and crafts but might respond more favorably when offered the opportunity to manage a group activity. Malone and Camp75 give an example of how to engage a reluctant client in personally meaningful activity. A therapist knew that a client had formerly been a gardener, so she invited the client to arrange flowers with her. Initially, the client refused, so the therapist asked whether she could stay in the client’s room and arrange the flowers herself. The client agreed to this. The therapist continued to approach the client daily without pushing her unduly. Eventually, the client began to offer suggestions and to start doing the activity herself.
Health-care professionals should also assess clients’ levels of engagement with activity to better identify clients at increased risk for functional decline. This can be done using formal tools such as the EMAS or may be done qualitatively by observing for levels of commitment, motivation, and participation.
Acknowledgments
The author gratefully acknowledges the assistance of Dr Kathryn Daniel who read and critiqued the manuscript.
Footnotes
Declaration of conflicting interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Ethical approval: Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the University of Texas at Arlington Institutional Review Board 2015–022.
Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by the Ferne Kyba Endowed Fellowship from the University of Texas at Arlington, College of Nursing and Health Innovation.
Informed consent: Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects before the study.
References
- 1. Wang TJ. Concept analysis of functional status. Int J Nurs Stud 2004; 41(4): 457–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Graf C. The Lawton instrumental activities of daily living scale. Am J Nurs 2008; 108(4): 52–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Inouye SK, Bogardus SJ, Baker DI, et al. The hospital elder life program: a model of care to prevent cognitive and functional decline in older hospitalized patients. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000; 48(12): 1697–1706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Lequerica AH, Kortte K. Therapeutic engagement: a proposed model of engagement in medical rehabilitation. Am J Phys Med Rehab 2010; 89(5): 415–422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. World population prospects: the 2012 revision, volume I: comprehensive tables. ST/ESA/SER.A/336, 2013, https://esa.un.org/unpd/wpp/publications/Files/WPP2012_Volume-I_Comprehensive-Tables.pdf
- 6. Lucas JW, Benson V. Tables of summary health statistics for the US population: 2015 National Health Interview Survey. National Center for Health Statistics, 2017, http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis/SHS/tables.htm (accessed 20 April 2017). [Google Scholar]
- 7. Annual estimates of the resident population for selected age groups by sex for the United States, states, counties, and Puerto Rico Commonwealth and Municipios: April 1, 2010 to July 1, 2014. U.S. Census Bureau, Population Division Release, June 2015 (accessed 20 April 2017). [Google Scholar]
- 8. Harris-Kojetin L, Sengupta M, Park-Lee E, et al. Long-term care providers and service users in the United States: data from the National Study of Long-Term Care Providers, 2013–2014. National Center for Health Statistics. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 3, Number 38, 2016, https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/series/sr_03/sr03_038.pdf [PubMed]
- 9. Halvorsrud L, Kirkevold M, Diseth A, et al. Quality of life model: predictors of quality of life among sick older adults. Res Theor Nurs Pract 2010; 24(4): 241–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Simone PM, Haas AL. Frailty, leisure activity and functional status in older adults: relationship with subjective well being. Clin Gerontologist 2013; 36(4): 275–293. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Thanakwang K, Soonthorndhada K, Mongkolprasoet J. Perspectives on healthy aging among Thai elderly: a qualitative study. Nurs Health Sci 2012; 14(4): 472–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Wallace M, Shelkey M. Monitoring functional status in hospitalized older adults. Am J Nurs 2008; 108(4): 64–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Decourcelle V, Maréchaux S, Pinçon C, et al. Impact of functional decline on outcome in elderly patients with acute coronary syndromes. Am J Crit Care 2013; 22(1): e1–e11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Kaye HS, Harrington C, LaPlante MP. Long-term care: who gets it, who provides it, who pays, and how much? Health Affairs 2010; 29(1): 11–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Zhu CW, Leibman C, McLaughlin T, et al. The effects of patient function and dependence on costs of care in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008; 56(8): 1497–1503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Gillespie P, O’Shea E, Cullinan J, et al. The effects of dependence and function on costs of care for Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in Ireland. Int J Geriatr Psych 2013; 28(3): 256–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Nichols LO, Martindale-Adams J, Greene WA, et al. Dementia caregivers’ most pressing concerns. Clin Gerontologist 2008; 32(1): 1–14. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Willette-Murphy K, Todero C, Yeaworth R. Mental health and sleep of older wife caregivers for spouses with Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders. Issues Ment Health N 2006; 27(8): 837–852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Kortte KB, Falk LD, Castillo RC, et al. The Hopkins Rehabilitation Engagement Rating Scale: development and psychometric properties. Arch Phys Med Rehab 2007; 88(7): 877–884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Cohen-Mansfield J, Shmotkin D, Goldberg S. Predictors of longitudinal changes in older adults’ physical activity engagement. J Aging Phys Activ 2010; 18(2): 141–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Bielak AA, Anstey KJ, Christensen H, et al. Activity engagement is related to level, but not change in cognitive ability across adulthood. Psychol Aging 2012; 27(1): 219–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Hinterlong JE, Morrow-Howell N, Rozario PA. Productive engagement and late life physical and mental health: findings from a nationally representative panel study. Res Aging 2007; 29(4): 348–370. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Small BJ, Dixon RA, McArdle JJ, et al. Do changes in lifestyle engagement moderate cognitive decline in normal aging? Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology 2012; 26(2): 144–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Kolt G, Yorston Land Rosenkranz R. Physical activity and physical function in older adults: the 45 and up study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2012; 60(4): 719–725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Huang HT, Chang CM, Liu LF, et al. Trajectories and predictors of functional decline of hospitalised older patients. J Clin Nurs 2013; 22(9): 1322–1331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Song H, Meade K, Akobundu U, et al. Depression as a correlate of functional status of community-dwelling older adults: utilizing a short-version of 5-item Geriatric Depression Scale as a screening tool. J Nutr Health Aging 2014; 18(8): 765–770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. McLaughlin D, Leung J, Pachana N, et al. Social support and subsequent disability: it is not the size of your network that counts. Age Ageing 2012; 41(5): 674–677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Bonnefoy M, Boutitie F, Mercier C, et al. Efficacy of a home-based intervention programme on the physical activity level and functional ability of older people using domestic services: a randomised study. J Nutr Health Aging 2012; 16(4): 370–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Cohen-Mansfield J, Marx MS, Regier NG, et al. The impact of personal characteristics on engagement in nursing home residents with dementia. Int J Geriatr Psych 2009; 24(7): 755–763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Blace NP. Functional ability, participation in activities and life satisfaction of the older people. Asian Social Science 2012; 8(3): 75–87. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Angleman SB, Santoni G, Von Strauss E, et al. Temporal trends of functional dependence and survival among older adults from 1991 to 2010 in Sweden: toward a healthier aging. J Gerontol A: Biol 2015; 70(6): 746–752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Shrira A, Litwin H. The effect of lifetime cumulative adversity and depressive symptoms on functional status. J Gerontol B: Psychol 2014; 69(6): 953–965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Bekhet AK, Zauszniewski JA. Chronic conditions in elders in assisted living facilities: associations with daily functioning, self-assessed health, and depressive symptoms. Arch Psychiat Nurs 2014; 28(6): 399–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Batsis JA, Germain CM, Vásquez E, et al. Waist circumference, physical activity, and functional impairments in older U.S. adults: results from the NHANES 2005–2010. J Aging Phys Activ 2015; 23(3): 369–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Yang S, Hoshi T, Wang S, et al. Socioeconomic status, comorbidity, activity limitation, and healthy life expectancy in older men and women: a 6-year follow-up study in Japan. J Appl Gerontol 2014; 33(7): 831–847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Quiben MU, Hazuda HP. Factors contributing to 50-ft walking speed and observed ethnic differences in older community-dwelling Mexican Americans and European Americans. Phys Ther 2015; 95(6): 871–883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Lee EKO, Lee J. Education, functional limitations, and life satisfaction among older adults in South Korea. Educ Gerontol 2013; 39(7): 514–526. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Pennarts HM, Schouws SNTM, Bongers IMB. Cognitive functioning in relation to self-care in elderly persons with a bipolar disorder. Clin Gerontologist 2014; 37(5): 419–428. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Luo H, Tang JYM, Wong GHY, et al. The effect of depressive symptoms and antidepressant use on subsequent physical decline and number of hospitalizations in nursing home residents: a 9-year longitudinal study. J Am Med Dir Assoc 2015; 16(12): 1048–1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Sin NL, Yaffe K, Whooley MA. Depressive symptoms, cardiovascular disease severity, and functional status in older adults with coronary heart disease: the Heart and Soul Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2015; 63(1): 8–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Kennison RF, Cox J. Health and functional limitations predict depression scores in the health and retirement study: results straight from MARS. Calif J Health Promot 2013; 11(1): 97–108. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Lim EJ. Gender differences in the relationship between physical functioning and depressive symptoms in low-income older adults living alone. Nurs Health Sci 2014; 16(3): 381–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Weil J, Hutchinson SR, Traxler K. Exploring the relationships among performance-based functional ability, self-rated disability, perceived instrumental support, and depression: a structural equation model analysis. Res Aging 2014; 36(6): 683–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Kouvonen A, De Vogli R, Stafford M, et al. Social support and the likelihood of maintaining and improving levels of physical activity: the Whitehall II Study. Eur J Public Health 2012; 22(4): 514–518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Benka J, Nagyova I, Rosenberger J, et al. Social support as a moderator of functional disability’s effect on depressive feelings in early rheumatoid arthritis: a four-year prospective study. Rehabil Psychol 2014; 59(1): 19–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Siu K-C, Rajaram SS, Padilla C. Impact of psychosocial factors on functional improvement in Latino older adults after Tai Chi exercise. J Aging Phys Activ 2015; 23(1): 120–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Hatch J, Lusardi MM. Impact of participation in a wellness program on functional status and falls among aging adults in an assisted living setting. J Geriatr Phys Ther 2010; 33(2): 71–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Mullen SP, McAuley E, Satariano WA, et al. Physical activity and functional limitations in older adults: the influence of self-efficacy and functional performance. J Gerontol B: Psychol 2012; 67(3): 354–361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Dondzila CJ, Gennuso KP, Swartz AM, et al. Dose-response walking activity and physical function in older adults. J Aging Phys Activ 2015; 23(2): 194–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Treiber KA, Carlson MC, Corcoran C, et al. Cognitive stimulation and cognitive and functional decline in Alzheimer’s disease: the Cache County Dementia Progression Study. J Gerontol B: Psychol 2011; 66(4): 416–425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Chen L, Liu L, Liu C, et al. Predicting functional decline of older men living in veteran homes by Minimum Data Set: implications for disability prevention programs in long term care settings. J Am Med Dir Assoc 2013; 14(4): 309.e9–309.e13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Rosso AL, Taylor JA, Tabb LP, et al. Mobility, disability, and social engagement in older adults. J Aging Health 2013; 25(4): 617–637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Soubelet A. The role of social activity in age–cognition relations. Educ Gerontol 2013; 39(8): 558–568. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Hsu HC. Does social participation by the elderly reduce mortality and cognitive impairment? Aging Ment Health 2007; 11(6): 699–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Eakman AM, Carlson ME, Clark FA. Factor structure, reliability, and convergent validity of the Engagement in Meaningful Activities Survey for older adults. Otjr Occup Part Heal 2010; 30(3): 111–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Atchley RC. A continuity theory of normal aging. Gerontologist 1989; 29(2): 183–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Goldberg B, Brintnell ES, Goldberg J. The relationship between engagement in meaningful activities and quality of life in persons disabled by mental illness. Occup Ther Ment Health 2002; 18(2): 17–44. [Google Scholar]
- 58. Groll DL, To T, Bombardier C, et al. The development of a comorbidity index with physical function as the outcome. J Clin Epidemiol 2005; 58(6): 595–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Chiang KS, Green KE, Cox EO. Rasch analysis of the Geriatric Depression Scale-Short Form. Gerontologist 2009; 49(2): 262–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA, Brooks JO, et al. Proposed factor structure of the Geriatric Depression Scale. Int Psychogeriatr 1991; 3(1): 23–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Wall JR, Lichtenberg PA, MacNeill SE, et al. Depression detectionin geriatric rehabilitation: Geriatric Depression Scale short form vs. long form. Clin Gerontologist 1999; 20(3): 13–21. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Katz S, Akpom CA. 12. Index of ADL. Med Care 1976; 14(5): 116–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Hartigan I. A comparative review of the Katz ADL and the Barthel Index in assessing the activities of daily living of older people. Int J Older People Nurs 2007; 2(3): 204–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 1969; 9(3): 179–186. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Alosco ML, Spitznagel MB, Raz N, et al. Executive dysfunction is independently associated with reduced functional independence in heart failure. J Clin Nurs 2014; 23(5): 829–836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing [software]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2014, http://www.R-project.org (accessed 4 April 2016). [Google Scholar]
- 67. Hui WLW, Gel YR, Gastwirth JL. lawstat: an R package for law, public policy, and biostatistics. J Stat Softw 2008; 28(3): 1–26. http://www.jstatsoft.org (accessed 4 April 2016).27774042 [Google Scholar]
- 68. Van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. Mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw 2011; 45(3): 1–67. http://www.jstatsoft.org (accessed 4 April 2016). [Google Scholar]
- 69. MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, et al. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychol Methods 2002; 7(1): 19–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Marengoni A, Von Strauss E, Rizzuto D, et al. The impact of chronic multimorbidity and disability on functional decline and survival in elderly persons. A community-based, longitudinal study. J Intern Med 2009; 265(2): 288–295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Mehta KM, Yaffe K, Covinsky KE. Cognitive impairment, depressive symptoms, and functional decline in older people. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50(6): 1045–1050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Glass TA, De Leon CFM, Bassuk SS, et al. Social engagement and depressive symptoms in late life: longitudinal findings. J Aging Health 2006; 18(4): 604–628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Hall NC, Chipperfield JG, Heckhausen J, et al. Control striving in older adults with serious health problems: a 9-year longitudinal study of survival, health, and well-being. Psychol Aging 2010; 25(2): 432–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Kratz AL, Hirsh AT, Ehde DM, et al. Acceptance of pain in neurological disorders: associations with functioning and psychosocial well-being. Rehabil Psychol 2013; 58(1): 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Malone ML, Camp CJ. Innovative practice. Montessori-based dementia programming: providing tools for engagement. Dementia 2007; 6(1): 150–157. [Google Scholar]