Abstract
Running comparative studies of laterality in mammals is a way to deepen our understanding of the evolution of the brain hemisphere functions. Studies on vision highlighted a possible task-sharing between hemispheres depending on the characteristics of the observers, the nature of the observed stimulus and the context of the observation, a phenomenon that could go beyond the monitoring of conspecifics. Cetaceans are predators that adapted to an aquatic habitat and display a clear crossing of fibers to the side of the brain opposite the eye of origin. Here, we analysed the interactions between humans and cetaceans when free-ranging orcas approach divers. Our study concentrated on the spontaneous exploratory behaviours of divers by orcas depending on their age and sex, and on the possible expression of a visual laterality. The results showed a significant preference for the use of the left eye but exclusively in adult females. Adult males had a more sustained attention than adult females, marked by a higher spatial proximity to divers, slower approaches and longer look durations. Adult females, probably more cautious, explored from the distance and more furtively. Our findings support a possible link between attentional/motivational states and visual laterality in mammals.
Introduction
Recent studies suggest that brain laterality is much older than we thought until now and certainly not unique to humans1, 2. We actually find behavioural descriptions of motor and perceptual laterality in nearly all the groups of vertebrates as well as in some invertebrates. Visual laterality has notably been documented in a large number of species (i.e. Fish:3, 4; Amphibians:5, 6; Reptiles:7, 8; Birds:9, 10; Mammals:11–15; Invertebrates:16, 17), but rarely in large marine mammals because of the difficulty of observing them in the field and their relative rarity in captivity. According to Karenina et al.18, when individuals need to coordinate their behaviour with others, the uniformity of preferential sides inside a population is advantageous. For many animal species, though a consensus appears around the dominant use of one eye over the other one (and therefore the dominant processing by a given hemisphere), the variations regarding which eye (i.e. left or right) is used preferentially are important and seem to depend on the nature of the observed stimulus (e.g. novelty, valence and complexity4, 19–26, as well as on the individual characteristics of the observer (e.g. vigilance state and level of stress:27; age:28) and on the social context of the interaction29–32. Laterality at population or group levels is pointed out in different contexts such as foraging for food23, 24, antipredator33 or social vigilance29, 34, 35, as well as environmental exploration13, 22.
Cetaceans, contrasting to what was once believed, because of their often dark natural habitat (i.e. deep water) and developed echolocation36, have good vision in the air as underwater37. Furthermore, cetaceans have eyes in a semi lateral position with a near total crossing of optical nerves (left eye - right hemisphere)38. A few studies actually underline the existence of a possible visual laterality in cetaceans, even though the factors determining the expression of this laterality are still not very well known. For example, in free-ranging belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) and orcas (Orcinus orca), the juvenile is often positioned on the right side when it swims near its mother, maintaining a visual contact with its left eye18, 39. This preference for a lateralized swim position was recently confirmed with a beluga population in managed care40. Nevertheless, differences are found between social and non-social monitoring. When hunting, orcas attack their prey by placing them in their right visual field, whereas no lateral bias is found when they jump out of water and fall back on their side41. The right hemisphere of cetaceans would thus play a predominant role for social interactions18, 39, 42, whereas in response to non-social or non-familiar stimuli, a predominance of the left hemisphere can be found26.
In captivity, the left eye was preferentially used by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) to observe a human being, familiar or not, approaching the pool17. The individual left eye lateralized preferences were later confirmed by studies with captive belugas and pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens)43. In the same experimental conditions, when the human being was replaced by an object, the degree of familiarity with the observed item was important. Indeed, Blois-Heulin et al.22 found that, while non-familiar objects are observed preferentially with the right eye, the left eye is used to look at very familiar (previously manipulated) objects. Both eyes are used indiscriminately for intermediary items, i.e. non-familiar objects which have become to some extent familiar from previous observations but never manipulated. All in all, the mother, the human (assimilated to the one that is providing care and food) and the toys in captivity are preferentially processed by the right hemisphere, whereas prey and new objects are processed by the left hemisphere. Interestingly, the degree of familiarity with the human or object observed also influenced gaze duration in several cetacean species21, 43–45. The question thus remains open as to whether or not the observer’s attention and motivational state may be a determining factor. More studies are now needed, notably with free-ranging cetaceans, and the cases of human – orca spontaneous encounters is particularly interesting because the former is a potential prey for these animals.
Orcas show numerous and complex capacities of social coordination, which involve both communication and cognition. They indeed show a great diversity of innovated and learned foraging techniques and feeding cooperation strategies46–48, individual and matrilineal acoustic signatures with dialectal cultural variations47–49 and a long period of maternal dependence for the single offspring50. Orcas are also curious animals characterized by a highly developed neophilia in captivity51 and frequent manipulation of various objects in the wild52. Recent studies have described cases of play behaviour innovations in resident orcas living in the U.S. west coast46, 53.
In several mammal species, innovations, risk-taking and exploratory behaviours vary with the age and sex of the individual (e.g. rats and primates:54–57). In captive dolphins, youngsters are generally more curious than adults58, and 80% of the new behaviours observed in plays come from juveniles59. However, Baird et al.60 found no influence of age and sex in diving rates in orcas, but male adults would dive more often during the day than female adults. The question of exploratory behaviour regarding age, sex, as far as we know, has never been studied in free-ranging orcas. Previous studies in cetaceans (e.g. dolphins and belugas) have shown that gaze duration (and possibly swimming speed and distance of approach) plays a key role during exploration of familiar and unfamiliar humans and objects21, 43–45. Although the exploratory behaviours of cetaceans have still been poorly described in the literature, particular behaviours like head and body movements, some being associated with vocalisations (notably echolocation), have also been previously described in this context61, 62.
How humans are perceived by free-ranging orcas is a current hot topic, notably because whale-watching and swimming-with-whales ecotouristic sites are multiplying. To this day, little research has been conducted scientifically about the nature of human - orca spontaneous encounters in the wild and in the diversity of associated behaviours. Orcas’ interest is expressed by behaviours like following boats and many spontaneous interactions with humans (cetologists, fishermen, tourists), including play and even actions of supposed cooperation with humans in their fishing activities63. In the wild, orcas are totally free of their movements and voluntarily approach divers. By using underwater videos, made in Norway, we therefore concentrated on gazes and other behaviours in the context of spontaneous approaches of divers by orcas. We studied an area where diving is organised every year. We have analysed the influence of individual’s age and sex on the behaviours displayed during these approaches and tested a possible eye-preference during visual monitoring.
Results
Among all approaches sampled, 24%, 61.3% and 14.7% consisted of adult males, adult females and subadults respectively. Monocular gazes were clearly the majority (82.5%) of all gazes sampled. Nevertheless, the eye preferentially chosen by the different individuals varied with their age and sex. Thus, while adult males (Wilcoxon matched-paired test, N = 18, Qobs = 40, p = 0.97) and subadults (N = 11, Qobs = 7.5, p = 1) did not preferentially use one eye over the other, adult females used their left eye more frequently (N = 60, Qobs = 191, p = 0.04) (Fig. 1).
In most cases (86%), the “approach consisted in a progressive movement towards the divers and more rarely (14%) in a “passing by” movement (i.e. quasi-linear trajectory, constant distance with the divers). Moreover, orcas approached preferentially from the front or below (98%) and in very rare cases from the back (2%). After having excluded the rare approaches from behind, we found that all units were relatively frequent (i.e. from the right side 39%, from the left side 29%, facing 14%, just under 10%, from deep water 8%).
However, certain types of approach were more frequently observed than others in the three age-sex classes of individuals (see all statistical intra-class comparisons in Table 1). First, concerning the social context and the minimum distance of approach, we found that all individuals approached significantly more often alone than accompanied, and more often close than far. However, while adult females approached more often close than very close, no difference was found for adult males and subadults. Second, concerning visual monitoring and swimming speed, while adult females and subadults displayed more short than long gazes, no difference was found for adult males. This has to be related to some extent to the swimming speed that was more frequently slow than fast in adult males but equally slow and fast in both adult females and subadults. Third, concerning head movements, while adult males displayed more horizontal than vertical movements, both types were equally observed in adult females and subadults. Fourth, all individuals approached significantly more often silently than accompanied by whistle calls. Finally, two behaviours were found only in adult females and subadults, and not in adult males: belly presentation (adult females 6.7%, subadults 18% of times), and body rotation (adult females 16.1%, subadults 16.4%).
Table 1.
Comparisons | Adult females | Adult males | Subadults |
---|---|---|---|
Alone versus with another orca(s) | 3.05 ± 0.06/1.16 ± 0.02 | 4.61 ± 0.65/0.88 ± 0.37 | 4.72 ± 2.04/0.72 ± 0.54 |
N = 56 z = 3.32 | N = 17 z = 5.51 | N = 11 z = 2.93 | |
p = 0.0009 | p = 0.0007 | p = 0.003 | |
Close versus Far | 3.86 ± 0.07/0.3 ± 0 | 5.11 ± 0.83/0.2 ± 0.1 | 5.45 ± 2.67/0.1 ± 0.09 |
N = 56 z = 6.23 | N = 18 z = 3.62 | N = 11 z = 2.93 | |
p = 0.00001 | p = 0.0003 | p = 0.003 | |
Very close versus Close | 0.96 ± 0.03/2.9 ± 0 | 3.11 ± 0.57/2.05 ± 0.59 | 2.91 ± 2.25/2.54 ± 0.55 |
N = 54 z = 4.89 | N = 15 z = 1.47 | N = 10 z = 1.68 | |
p < 0.0001 | p = 0.14 | p = 0.09 | |
Short versus Long gaze | 2.71 ± 0.04/1.46 ± 0.04 | 2.16 ± 0.61/3.33 ± 0.56 | 3.72 ± 1.84/1.63 ± 0.86 |
N = 40 z = 3.91 | N = 16 z = 1.75 | N = 9 z = 2.07 | |
p = 0.00009 | p = 0.08 | p = 0.04 | |
Slow versus Fast swimming | 4.25 ± 0.07/1.87 ± 0.03 | 4.22 ± 0.61/1.11 ± 0.43 | 4.45 ± 1.34/4.49 ± 1.35 |
N = 52 z = 1.48 | N = 17 z = 3.40 | N = 10 z = 0.96 | |
p = 0.14 | p = 0.0006 | p = 0.33 | |
Horizontal versus Vertical head movement | 0.36 ± 0.01/0.25 ± 0.01 | 0.5 ± 0.18/0.22 ± 0.10 | 0.36 ± 0.36/0.45 ± 0.36 |
N = 16 z = 0.98 | N = 6 z = 2.20 | N = 1 | |
p = 0.33 | p = 0.03 | ||
Whistling versus Silence | 1.05 ± 0.27/3.13 ± 0.50 | 1.27 ± 0.40/4.22 ± 0.65 | 1.83 ± 1.05/8.33 ± 4.41 |
N = 55 z = 4.47 | N = 16 z = 2 | N = 11 z = 3.059 | |
p = 0.00008 | p = 0.0006 | p = 0.003 |
In each cell: First line – mean number of occurrences (+/− standard error) for the two compared behavioural units; Second and third lines – Number of individuals taken into account in the statistical analysis (equal scores excluded), z score and p value of the Wilcoxon test (significances in bold). NA: test not applicable.
The above differences were confirmed for some of the measured variables when running comparisons between adult males, adult females and subadults (inter-classes comparisons). Indeed, adult males approached significantly the diver at a shorter distance than adult females (Mann-Whitney test, n1 = 18 n2 = 60, z = −3.11, p = 0.002), but only a trend was found with subadults (n1 = 18 n2 = 11, z = 1.77, p = 0.08) (Fig. 2). Also, adult females and subadults did not differ in the proportion of times they approached at a very close distance (z = 0.01, p = 0.99). Moreover, adult males displayed significantly fewer short gazes than adult females (z = 4.05, p = 0.0001) and than subadults (z = −2.89; p = 0.004), whereas adult males and subadults did not differ (z = −0.72; p = 0.47) (Fig. 3).
The swimming speed was also significantly slower in adult males than in subadults (z = 2.29, p = 0.007), while adult females’ speed did not differ (when applying Bonferroni correction) with those of adult males (z = −2.001, p = 0.045) and subadults (z = 1.35, p = 0.18) (Fig. 4).
No influence of age and sex was found for the proportion of solitary approaches (Kruskall-Wallis test: H(2.89) = 2.69, p = 0.26; males 88.56 ± 4.10%, females 69.91 ± 5.26% and subadults 93.39 ± 3.57%), horizontal head movements (H(2.89) = 3.68 p = 0.16; males 34.72 ± 10.91%, females 20.97 ± 4.98% and subadults 4.55 ± 4.55%), whistling approaches (H(2.89) = 0.74, p = 0.69; males 34.72 ± 5.25%, females 22.51 ± 4.23% and subadults 12.14 ± 4.37%).
In order to better understand the context of whistling, we tested whether certain variables were more often associated than others. Indeed, we found that while there were as many approaches with whistles when the orca approached alone or not (Wilcoxon matched-paired test: N = 83, Q = 1806, p = 0.743), there were significantly more approaches with whistles when the swimming speed was slow than fast (N = 83, Q = 2478, p = 0.0001).
Discussion
This first investigation of the nature of the exploratory behaviour of free-ranging orcas when approaching human divers shows two important findings. First, adult females prefer to visually monitor humans with their left eye than with their right eye, but no eye preference was found in adult males and subadults. Second, the approach behaviours differed significantly between different individuals; adult males demonstrated stronger attention, as well as potentially greater curiosity (shorter distance, slower swimming and longer gazes) than adult females and subadults, the latter being possibly more cautious.
The visual laterality in favour of the left eye is in accordance with the previous findings on social interactions in wild and captive cetaceans18, 39 and with captive dolphins when looking at humans21 and at familiar toys22. It therefore confirms the dominance of the right hemisphere in the visual exploration of some salient and familiar items by cetaceans (see also ref. 64) and allows us to support the hypothesis that the human diver is a source of interest for the free-ranging orcas habituated to their presence (for about 18 years). This hypothesis is also supported by the fact that no aggressiveness or sudden withdrawal has been noticed.
However, not all individuals preferred a left-side gazing in our study. Several non-mutually exclusive hypotheses may explain the individual variations observed. First, it is possible that laterality is more pronounced in older individuals than younger ones, due to their experiences with those unusual encounters, which would account for the absence of laterality in subadults. Indeed, the strength of laterality in animals is frequently related to the degree of familiarity with the observed item22. This is also supported by the fact that more adult females are in contact with divers than adult males in this area, and thus more experienced with human encounters. Over two consecutive years, the number of approaches by adult females was 4.5 times higher than the number of approaches by adult males. Second, it is possible that the emotional state of male and female adults differed in our study, as this is another factor of variation in humans and animals for both strength and direction of laterality65– 67. The fact that females generally keep a greater distance than males here and only observe humans briefly could be the sign of cautiousness in females that, while approaching, would remain more vigilant. Third, it is very likely that there is a link between the direction of the laterality and the degree of attention of the observer, as it was found earlier in birds68. Males, being more attentive, perhaps integrate more details, thus mobilizing their two hemispheres. The left hemisphere is known to play a key role for detailed visual exploration in various animal species69–73. Fourth, the hormonal difference between males and females may also be responsible for the sex-dependent laterality observed here. A study on testosterone-treated chicks found that an effect of testosterone was greater reliance on the frontal visual field74, which could be a possible explanation for the absence of lateral bias due to a stronger collaboration between the two hemispheres in male orcas. Fifth, differences in cognitive abilities between males and females could explain the sex-dependent laterality found in our study. In humans, females perform better than males in various memory tasks75, 76. This female superiority would be explained by a more effective use of the abilities of the right hemisphere for accessing memory of patterns made up of multiple items2.
Also in our study, adult males approached less often than adult females; this is probably due simply to their smaller number in the overall population. The fact that males approached divers at a closer distance and for longer exploration sequences is an important point of discussion. One abovementioned explanation is that males may be more curious. Unfortunately, to our knowledge, there is no such “personality type” study done with cetaceans that could confirm this hypothesis. However, sex-specific responses to human presence have been found earlier in cetaceans. Williams et al.77 studied the habituation to human activities by orcas in Johnson Detroits (British Columbia). They found that the animal’s reaction to boats changed in a sex-specific way with time. While females tended to accelerate their swimming speed and increase the angle of their diving routes, males kept their swimming speed constant with less predictable trajectory. This said, the fact that females approached less closely in our study does not mean that they did not “observe”; cetaceans may use echolocation to complete their visual exploration36, 78. It is possible that females prioritized long-distance echolocation over short-distance eye observation. In line with this, vertical head movements, typically associated with echolocation61, were particularly frequent in adult females. Scheer et al.61 also described this echolocation behaviour directed to humans at a 20-meter distance in free-ranging pilot whales (Globicephala macrorhynchus). As Blois-Heulin et al.22, we suppose that echolocation, associated to a binocular vision, completes the amount of information collected on objects, facilitating the construction of its representation.
The approaches described here are signs of a certain curiosity, and possibly attempts to initiate interactions with divers. The fact that the vast majority of the approaches come from the front clearly indicates that animals behave to remain visible. Scheer et al.61 with free-ranging pilot whales and Duroyon62 with captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) have also described behaviours like belly presentation, body rotation, horizontal head movements that were clearly associated with non-agonistic encounters and interaction initiations. To our knowledge, the function of whistling in this context has never been studied, but it is intriguing to find more calls associated with closer and longer approaches. This also shows that orcas do not seek to be silent, whereas they do when hunting mammals79–81. We must acknowledge however that we could not identify callers and more solid conclusions require further investigation.
In conclusion, the fact that visual laterality and exploratory behaviours are sex- and age-dependent has interesting perspectives in both basic (understanding of the underlying, notably psychological, mechanisms and evolutionary processes) and applied research fields. Indeed, human-cetacean encounters are increasing all around the world82, 83 and the development of ecotourism over the last 20 years has created a significant pressure on the environment of cetaceans, and this is not limited to cetaceans. Complementary studies are necessary to explore further the different hypotheses raised here, and notably to test to which extent the motivation, the temperament, the degree of attention and the level of experience of animals are determining factors of behavioural variations.
Materials and Methods
Study animals, context of the study and video recordings
The presence of orcas in the Kalkfjord (Norway) is directly linked to the massive presence of specific prey, i.e. atlantic herrings (Clupea harengus). As a result, the same population of orcas comes back to this location every year84. Individuals are identified based on classic criterion for that species, such as the size and the shape of the dorsal fin and the saddle patch as well as bodies scars85. Only individuals clearly identified and observed at least twice during the study have been included in the data set, i.e. 60 adult females, 18 adult males, 11 unsexed subadults.
The underwater videos used for the analyses have been made by Pierre Robert de Latour during ecotouristic dives organised in Norway on winters 2014 and 2015 (November to February). The expeditions have been set up from the base M/S SULA and the dives started from a small barge, within a narrow period of daylight (i.e. 10 am–2 pm). Orcas are first observed at a distance, and then the barge is used to approach the animals following the USEA (Undersea Soft Encounter Alliance) method developed by Pierre Robert de Latour. This method is based on the principle of a limited intrusion into the orcas’ space, who can then decide to approach the barge and the divers further or not. The divers (including snorkelers), with a maximum of eight people simultaneously in the water, are gathered just behind the guide, Pierre Robert de Latour. All people are commanded to stay as a group, move slowly and silently.
Video recordings have been made with an underwater camera Sony Ilce A6000 with a Nauticam NA 6000 housing, and equipped with a 24 mm focal equivalent to 24*36 (shutter speed: 1/25 to 1/100, aperture: f2.8, ISO: automatic limited at 3200, White Balance: 4800 k). All in all, 902 video sequences (collected with an ad libitum sampling method, i.e. at various times of the diving session and with various durations) have been analysed, that is 540 minutes of film sampled (2014: 323 video sequences lasting 163 minutes in total; 2015: 579 video sequences lasting 377 minutes in total).
Our study is observational and totally non-invasive, based on video scoring. Videos come from the archive of the USEA – Orques Sans Frontieres program (permit #USEA2017-0007).
Behavioural repertoire and data collection
A detailed behavioural repertoire has been established to sample the different movements and other exploratory behaviours of orcas when approaching the divers (see photo examples in Fig. 5). Here, an approach is defined as follows1: an approach is systematically an animal movement oriented in the direction of the divers2; an approach starts from the moment where an orca enters the camera range and ends as soon as it gets out. The behavioural repertoire, composed of 10 measured variables (most of them being divided into mutually exclusive units), is given in Table 2. For each single approach, we used a 1-0 sampling method to score all measured variables.
Table 2.
Measured variables | Behavioural unit definitions |
---|---|
Distance (minimum distance between the orca and the diving guide reached during a given approach) | The orca approaches the diver from: |
- Very close (less than 5 meters) | |
- Close (between 5 and 20 meters) | |
- Far (more than 20 meters). | |
Angle of approach | The orca approaches the diver from: |
- The front: | |
* from the right side of the diver (angle: 30 to 120°) | |
* from the left side of the diver (angle: 200 to 330°) | |
* facing the diver (angle: 0°) | |
- Below: | |
* moving just under the diver | |
* arriving vertically from deep water | |
- The back. | |
Swimming speed | - Slow: the orca moves but the movements of its caudal fin are hardly detectable |
- Fast: at least one caudal fin movement per second. | |
Gaze laterality | The orca looks at the diver with: |
- its right eye | |
- its left eye | |
- both eyes (bilateral) | |
For a given approach, the eye used may change during the behavioural sequence and each gaze change is counted for the laterality analysis. | |
Gaze duration (mono- and binocular pooled) | The orca looks at the diver for: |
- less than 6 seconds (Short gaze) | |
- more than 6 seconds (Long gaze) | |
For a given approach, we measured the total gazing duration. | |
Head movements | - Horizontal (the orca turns slightly its head towards the diver) |
- Vertical (the orca is facing the diver and moves the head up and down several times in a row). | |
Whistle calls | - Whistling (a high-pitched modulated frequency is heard) |
- Silence (no species-specific sound is heard) | |
It is not possible to confirm whether the caller is the observed orca or a neighbouring one. | |
Belly presentation | The orca moves the head up presenting its belly to the humans. |
Body rotation | The orca rotates its body along the horizontal axis. |
Statistical analyses
In a first step, to study the possible existence of a visual laterality bias during approaches at the population level, we compared, using Wilcoxon tests for matched samples, the number of gazes from the left eye with the number of gazes from the right eye directed to the humans based on all individuals sampled in each age-sex class.
In a second step, we tested whether the different individuals differed in the way they approached humans. On the one hand, using Wilcoxon tests for matched samples, we compared, for each measured variable and each age-sex class of individuals, the number of occurrences of the different behavioural units sampled. On the other hand, we calculated for each individual sampled the percentage of expression of a given unit over all the behavioural units of each measured variable. These individual percentages were then used to compare the behavioural profiles of adult males, adult females and unsexed subadults using Kruskal-Wallis and post-hoc Mann - Whitney tests (applying Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons). Significant threshold was set at 0.05 (and 0.0167 in case of Bonferroni correction).
Acknowledgements
We thank the USEA – Orques Sans Frontières program for providing the videos used in this study. We are very grateful to Veronique Biquand for her assistance with the statistics and to Hadley Couraud for correcting our English.
Author Contributions
P.R.L. collected the videos. S.C. scored the videos. S.C., C.B.H. and A.L. analysed the data and wrote the paper.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Rogers, L. J. & Andrew, R. Comparative vertebrate lateralization (Cambridge University Press, 2002).
- 2.Rogers, L. J., Vallortigara, G. & Andrew, R. J. Divided brains: the biology and behaviour of brain asymmetries (Cambridge University Press, 2013).
- 3.Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. Lateralization of response to social stimuli in fishes: A comparison between different methods and species. Physiol. Behav. 2001;74:237–244. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00552-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lippolis G, Joss JMP, Rogers LJ. Australian Lungfish (Neoceratodus forsteri): a missing link in the evolution of complementary side biases for predator avoidance and prey capture. Brain Behav. Evol. 2009;73:295–303. doi: 10.1159/000230674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Robins A, Rogers LJ. Lateralized prey catching responses in the toad (Bufo marinus): analysis of complex visual stimuli. Anim. Behav. 2004;68:567–575. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.12.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lippolis G, Bisazza A, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Lateralization of predator avoidance responses in three species of toads. Laterality. 2002;7:163–183. doi: 10.1080/13576500143000221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bonati, B. & Csermely, D. Lateralization in Lizards: Evidence of presence in several contexts in Behavioral Lateralization in Vertebrates (eds Csermely, D. & Regolin, L.) 35–38 (Springer, Heidelberg, 2011).
- 8.Deckel AW, Fuqua L. Effects of serotonergic drugs on lateralized aggression and aggressive displays in Anolis carolinensis. Behav. Brain. Res. 1998;95:227–232. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00048-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Franklin WE, Lima SL. Laterality in avian vigilance; do sparrow have a favourite eye? Anim. Behav. 2001;62:879–885. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1826. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ventolini N, et al. Laterality in the wild: preferential hemifield use during predatory and sexual behaviour in the black winged stilt (Himantopus himantopus) Anim. Behav. 2005;69:1077–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Chapelain AS, Blois-Heulin C. Eye preference in Campbell’s monkeys (Cercopithecus c. campbelli) Anim. Cogn. 2009;12:11–19. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0164-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ. Eye preference in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): influence of age, stimulus and hand preferences. Laterality. 1998;3:180–191. doi: 10.1080/713754297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bracchini SN, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Tecumseh Fitch W. Eye preferences in captive chimpanzees. Anim. Cogn. 2012;15:971–978. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0523-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Des Roches ADB, Richard-Yris MA, Henry S, Ezzaouïa M, Hausberger M. Laterality and emotions: visual laterality in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) differs with objects’ emotional value. Physiol. Behav. 2008;94:487–490. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Siniscalchi M, Sasso R, Pepe AM, Vallortigara G, Quaranta A. Dogs turn left to emotional stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;208:516–521. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Frasnelli E, Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. Response competition associated with right-left antennal asymmetries of new and old olfactory memory traces in honeybees. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;209:36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jozet-Alves C, et al. Visual lateralization is task- and age-dependent in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) Anim. Behav. 2012;83:1313–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.02.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Karenina K, Giljov A, Ivkovich T, Burdin A, Malashichev Y. Lateralization of spatial relationships between wild mother and infant orcas (Orcinus orca) Anim. Behav. 2013;86:1225–1231. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.025. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cantalupo C, Bisazza A, Vallortiga G. Lateralization of predator evasion response in a teleost fish (Girardinus falcatus: Poeciliidae) Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:1637–1646. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00043-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Quaranta A, Siniscalchi M, Vallortigara G. Asymmetric tail wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:199–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Thieltges H, Lemasson A, Kuczaj S, Böye M, Blois-Heulin C. Visual laterality in dolphins when looking at (un)familiar humans. Anim. Cogn. 2011;14:303–308. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0354-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Blois-Heulin C, Crevel M, Lemasson A, Böye M. Visual laterality in dolphins: importance of the familiarity of stimuli. BMC Neuroscience. 2012;13:9. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Giljov AN, Karenina KA, Malashichev YB. An eye for a worm: lateralisation of feeding behaviour in aquatic anamniotes. Laterality. 2009;14(3):273–286. doi: 10.1080/13576500802379665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rogers LJ, Zucca P, Vallortigara G. Advantage of having a lateralized brain. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2004;271:S420–S422. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rogers, L. J. & Kaplan, G. An eye for a predator: lateralization in birds, with particular reference to the Australian magpie in Behavioral and morphological asymmetries in vertebrates (eds Malashichev, Y. & Deckel, W.) 47–57 (Austin, Landes Bioscience, 2006).
- 26.Siniscalchi M, Dimatteo S, Pepe AM, Sasso R, Quaranta A. Visual lateralisation in wild striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoabla) in response in stimuli with different degrees of familiarity. PLoS One. 2012;7(1):e3001. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ. Eye preferences in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): influence of age, stimulus and hand preferences. Laterality. 1998;3:109–130. doi: 10.1080/713754297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kruper DC, Boyle BE, Patton RA. Eyes and hand preferences in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Psycho. Sciences. 1966;5:277–278. doi: 10.3758/BF03328393. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Baraud I, Buytet B, Bec P, Blois-Heulin C. Social laterality and ‘transversality’ in two species of mangabeys: influence of rank and implication for hemispheric specialization. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;198:449–458. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Casperd JM, Rim D. Asymmetries in the visual processing of emotional cues during agonistic interactions by gelada baboons. Behav. Process. 1996;37:57–65. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(95)00075-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hopkins WD, Bennett AJ. Handedness and approach-avoiding behaviour in chimpanzees. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1994;20:413–418. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.20.4.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Quaresmini C, Forrester GS, Spiezio C, Vallortigara G. Social environment elicits lateralized behaviors in gorillas and chimpanzees. J. Comp. Psychol. 2014;128:276–284. doi: 10.1037/a0036355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rogers LJ. Evolution of hemispheric specialisation: advantages and disadvantages. Brain Lang. 2000;73:236–253. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Casperd LM, Dunbar RIM. Asymmetries in the visual processing of emotional cues during agonistic interactions by gelada baboons. Behav. Processes. 1996;37:57–65. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(95)00075-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Austin NP, Rogers LJ. Lateralization of agonistic and vigilance responses in Przewalski horses (Equus przewalskii) Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014;151:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.11.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pack AA, Herman LM, Hoffmann-Khunt M, Branstetter BK. The object behind the echo: dolphins (Tursiop truncatus) perceive object shape globally through echolocation. Behav. Process. 2002;58:1–26. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(01)00200-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dawson, W. W. The cetacean eye in Cetacean Behavior (eds Herman, L. M.) 53–100 (Wiley, New York, 1980).
- 38.Marino, L. Evolution of the brain and cognition in cetaceans in Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience (eds Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P. & Shackelford, T. K.) 163–193 (Cambridge MIT Press, 2006).
- 39.Karenina K, Giljov A, Glazov D, Malashiche Y. Social laterality in wild beluga whale infants: comparisons between locations, escort conditions, and ages. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2013;67:1195–1204. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1545-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hill HM, et al. Lateralized swim positions are conserved across environments for beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) mother-calf pairs. Behav Processes. 2017;138:22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Karenina K, Giljov A, Ivkovich T, Burdin A, Malashichev Y. Evidence for the perceptual origin of right-sided feeding biases in cetaceans. Anim. Cogn. 2015;19(1):239–243. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0899-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sakai M, Hishii T, Takeda S, Kohshima S. Laterality of flipper rubbing and behaviour in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aducuns): caused by asymmetry of eye use? Behav. Brain Res. 2006;170(2):204–210. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Yeater DB, Hill HM, Baus N, Farnell H, Kuczaj SA. Visual laterality in belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) and Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens) when viewing familiar and unfamiliar humans. Anim. Cogn. 2014;17(6):1245–1259. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0756-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Guarino, S., Yeater, D., Lacy, S., Dees, T. & Hill, H.M. Responses to familiar and unfamiliar objects by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas), bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), and Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens). Anim. Cogn. (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 45.Hill HM, et al. Responses to familiar and unfamiliar humans by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas), bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), & Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens): a replication and extension. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2016;29:1–21. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Baird, R. W. Killer Whales of the world: Natural history and conservation. Stillwater (MN: Voyageur Press, 2002).
- 47.Rendell L, Whitehead H. Culture in whales and dolphins. Behav. Brain Sci. 2001;24:309–324. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0100396X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Yurk H, Barrett-Lennard LG, Ford JKB, Matkin C. Cultural transmission within marternal lineages: Vocal clans in resident killer whales in southern Alaska. Anim. Behav. 2002;63:1103–1119. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.3012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ford JKB. Acoustic behavior of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1989;67:727–745. doi: 10.1139/z89-105. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Whitehead, H. & Mann, J. Female reproductive strategies of cetaceans in Cetacean societies, field studies of dolphins and whales (eds Mann, J., Connor, R.C., Tyack, P.L. & Whitehead, H.) 219–246 (Chicago, The University of Chicago Press, 2000).
- 51.Nakahra F, Takemura A. A survey on the behaviour of captive ondotocetes in Japan. Aquatic Mammals. 1997;23.3:135–143. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Paulos R, Trone M, Kuczaj SA. Play in wild in captive cetaceans. Int. J. Comp. Psychology. 2010;23:701–722. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Whitehead H, Rendell LE, Osborne RW, Würsing B. Culture and conservation of non-humans with reference to whales and dolphins Review and new directions. Biol. Conserv. 2004;120:427–437. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2004.03.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lynn DA, Brown GR. The Ontogeny of Exploratory Behavior in Male and Female Adolescent Rats (Rattus norvegicus) Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51(6):513–520. doi: 10.1002/dev.20386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Reader, S.M. & Laland, K.N. Primate Innovation: sex, age and social rank differences. Int. J. Primatol. 22(5) (2001).
- 56.Fairbanks LA. Risk-Taking by Juvenile Vervet Monkeys. Behaviour. 1993;124(1/2):57–72. doi: 10.1163/156853993X00506. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K. Anti-predator strategies of free-ranging Campbell’s monkeys. Behaviour. 2009;146:1687–1708. doi: 10.1163/000579509X12469533725585. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kuczaj, S. A. & Walker, R. T. How do dolphins solve problems? In Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence (eds Wasserman, E.A. & Zentall, T.R.) 580–601 (New-York: Oxford University, 2006).
- 59.Kuczaj SA, Makecha R, Trone M, Paulos RD, Ramos JA. Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: Evidence from the play of dolphin calves. International J. Comp. Psychol. 2006;19:223–240. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Baird RW, Bradley Hanson M, Lawrence MD. Factors influencing the diving behaviour of fish-eating killer whales: sex differences and diel and interannual variation in diving rates. Can. J. Zool. 2005;83:257–267. doi: 10.1139/z05-007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Scheer, M., Hofmann, B. & Behr, I.P. Ethogram of selected behaviors initiated by free-ranging short-finned pilot whales (Globicephala macrorhynchus) and directed to human swimmers during open water encounters. Anthrozoös17(3) (2004).
- 62.Duroyon, C. Problématique de la constitution d’un répertoire comportemental chez une espèce de mammifères marins, le dauphin souffleur (Tursiops truncatus). Thèse vétérinaire. Ecole Nationale Vétérinaire d’Alfort (2009).
- 63.Patterson, E. M. & Mann, J. Cetacean Innovation in Animal creativity and innovation (eds. Kaufman, A.B. & Kaufman, J.) 73–120 (Academic Press, 2015).
- 64.Siniscalchi M, Dimatteo S, Pepe AM, Sasso R, Quaranta A. Visual lateralization in wild striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) in response to stimuli with different degrees of familiarity. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(1):e30001. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Erhan H, Borod JC, Tenke CE, Bruder GE. Identification of emotion in a dichotic listening task: event-related brain potential and behavioral findings. Brain Cogn. 1998;37:286–307. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.0984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Borod JC. Interhemispheric and intrahemispheric control of emotion: a focus on unilateral brain damage. J. Consult. Clinic. Psychol. 1992;60:339–348. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.60.3.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Andrew, R. J. & Rogers, L. J. The nature of lateralization in tetrapods in Comparative vertebrate lateralization (eds Rogers, L. J. & Andrew, R. J.) 94–125 (Cambridge University Press, 2002).
- 68.George I, Vernier B, Richard J-P, Hausberger M, Cousillas H. Hemispheric Specialization in the Primary Auditory Area of Awake and Anesthetized Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;118(3):597–610. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.3.597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Fagard, J. & Streri, J. Développement des asymétries chez le bébé, in Droitiers et gauchers, des asymétries dans tous les sens (ed. Fagard, J.) 49–70 (Solal Press, 2006).
- 70.MacNeilage PF, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. Origins of the left and right brain. Scientific American. 2009;301:60–67. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0709-60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Tommasi L, Vallortigana G. Encoding of geometric and landmark information in the left and right hemispheres of the avian brain. Behav. Neurosci. 2001;115:602–613. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.3.602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Tommasi L, Vallortigana G. Hemispheric processing of landmark and geometric information in male and female domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) Behav. Brain Res. 2004;155:85–96. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Vallortigana G, Rogers LJ. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav. Process. 2005;33:41–57. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(94)90059-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Rogers LJ, Andrew RJ. Frontal and lateral visual field use after treatment with testosterone. Anim. Behav. 1989;38:394–405. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80032-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.James TW, Kimura D. Sex differences in remembering the locations of objects in an array: location-shift versus location-exchanges. Evol. Hum. Behav. 1997;18:155–163. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00004-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Herlitz A, Nilsson L-G, Backman L. Gender differences in episodic memory. Mem. Cognit. 1997;25:801–811. doi: 10.3758/BF03211324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Williams R, Trites AW, Bain DE. Behavioral responses of killer whales (Orcinus orca) to whale-watching boats: opportunistic observations and experimental approaches. J. Zool. Lond. 2002;256:255–270. doi: 10.1017/S0952836902000298. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Barrett-Lennard LG, Ford JKB, Heise KA. The mixed blessing of echolocation: differences in sonar use by fish-eating and mammal-eating killer whales. Anim. Behav. 1996;51:553–565. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Morton, A. B. A quantitative comparison of the behaviour of resident and transient forms of the killer whale off the central British Columbia coast. Report of the International Whaling Commission (special issue 12), 245–248 (1990).
- 80.Simon M, McGregor PK, Ugarte F. The relationship between the acousticbehaviour and surface activity of killer whales (Orcinus orca) that feed onherring (Clupea harengus) Acta Ethol. 2007;10:47–53. doi: 10.1007/s10211-007-0029-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Simon M, Wahlberg M, Miller LA. Echolocation clicks from killer whales (Orcinus orca) feeding on herring (Clupea harengus) (L.) J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007;121:749–752. doi: 10.1121/1.2404922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hoyt, E. The Worldwide Value and Extent of Whale Watching (Bath, UK: Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society), (1995).
- 83.Hoyt, E. Whale Watching in Worldwide Tourism Numbers, Expenditures, and Expanding Socioeconomic Benefits (Crowborough, UK: International Fund for Animal Welfare, 2001).
- 84.Vester H, Hammerschmidt K. First record of killer whales (Orcinus Orca) feeding on Atlantic salmon (Salmon salar) in northern Norway suggest a multi-prey feeding type. Marine Biodiversity Records. 2012;6:1–5. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Ford, J. K. B., Elis, G. & Balcomb, K. Killer Whales (UBC Press, 1994).