Skip to main content
Journal of Neurophysiology logoLink to Journal of Neurophysiology
. 2017 Jun 21;118(3):1622–1636. doi: 10.1152/jn.00451.2017

The cerebellum does more than sensory prediction error-based learning in sensorimotor adaptation tasks

Peter A Butcher 1,2,, Richard B Ivry 3,4, Sheng-Han Kuo 5, David Rydz 5, John W Krakauer 6, Jordan A Taylor 1,2
PMCID: PMC5596119  PMID: 28637818

Individuals with cerebellar pathology are impaired in sensorimotor adaptation. This deficit has been attributed to an impairment in error-based learning, specifically, from a deficit in using sensory prediction errors to update an internal model. Here we show that these individuals also have difficulty in discovering an aiming solution to overcome their adaptation deficit, suggesting a new role for the cerebellum in sensorimotor adaptation tasks.

Keywords: visuomotor rotation, cerebellum, implicit learning, explicit learning, aiming, motor learning

Abstract

Individuals with damage to the cerebellum perform poorly in sensorimotor adaptation paradigms. This deficit has been attributed to impairment in sensory prediction error-based updating of an internal forward model, a form of implicit learning. These individuals can, however, successfully counter a perturbation when instructed with an explicit aiming strategy. This successful use of an instructed aiming strategy presents a paradox: In adaptation tasks, why do individuals with cerebellar damage not come up with an aiming solution on their own to compensate for their implicit learning deficit? To explore this question, we employed a variant of a visuomotor rotation task in which, before executing a movement on each trial, the participants verbally reported their intended aiming location. Compared with healthy control participants, participants with spinocerebellar ataxia displayed impairments in both implicit learning and aiming. This was observed when the visuomotor rotation was introduced abruptly (experiment 1) or gradually (experiment 2). This dual deficit does not appear to be related to the increased movement variance associated with ataxia: Healthy undergraduates showed little change in implicit learning or aiming when their movement feedback was artificially manipulated to produce similar levels of variability (experiment 3). Taken together the results indicate that a consequence of cerebellar dysfunction is not only impaired sensory prediction error-based learning but also a difficulty in developing and/or maintaining an aiming solution in response to a visuomotor perturbation. We suggest that this dual deficit can be explained by the cerebellum forming part of a network that learns and maintains action-outcome associations across trials.

NEW & NOTEWORTHY Individuals with cerebellar pathology are impaired in sensorimotor adaptation. This deficit has been attributed to an impairment in error-based learning, specifically, from a deficit in using sensory prediction errors to update an internal model. Here we show that these individuals also have difficulty in discovering an aiming solution to overcome their adaptation deficit, suggesting a new role for the cerebellum in sensorimotor adaptation tasks.


visuomotor rotation tasks, which induce a discrepancy between movements of the limb and visual feedback, are a powerful tool for elucidating principles of motor learning (for a review see Krakauer 2009). While learning in these tasks has typically been thought to be implicit, reflecting the training of an internal forward model via sensory prediction errors (Mazzoni and Krakauer 2006; Schlerf et al. 2012; Tseng et al. 2007), it has become clear that multiple processes can contribute to performance. These include reinforcement learning (Galea et al. 2011; Huang et al. 2011; Nikooyan and Ahmed 2015), use-dependent plasticity (Diedrichsen et al. 2010; Huang et al. 2011; Verstynen and Sabes 2011), and instructed aiming strategies (Benson et al. 2011; Mazzoni and Krakauer 2006; Taylor and Ivry 2011).

Most relevant to the present study is our recent work dissociating changes in performance that arise from modifications in aiming, which can be explicitly reported, and learning processes that are implicit, such as sensory prediction error-based learning (Bond and Taylor 2015; McDougle et al. 2015; Taylor et al. 2014). The latter has been associated with the cerebellum, with compelling evidence coming from studies showing that individuals with cerebellar pathology display significant impairments in a range of sensorimotor adaptation tasks (Martin et al. 1996; Rabe et al. 2009; Schlerf et al. 2013; Smith and Shadmehr 2005; Weiner et al. 1983). For example, in visuomotor rotation tasks, people with spinocerebellar ataxia (SCA) exhibit a reduced ability to counter experimentally imposed perturbations. Importantly, they show attenuated aftereffects when the perturbation is removed (Schlerf et al. 2013; Werner et al. 2009). This is consistent with the idea that an intact cerebellum is required to compute sensory prediction errors via a forward model (Haruno et al. 2001; Izawa et al. 2012; Miall and Wolpert 1996). This hypothesis is further supported by neuroimaging in humans (Schlerf et al. 2012) and neurophysiology in nonhuman species (Brooks et al. 2015) showing cerebellar activity that is correlated with sensory prediction errors.

In the context of a visuomotor rotation, we define aiming as choosing to move the hand toward a location other than the target with the goal of having the cursor land on the target. A priori, there is no reason to believe that aiming is cerebellum dependent. Indeed, when individuals with cerebellar degeneration are provided with an explicit aiming strategy and visual cues to support the implementation of that strategy, they show near-perfect performance in a visuomotor adaptation task (Taylor et al. 2010).

Given their ability to use an instructed strategy, it is puzzling that individuals with cerebellar damage are impaired in visuomotor rotation tasks when they are not provided with an explicit strategy. That is, why do they fail on their own to come up with an aiming solution to compensate for their impaired implicit learning, especially given that the perturbation results in a salient error that remains for many trials? This paradox suggests that the cerebellum may be necessary not only for implicit learning but also for discovering, implementing, and/or adjusting an appropriate aiming solution when it is not provided directly through instruction (see also Vaca-Palomares et al. 2013).

We have developed a simple method to assess trial-by-trial fluctuations in aiming during visuomotor rotation tasks (Taylor et al. 2014) that allows for continuous assessment of aiming behavior and implicit learning. In the present set of experiments, we employed this method to determine the source(s) of impairment in individuals with cerebellar damage.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Experiment 1.

Ten individuals with SCA (average age = 53.7 yr, SD = 12.6 yr; 2 women, 8 men; 5 right-handed) were recruited for the study at the 2012 National Ataxia Foundation Annual Meeting (San Antonio, TX) and from the Berkeley, California community. SCA participants were only included if they did not exhibit clinical signs of cerebellar-type multiple system atrophy or evidence of moderate cognitive impairment. Four of the SCA group had a confirmed genetic subtype; for the others, the diagnosis was sporadic adult-onset ataxia of unknown etiology (Table 1). The severity of ataxia was assessed with the International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale (ICARS; Trouillas et al. 1997). The SCA participants had an average ICARS score of 26.5 (SD = 14.7) out of a maximum score of 100. The participants were also screened for cognitive impairment with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; average = 28.1, SD = 1.5; all scored within normal range of 26–30) (Nasreddine et al. 2005).

Table 1.

SCA neuropsychology and demographic information

Participant Sex Age, yr Ed, yr Handedness Type Diag, yr Ataxia Scale MoCA
Abrupt
    ARSCA1 Female 63 16 Left SCA8 11 27
    ARSCA2 Male 73 20 Left SAOA 4 20.5 28
    ARSCA3 Male 41 16 Left SAOA 19 27 29
    ARSCA4 Female 55 14 Right SCA2 13 48 26
    ARSCA5 Male 47 16 Left SAOA 19 22 29
    ARSCA6 Male 42 16 Right SCA2 2 41 28
    ARSCA7 Male 62 19 Right SAOA/MSA 4 12 30
    ARSCA8 Male 70 16 Right SAOA/family hist 9 11 27.5
    ARSCA9 Male 44 22 Left SAOA/MSA 2 12 30
    ARSCA10 Male 40 16 Right SCA2 11 45 26
Gradual
    GRSCA1 Female 35 17 Right SCA1 4 6* 24
    GRSCA2 Female 30 17 Left SCA2 3 17*
    GRSCA3 Male 44 17 Right SCA1 1 9* 19
    GRSCA4 Male 79 13 Right SAOA 6 11* 14
    GRSCA5 Male 46 17 Right ADCA 1 7* 20
    GRSCA6 Male 41 17 Right SAOA 23.5* 23
    GRSCA7 Male 49 17 Right SAOA 3 8* 22
    GRSCA8 Male 36 17 Right SCA1 3 10.5* 25
    GRSCA9 Female 69 19 Left SAOA 8 3* 30
    GRSCA10 Female 52 22 Right SCA6 14 1 28

Ed, education; Diag, years since diagnosis; SAOA, sporadic adult-onset ataxia; MSA, multiple system atrophy; ADCA, autosomal dominant cerebellar ataxia.

*

SARA; otherwise ICARS.

Twelve age-matched control participants were recruited from the Berkeley and Princeton, New Jersey communities. These participants, based on self-reports, had no known neurological conditions. The data from two of the control participants were not included in the final analysis: One consistently moved too slowly, and the other failed to report the aiming locations on many trials (see below). Thus the control group consisted of 10 participants (average age = 59.7 yr, SD = 14.7 yr; 4 women, 6 men; all right-handed). The control participants were also administered the MoCA (average = 27.6, SD = 2.2), with two scoring just below the normal range cutoff of 26 (23 and 25). They were included in the analyses given that their performance was similar to the other control participants on the experimental task.

Experiment 2.

Twelve individuals with SCA were recruited from the Princeton community and from the Columbia University Medical Center (CUMC). These participants were selected after a clinical assessment determined that they did not exhibit symptoms of extracerebellar pathology (Parkinson’s disease, cerebellar-type multiple system atrophy). One individual was excluded from the analysis after failing to provide aiming reports, and a second was unable to complete the task in the time allotted, resulting in a final data set of 10 participants (average age = 48.1 yr, SD = 16.2 yr; average years of education = 17, SD = 1.6; 4 women, 6 men; 8 right-handed; 6 confirmed genetic subtype; Table 1). The severity of ataxia symptoms was evaluated with the Scale for the Assessment and Rating of Ataxia Severity (SARA; Schmitz-Hübsch et al. 2006). The SCA participants had an average SARA score of 10.6 (SD = 6.2) out of a maximum score of 40. The SARA scale was used in experiment 2, reflecting the preference of the CUMC neurologists. Because of time constraints, the MoCA was not administered to one of the SCA participants. Surprisingly, eight of the nine remaining SCA participants scored below the normal range on the MoCA (average score: 22.7, SD 4.8). In part, this may reflect the fact that for four of these participants English was a second language. It is also possible that this reflects a more severely compromised sample (although this is not supported by their ataxia scores) or more stringent scoring criteria. Ten age-matched control participants were recruited from the Princeton community (average age = 53.3 yr, SD = 10.5 yr; 5 women, 5 men; 10 right-handed). The control participants were administered the MoCA (average = 25.5, SD = 1.1); five participants scored just below the normal range (three 25 and two 24).

Experiment 3.

Twenty young adults were recruited from the research participation pool of the Department of Psychology at Princeton University (average age = 19.7 yr, SD = 1.5 yr; 9 women, 11 men; all right-handed).

The experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the University of California, Berkeley (experiment 1), CUMC (experiment 2), and Princeton University (experiments 2 and 3). All participants provided written informed consent. Participants in experiments 1 and 2 were paid an honorarium of $20/h, while participants in experiment 3 were compensated with class credit or $12.

Experimental Apparatus

In all three experiments, participants made 7-cm horizontal reaching movements to visual targets. The targets were displayed on a 15-in. (experiment 1) or 17-in. (experiments 2 and 3) LCD monitor. The monitor was mounted horizontally, positioned ~25 cm above a digitizing tablet (Intuous Pro Large, Wacom). Given the position of the monitor, vision of the limb was occluded. All participants, regardless of handedness, were tested with their right hand. A digitizing pen was held in the right hand—regardless of handedness preference—and the movement required sliding the pen across the surface of the tablet. Feedback of hand position, when provided, was displayed in the form of a circular cursor displayed on the monitor.

Procedure

We employed a variant of a visuomotor rotation task that requires participants to verbally report their aiming location on each trial. This procedure has been described in detail in a previous report (Taylor et al. 2014). At the start of each trial, a white ring was presented, indicating the distance of the participant’s hand from a start position (5-mm circle at center of screen). By continually making the ring smaller the participant could guide his or her hand to the start position. When the hand was within 1 cm of the center of the start position, the ring was replaced by a cursor, allowing the participant to precisely position the hand inside the start circle (Fig. 1A).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

General task outline. A: on each trial a single target appeared at 1 of 8 possible locations arranged equally around the start position. After the target appeared, but before moving, participants were asked to verbally report which number they were aiming toward for the current trial. B: in all 3 experiments, participants first completed 56 baseline trials with veridical feedback. This was followed by a rotation block in which the visual feedback was rotated about the start position. Finally, participants completed a no-feedback washout block, where both visual and auditory feedback were withheld. In experiments 1 and 3 participants completed 128 rotation trials for a total of 264 trials, while in experiment 2 participants completed 220 rotation trials for a total of 456 trials.

After the start position was maintained for 1 s, a green target circle (7-mm diameter) was presented. The target appeared at one of eight locations, separated by 45° along an invisible ring (radial distance from start circle of 7 cm). Each of the eight locations was presented within a block of eight trials, and within a block the locations were randomly selected.

Participants were instructed to make a ballistic reaching movement, with the goal of getting the feedback cursor to appear at the target location. Participants were encouraged to reach beyond the target location, effectively shooting through the target. We chose to have the participants reach past the target to minimize the impact of intention tremor on accuracy, given that this symptom can become pronounced at the end of a rapid movement. Feedback was presented as an end-point location in experiments 1 and 3. In these experiments, the cursor disappeared when the movement amplitude exceeded the 5-mm start circle and did not reappear until the amplitude reached 7 cm. End-point feedback was presented for 1.5 s at this location (subject to the visual perturbation—see below) in the form of a red cursor (3.5-mm diameter). In experiment 2, the cursor was visible during the outbound portion of the reach (online feedback). Once the amplitude reached 7 cm, the cursor position was frozen for 1.5 s. In all experiments, a pleasant “ding” sound was played if the feedback cursor intersected the target region; otherwise a mildly aversive “buzz” sound was played.

To encourage participants to make fast movements, a digital vocal sample saying “too slow” was played if the movement time was >400 ms. The movement protocol, involving an emphasis on fast movements with limited feedback (experiments 1 and 3), was adopted to minimize feedback corrections, a problem for individuals with SCA (Tseng et al. 2007).

The visual workspace included a ring of numbered “landmarks” spaced at regular intervals of 5.6° (Fig. 1A). The numbers increased and decreased in the clockwise and counterclockwise directions, respectively, from the target. As such, the order of the landmarks rotated with the target. Before each movement on aiming report trials, the participant verbally reported the landmark he/she planned to reach toward. These verbal reports were recorded by the experimenter. Trials in which participants failed to report their aim were excluded from the analysis. In experiment 1, the SCA participants failed to provide a report on 15.0% of the report trials, while control participants failed to provide a report on 1.8% of the trials. In experiment 2, the percentage of failed reports dropped to 1.6% and 0.4% for the SCA and control participants, respectively. In experiment 3, the college-age control participants failed to provide reports on 1.1% and 1.0% of the trials for the No-Variance and Variance-Added groups, respectively. Refinements in the clarity of task instructions are likely responsible for the higher response rate in experiments 2 and 3.

The experiment was divided into five blocks: baseline, baseline-report, rotation, washout no-feedback, and washout with feedback. The participants first completed a baseline block of 48 trials with veridical feedback (Fig. 1B). The report task was then described, and participants completed eight trials, verbally reporting the aiming landmark before each reach. Feedback was veridical on these trials (and participants almost always reported the aiming location as “zero”).

The visual rotation was introduced in the rotation block. In experiment 1, this was a 45° counterclockwise rotation, imposed for 128 trials. In experiment 2, the rotation was introduced gradually over 320 trials, with 0.144° added on each trial until the full 45° counterclockwise rotation was achieved. For experiment 3, a counterclockwise rotation was present for 128 trials. For one group of participants (No-Variance), the size of the rotation was constant at 45°; for a second group of participants, the size of the rotation on a given trial was drawn from a Gaussian distribution with a mean of 45° and a standard deviation of 11° (Variance-Added). In all three experiments, participants reported the aiming location before making their reach.

After the rotation block participants made an additional 40 reaches, but the rotation, cursor feedback, and aiming landmarks were removed (washout no-feedback). For this block, participants were explicitly instructed to aim directly to the green target. Neutral auditory feedback, in the form of a “knock” sound, indicated when the reach amplitude exceeded 7 cm but otherwise provided no information related to target accuracy. Veridical feedback was restored for a final 40-trial block (washout with feedback).

Movement Analysis

All initial data analyses were performed with MATLAB (MathWorks), and statistical analyses were performed in SPSS (IBM 2011). Task performance was assessed by calculating the angular difference between the target and the initial heading angle of the hand. A participant’s hand location could drift within the start circle during the report period of the trial; therefore we computed heading angle relative to the location where the hand left the start circle. This was done by fitting a straight line between samples taken at 1 and 3 cm from the start position (Taylor et al. 2014). We used initial heading angle rather than end-point angle to allow for a visual comparison between the online and end-point feedback conditions. The initial heading angle is also less susceptible to noise that might come about from the SCA participants’ ataxia.

For averaging across trials, movement trajectories were rotated to a common axis (e.g., as though the target was always located at 0°). With this convention, a positive angle indicates a deviation in the clockwise direction and a negative angle indicates a deviation in the counterclockwise direction. Note that the heading angles are reported in hand space rather than as target error. With this convention, hand heading angle will change in the opposite direction of the rotation as performance improves.

The mean hand heading angle was calculated on an individual basis for four different epochs: 1) the last eight trials of the baseline block, the 2) first and 3) last eight trials of the rotation block (early and late rotation), and 4) the first eight trials of the no-feedback washout block (washout). Trials were binned into groups of eight trials; we report the mean and standard error for each bin. Since the eight targets were presented in a random order within each cycle, this ensured that all targets were equally represented in each bin. In experiment 1, one participant with SCA failed to complete the last eight trials of the rotation block, so the mean of the second to last bins of eight trials was used. We did not fit an exponential function to the time series of hand heading angles during the rotation block given the nonmonotonic nature of the aim report data (Taylor et al. 2014).

To obtain an estimate of implicit learning, the reported aiming angle was subtracted from the measured hand heading angle on each trial. We refer to this measure as implicit learning since it could contain a number of processes in addition to error-based updating of a forward model, such as use-dependent learning and reinforcement learning. The mean aiming angle and implicit learning were calculated for three different epochs: 1) the last eight trials of the baseline block and the 2) first and 3) last eight trials of the rotation block (early and late rotation).

In terms of kinematics, we measured velocity and movement time. Velocity was computed by submitting the hand position data to a fourth-order Savitzky-Golay filter (Savitzky and Golay 1964; Smith et al. 2000). Movement onset could not always be based on the time at which the hand left the start circle because the hand occasionally wandered from this position during the report phase. As such, movement onset was estimated by a two-part procedure. We first identified the point in the time series when the movement amplitude reached 2 cm from the start position. From this point, the time series was searched backward to find the time point either when the participant left the start circle or when the movement started from outside the start position, the time point with the minimum radial distance from the start position.

We also excluded trials from the rotation block if the implicit learning estimate for that trial was >3 standard deviations outside the median implicit learning estimate for that participant. We used this criterion as a proxy to identify trials in which the participant may not have provided an accurate report of his/her aiming location or the movement itself was highly discrepant. With this criterion, >1% of the trials were excluded for the SCA participants over experiments 1 and 2 and control participants in experiment 2. In experiment 1, control participants had 2.1% of trials excluded. For experiment 3, 1.5% and 1.6% of trials were excluded for the Variance-Added and No-Variance groups, respectively.

Power Analysis

We performed a power analysis to estimate the minimum sample size required to obtain an expected effect size, using the data set from Taylor et al. (2010). Specifically, we focused on two “pure” measures of implicit learning, the extent of hand angle drift when participants were provided with an aiming strategy and the magnitude of the aftereffect, comparing these measures between SCA and control participants. Power was estimated for an independent-samples t-test, using a two-tailed α of 0.05 and power of 0.95. Based on the group means and standard deviations from the measure of implicit learning (i.e., drift) in Taylor et al. (2010), the effect size is d = 2.63 (μControl = 11.3°, σControl = 2.2°, μSCA = 5.9, σSCA = 1.9°). From this value, a minimum sample size of five participants is required in each group. A similar estimate of sample size is obtained using the aftereffect values (μControl = 6.2°, σControl = 2.4°, μSCA = 0.3, σSCA = 1.6°; d = 2.89). To be conservative, we doubled this number and recruited a minimum of 10 participants for each group for all of the experiments (Button et al. 2013).

RESULTS

Experiment 1: Cerebellar Damage Impairs Both Aiming and Adaptation to an Abrupt Rotation

After a baseline block with veridical end-point feedback, we introduced the aiming report task, requiring participants to indicate the number corresponding to the aiming location before each movement (Fig. 1A). For the first eight trials in which feedback remained veridical (baseline-report block), participants in both groups tended to report aiming directly at the target (control: −0.5 ± 0.3°, SCA: −0.3 ± 0.4°; group comparison: t18 = 0.5, P = 0.65). Consistent with their aiming reports, the heading angles of the reaches were directed toward the target with a small clockwise bias (control: 2.9 ± 0.9°, SCA: 2.0 ± 1.8°; group comparison: t18 = 0.5, P = 0.65). In sum, the behavior during the baseline phase was similar between the two groups.

The introduction of the perturbation (rotation block) induced changes in heading angle from baseline for both groups (Fig. 2A). To examine the initial phase of learning, we focused on the first eight trials. Participants with SCA displayed smaller heading angles (5.0 ± 3.8°) over these trials compared with the control participants (20.7 ± 3.5°; Fig. 2D). The difference in performance was even more pronounced at the end of the rotation block. Over the final eight trials, the control participants had almost completely countered the perturbation (41.3 ± 5.0°); in contrast, SCA participants were only partially countering the perturbation (16.5 ± 7.9°). To determine whether there were any differences in performance over the course of the rotation block, we performed a mixed factorial repeated-measures ANOVA with factors of Group (control and SCA) and Time (early and late rotation). A main effect of time is expected, since participants should compensate for more of the perturbation at the end of the rotation block than in the beginning. There are two, non-mutually exclusive, ways in which SCA participants’ performance could differ from control participants. SCA participants might be generally impaired in compensating for the perturbation relative to control participants, in which case a main effect of group would be expected. Additionally, SCA participants could be slower to respond to the perturbation, in which case an interaction would be expected. The ANOVA revealed a main effect of Group [F(1,18) = 12.6, P = 0.002] and a main effect of Time [F(1,18) = 10.4, P = 0.005]. No Group × Time interaction was present [F(1,18) = 0.8, P = 0.37]. Thus the performance of both groups improved (cursor terminated closer to the target) over the course of the rotation block, and the performance improvement was considerably greater for the control participants compared with the SCA participants.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Experiment 1: abrupt rotation performance metrics. A–C: group-averaged data for hand heading angle (A), aim reports (B), and estimates of implicit learning (C; hand heading angle − aim). Data are based on averages taken over bins of 8 trials for each participant and then averaged across participants for each group. Shaded lines represent confidence intervals around the mean. Bins are marked with the trial number of the last trial of that bin. D–F: individual participant data (dots) and group means (horizontal bar) for hand heading angle (D), aim report (E), and implicit learning estimate (F), with the data averaged over the first 8 trials of the rotation block, the last 8 trials of the rotation block or the first 8 trials of the no-feedback washout block. FB, feedback; IM, implicit learning estimate.

To measure the size of the aftereffect, participants completed a no-feedback block in which they were instructed to aim directly to the target. Feedback in this block was limited to an auditory tone indicating when the movement amplitude had exceeded 7 cm. A comparison of the first eight trials of this block to the eight baseline trials revealed a reliable aftereffect for both control participants (12.2 ± 1.3°; t9 = 5.5, P = 0.0004) and SCA participants (7.1 ± 2.2°; t9 = 3.0, P = 0.02; Fig. 2, A and D). However, when comparing the two groups the magnitude of the aftereffect was significantly larger in the control group compared with the SCA group (t18 = 2.0, P = 0.03). Additionally, our measure of implicit learning provides complementary evidence that this process is impaired in the SCA group (see below).

Verbal reports.

The time series of the aiming reports revealed that a large portion of the performance changes for the control participants was due to a change in their reported aiming location (Fig. 2B). For control participants, the mean aiming location was shifted from 13.3 ± 3.3° over the first eight trials to 22.1 ± 5.9° by the last eight trials of the rotation block. On average, these values were attenuated in the SCA participants (Fig. 2E). Here, the early and late aiming angles were shifted from the target location by 1.6 ± 2.8° and 13.3 ± 7.0°, respectively. As with hand heading angle, to determine whether there were any differences in the verbal aiming reports over the rotation block, the aiming reports for early and late in the rotation block were submitted to a mixed factorial ANOVA with the same factors of Group (control and SCA) and Time (early and late rotation). The ANOVA revealed an effect of Time [F(1,18) = 6.3, P = 0.03], suggesting that aiming angles increased over the rotation block, but only a trend for an effect of Group [F(1,18) = 3.2, P = 0.09]. No Group × Time interaction [F(1,18) = 0.1, P = 0.74] was present. From visual inspection of the aiming time series, control participants appeared to have an initial large shift in aiming angle, which then began to decrease slowly over the course of the rotation block. Given this nonmonotonic nature of the aiming report time series, as well as the high variance in the initial trials, we performed an additional post hoc analysis comparing the aim reports for the two groups across the whole rotation phase. On this composite measure, the aiming reports indicated larger shifts in aiming location for control (25.7 ± 3.8°) compared with SCA (11.2 ± 5.1°; t18 = 2.3, P = 0.035) participants.

To estimate implicit learning, we subtracted the reported aiming angle from the hand heading angle for each trial (Fig. 2C). We again focused on the mean for the first and last eight trials. For control participants, the estimate of implicit learning increased from 2.8 ± 1.4° early in the rotation block to 14.6 ± 2.3° by the end. By comparison, SCA participants had an estimate of no implicit learning (0.6 ± 2.1) early in the rotation block, which only barely increased to 2.9 ± 3.4° by the end of the block. As was done for hand heading angle and the verbal aim reports, to compare changes in implicit learning over the rotation block these values were submitted to an ANOVA with factors of Group and Time. The ANOVA revealed significant main effects of Group [F(1,18) = 5.6, P = 0.03], resulting from generally impaired implicit learning for SCA participants, and Time [F(1,18) = 17.7, P = 0.001], due to implicit learning estimates increasing from early to late in the rotation block. A Group × Time interaction [F(1,18) = 8.1, P = 0.01] was also present, suggesting that the impaired implicit learning in SCA participants differed relative to control participants between early and late in the rotation. As can be seen in Fig. 2F, the estimate of implicit learning was markedly lower for the SCA participants, an effect that was especially pronounced late in the rotation block. Thus by both the estimate of implicit learning during the rotation block and the aftereffect measure, adaptation was impaired in SCA participants compared with control participants.

In summary, the individuals with SCA exhibited a performance impairment when presented with a 45° visuomotor rotation, similar to that observed in previous studies of sensorimotor adaptation (e.g., Martin et al. 1996; Schlerf et al. 2013). By obtaining verbal reports, we were able to dissociate adjustments in aiming from implicit learning. The results indicate a dual impairment: Not only was implicit learning attenuated in the SCA participants, but they also tended to aim to locations closer to the target during the early stages of the rotation block compared with control participants. While SCA participants did modify their aim, these adjustments failed to effectively counter the rotation such that their overall performance only compensated for about half of the perturbation by the end of training. Thus SCA participants were impaired in self-discovery of an aiming strategy, which stands in contrast to our previous finding that SCA participants are quite competent in carrying out strategy if it is provided by instruction (Taylor et al. 2010).

Experiment 2: Gradually Introducing a Rotation Fails to Alleviate Impairment in Aiming and Adaptation as a Result of Cerebellar Damage

With an abruptly introduced rotation, there appear to be two stages of aiming: an initial large change in aim to compensate for the salient error induced by the perturbation, followed by small trial-to-trial adjustments to maintain accurate performance in the presence of small errors. It is likely that this first initial stage is the selection and implementation of an explicit general aiming strategy; however, the extent to which the second smaller adjustment phase is explicitly generated is less clear. It may be that the small trial-to-trial adjustments are achieved by something like implicit aiming, where an implicit mechanism is used to generate the adjustment to which explicit access is gained afterwards. Providing an instructed aiming strategy would direct the initial large change in aim; however, as adaptation increases and compensates for more of the perturbation, to maintain accurate performance and offset adaptation small adjustments in aim may be necessary from trial to trial. Given that when SCA individuals are provided with an instructed aiming strategy they can counter an abrupt rotation, and maintain performance, they may not be impaired when only small trial-to-trial changes in aim are necessary. To investigate this, in experiment 2 we introduced the perturbation gradually over the rotation block. It should be noted that we extended the rotation block from 128 to 320 trials and provided online cursor feedback as the participants reached toward the target to increase the contribution of implicit learning.

For the first eight trials of aiming under veridical feedback (baseline-report block), participants in both groups tended to report aiming toward the target (control: −2.4 ± 2.2°, SCA: −0.9 ± 1.2°; group comparison: t18 = 0.6, P = 0.56). Consistent with their aim reports, the heading angles for these first eight baseline trials were directed toward the target with a small clockwise bias (control: 1.4 ± 0.6°, SCA: 3.9 ± 1.6°; group comparison: t18 = 1.5, P = 0.15). As in experiment 1, the emphasis on participants making quick slicing movements toward the target resulted in relatively similar behavior for both groups of participants.

After the baseline block, a 45° counterclockwise visuomotor rotation was gradually introduced over 320 trials. The small perturbation (only 1.15° on the 8th trial) did not induce reliable changes from baseline in hand heading angle over the first eight trials of the rotation block for either the control participants (−0.7 ± 2.8°) or the SCA participants (1.6 ± 1.2°; Fig. 3D). By the end of the rotation block, when the full 45° rotation was present, both groups had adjusted their hand heading angles in response to the perturbation (Fig. 3A). This change, averaged over the last eight trials, was substantially larger in the control participants (35.8 ± 1.3°) compared with the SCA participants (18.2 ± 4.2°). As in experiment 1, to compare performance over the rotation block these values were submitted to a mixed factorial ANOVA. The ANOVA revealed a main effect of Group [F(1,18) = 8.8, P = 0.008], a main effect of Time [F(1,18) = 93.9, P < 0.0001], and a Time × Group interaction [F(1,18) = 13.3, P = 0.002]. Thus, as in experiment 1, the main effect of Group reveals that SCA participants compensated less for the perturbation than control participants, showing that their performance impairment is observed with both abrupt and gradual perturbations (see Schlerf et al. 2013). Additionally, the presence of an interaction suggests that the impairment of SCA participants relative to control participants differed between early and late in the rotation. Visual inspection suggests that the difference in performance was larger late in the rotation block, which is expected given that the gradual introduction of the perturbation resulted in only a ~1° perturbation during this early phase.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Experiment 2: gradual rotation performance metrics. A–C: group-averaged data for hand heading angle (A), aim reports (B), and estimates of implicit learning (C; hand heading angle − aim). Data are based on averages taken over bins of 8 trials for each participant and then averaged across participants for each group. Shaded lines represent confidence intervals around the mean. Bins are marked with the trial number of the last trial of that bin. D–F: individual participant data (dots) and group means (horizontal bar) for hand heading angle (D), aim report (E), and implicit learning estimate (F), with the data averaged over the first 8 trials of the rotation block, the last 8 trials of the rotation block, or the first 8 trials of the no-feedback washout block.

On the no-feedback washout block, cursor feedback was withheld and the participants were instructed to aim directly for the target. Comparing the first eight trials of this block to the eight baseline trials revealed a reliable aftereffect for control (26.0 ± 1.5°; t9 = 15.8, P < 0.0001) and SCA (16.6 ± 1.8°; t9 = 5.2, P = 0.0005) participants. However, the magnitude of the aftereffect was smaller in SCA compared with control participants (t18 = 4.0, P = 0.0004; Fig. 3, A and D). Thus on this measure of implicit learning the SCA participants were impaired relative to the control group.

Verbal reports.

In contrast to experiment 1, the time series of the aiming reports revealed that only a small portion of the change in hand heading angle for the control participants was due to a change in their aiming location (Fig. 3B). Over the first eight trials of the rotation block, their mean aiming location was −2.4 ± 2.3°. It should be noted that this shift is in the counterclockwise direction and would effectively increase the perturbation; we assume this reflects noise or an attempt to negate the effects of intrinsic bias (Ghilardi et al.1995; Gibo et al. 2013; Vindras et al. 1998, 2005). By the last eight trials, the aim was shifted by 5.3 ± 3.1° in the clockwise direction, effectively helping to counter the rotation. In contrast, the SCA participants did not consistently shift their aiming location over the course of the rotation block (Fig. 3E). Compared with baseline, they showed a shift of −1.7 ± 0.8° over the first eight trials and a shift of −0.6 ± 0.4° over the last eight trials. When the verbal aiming reports were compared with an ANOVA, the effect of Time was significant [F(1,18) = 6.1, P = 0.024] but the effect of Group was not [F(1,18) = 1.5, P = 0.24]. There was a trend toward a Time × Group interaction [F(1,18) = 3.3, P = 0.085].

We performed two additional post hoc comparisons to quantify the extent of aiming in control and SCA participants. First, to determine whether aiming direction changed over the course of the rotation block, a within-subject t-test was conducted comparing aiming during the last eight baseline trials and the last eight rotation trials. By this measure, the control participants adjusted their aim by the end of the rotation block (t9 = 2.2, P = 0.051), although this was only marginally significant. In contrast, the SCA participants did not exhibit a reliable shift in aim (t9 = 0.3, P = 0.79). Second, to compare overall aiming between the two groups, a t-test was performed comparing the aim reports averaged across the entire rotation phase. Using this measure, we observed a reduced shift in aiming direction for the SCA group compared with the control participants (t18 = −2.1, P = 0.047). Thus despite the relatively small changes in aiming observed for the control participants, these comparisons suggest larger changes in aim for control participants compared with SCA participants. Indeed, the SCA participants failed to adjust their aim in a consistent manner during the rotation block, despite the fact that the target error became quite pronounced by the end of the block.

We employed the subtractive procedure to estimate implicit learning (Fig. 3C) and focused on the first and last eight trials during the rotation block for our statistical analysis (Fig. 3F). For control participants, the estimate of implicit learning increased from 3.8 ± 2.1° early in the rotation block to 33.1 ± 4.5° by the end. By comparison, SCA participants had an estimate of no implicit learning (−2.3 ± 3.1) early in the rotation block, which increased to 13.2 ± 5.5° by the end of the block. An ANOVA on these values revealed main effects of both Group [F(1,18) = 7.0, P = 0.016] and Time [F(1,18) = 59.7, P < 0.0001]. The main effect of Group results from smaller implicit learning estimates overall for SCA participants compared with control participants. In addition, the Group × Time interaction was significant [F(1,18) = 5.6, P = 0.030], reflecting the fact that the control participants had a larger increase in implicit learning over the course of the rotation block compared with the SCA participants. This result converged with that observed in the measure of the aftereffect, replicating the impaired adaptation for SCA participants shown in experiment 1.

While our results are comparable across experiments 1 and 2, we caution against drawing any strong inferences from any differences in results between the experiments. First, there is the problematic nature of null results (the lack of a difference between control and SCA participants on the aftereffect data in experiment 1). Second, there are substantial differences between the two tasks. In experiment 2, participants completed more than twice as many rotation trials as in experiment 1 and received online cursor feedback. We would expect both factors to enhance implicit learning in control participants, offering greater sensitivity when comparing their performance to the SCA participants.

The results of experiment 2 demonstrate that the SCA participants were impaired in responding to a 45° gradual visuomotor rotation. As in experiment 1, their deficit appears to be manifest in measures of both implicit learning and aiming. The aiming deficit was apparent in experiment 2, even though only small adjustments in aiming location are necessary from trial to trial to maintain performance. We note that, for both groups, the amount of aiming was markedly attenuated in this experiment, and this may have contributed to the fact that the target error remained substantial at the end of the experiment. For control participants, the final error was ∼9°; for the SCA group, the final error was ∼27°. Despite this large error, the SCA participants failed to alter their aiming locations; they were unable to compensate for their impairment in implicit learning by deploying an aiming solution.

Given the variable performance of SCA participants in experiments 1 and 2, and the fact that SCA leads to highly heterogeneous damage to the cerebellum, it may be tempting to map behavior to damage in cerebellar subregions or with specific subtypes of SCA. However, given this variability, a larger sample size than we have here would be necessary (see Kansal et al. 2017). With the present data set, any conclusions involving SCA subtypes, or more specific regions of the cerebellum, would likely be driven by lesions and performance in only a few participants, where lesion reconstruction is unlikely to yield reliable results (Kimberg et al. 2007; Rorden et al. 2007). Additionally, correlating behavioral deficits to damage in cerebellar subregions is likely to be best tested in individuals with focal lesions, where the pathology is more localized than the broad deterioration in SCA.

Experiment 3: Higher Motor Variability Does Not Account for the Aiming and Adaptation Deficits Due to Cerebellar Damage

A feature of SCA is the presence of increased movement variability. Indeed, even though we focused on the initial heading angle in experiments 1 and 2, the reaching movements for the SCA participants were more variable than those for the control participants. For example, limiting the analysis to the initial baseline block of experiment 1 (before the aiming task was introduced), the standard deviations of the heading angles for the SCA and control groups were 11° and 5°, respectively. This increase in movement variability may make it difficult for individuals with SCA to develop a reliable aiming solution because they cannot converge on a consistent direction. To make this concrete, consider the situation where an individual, after encountering a 45° clockwise perturbation as in experiment 1, opts to aim to a location that is 30° in the counterclockwise direction. Evaluating the utility of this aiming solution will be hampered if the reach deviates widely from the selected trajectory (independent of adjustments induced by implicit learning). Similarly, increased motor variability in experiment 2 might make it difficult for the SCA participants to gauge the effects of the increasing perturbation.

The impact of motor variance on learning has been explored in previous studies of visuomotor adaptation (He et al. 2016; Therrien et al. 2016; Wu et al. 2014). In terms of the effects of cerebellar pathology, Schlerf et al. (2013) found that individuals with SCA exhibited impaired implicit learning, even when one considers how increased motor variability might impinge upon learning and performance. Here we ask how an increase in motor variability might influence the discovery of an aiming solution. Rather than creating conditions in which we directly manipulate motor variability, we added noise to the movement feedback presented to adults and examined the effect this had as they learned to respond to a 45° perturbation.

College-age adults were randomly assigned to one of two groups in experiment 3. In the No-Variance group, the procedure was identical to experiment 1 with the participants exposed to a constant 45° counterclockwise perturbation during the rotation block. In the Variance-Added group, we (crudely) simulated the effects of ataxia by pseudorandomly varying the size of the rotation on each trial during the rotation block (Fig. 4). To this end, a noise term was added to the 45° perturbation. The size of the rotation on each trial was based on a random sample from a Gaussian distribution with a mean of 45° and standard deviation of 11°. The value of 11° was chosen because it is the mean of the individual standard deviations of the hand heading angle for the SCA participants in experiment 1. Given that feedback is limited to the reach end point (at 7 cm), the participants in the Variance-Added group would experience a noisy 45° perturbation.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Sample perturbation schedule for Variance-Added group in experiment 3. The visuomotor rotation for each trial in the rotation block was drawn from a Gaussian distribution with a mean of 45° and standard deviation of 11°. The variance of the Gaussian was based on the mean variance in hand heading angle seen in participants with ataxia in experiment 1. Participants in the No-Variance control group experienced a constant 45° rotation on all trials in the rotation block (not shown).

During the initial baseline block (before reporting aim) the two groups had similar standard deviations of their heading angles (No-Variance: 6.3 ± 0.4°, Variance-Added: 6.4 ± 0.9°; t18 = 0.04, P = 0.97), suggesting there were no baseline differences in movement variance between the two groups. For the first eight trials of aiming with veridical feedback (baseline-report block), the participants in both groups tended to report aiming toward the target (No-Variance: −1.1 ± 0.6°, Variance-Added: 0.1 ± 0.2°; group comparison: t18 = 0.6, P = 0.56). The hand heading angles were in the direction of the target, although the Variance-Added group exhibited a significant, albeit small clockwise bias (2.7 ± 0.7°) that was not observed in the No-Variance group (0.0 ± 0.5°; t18 = 3.1, P = 0.01). Given that the two groups received identical (veridical) feedback in these first two blocks, this difference likely represents chance variation in the participants’ baseline reaching bias (Ghilardi et al. 1995).

The 45° perturbation was introduced in the rotation block, along with the increase in end-point variance for the Variance-Added group. Both groups displayed adjustments in heading angle in response to the perturbation (Fig. 5A). These adjustments were similar for the No-Variance and Variance-Added participants, both early (No-Variance mean: 12.6 ± 4.5°, Variance mean: 11.7 ± 6.2°) and late (No-Variance mean: 44.2 ± 1.2°, Variance-Added mean: 41.8 ± 6.6°; Fig. 5D) in the rotation block. As in previous experiments, a mixed factorial ANOVA was used to compare learning over the rotation blocks, although in this case the factor of Group is comparing Variance-Added and No-Variance groups. There was a main effect of Time [F(1,18) = 76.1, P < 0.0001] but no effect of Group [F(1,18) = 0.07, P = 0.80] or Group × Time interaction [F(1,18) = 0.04, P = 0.84]; thus no differences in performance were present between the groups. The groups also exhibited similar aftereffects in the no-feedback block, in which they were instructed to aim directly for the target. Both groups exhibited a significant aftereffect (No-Variance: 6.3 ± 0.9°, t9 = 4.8, P = 0.0009; Variance-Added: 7.9 ± 0.8°, t9 = 5.2, P = 0.0006), and the magnitude of the aftereffect was similar for the two groups (t18 = 1.4, P = 0.90). These results suggest that the imposition of added end-point variance did not impact overall performance in response to a visuomotor perturbation.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Experiment 3: abrupt rotation with variable rotation performance metrics. A–C: group-averaged data for hand heading angle (A), aim reports (B), and estimates of implicit learning (C; hand heading angle − aim). Data are based on averages taken over bins of 8 trials for each participant and then averaged across participants for each group. Shaded lines represent confidence intervals around the mean. Bins are marked with the trial number of the last trial of that bin. D–F: individual participant data (dots) and group means (horizontal bar) for hand heading angle (D), aim report (E), and implicit learning estimate (F), with the data averaged over the first 8 trials of the rotation block, the last 8 trials of the rotation block, or the first 8 trials of the no-feedback washout block.

Per experimenter instructions, participants in both groups moved rapidly, and there was no difference in movement duration between the groups during the rotation block (No-Variance: 274 ± 57 ms, Variance-Added: 214 ± 14 ms; t18 = 1.02, P = 0.32).

Verbal reports.

The aiming report data reveal that a large portion of learning was due to an adjustment in aiming location (Fig. 5B) and that this effect was comparable for the two groups. For the No-Variance group, the mean aiming location was shifted 7.3 ± 5.0° over the first eight trials and increased to 34.8 ± 1.4° by the last eight trials of the rotation block. Participants in the Variance-Added group had an initial shift of 12.3 ± 5.0°, which increased to 30.8 ± 5.8° by the last eight trials of the rotation block. A mixed factorial ANOVA comparing aiming over the rotation block revealed that the main effect of Time was significant [F(1,18) = 33.3, P < 0.0001], with participants increasing the angle of their aiming over the course of the rotation block (Fig. 5E). However, there was no main effect of Group [F(1,18) = 0.01, P = 0.92] or Group × Time interaction [F(1,18) = 1.3, P = 0.27]. While the overall pattern suggests that the aiming shift over the entire rotation block was larger for the No-Variance group (33.7 ± 2.3°) compared with the Variance-Added (29.0 ± 3.9°) group, this difference was not significant (comparison of means taken over the whole rotation block: t18 = 1.0, P = 0.31). Indeed, the group difference reflects the performance of one participant in the Variance-Added group who only reported small aiming angles.

Implicit learning was estimated for each trial by subtracting the aiming angle from the hand heading angle (Fig. 5, C and F). Participants in the No-Variance group had an estimate of implicit learning that increased from 5.3 ± 1.5° early in the rotation block to 9.4 ± 1.3° by the end. In the Variance-Added group participants had an estimate of implicit learning that increased from 0.0 ± 1.6 early in the rotation block to 9.4 ± 1.4° by the end of the block. Implicit learning was compared with an ANOVA based on the first and last eight trials of the rotation block, revealing a main effect of Time [F(1,18) = 26.0, P = 0.0001] but no main effect of Group [F(1,18) = 2.9, P = 0.11] and only a trend for an interaction between these factors [F(1,18) = 4.2, P = 0.055]. While the Group effect and interaction were not significant, a post hoc comparison of the groups early [t18 = 2.5, P = 0.02] and late [t18 = 0.0, P = 0.99] in the rotation block suggests that while participants in the Variance-Added group may have initially had less implicit learning early during the rotation block, their implicit learning caught up to the No-Variance group by the end of the rotation. When a further post hoc comparison was made of implicit learning estimates averaged across the entire rotation block, no difference was present between the No-Variance group (7.2 ± 0.9) and the Variance-Added group (6.5 ± 1.2; t18 = 0.5, P = 0.62), confirming that no difference in overall implicit learning was present.

In summary, the results of experiment 3 show that the injection of random end-point noise had only a modest effect on performance. Interestingly, the effect of noise was, at least in terms of overall performance, limited to a possible slight reduction in the magnitude of implicit learning as estimated early during the rotation block, but no differences were present late in the rotation block or in aftereffect. There were also differences between the groups in how they modified their aim on a trial-by-trial basis, with the Variance-Added group continually modifying their aiming location in response to the added noise. Nonetheless, these participants were able to learn the mean of the 45° rotation and they changed their mean aiming angle to a similar degree as participants who experienced a constant perturbation. Taken together, these results suggest that an increase in end-point variability does not result in profound deficits in implicit and aiming components of visuomotor learning. Thus the difficulty exhibited by individuals with SCA in adopting an appropriate aiming solution is likely unrelated to their increased movement variability.

DISCUSSION

Previous work has repeatedly demonstrated that individuals with damage to the cerebellum are impaired in sensorimotor learning. The emphasis in this literature has focused on implicit deficits in error-based learning. However, here we have shown that SCA also leads to an impairment in the ability to discover and maintain an aiming solution to offset a visuomotor perturbation.

Impaired Aiming in SCA

To study the use of aiming behavior, participants reported their aiming location before each reach (Taylor et al. 2014). This task provides a direct probe on the use and evolution of an aiming solution and, by subtractive logic, a continuous estimate of implicit learning. We observed a dual deficit in participants with SCA when presented with a visuomotor rotation. Not only did the SCA group exhibit impaired implicit learning, they also showed a failure to aim to counter the observed target error. They adjusted their aim across the perturbation block, choosing locations that tended to be in the appropriate direction to counter the rotation, but failed to fully compensate for the perturbation.

This aiming deficit helps answer the question posed in the introduction: If people can compensate for a rotation through a multiplicity of processes, why do individuals with SCA not compensate for an implicit learning deficit through an increased reliance on aiming? The present results indicate that the ability to discover an aiming solution is also compromised.

This dual deficit was observed not only when a 45° perturbation was introduced abruptly (experiment 1) but also when the 45° perturbation was introduced gradually (experiment 2). It has been assumed that aiming makes a minimal contribution to performance when the perturbation is introduced in a gradual manner. However, this assumes that implicit learning—the form arising from sensory prediction error-based learning—will be sufficient to compensate for the perturbation. The present data, as well as a recent report (Bond and Taylor 2015), suggest that implicit learning is insufficient to achieve good performance when the perturbation is large (for control as well as SCA participants) and thus the end-point error will grow over the course of the rotation block. In experiment 2, the control participants began to adjust their aim, with deviations in the aiming location becoming consistent after ~150 trials into the rotation block. In contrast, the SCA group failed to invoke compensatory aiming even when the error became quite large. A priori, we might have expected the SCA participants to have larger aiming angles than the control participants in order to compensate for their impaired implicit learning.

The inclusion of participants with SCA types 1 and 2 in our sample may be a point of concern given previous reports indicating greater extracerebellar involvement in these groups compared with other subtypes of SCA (Bürk et al 2003). In particular, might the aiming deficit in the SCA group analyses arise from pathology in these extracerebellar regions? The SCA2 participants in experiment 1 exhibited some of the greater impairments in aiming. However, analyses conducted after removal of these three participants in experiment 1, as well as the three SCA1 participants and one SCA2 participant in experiment 2, yield a trend similar to the reported analyses in terms of overall performance, aberrant aiming, and attenuated implicit adaptation. If aiming processes remained intact we would expect to see comparable overall performance, as SCA participants could use larger changes in aim to compensate for their impaired implicit adaptation. This was not observed in any of the participants or in the literature, where cerebellar damage has consistently been linked with impairments in both overall performance and aftereffects (Martin et al. 1996; Rabe et al. 2009; Schlerf et al. 2013; Smith and Shadmehr 2005; Weiner et al. 1983), suggesting they are not effectively using aiming or other compensatory strategies to make up for their impaired implicit adaptation. Most relevant to our experiments, to minimize the chance of extracerebellar involvement in our sample, we only included SCA participants with pure cerebellar symptoms, excluding any participants with resting tremor, rigidity, or bradykinesia, which would suggest parkinsonism and basal ganglia involvement. Additionally, the SCA participants in experiment 2 were examined in detail by a movement disorders specialist (S.-H. Kuo).

It is also important to note that the SCA2 participants who showed aiming impairments also tended to have more advanced ataxia as measured in the neurological assessment (Table 1 ataxia scale). As such, we cannot dissociate disease progression from SCA subtype. Moreover, the patterns of pathology show considerable overlap at more advanced disease stages, independent of subtype, and the extracerebellar pathology in SCA1 and SCA2 is not evident in areas associated with executive function or strategy use (Seidel et al. 2012). When measured macroscopically, the extracerebellar pathology is primarily in the brain stem, with occasional extension to midbrain regions (Della Nave et al. 2008; Mascalchi et al. 2014; Paulson 2009; Seidel et al. 2012).

We are not making the claim that the aiming deficits reported here should be taken to mean that aiming is solely dependent on cerebellar function. There is extensive evidence that cortico-cerebellar networks encompass almost the entire neocortex, with prominent projections to prefrontal cortex (Bostan et al. 2013; Buckner et al. 2011; Caligiore et al. 2017; Kelly and Strick 2003). Even if the pathology in our sample was limited to just the cerebellum, behavioral changes likely reflect disruption in a network that spans cerebellar and extracerebellar regions. Indeed, this point is made clear in studies of diaschisis showing hypometabolism of frontal cortex in patients with cerebellar degeneration (Meyer et al. 1993).

One could take the position that the deficits in planning, including something like the implementation of an aiming strategy, in individuals with SCA should be attributed to brain areas such as prefrontal cortex known to be essential for these aspects of cognition. This inference (Klinke et al. 2010), however, seems questionable when we consider that complex processes such as those subsumed by the term “executive function” surely require a network perspective. It is likely that the neuropsychological impairments observed in individuals with SCA reflect a disruption of cerebello-cortical networks. More generally, concerns with using lesion methods to make direct structure-function inferences are relevant for all neuropsychological research in humans and nonhuman species. Although it is important to keep these issues in mind, it is also clear that there is some degree of commonality in patients with cerebellar degeneration, supporting the idea that, despite their heterogeneity, we can learn something about cerebellar function through the study of this group.

Taken together, experiments 1 and 2 demonstrate that SCA participants have difficulty adjusting their aim to counter a visuomotor rotation. Thus, in addition to an impairment in sensory prediction error-based learning, these individuals have difficulty using the feedback to develop and implement an appropriate aiming solution. Below, we consider possible explanations for this impairment in aiming.

Increased Movement Variability Does Not Account for Impairments in Aiming

In experiment 3, the addition of trial-by-trial noise to a constant perturbation was used to test the hypothesis that the observed aiming impairment is due to ataxia-related movement variability. This manipulation, however, only had a modest effect on the performance of the young healthy adults. Most relevant to the present study, the additional variability did not produce a significant effect on either overall performance or aiming behavior. The Variance-Added group altered their aiming direction shortly after the onset of the (noisy) perturbation. While they continued to make aiming adjustments across the rotation block, the asymptotic size of the aiming shift was statistically indistinguishable from that observed in the No-Variance group.

We recognize that our noise manipulation is, at best, a weak approximation of the consequences of ataxia. We assume that the college students attribute the increased variability to the environment and not their motor system. Moreover, the increased variance is transient, unlike the chronic nature of ataxia. Nonetheless, the results of experiment 3 suggest that an inability to develop an appropriate aiming solution is not solely due to the motor variability associated with ataxia.

The Variance-Added group did exhibit a small reduction in the estimate of implicit learning early in the rotation block. However, there was no difference in the implicit learning estimate by the end of the rotation block or when compared across the entire rotation block. However, this delayed onset of implicit learning is likely distinct from the implicit learning deficit observed in SCA. Under conditions of high external noise, the learning rate should be reduced (He et al. 2016; Kalman 1960). Moreover, using a computational model, Schlerf and colleagues (Schlerf et al. 2013) reported that the slower learning rates in SCA are unlikely to be the result of increased motor noise. Indeed, when directly examined, minimal correlation is found between impairments in visuomotor learning and the severity of ataxia (Martin et al. 1996; Schlerf et al. 2013).

Flexibility in Aiming Is Not Directly Driven by Sensory Prediction Errors

A second hypothesis is that sensory prediction errors are necessary to form an effective aiming solution. While some form of performance error would be needed to adjust aiming, it is unlikely that a sensory prediction error is the primary signal. A sensory prediction error occurs whenever there is a mismatch between the expected and actual consequences of a movement. This sensory prediction error is used to update a forward model to improve prediction. In a standard visuomotor rotation task, this error is defined as the difference between the cursor position and target (when taken as a proxy of the location for expected feedback). However, when participants use an instructed aiming strategy to counter the rotation, their hand slowly drifts away from the aiming location across trials (Mazzoni and Krakauer 2006). This occurs because a persistent sensory prediction error, the mismatch between the cursor position and aiming location in this context, continues to result in implicit updating of a forward model. Eventually, this implicit learning leads to poor performance (i.e., large target errors) and participants have to change their aim to offset continued implicit learning (Taylor and Ivry 2011). Models of performance in this aiming task suggest that changes in aim are driven by performance error, the difference between the target and cursor feedback locations, rather than sensory prediction error (Taylor et al. 2014; Taylor and Ivry 2011). Sensory prediction errors, on the other hand, appear to lead to implicit learning even when irrelevant to the task goal (Mazzoni and Krakauer 2006; Morehead et al. 2014; Schaefer et al. 2012).

Another problem with the hypothesis that the aiming deficit is related to an impairment in learning from sensory prediction errors is that the time course of aim reports looks quite different from that observed for implicit learning. First, with large perturbations (e.g., experiment 1), the aim reports are nonmonotonic, with an initial large increase and then a gradual reduction over the course of the rotation block. Second, while the average data might look smooth, the aiming reports for individuals can change abruptly and in either direction, behavior that is reminiscent of exploration (see Taylor et al. 2014). This stands in contrast to the slow, monotonic updating of an internal model that is considered to be the hallmark of motor adaptation (Huberdeau et al. 2015). Thus it seems unlikely that the output of a forward model is the driving force for finding an aiming solution.

An Action-Outcome Maintenance Account of Impaired Aiming in SCA

A third hypothesis considers less direct ways in which the cerebellum might support learning and, by extension, the cognitive capacity required for discovering an aiming solution. The cerebellum is known to be highly connected with much of the cerebral cortex, including prefrontal cortex (Buckner et al. 2011), and damage to the cerebellum can produce a constellation of neuropsychological impairments similar, albeit in milder form, to that observed in patients with lesions of prefrontal cortex (Bodranghien et al. 2016; Buckner 2013). Drawing on ideas developed in perceptual domains (Cohen et al. 1997; Gazzaley et al. 2005; Prabhakaran et al. 2000), we have proposed that a network including prefrontal cortex and cerebellum forms something akin to a motor working memory system, one essential in the action domain (Ivry and Fiez 2000; Spencer and Ivry 2009). By this idea, the cerebellum helps represent and/or maintain task-relevant stimulus-response associations across trials. Theories of prefrontal cortex function have suggested a role in implementing and maintaining task sets across trials (Dosenbach et al. 2006, 2007), possibly in a hierarchical manner where abstraction increases rostrally, allowing for the simultaneous search for action rules at multiple levels of abstraction (Badre et al. 2010; Badre and D’Esposito 2009). By extension, the task set implemented by the prefrontal cortex would include the task-relevant action-outcome associations, a network involving the cerebellum. Consistent with this theory, Spencer and Ivry (2009) showed that the impact of SCA on sequence learning was considerable when the task involved indirect, arbitrary stimulus-response associations but that SCA participants performed as well as control participants in sequence learning when the stimulus-response associations were direct.

An extension of this hypothesis may account for the aiming deficit observed in the present study. Converging on an appropriate aiming solution entails a cyclic process of hypothesis testing, generating possible solutions and then evaluating their efficacy. This process would require the maintenance of stimulus-response associations or, perhaps in the case of visuomotor rotations, action-outcome associations.

Moreover, the memory demands are likely greatly increased when there are eight target locations, especially with a visuomotor rotation (Krakauer et al. 2000), where implicit generalization appears quite narrow (Heuer and Hegele 2011). Here an aiming hypothesis generated in response to an action at one target may appear appropriate when applied to neighboring targets but fail miserably when applied to distant targets. For example, aiming above the target location would be effective in countering a clockwise rotation for targets presented on the right side of the display but would be counterproductive if applied to targets on the left side of the display. Thus the participant would need to maintain action-outcome associations at multiple target locations to eventually learn that the solution to the perturbation is common to all target locations in a polar coordinate reference frame. This idea would suggest that if the SCA participants had a compromised ability to maintain action-outcome association, then they would naturally have an impaired ability to counter a visuomotor rotation across the workspace.

Our action-outcome maintenance hypothesis makes three predictions. First, we would expect that the ability to employ an aiming solution might be related to the individual’s cognitive status. Second, we would expect that individuals with SCA would be able to develop an appropriate aiming solution if the perturbation was simpler. For example, these individuals might be able to use aiming to compensate for a translational perturbation. Third, we would predict that individuals with ataxia would disproportionately benefit if the demands on working memory were reduced, say by displaying the previous aim choice for each target or by the use of only a single target location. These last two predictions can motivate future work on the multifaceted contribution of the cerebellum to sensorimotor learning. At present, the results presented here underscore a role for the cerebellum, not only in implicit aspects of motor performance but also when those tasks require a more explicit association to link a stimulus with an appropriate response to meet task goals.

Here we observed a dual deficit in sensory prediction error-based learning (e.g., a forward model) and aiming. While at first glance the latter may seem odd, it perhaps should not be all that surprising given the cerebellum’s involvement in learning for many different types of tasks, from eyeblink conditioning to sequence learning. Indeed, the nearly uniform circuitry of the cerebellum, along with its connections to many areas of the rest of the brain (Buckner et al. 2011), suggests that it may be contributing to learning processes in a generalized manner that remains to be determined.

GRANTS

P. A. Butcher and J. A. Taylor were supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) Grant R01 NS-084948 and the Princeton Neuroscience Institute’s Innovation Fund. R. B. Ivry was supported by NINDS Grants NS-074917 and NS-092079.

DISCLOSURES

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

P.A.B., R.B.I., J.W.K., and J.A.T. conceived and designed research; P.A.B., S.-H.K., D.R., and J.A.T. performed experiments; P.A.B. and J.A.T. analyzed data; P.A.B., R.B.I., S.-H.K., D.R., J.W.K., and J.A.T. interpreted results of experiments; P.A.B. prepared figures; P.A.B. drafted manuscript; P.A.B., R.B.I., S.-H.K., D.R., J.W.K., and J.A.T. edited and revised manuscript; P.A.B., R.B.I., S.-H.K., D.R., J.W.K., and J.A.T. approved final version of manuscript.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Sam McDougle for feedback on the manuscript.

REFERENCES

  1. Badre D, D’Esposito M. Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 659–669, 2009. doi: 10.1038/nrn2667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Badre D, Kayser AS, D’Esposito M. Frontal cortex and the discovery of abstract action rules. Neuron 66: 315–326, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benson BL, Anguera JA, Seidler RD. A spatial explicit strategy reduces error but interferes with sensorimotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol 105: 2843–2851, 2011. doi: 10.1152/jn.00002.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bodranghien F, Bastian A, Casali C, Hallett M, Louis ED, Manto M, Mariën P, Nowak DA, Schmahmann JD, Serrao M, Steiner KM, Strupp M, Tilikete C, Timmann D, van Dun K. Consensus Paper: revisiting the symptoms and signs of cerebellar syndrome. Cerebellum 15: 369–391, 2016. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0687-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bond KM, Taylor JA. Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. J Neurophysiol 113: 3836–3849, 2015. doi: 10.1152/jn.00009.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bostan AC, Dum RP, Strick PL. Cerebellar networks with the cerebral cortex and basal ganglia. Trends Cogn Sci 17: 241–254, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brooks JX, Carriot J, Cullen KE. Learning to expect the unexpected: rapid updating in primate cerebellum during voluntary self-motion. Nat Neurosci 18: 1310–1317, 2015. doi: 10.1038/nn.4077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buckner RL. The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80: 807–815, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BT. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106: 2322–2345, 2011. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bürk K, Globas C, Bösch S, Klockgether T, Zühlke C, Daum I, Dichgans J. Cognitive deficits in spinocerebellar ataxia type 1, 2, and 3. J Neurol 250: 207–211, 2003. doi: 10.1007/s00415-003-0976-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, Munafò MR. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14: 365–376, 2013. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Caligiore D, Pezzulo G, Baldassarre G, Bostan AC, Strick PL, Doya K, Helmich RC, Dirkx M, Houk J, Jörntell H, Lago-Rodriguez A, Galea JM, Miall RC, Popa T, Kishore A, Verschure PF, Zucca R, Herreros I. Consensus Paper: towards a systems-level view of cerebellar function: the interplay between cerebellum, basal ganglia, and cortex. Cerebellum 16: 203–229, 2017. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0763-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Noll DC, Jonides J, Smith EE. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 386: 604–608, 1997. doi: 10.1038/386604a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Della Nave R, Ginestroni A, Tessa C, Salvatore E, De Grandis D, Plasmati R, Salvi F, De Michele G, Dotti MT, Piacentini S, Mascalchi M. Brain white matter damage in SCA1 and SCA2. An in vivo study using voxel-based morphometry, histogram analysis of mean diffusivity and tract-based spatial statistics. Neuroimage 43: 10–19, 2008. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diedrichsen J, White O, Newman D, Lally N. Use-dependent and error-based learning of motor behaviors. J Neurosci 30: 5159–5166, 2010. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5406-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RA, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 11073–11078, 2007. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dosenbach NU, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, Kang HC, Burgund ED, Grimes AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50: 799–812, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Galea JM, Vazquez A, Pasricha N, Orban de Xivry JJ, Celnik P. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. Cereb Cortex 21: 1761–1770, 2011. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gazzaley A, Cooney JW, McEvoy K, Knight RT, D’Esposito M. Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity. J Cogn Neurosci 17: 507–517, 2005. doi: 10.1162/0898929053279522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ghilardi MF, Gordon J, Ghez C. Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. J Neurophysiol 73: 2535–2539, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gibo TL, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Okamura AM, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar motor learning: are environment dynamics more important than error size? J Neurophysiol 110: 322–333, 2013. doi: 10.1152/jn.00745.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Haruno M, Wolpert DM, Kawato M. Mosaic model for sensorimotor learning and control. Neural Comput 13: 2201–2220, 2001. doi: 10.1162/089976601750541778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. He K, Liang Y, Abdollahi F, Fisher Bittmann M, Kording K, Wei K. The statistical determinants of the speed of motor learning. PLoS Comput Biol 12: e1005023, 2016. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Heuer H, Hegele M. Generalization of implicit and explicit adjustments to visuomotor rotations across the workspace in younger and older adults. J Neurophysiol 106: 2078–2085, 2011. doi: 10.1152/jn.00043.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Huang VS, Haith A, Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW. Rethinking motor learning and savings in adaptation paradigms: model-free memory for successful actions combines with internal models. Neuron 70: 787–801, 2011. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Huberdeau DM, Krakauer JW, Haith AM. Dual-process decomposition in human sensorimotor adaptation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 33: 71–77, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. IBM IBM SPSS Statistics 20 Core System User’s Guide. Armonk, NY: IBM, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ivry RB, Fiez JA. Cerebellar contributions to cognition and imagery. In: The New Cognitive Neurosciences, edited by Gazzaniga MS. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000, p. 999–1011. [Google Scholar]
  29. Izawa J, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R. Cerebellar contributions to reach adaptation and learning sensory consequences of action. J Neurosci 32: 4230–4239, 2012. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6353-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kalman RE. A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. J Basic Eng 82: 35–45, 1960. doi: 10.1115/1.3662552. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kansal K, Yang Z, Fishman AM, Sair HI, Ying SH, Jedynak BM, Prince JL, Onyike CU. Structural cerebellar correlates of cognitive and motor dysfunctions in cerebellar degeneration. Brain 140: 707–720, 2017. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci 23: 8432–8444, 2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kimberg DY, Coslett HB, Schwartz MF. Power in voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 1067–1080, 2007. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Klinke I, Minnerop M, Schmitz-Hübsch T, Hendriks M, Klockgether T, Wüllner U, Helmstaedter C. Neuropsychological features of patients with spinocerebellar ataxia (SCA) types 1, 2, 3, and 6. Cerebellum 9: 433–442, 2010. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0183-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Krakauer JW. Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation. Adv Exp Med Biol 629: 405–421, 2009. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-77064-2_21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C. Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci 20: 8916–8924, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT. Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain 119: 1183–1198, 1996. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mascalchi M, Diciotti S, Giannelli M, Ginestroni A, Soricelli A, Nicolai E, Aiello M, Tessa C, Galli L, Dotti MT, Piacentini S, Salvatore E, Toschi N. Progression of brain atrophy in spinocerebellar ataxia type 2: a longitudinal tensor-based morphometry study. PLoS One 9: e89410, 2014. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW. An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 26: 3642–3645, 2006. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5317-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McDougle SD, Bond KM, Taylor JA. Explicit and implicit processes constitute the fast and slow processes of sensorimotor learning. J Neurosci 35: 9568–9579, 2015. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5061-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Meyer JS, Obara K, Muramatsu K. Diaschisis. Neurol Res 15: 362–366, 1993. doi: 10.1080/01616412.1993.11740164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Miall RC, Wolpert DM. Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw 9: 1265–1279, 1996. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(96)00035-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Morehead JR, Taylor JA, Parvin D, Marrone E, Ivry RB. Implicit adaptation via visual error clamp. Translational and Computational Motor Control Meeting, Am Soc Neurorehabilitation, Washington, DC, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  44. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 53: 695–699, 2005. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nikooyan AA, Ahmed AA. Reward feedback accelerates motor learning. J Neurophysiol 113: 633–646, 2015. doi: 10.1152/jn.00032.2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Paulson HL. The spinocerebellar ataxias. J Neuroophthalmol 29: 227–237, 2009. doi: 10.1097/WNO0b013e3181b416de. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Prabhakaran V, Narayanan K, Zhao Z, Gabrieli JD. Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nat Neurosci 3: 85–90, 2000. doi: 10.1038/71156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rabe K, Livne O, Gizewski ER, Aurich V, Beck A, Timmann D, Donchin O. Adaptation to visuomotor rotation and force field perturbation is correlated to different brain areas in patients with cerebellar degeneration. J Neurophysiol 101: 1961–1971, 2009. doi: 10.1152/jn.91069.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rorden C, Karnath HO, Bonilha L. Improving lesion-symptom mapping. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 1081–1088, 2007. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Savitzky A, Golay MJ. Smoothing and differentiation of data by simplified least squares procedures. Anal Chem 36: 1627–1639, 1964. doi: 10.1021/ac60214a047. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schaefer SY, Shelly IL, Thoroughman KA. Beside the point: motor adaptation without feedback-based error correction in task-irrelevant conditions. J Neurophysiol 107: 1247–1256, 2012. doi: 10.1152/jn.00273.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schlerf J, Ivry RB, Diedrichsen J. Encoding of sensory prediction errors in the human cerebellum. J Neurosci 32: 4913–4922, 2012. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4504-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Schlerf JE, Xu J, Klemfuss NM, Griffiths TL, Ivry RB. Individuals with cerebellar degeneration show similar adaptation deficits with large and small visuomotor errors. J Neurophysiol 109: 1164–1173, 2013. doi: 10.1152/jn.00654.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schmitz-Hübsch T, du Montcel ST, Baliko L, Berciano J, Boesch S, Depondt C, Giunti P, Globas C, Infante J, Kang JS, Kremer B, Mariotti C, Melegh B, Pandolfo M, Rakowicz M, Ribai P, Rola R, Schöls L, Szymanski S, van de Warrenburg BP, Dürr A, Klockgether T. Scale for the assessment and rating of ataxia: development of a new clinical scale. Neurology 66: 1717–1720, 2006. [Erratum]. Neurology 67: 299, 2006. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219042.60538.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Seidel K, Siswanto S, Brunt ER, den Dunnen W, Korf HW, Rüb U. Brain pathology of spinocerebellar ataxias. Acta Neuropathol 124: 1–21, 2012. doi: 10.1007/s00401-012-1000-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Smith MA, Brandt J, Shadmehr R. Motor disorder in Huntington’s disease begins as a dysfunction in error feedback control. Nature 403: 544–549, 2000. doi: 10.1038/35000576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Smith MA, Shadmehr R. Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington’s disease but not cerebellar degeneration. J Neurophysiol 93: 2809–2821, 2005. doi: 10.1152/jn.00943.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Spencer RM, Ivry RB. Sequence learning is preserved in individuals with cerebellar degeneration when the movements are directly cued. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 1302–1310, 2009. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Taylor JA, Ivry RB. Flexible cognitive strategies during motor learning. PLoS Comput Biol 7: e1001096, 2011. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Taylor JA, Klemfuss NM, Ivry RB. An explicit strategy prevails when the cerebellum fails to compute movement errors. Cerebellum 9: 580–586, 2010. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0201-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Taylor JA, Krakauer JW, Ivry RB. Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation task. J Neurosci 34: 3023–3032, 2014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3619-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Therrien AS, Wolpert DM, Bastian AJ. Effective reinforcement learning following cerebellar damage requires a balance between exploration and motor noise. Brain 139: 101–114, 2016. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Trouillas P, Takayanagi T, Hallett M, Currier RD, Subramony SH, Wessel K, Bryer A, Diener HC, Massaquoi S, Gomez CM, Coutinho P, Ben Hamida M, Campanella G, Filla A, Schut L, Timann D, Honnorat J, Nighoghossian N, Manyam B. International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale for pharmacological assessment of the cerebellar syndrome. The Ataxia Neuropharmacology Committee of the World Federation of Neurology. J Neurol Sci 145: 205–211, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Tseng YW, Diedrichsen J, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ. Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J Neurophysiol 98: 54–62, 2007. doi: 10.1152/jn.00266.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Vaca-Palomares I, Díaz R, Rodríguez-Labrada R, Medrano-Montero J, Vázquez-Mojena Y, Velázquez-Pérez L, Fernandez-Ruiz J. Spinocerebellar ataxia type 2 neurodegeneration differentially affects error-based and strategic-based visuomotor learning. Cerebellum 12: 848–855, 2013. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0496-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Verstynen T, Sabes PN. How each movement changes the next: an experimental and theoretical study of fast adaptive priors in reaching. J Neurosci 31: 10050–10059, 2011. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6525-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Vindras P, Desmurget M, Prablanc C, Viviani P. Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. J Neurophysiol 79: 3290–3294, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Vindras P, Desmurget M, Viviani P. Error parsing in visuomotor pointing reveals independent processing of amplitude and direction. J Neurophysiol 94: 1212–1224, 2005. doi: 10.1152/jn.01295.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Weiner MJ, Hallett M, Funkenstein HH. Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system. Neurology 33: 766–772, 1983. doi: 10.1212/WNL.33.6.766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Werner S, Bock O, Timmann D. The effect of cerebellar cortical degeneration on adaptive plasticity and movement control. Exp Brain Res 193: 189–196, 2009. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1607-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wu HG, Miyamoto YR, Gonzalez Castro LN, Ölveczky BP, Smith MA. Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability. Nat Neurosci 17: 312–321, 2014. doi: 10.1038/nn.3616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurophysiology are provided here courtesy of American Physiological Society

RESOURCES