Abstract
This study tested a model linking sensitivity to punishment (SP) and reward (SR) to marijuana use and problems via affect lability and poor control. A 6-month prospective design was used in a sample of 2,270 young-adults (64% female). The hypothesized SP x SR interaction did not predict affect lability or poor control, but did predict use likelihood at baseline. At low levels of SR, SP was associated with an increased likelihood of abstaining, which was attenuated as SR increased. SP and SR displayed positive main effects on both affect lability and poor control. Affect lability and poor control, in turn, mediated effects on the marijuana outcomes. Poor control predicted both increased marijuana use and, controlling for use level, greater intensity of problems. Affect lability predicted greater intensity of problems, but was not associated with use level. There were few prospective effects. SR consistently predicted greater marijuana use and problems. SP however, exhibited both risk and protective pathways. Results indicate that SP is associated with a decreased likelihood of marijuana use. However, once use is initiated SP is associated with increased risk of problems, in part, due to its effects on both affect and behavioral dysregulation.
Keywords: Reinforcement sensitivity theory, Affect lability, Impulsivity, Marijuana use, Marijuana problems
Marijuana is most commonly used illicit substance in the United States (SAMHSA, 2013; Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, Schulenberg, & Miech, 2015). It is especially prevalent among young adults, with approximately 20% of 18 to 25-year-olds reporting marijuana use in the past 30 days (SAMHSA, 2013; Johnston et al., 2015) and up to 10% of young adult college students exhibiting marijuana-related problems significant enough to warrant diagnosis of a marijuana use disorder (Caldeira, Arria, O’Grady, Vincent, & Wish, 2008; SAMHSA, 2010). Given the number of negative consequences associated with marijuana use (e.g., Pearson, Liese, Dvorak, & Marijuana Outcomes Study Team [MOST], 2017), understanding individual difference factors that contribute to marijuana use and marijuana-related problems is an important area of research.
Substance use contains both strong approach (e.g., euphoric effects) and inhibitory cues (e.g., legal, health risks). As such, differential responsiveness to punishment versus reward may be associated with increases or decreases in use through reinforcement processes. It follows that underlying individual differences in sensitivity to punishment and reward are associated with variability in substance use (Colder et al., 2013; Franken & Muris, 2006), including marijuana use (Simons, Dvorak, & Lau-Barraco, 2009b). However, because sensitivity to punishment (SP) and sensitivity to reward (SR) are related to diverse personality/regulatory constructs of relevance to substance use (e.g., neuroticism, impulsivity; Braddock et al., 2011; Corr & McNaughton, 2008), the linkage between SR, SP, intermediate regulatory constructs, and risky substance use needs to be further examined. The goal of the current study is to delineate the independent and joint effects of SR and SP on more proximal affective and behavioral regulatory constructs and marijuana use and problems.
Sensitivity to Punishment and Reward
There are several neuropsychological theories of personality (for review, see DeYoung & Gray, 2009). One prominent model is reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST; Corr, 2004; Corr & McNaughton, 2008; Gray & McNaughton, 2000) which posits that individual differences in personality, emotions, motivation, and behavior arise from three conceptual brain systems thought to differentially respond to reward and punishment. Consequently, they are believed to govern approach and avoidance behavior. One system, the Behavioral Approach System (BAS), is suggested to be sensitive to stimuli that signal reward and underlies approach motivation, positive affect, and reinforcement learning processes. Evidence suggests that individuals with a hypersensitive BAS tend to be impulsive, sensation-seekers who respond to reward-related cues with increases in positive affect and approach motivation (Carlson, Pritchard, & Dominelli, 2013; Corr, 2008; Gray, 1987). Hence, a hypersensitive BAS (i.e., sensitivity to reward) may be a risk factor for substance use motivated by positive reinforcement. Several studies have shown those with a high BAS display problematic substance use (Simons, Dvorak, & Batien, 2008; Wardell, O’Connor, Read, & Colder, 2011) and poor control over behavior motivated by reward (Braddock et al., 2011; Franken & Muris, 2006).
The two remaining systems, the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) and the Fight-Flight-Freezing System (FFFS), are thought to govern responses to aversive stimuli and be engaged during avoidance behavior. The BIS inhibits behavior through increased arousal and risk assessment in response to stimuli signaling the loss of expected reward, uncertainty, and goal conflict. The BIS gives rise to emotional distress and is associated with trait negative affect, especially anxiety (Corr, 2008; Gray, 1982; Smillie, Pickering, & Jackson, 2006). The FFFS mediates responses to cues of punishment and non-reward, is responsible for escape behaviors, and gives rise to the emotions fear and panic. These two systems jointly control avoidance motivation and punishment sensitivity can be conceptualized as hypersensitivity of these two systems combined (Corr, 2004).
Given the primary combined function of these systems is to effectively inhibit behavioral approach to avoid punishment, some researchers have speculated that elevated SP may serve as a protective factor, deterring hazardous use to avoid negative consequences (Franken & Muris, 2006). In keeping with this view, several studies have found an inverse relationship between SP and substance use (Pardo, Aguilar, Molinuevo, & Torrubia, 2007; Simons & Arens, 2007; Wray, Simons, & Dvorak, 2011). Despite this support, SP is also heavily associated with trait negative affectivity (Erdle & Rushton, 2010; Smillie et al., 2006), which is linked to increased substance use and related problems (McCarthy, Curtin, Piper, & Baker, 2010; Simons, Gaher, Oliver, Bush, & Palmer, 2005b; Simons, Wills, & Neal, 2014). Hence, there may be an alternate pathway linking SP to increases in substance use and related problems. That is, because SP is associated with heightened negative affectivity it is possible that SP might lead to problematic substance use through negative reinforcement mechanisms. Indeed, some researchers have found positive relationships between SP and problematic substance use (Knyazev, 2004; Voigt et al., 2009). Taken together, it is clear that these relationships are complex and, as some researchers suggest (Wardell et al., 2011), it may be necessary to examine interactions between SP and SR to appropriately characterize the role of SP in the context of risky substance use.
Traditional views of RST (Gray, 1987) advocate that SR and SP stem from independent brain systems, while suggesting their influence on behavior is the result of a joint effort. This view is outlined by the joint subsystems hypothesis, which contends that these factors can interact to predict behavior and emotional experiences, with the effects being conditional upon each other (Corr, 2004, 2013). In line with this, research has examined the interaction between SR and SP as it relates to substance use. However, this has produced inconsistent findings, with some finding a positive relationship between SP and substance use, which is then amplified by high levels of SR (O’Connor, Stewart, & Marlatt, 2009; Wardell et al., 2011), while others have found a negative effect of SP on substance use, that was attenuated by high level of SR (Simons & Arens, 2007; Simons et al., 2008). As a result, the exact nature of these associations remains unclear. One way of clarifying this may lie in examining how SR and SP might be related to different dimensions of personality. In support of this, the joint subsystems hypothesis further advocates that greater SR and lower SP culminate in maximum poor behavioral control, whereas, the combined strength of SR and SP (with stronger input from SP than SR) gives rise to maximum affective instability (Corr, 2004, 2013). In that regard, SR and SP are associated with characteristic regulatory deficits in emotion and behavior (Braddock et al., 2011; Carlson et al., 2013; Markarian, Pickett, Deveson, & Kanona, 2013; Tull, Gratz, Latzman, Kimbrel, & Lejuez, 2010), which may mediate associations between SP and SR and substance use outcomes.
Affect Lability
Affect lability is a central feature of affect dysregulation (Jahng et al., 2011) and refers to the frequency, speed, and range of changes in affective states an individual experiences (Harvey, Greenberg, & Serper, 1989). This variation is characterized by large, erratic, shifts in feeling states in response to internal or external cues. Individuals high in affect lability are unable to dampen their emotional responses and therefore lack emotional stability. Affect lability is associated with substance use and associated problems (Simons & Carey, 2002, 2006; Wills, Walker, Mendoza, & Ainette, 2006) and often demonstrates unique associations with substance dependence symptoms, independent of use (Simons, Carey, & Wills, 2009a; Simons, Oliver, Gaher, Ebel, & Brummels, 2005c; Simons et al., 2014). Furthermore, affective instability has been shown to be a key risk factor, differentiating problematic use from non-problem use (Kuvaas, Dvorak, Pearson, Lamis, & Sargent, 2014).
The relationship between affective dysregulation and substance use is complex, with emotional instability appearing to contribute to the development of problematic use in a number of ways. First, affective processing models of negative reinforcement implicate fluctuations in negative affect paired with repeated substance use as the core mechanism behind the development and maintenance of substance use disorders (Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004; McCarthy et al., 2010). The frequent and intense affective shifts experienced by those with high affect lability may strengthen the conditioned associations between emotional arousal and substance use (Simons et al., 2014). Sudden increases in emotional arousal have also been shown to impair deliberative control processes and increase the effects of more automatic appetitive processes on behavior (Baker et al., 2004; Lieberman, 2007; Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999). Taken together, affect lability seems to be associated with problematic use patterns, but not more normative consumption (Simons & Carey, 2002; Simons et al., 2009a; Simons, Gaher, Correia, Hansen, & Christopher, 2005a; Simons et al., 2014).
In personality trait models such as the Five Factor Model (Costa & McCrae, 1996), emotional instability (i.e., lability) is subsumed by the trait neuroticism. In RST, neuroticism is posited to result from the joint effects of SP and SR, whereby those with high SP and high SR display the greatest emotional disturbance. That is, high SP is positively associated with emotional symptoms and the magnitude of that relationship (i.e., slope) is greatest in high SR individuals. Whereas, that relationship decreases in strength at lower levels of SR (Corr, 2004; Heym, Ferguson, & Lawrence, 2008). Hence, affect lability may be one mechanism linking SP and SR to substance use outcomes. However, research also identifies SP and SR as underlying characteristic differences in behavioral control or impulsivity (Carlson et al., 2013; Torrubia, Avila, Moltó, & Caseras, 2001), which are also closely tied to substance use behavior (Lejuez et al., 2010; Verdejo-García, Lawrence, & Clark, 2008).
Poor Control
Poor control of behavior (e.g., impulsivity) has been extensively linked to problematic substance use in both human and animal models (Dawe & Loxton, 2004; Lejuez et al., 2010) and is considered an important risk factor in the genesis and course of substance use behavior (Collado, Felton, MacPherson, & Lejuez, 2014; Simons et al., 2009a; Verdejo-García et al., 2008; Wills et al., 2013). Poor behavioral control is characterized by lack of planning and forethought, failure to consider risks, premature responding, inability to delay gratification, and difficulty restricting behavior to those required by situational demands (Daruna & Barnes, 1993; Plutchik & Van Praag, 1995; Wills et al., 2006). Poor control is thought to increase risk for substance use because an individual’s behavior is governed by more immediate hot cognition and less by cooler, thoughtful processing of long-term consequences (Carver, 2005; Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999) and substance-related problems ensue as a result of deficits in the capacity to control behavior commensurate with situational demands (Tarter et al., 2003). Poor control is connected to use of a variety of licit and illicit substances (e.g., heroin, alcohol; Dissabandara et al., 2014; Simons & Carey, 2006; Simons et al., 2009a), including marijuana use (Day, Metrik, Spillane, & Kahler, 2013; Simons & Carey, 2002). Altogether, poor control appears to contribute significantly both directly to substance-related problems (Dvorak & Day, 2014; Simons et al., 2009a) and indirectly through its effects on use behavior (Simons et al., 2009a).
In the RST, impulsivity, or poor behavioral control, is posited to arise from the joint effects of SP and SR (Corr, 2004, 2013). Specifically, those with the poor behavioral control (i.e., inhibitory deficits) are characterized by high SR and low SP. This constellation minimizes the reciprocal inhibitory feedback loop between punishment and reward mechanisms and maximizes behavioral disinhibition. Subsequently, as SP raises it attenuates the positive relationship between SR and poor behavioral control, and vice versa (Avila & Parcet, 2001; Corr, 2004; Heym et al., 2008). Hence, poor behavioral control may mediate associations between SP, SR and substance use behavior, given poor control’s role as a propagator of substance use behavior.
The Current Study
In this study, we tested a reinforcement sensitivity model for marijuana use and problems using a 6-month prospective design. The hypothesized model is depicted in Figure 1. SP and SR were hypothesized to be indirectly associated with marijuana use and associated problems via affect lability and poor control. SP and SR were expected to interact in predicting the intermediary constructs of affect lability and poor control. Specifically, the positive association between SR and poor control is expected to be conditional upon levels of SP, such that the association is attenuated at higher levels of SP. Similarly, the positive association between SP and affect lability is expected to be conditional upon levels of SR, such that the association would be potentiated at higher levels of SR. Affect lability at baseline was expected to have direct associations with marijuana problems at Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2). Poor behavioral control was hypothesized to have direct effects on both marijuana use and problems at T1 and T2.
Methods
Participants
Participant were 2,270 college students (64% female) that ranged in age from 18 to 25 years (M = 19.59, SD = 1.51). Ninety-four percent of the participants were White, 1% Asian, 1% African American, 1% Native American/Alaskan Native, and 3% other or do not wish to respond. Ninety-eight percent were non-Hispanic. Participants were recruited through e-mail and student newspaper advertisements. Of the participants who completed the baseline measures, 87% returned for a 6-month follow-up (T2), and of returning participants 94% were successfully matched to their baseline (T1) data. Four articles have been published from this dataset (Gaher, Hahn, Shishido, Simons, & Gaster, 2015; Simons et al., 2009a; Simons et al., 2008; Wray et al., 2011).
Procedure
Participants completed online questionnaires in a private space under the supervision of a research assistant and were provided informed consent. The study was approved by the institutional review board. To ensure participants’ anonymity, each participant was assigned a unique code during their completion of the baseline questionnaires. Participants received $20 for completing the baseline assessment and $30 for completing the 6-month follow-up assessment. The average time interval between assessments was 203 days (SD = 50.35).
Measures
Sensitivity to Punishment and Reward
Sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward were assessed by the Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ; Torrubia et al., 2001). The SPSRQ is a 48-item scale with a dichotomous (yes/no) response format. For the current study, the factor solution obtained by O’Connor, Colder, and Hawk (2004) was used. Thus, the sensitivity to reward (SR) scale consisted of 17 items (α = .78; sample item: “Do you often do things to be praised?”) and the sensitivity to punishment (SP) scale consisted of 18 items (α = .85; sample item: “Do you often refrain from doing something because of your fear of being embarrassed?”). Construct validity of these scales as indicators of behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation systems is supported by expected relationships with other individual difference measures (e.g., anxiety, impulsivity, neuroticism; Caseras, Àvila, & Torrubia, 2003; Torrubia et al., 2001). In the current model, SP and SR were latent variables with 3 indicators each, which were parcels formed by the item-to-construct method (Little, Cunningham, Shahar, & Widaman, 2002).
Affect lability
Affect lability was a latent variable measured by three indicators derived from the subscales of the Affective Lability Scale- Short Form (ALS; Oliver & Simons, 2004). Items are rated on a 4-point scale (1 = very undescriptive and 4 = very descriptive; sample item: “I switch back and forth between being extremely energetic and having so little energy that it’s a huge effort just to get where I am going”). The three subscales assess affective lability in respect to depression or elation (8 items, α = .87), anxiety or depression (5 items, α = .86), and anger (5 items, α = .83).
Poor control
Poor control was a latent variable defined by three indicators. First, seven items from the Eysenck Impulsivity Scale (I7; Eysenck, Pearson, Easting, & Allsopp, 1985) assessed difficulty in controlling behavior (α = .75; sample item: “Do you generally do and say things without stopping to think?”). Second, we derived two scales from a measure of self-regulation (Kendall & Williams, 1982): a 3-item scale representing impatience (α =.52; sample item: “I have to have everything right away”) and a 6-item scale representing distractibility (α =.81; sample item: “I like to switch from one thing to another”). These items were rated on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all true of me and 5 = very true of me). Previous research has demonstrated that these measures of impulsivity, impatience, and distractibility form a replicable subscale structure and load significantly on a factor of poor control (Wills et al., 2001; Wills, Sandy, & Yaeger, 2002).
Marijuana use
Marijuana use was an observed variable assessed by a 1-week grid with 4, 6-hour, periods per day (Williams, Adams, Stephens, & Roffman, 2000). Participants indicated the number of time periods where they consumed marijuana in a typical week in the past 6 months. This measure of marijuana use has demonstrated good criterion validity in previous research (Simons & Carey, 2006; Simons et al., 2005a) and moderate relations with DSM-IV marijuana abuse and dependence (Williams et al., 2000).
Marijuana problems
Marijuana problems was an observed variable assessed by a substance use problems scale (Wills et al., 2002), reworded to focus on marijuana. This 16-item inventory measures marijuana-related problems in the past 6 months. Items were scored to indicate the presence or absence of each item with total scores ranging from 0 – 16. Sample items include: “You worried about your use,” “A friend told you to cut down.” Validity of the parent measure of substance-related problems is supported by moderate to strong expected associations with level of substance use, use motives, as well as peer and parental substance use levels across multiple samples (Wills, Pokhrel, Morehouse, & Fenster, 2011; Wills et al., 2002).
Analysis Plan
Structural equation models were estimated in Mplus 7 (Muthén & Muthén, 2013) using maximum likelihood robust estimation. We utilized a model building approach (i.e., increasing model complexity in steps) to sequentially examine the hypotheses and create a parsimonious model (see results). SR, SP, affect lability, and poor control were specified as continuous latent variables. Given that only 30% of participants reported marijuana use at T1 and just 23% at T2, use and problem variables were expected to have an "excess" of zeroes. Thus, marijuana use and problems were specified as observed variables with zero-inflated negative binomial (ZINB) distributions (Simons, Neal, & Gaher, 2006; Simons et al., 2014). The ZINB reference distribution is ideal for count outcomes that are positively skewed and include excessive zeros. The ZINB model includes two parts; the zero-inflated portion of the model, which determines the probability that an observation is “always” zero (i.e., in excess of what would be expected from the negative binomial distribution), and a count portion, which models the count of the outcome conditional upon the likelihood of the observation being potentially non-zero (Cameron & Trivedi., 1998; Collins & Spelman, 2013). Gender was included as a covariate. As recommended by Kline (2011), a two-step approach was utilized, such that the measurement model was examined followed by the structural model. Guidelines for what constitutes a good fit vary, although a CFI ≥ .95 is thought to represent very good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2011), and RMSEA < .06 as well as SRMR values of .08 or lower are thought to indicate a close fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Chi-square values that are closer to zero and not significant are suggestive of good fit (Kline, 2011). However, in models that require numeric integration (such as ours), traditional model fit indices used to interpret the fit of structural equation models (e.g., CFI, RMSEA, SRMR, and χ2) have not been developed. Instead, an alternative method for assessing the fit of each model needs to be employed (Klein & Moosbrugger, 2000; Maslowsky, Jager, & Hemken, 2015), whereby nested models are compared using a likelihood ratio test. This is operationalized as the relative fit of the base model and the expanded alternative model.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
One participant was excluded from the analysis because the majority of their data were missing. The analysis sample consisted of 2,269 individuals. Descriptive statistics for observed variables are in Table 1. At T1, participants reported using marijuana in an average of 1.68 (SD = 4.25) 6-hour time periods per week in the past 6 months, with approximately 30% of the total sample reported using marijuana at least once per week and approximately 23% reported at least one marijuana-related problem during that time. At T2, participants reported using marijuana an average of 1.23 (SD = 3.76) 6-hour time periods per week in the previous 6 months. At follow-up, approximately 23% endorsed using in at least once per week and approximately 16% reported having at least one marijuana-related problem in the 6 months since the baseline. Marijuana use and problems at T1 were highly correlated (r = .67, p < .001) as were use and problems at T2 (r = .67, p < .001). Marijuana use and associated problems were moderately stable over time with correlations between the T1 and T2 assessments of use (r = .59, p < .001) and problems (r = .52, p < .001), respectively.
Table 1.
M (SD) | Median | Range | Skew | Kurtosis | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
ALS Anxiety-Depression | 8.73 (3.61) | 8 | 5–20 | 0.87 | 2.97 |
ALS Depression-Elation | 16.61 (5.30) | 17 | 8–32 | 0.28 | 2.50 |
ALS Anger | 7.94 (3.22) | 7 | 5–20 | 1.26 | 4.15 |
PC Impatience | 5.96 (2.14) | 6 | 3–15 | 0.82 | 3.61 |
PC Distractibility | 15.14 (4.85) | 15 | 6–30 | 0.35 | 2.65 |
PC Impulsivity | 2.63 (2.12) | 2 | 0–7 | 0.44 | 2.06 |
SR Parcel 1 | 2.47 (1.39) | 3 | 0–5 | −0.05 | 2.20 |
SR Parcel 2 | 3.40 (1.54) | 3 | 0–6 | −0.18 | 2.24 |
SR Parcel 3 | 2.68 (1.54) | 3 | 0–6 | 0.09 | 2.27 |
SP Parcel 1 | 2.87 (1.71) | 3 | 0–6 | 0.09 | 2.02 |
SP Parcel 2 | 3.35 (1.72) | 3 | 0–6 | −0.21 | 2.11 |
SP Parcel 3 | 2.52 (1.79) | 2 | 0–6 | 0.23 | 2.03 |
T1 Marijuana Use | 1.68 (4.25) | 0 | 0–28 | 3.63 | 17.22 |
T1 Marijuana Problems | 0.72 (1.75) | 0 | 0–14 | 3.26 | 15.29 |
T2 Marijuana Use | 1.23 (3.76) | 0 | 0–28 | 4.53 | 25.84 |
T2 Marijuana Problems | 0.46 (1.43) | 0 | 0–16 | 4.57 | 30.59 |
Note. N = 2,270. ALS = Affect Lability; PC = Poor Control; SR = Sensitivity to Reward; SP = Sensitivity to Punishment.
Measurement Model
An initial measurement model was estimated using maximum likelihood robust estimation that had latent factors for SP, SR, affect lability, and poor control. Gender was included as a covariate with paths to each factor. The initial measurement model was an adequate fit to the data χ2 (56, N = 2269) = 782.02, p < .001; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .076 90% CI [.071, .080]; comparative fit index (CFI) = .93; standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) = .048. The modification indices were examined to evaluate whether to free additional constrained parameters in the model. Parameters were sequentially freed and the model re-estimated. This process resulted in four modifications to the model: 1) measurement errors of the depression-elation indicator and the distractibility indicator were allowed to covary; 2) the measurement errors for SP-parcel 3 and SR-parcel 2 were allowed to covary; 3) a path from gender to the impatience indicator was freed; and 4) the measurement errors of SP-parcel 3 and SR-parcel 1 allowed to covary. Each of the freed parameters had a modification index greater than 29.00. Freeing these parameters resulted in an improved final model that was a good fit to the data χ2 (52, N = 2269) = 517.56, p < .001; RMSEA = .063 90% CI [.058, .068]; CFI = .95; SRMR = .042. Standardized factor loadings in the final model ranged from .62 to .87. Standardized correlations between latent variables were all significant (p < .000) and positive, ranging from .18 to .60.
Structural Model
Given of the complexity of the analysis, we used a model-building strategy designed to minimize Type I error and to develop a parsimonious model. At each step, joint tests of conceptually related effects (i.e., a test of all effects being zero) were conducted and likelihood ratio tests were examined to determine whether to add the hypothesized effects. Table 3 reports the result of the model building steps, linking each step to the study hypotheses. The final model is depicted in Figure 2 along with the resulting parameter estimates.
Table 3.
Model Test | Comparison Model |
Parameters | Loglikelihood | AIC BIC |
χ2 Difference | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hypothesized Model | Base model. | - | 78 | −56191.39 | 112538.39 112985.69 | - | - |
Step 2. Test direct effects of PC to T2 use | Base | 80 | −56182.934 | 112525.94 112984.04 | χ2(2) = 15.59 | .000 | |
Step 3. Test direct effects of PC and ALS on T2 problems | Step 2 | 84 | −56178.32 | 112524.65 113005.72 | χ2(4) = 6.64 | .156 | |
Alternative Model with Direct Effects | Step 4. Test direct effects of SR, SP and SP x SR on T1 use | Step 2 | 86 | −56154.41 | 112480.82 112973.35 | χ2(6) = 60.28 | .000 |
Step 5. Test direct effects of SR, SP and SP x SR on T1 problems | Step 4 | 92 | −56146.26 | 112476.53 113003.42 | χ2(6) = 16.16 | .013 | |
Step 5.1. Test direct effects of SR and SP on T1 problems inflation | Step 4 | 88 | −56147.85 | 112471.70 112975.68 | χ2(2) = 14.79 | .001 | |
Step 6. Test prospective direct effects of SR, SP, and SR x SP on T2 use | Step 5.1 | 94 | −56143.56 | 112475.12 113013.47 | χ2(6) = 9.21 | .162 | |
Step 6.1. Test prospective direct effects of SR and SP on T2 use | Step 5.1 | 92 | −56144.12 | 112472.24 112999.13 | χ2(4) = 7.289 | .121 | |
Step 7. Test prospective direct effects of SR, SP, and SR x SP on T2 problems | Step 5.1 | 94 | −56138.16 | 112464.32 113002.67 | χ2(6) = 13.98 | .029 | |
Step 7.1. Test prospective direct effects of SR and SP on T2 problems | Step 5.1 | 92 | −56139.17 | 112462.35 112989.24 | χ2(4) = 11.80 | .019 |
Note. SP = Sensitivity to Punishment; SR = Sensitivity to Reward; ALS = Affect Lability; PC = Poor Control
First, a latent variable structural model was estimated with gender as a covariate. This model included paths from SR and SP to affect lability and poor control, respectively, as well as a correlation between SR and SP. This model fit the data well (χ2 (52, N = 2269) = 517.56, p < .001; RMSEA = .063 90% CI [.058, .068]; CFI = .95; SRMR = .042). Next, a SP x SR latent variable interaction was introduced to the model with paths to both affect lability (b = 0.03, p = .057) and poor control (b = 0.02, p = .468). This addition did not significantly improve the model Δ χ2 (2, N = 2269) = 3.70, p = .157, and thus the interaction effects were removed.
Next, the observed marijuana outcomes (i.e., marijuana use and marijuana problems at both T1 and T2) were introduced to the previously fit latent variable model and a new structural model was estimated (Step 1). The observed marijuana outcomes were each specified as a count with a ZINB distribution. This new base model included cross-sectional direct paths from poor control to T1 use and problems and from affect lability to T1 problems as well as prospective direct effects of T1 marijuana use on T2 use and problems and prospective direct effect of T1 problems on T2 problems. Lastly, gender had direct paths to each of the substance use outcomes. Results from the base model indicated that gender was not associated with either substance use outcome at T2 nor was it significantly associated with T1 problems. These nonsignificant gender effects were subsequently dropped for a more parsimonious model. In Step 2, we tested the prospective effects of poor control on T2 marijuana use. This was a significant addition to the model, and thus, was retained (see Table 3). Step 3 tested the prospected effects of affect lability and poor control on T2 marijuana problems. This did not result in a significant addition and was dropped from the model.
Alternative model
The model estimation thus far tested the significance of expected effects and found partial support for hypothesized paths. In addition to the estimated paths, the hypothesized model (see figure 1) was such that the effects of SR and SP were entirely indirect through affect lability and poor control. That is, the model specifies that direct effects of SP and SR on the marijuana outcomes are non-significant (i.e., zero). The following section tests whether this hypothesized structure is tenable by estimating a series of alternative models that test whether direct effects of SP and SR on the marijuana outcomes are non-significant.
In Step 4, we added directs effects of SR, SP and SP x SR latent variable interaction on T1 use. This was a significant addition to the model and was retained (see Table 3). The SP x SR interaction was significant in predicting the inflation (b = −0.25, p = .002) but not count (b = 0.05, p = .196) outcome. The interaction is depicted in Figure 3. Consistent with research on this and other samples (Simons & Arens, 2007; Simons et al., 2008), SP had a conditional direct effect on the likelihood of being abstinent at T1. At low levels of SR (−1 SD below the mean) SP was associated with an increased likelihood of abstaining (b = 0.89, p > .001) this effect was attenuated as levels of SR increased (+1 SD above the mean; b = 0.19, p = .198). In Step 5, the effects of SP, SR and SP x SR interaction on T1 problems were tested and represented a significant addition to the model. Yet, the SP x SR interaction effect was not significant for either the inflation (b = −0.08, p = .406) or count portions (b = 0.04, p = .232). Hence, the interaction was dropped and Step 5 re-estimated. The joint test of the main effects on the outcomes was significant Δ χ2 (4, N = 2269) = 12.88, p = .012. However, the inclusion of the effects of SR and SP resulted in all of the effects on the count portion (i.e., affect lability, poor control, SR, and SP) to be nonsignificant. Thus, to have a more interpretable and parsimonious model, the effects of SR and SP were retained for the inflation portion, but dropped from the count portion (see Step 5.1 in Table 3). Step 6 tested the prospective direct effects of SR, SP, and SR x SP interaction on T2 use. The Step did not significantly improve the model, nor did addition of the main effects without the interaction terms (see Step 6.1 in Table 3). Hence, no additional paths to T2 use were retained. Lastly, Step 7 tested the prospective direct effects of SR, SP, and SR x SP interaction on T2 problems. This resulted in a significant improvement in the overall model, but the interaction effects were not significant: inflation (b = 0.12, p = .373); count (b = 0.05, p = .354). Thus, for parsimony, the interaction effects were dropped and the Step re-estimated. This resulted in retaining direct paths from SP and SR to T2 problems (see Step 7.1 in Table 3). Finally, results of the loglikelihood ratio test for the final model (Step 7.1; AIC = 112538.39, BIC =112985.69) versus the base model (AIC =112462.32, BIC = 112989.24) favored the final model Δ χ2 (14, N = 2269) = 94.27, p > .000.
Structural model summary
The final model estimates are presented in Figure 2. Importantly, gender was included as a covariate in the final model, however, it was omitted from the figure for clarity. Thus, those effects are listed here. Female gender was associated with SP (b = −0.54, p > .001), affect lability (b = −0.10, p = .009), and the likelihood of being a abstinent at T1 (i.e., T1 marijuana use inflation; b = −0.47, p = .018); whereas male gender was associated with SR (b = 0.54, p > .001) and the intensity of marijuana use at T1 (i.e., T1 marijuana use count; b = 0.38, p = .001). Gender was not associated with poor control (b = −0.05, p = .544). Also omitted for clarity from the figure of the final model was the correlation between the disturbance terms of affect lability and poor control. These were significantly related (b = 0.40, p > .001), indicating that these constructs have some shared sources of residual variance.
Indirect effects
The zero-inflated variables cannot be included as mediators in traditional tests of indirect effects. Hence, we provide tests of indirect effects that do not include marijuana use or problems as a mediator (see Table 4) using bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals (MacKinnon, Lockwood, & Williams, 2004). As hypothesized, SR and SP demonstrated indirect effects on marijuana use and problems via affect lability and poor control. For example, SP was indirectly associated with a decreased likelihood of being abstinent (i.e., inflation) and increased use intensity (i.e., count) at T1 via poor control. However, the total effect of SP was significantly associated with an increased likelihood of being abstinent at T1. Similarly, SP was indirectly related with increased problem intensity at T1 through affect lability and poor control. SR exhibited indirect associations with a decreased likelihood of being abstinent, increased use intensity, and increased problem intensity at T1 via poor control. The indirect relationship between SR and T1 problem intensity via affect lability was also significant. As for T2 use, both SP and SR showed significant prospective indirect associations with a decreased likelihood of being abstinent at T2 through poor control. Whereas, the prospective indirect effects of SP and SR on use intensity at T2 via poor control were not significant.
Table 4.
Variables/Path | b | SE | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|
Sensitivity to Punishment (SP) | |||
SP → ALS → T1 Problem Intensity | 0.06 | 0.03 | [0.006, 0.129] |
SP → PC → T1 Likelihood of Abstaining | −0.17 | 0.05 | [−0.288, −0.072] |
SP → PC → T1 Use Intensity | 0.09 | 0.03 | [0.033, 0.151] |
SP → PC → T1 Problem Intensity | 0.06 | 0.02 | [0.015, 0.098] |
SP → PC → T2 Likelihood of Abstaining | −0.16 | 0.05 | [−0.260, −0.066] |
SP → PC → T2 Use Intensity | −0.01 | 0.02 | [−0.053, 0.032] |
Sensitivity to Reward (SR) | |||
SR → ALS → T1 Problem Intensity | 0.02 | 0.01 | [0.003, 0.054] |
SR → PC → T1 Likelihood of Abstaining | −0.25 | 0.07 | [−0.418, −0.117] |
SR → PC → T1 Use Intensity | 0.13 | 0.04 | [0.048, 0.226] |
SR → PC → T1 Problem Intensity | 0.08 | 0.03 | [0.024, 0.148] |
SR → PC → T2 Likelihood of Abstaining | −0.23 | 0.07 | [−0.369, −0.081] |
SR → PC → T2 Use Intensity | −0.02 | 0.03 | [−0.083, 0.043] |
Total Effects | |||
SP → T1 Likelihood of Abstaining | 0.37 | 0.10 | [0.190, 0.608] |
SP → T1 Use Intensity | 0.06 | 0.07 | [−0.113, 0.183] |
SR → T1 Likelihood of Abstaining | −0.76 | 0.12 | [−1.082, −0.552] |
SR → T1 Use Intensity | 0.00 | 0.07 | [−0.153, 0.158] |
Note. ALS = Affect Lability; PC = Poor Control; SR = Sensitivity to Reward; SP = Sensitivity to Punishment; Intensity = Count Portion of Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial (ZINB) Count Model; Likelihood = Inflation Portion of ZINB Count Model. The inflation portion of the ZINB count model reflects the likelihood of obtaining a zero (i.e., abstaining or having no problems, respectively). Therefore, a positive value represents an increased likelihood of obtaining a zero and a negative value indicates a decrease likelihood of obtaining a zero value (i.e. using). Indirect and total effects were calculated using bias corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals. Significance is represented by a confidence interval that does not contain zero. Total effect represent the effect of one exogenous variable at the mean level of the other.
Several total effects could not be estimated, given some paths had mediators that were zero-inflated. Therefore, in order to gain a sense of the overall pattern of the effects of SP and SR on the primary outcomes (i.e., marijuana use, marijuana problems) a series of t tests were conducted. We first compared levels of SP and SR between marijuana users (past 6 month use at baseline or follow-up, n = 799) and non-users (no past 6-month use at baseline or follow-up, n = 1471). Those who used marijuana showed significantly lower levels of SP (M = 8.20, SD = 4.53) than those who did not (M = 9.05, SD = 4.47), t(1618.84) = 4.31, p < .001, d = −0.19. In contrast, SR was higher among marijuana users than non-users (M = 9.65, SD = 3.67 versus M = 7.96, SD = 3.64), t(1629.73) = −10.53, p < .001, d = 0.46. We next examined levels of SP and SR as a function of presence or absence of marijuana-related problems at T2. SP among those with problems (n = 1917, M = 8.75, SD = 4.37) did not significantly differ than those without problems (n = 352, M = 8.75, SD = 4.53), t(499.50) = −0.002, p = .99. However, those with problems had higher levels of SR (M = 9.66, SD = 3.76) than those who did not experience problems (M = 8.35, SD = 3.69), t(481.72) = −6.00, p < .001, d = 0.35. Finally, we compared levels of SP and SR among marijuana users who reported marijuana related problems at follow-up (n = 323), versus those who did not (n = 475). Those reporting marijuana problems (M = 8.74, SD = 4.36) displayed higher levels of SP than those without problems (M = 7.83, SD = 4.60), t(714.58) = −2.84, , p < .001, d = 0.20; while those with problems (M = 9.81, SD = 3.77) did not significantly differ on levels of SR than those without problems (M = 9.55, SD = 3.59), t(669.65) = −0.96, p = .34.
Discussion
The purpose of this prospective study was to test a model of associations between individual differences in reinforcement sensitivity, affective and behavioral dysregulation, and marijuana outcomes at two time points in young adult college students. The effects of sensitivity to punishment and reward were expected to be indirect via affect lability and poor control. We also examined a series of theoretically-based interaction effects to test conditional relationships. The analytic approach distinguished between variables that predicted the likelihood of using marijuana or having marijuana problems versus not and variables that predicted the amount (or intensity) of marijuana use or problems, respectively, when a person had experienced one of these outcomes. In general, there was support for the hypothesized indirect effects, but little evidence of interaction effects. In the following sections, we discuss the findings in greater detail.
Effects of Sensitivity to Punishment and Reward on Dysregulated Affect and Behavior
A major aim of this study was to examine if the effects of SP and SR on dysregulated affect and behavior were consistent with the joint subsystems hypothesis (Corr, 2004, 2013). In short, the joint subsystems hypothesis states the neuroticism and extraversion are produced by the joint effects of punishment and reward sensitivity. Accordingly, SP and SR should interact to produce the turbulent affective experience characterized by those high in neuroticism and the poor behavioral control seen in those high in extraversion. In the case of affect lability, it was hypothesized that the positive association between SP and affect lability would be potentiated at higher levels of SR. This prediction is grounded in evidence showing that high SR is associated with increased positive affect whereas high SP is related to increased negative affect (e.g., O’Connor et al., 2004). Thus, those individuals high in both SP and SR would likely experience competing valenced emotions, effectively promoting large and erratic vacillations from positive to negative feelings states. Yet, despite this strong rationale, the interaction between SP and SR failed to significantly predict affect lability. Instead, both SP and SR exhibited positive direct effects on affect lability. The pattern is consistent with an alternative formulation of the joint subsystems hypothesis that predicts two main effects, with SP having greater magnitude than SR (Corr, 2013). In these data, both SP and SR exhibited positive direct effects on affect lability in the appropriate magnitude cited by the joint subsystems hypothesis (i.e., SP > SR). Hence, SP and SR have additive rather than multiplicative effects on lability resulting in large, erratic, shifts in feeling states.
Similarly, the SP x SR interaction was not significantly associated with poor control. This finding was unexpected, given that the joint subsystems hypothesis clearly states that impulsivity should arise from an interaction between SP and SR, where the positive relationship between SR and poor control is maximized at low levels of SP (Corr, 2004; Simons et al., 2008). Despite this, we again found evidence of two main effects. SR exhibited a positive direct effect on poor control, as hypothesized. However, contrary to expectation, SP demonstrated a positive direct effect on poor control as opposed to the anticipated inverse relationship. This unexpected finding is inconsistent with the larger literature showing inverse associations between SP and disinhibition (e.g., Carlson et al., 2013; Torrubia et al., 2001) and it implies being sensitive to punishment in some way gives rise to poorly controlled behavior. There are several potential explanations for this finding. For example, this could reflect the positive relationship between neuroticism and impulsivity (e.g., Eysenck & Eysenck, 1977; Valero et al., 2014). Indeed, the construct negative urgency is characterized by high neuroticism and emotionally-driven impulsive behavior, thus, integrating negative emotionality (i.e., neuroticism) and reckless action (i.e., impulsivity) into a single construct (Settles et al., 2012; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). Therefore, this finding may reflect negative emotionally-driven impulsive behavior related to avoidance.
Sensitivity to Reward and Marijuana-Related Outcomes
The pattern of relationships found here consistently indicated that SR was associated with increased risk for marijuana use and problems. This is in line with mounting evidence suggesting that SR is a risk factor for substance use and associated problems (e.g., O’Connor & Colder, 2005; Pardo et al., 2007; Simons et al., 2009b). However, this study extends previous research by explicating the intermediate regulatory constructs by which being high in SR leads to increased marijuana use and problems. Importantly, this was the case for dysregulated affect and behavior as SR exhibited positive indirect effects on the marijuana-related outcomes through both affect lability and poor control.
The indirect effects of SR through poor control were consistent with the hypothesized model where poor control was expected to mediate associations between SR and substance use behavior, given poor control’s role as a propagator of substance use behavior (Day et al., 2013; Simons & Carey, 2002). Specifically, SR exhibited an inverse indirect effect via poor control on the likelihood of being abstinent at T1 and T2. Similarly, SR demonstrated positive indirect effects through poor control on use and problem intensity at T1. These results are theoretically consistent with RST and makes sense in the context of a positive reinforcement pathway to increased marijuana use and problems. Individuals high in SR display a sensitivity to positive incentives that increase the propensity for approach behavior and decrease inhibitory control of behaviors motivated by reward (Corr, 2004; Franken & Muris, 2006) which, at least in part, places them at risk for learning the positive reinforcing effects of marijuana, effectively increasing the likelihood and intensity of use. This is consistent with the contemporary view of the central importance of reward neural circuitry in addictive behaviors (Koob & Volkow, 2010) and with studies suggesting that increased motivation for reward underlies impulsive behavior leading to increased risky health behavior (Braddock et al., 2011). In contrast to poor control, affect lability had significant direct paths to problem intensity, but not use. Interestingly, SR also exhibited direct effects on both use and problems, indicating that the sensitivity to positive incentives contributes to marijuana-related outcomes above and beyond its effects via dysregulated behavioral and affective control.
It is important to note that recent research suggests that the BAS construct is multidimensional with factors representing various processes involved in identifying and attaining reinforcers (Krupić, Corr, Ručević, Križanić, & Gračanin, 2016a; Krupić, Gračanin, & Corr, 2016b). These individual processes reflect differences in identification of reinforcers, planning behavior, execution of behaviors designed to obtain reinforcers, and the emotional reactions to receiving reward. SR, as measured here, has been found to load onto a factor related to execution of behavior to obtain reward (Krupić et al., 2016a). For these reasons, future research would be advanced by inclusion of measures that allow for the BAS to be decomposed to facilitate more granular examination of the individual aspects of BAS functioning on substance use and problems (Corr & Cooper, 2016).
Sensitivity to Punishment and Marijuana-Related Outcomes
In contrast to SR, there have been a series of mixed findings in regard to the associations between SP and substance use, with some studies showing increased risk (O’Connor et al., 2009; Wardell et al., 2011) others showing decreased risk (Simons et al., 2008; Wray et al., 2011), and some finding no relationship (Colder et al., 2013). The results of this study clarify several of the existing discrepancies. The associations between SP and marijuana use and problems were complex. The relationship between SP and the likelihood using marijuana varied as a function of SR, such that the positive relationship between SP and the likelihood of abstaining was attenuated at high levels of SR. This finding is consistent with previous studies using these data (Simons et al., 2008) and others (Simons & Arens, 2007). This suggests that being sensitive to potential aversive consequences is a protective factor, buffering against use. However, as SR rises the protective effect of SP is diminished.
In addition, the effects of SP varied across outcomes within the model, exhibiting both risk protecting and risk promoting pathways. For example, SP was associated with an increased probability of abstaining from marijuana use at T1. In turn, this decreased probability of use at T1 is expected to have an inverse cascading effect on outcomes that come after it in the model contributing to lower overall risk. However, SP exhibited risk promoting effects as well. For example, SP was positively associated with T1 problem intensity via its effect on poor control and affect lability.
Unfortunately, we are unable to calculate the total effects due to distributions of the T1 marijuana use and problem variables. To remedy this, we conducted a series of t-tests to facilitate interpreting the overall effects of SP. Here we found that the net effect of SP on problems in the full sample was zero, suggesting that the risk promoting and risk protective effects of SP cancel each other out. However, among marijuana users those high in SP were more likely to exhibit problems. Hence, the results of these analyses are consistent with the idea that SP decreases the likelihood initiating marijuana use, but once an individual has initiated use behavior, SP may result in increased risk for problems. This would explain the mixed findings seen across studies as the result would depend on different sample characteristics, the outcomes being modeled, and the interaction with SR. It is important to note that even though the net effect of SP was zero does not mean it is irrelevant. In fact, just the opposite. SP has different effects for different people in different contexts, making it an important factor to consider.
Clinical Implications
The pattern of results highlights that different intervention targets may be most appropriate at different stages of the marijuana use continuum. These findings implicate behavioral dysregulation as a core issue underlying the initiation of use. Preventative interventions, perhaps, may be best served focusing broadly on increased adaptive self-regulation skills, rather than emphasizing negative consequences resulting from use. Individuals most likely to respond to information about potential dangers (i.e., those high in SP) are already less likely to use. The results are consistent with conceptualizations of substance use disorder as manifestations of underlying behavioral and emotional dysregulation. Our findings indicate that affect dysregulation exhibits a direct effect on problems, over and above use level, suggesting that once use has been initiated those with emotion regulation deficits are more likely to have problems even at lower levels of use. Effective regulation of emotion and behavior is essential for adaptive functioning. Hence, prevention programs that target regulatory deficits have the potential to impact individual well-being in addition to minimizing negative substance use outcomes (Wills, Simons, Manayan, & Robinson, in press). In addition, targeted substance specific interventions may be useful in minimizing harm among those that use marijuana. For example, use of marijuana-related protective behavioral strategies is a robust predictor of lower marijuana use frequency and associated consequences (Bravo, Anthenien, Prince, Pearson, & MOST, 2017; Pedersen, Hummer, Rinker, Traylor, & Neighbors, 2016). Interestingly, those who exhibit impulsivity-like traits have been shown to use these protective strategies less (Bravo et al., 2017). Hence, such interventions may benefit those that need it most.
Limitations
There are several limitations of the current study that should be noted. First, there were several potential indirect and total effects that could not be calculated because the mediator was modeled as two-parts (zeros and the count). This makes it difficult to fully characterize the effects of reinforcement sensitivity, affect lability, and poor control on problematic marijuana use. Second, though this is a complex model it is limited in that it does not include additional important familial and environmental factors that are relevant for understanding young adult substance use (cf. Simons et al., 2009a). Third, the current sample focused on college students and consisted largely of white participants. Results should be generalized to other populations with caution. Finally, this model was focused specifically on marijuana use and related problems, and as such, may be limited in its application to other substance use.
Summary
Overall, the current study identified a number of risk and protective pathways by which individual differences in reinforcement sensitivity are associated with corresponding increases or decreases in marijuana use and problems. Sensitivity to punishment and reward did not interact to predict affect lability or poor control as hypothesized, but displayed positive main effects on both affect lability and poor control. These constructs, in turn, exhibited independent positive effects on the intensity of marijuana problems, over and above the effects of use. The pattern of relationships consistently indicated that SR was associated with increased risk for marijuana use and problems: the effects were mediated, in part, by emotional and behavioral dysregulation, and moderated by SP.
In contrast to the consistent risk promoting effects of SR, associations between SP and marijuana use and problems were complex. The inverse association between SP and the likelihood of using marijuana varied as a function of SR, with the competing effects of SR attenuating the association. Once controlling for use level, SP was indirectly associated with increased intensity of problems due to its effects on emotional and behavioral dysregulation. Though speculative, the pattern of findings suggests that SP may reduce likelihood of marijuana involvement yet, due to its associations with dysregulation contribute to more problematic use patterns once use is initiated. These findings highlight the importance of decomposing outcomes into likelihood versus amount of use and problems as they had differential associations with predictors and underscores the importance of affect lability and poor control in predicting problematic marijuana use.
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Gender | - | ||||||||||||||||
2. SR Parcel 1 | .20δ | - | |||||||||||||||
3. SR Parcel 2 | .17δ | .55δ | - | ||||||||||||||
4. SR Parcel 3 | .19δ | .57δ | .55δ | - | |||||||||||||
5. SP Parcel 1 | −.14δ | .09δ | .09δ | .16δ | - | ||||||||||||
6. SP Parcel 2 | −.17δ | −.01 | .01 | .07δ | .61δ | - | |||||||||||
7. SP Parcel 3 | −.10δ | −.12δ | −.06† | −.02 | .59δ | .65δ | - | ||||||||||
8. ALS Anxiety-Depression | −.10δ | .07δ | .05* | .14δ | .43δ | .38δ | .32δ | - | |||||||||
9. ALS Depression-Elation | −.08δ | .15δ | .12δ | .22δ | .34δ | .31δ | .22δ | .63δ | - | ||||||||
10. ALS Anger | −.01 | .15δ | .18δ | .20δ | .26δ | .22δ | .19δ | .58δ | .52δ | - | |||||||
11. PC Impulsivity | .02 | .30δ | .28δ | .34δ | .19δ | .12δ | .05† | .25δ | .33δ | .32δ | - | ||||||
12. PC Distractibility | .04 | .19δ | .15δ | .27δ | .28δ | .28δ | .19δ | .32δ | .49δ | .30δ | .42δ | - | |||||
13. PC Impatience | −.11δ | .23δ | .19δ | .25δ | .21δ | .15δ | .11δ | .29δ | .31δ | .29δ | .38δ | .47δ | - | ||||
14. T1 Marijuana Use | .15δ | .11δ | .08δ | .12δ | −.04* | −.04 | −.05* | .07† | .08δ | .08δ | .14δ | .11δ | .05* | - | |||
15. T2 Marijuana Use | .12δ | .07† | .06† | .07δ | −.02 | −.01 | −.03 | .07† | .08δ | .08δ | .09δ | .08δ | .01 | .59δ | - | ||
16. T1 Marijuana Problems | .13δ | .16δ | .12δ | .16δ | .02 | .01 | −.02 | .13δ | .16δ | .14δ | .19δ | .17δ | .13δ | .67δ | .45δ | - | |
17. T2 Marijuana Problems | .10δ | .10δ | .07δ | .09δ | −.01 | .01 | −.03 | .11δ | .09δ | .09δ | .11δ | .11δ | .07δ | .45δ | .67δ | .52δ | - |
Note. N = 2,270. ALS = Affect Lability; PC = Poor Control; SR = Sensitivity to Reward; SP = Sensitivity to Punishment.
p < .05
p < .01
p < .001.
Public Health Significance.
The current study identifies important aspects of dysregulated affect and behavior that contribute to initiation and maintenance of marijuana use and that distinguish between problematic from non-problematic use. Identification of these factors is becoming increasingly important given the rapidly changing landscape surrounding marijuana use in the US. Prevention and intervention programs that target regulatory deficits have the potential to minimize negative substance use outcomes as well as having broader impacts on individual well-being.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported was supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) grant R01AA014573 (JSS) and, in part, by NIAAA grant R01AA020519 (JSS).
Footnotes
All authors contributed in a significant way to the manuscript and that all authors have read and approved the final manuscript.
The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Contributor Information
Noah N. Emery, The University of South Dakota, Department of Psychology, 414 East Clark Street, Vermillion, South Dakota, USA 57069, Phone: 605-677-5353, Fax: 605-677-3195, noah.emery@usd.edu
Jeffrey S. Simons, The University of South Dakota, Department of Psychology, 414 East Clark Street, Vermillion, South Dakota, USA 57069, Phone: 605-677-5353, Fax: 605-677-3195, jsimons@usd.edu
References
- Avila C, Parcet MA. Personality and inhibitory deficits in the stop-signal task: The mediating role of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;31:975–986. [Google Scholar]
- Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. Addiction Motivation Reformulated: An Affective Processing Model of Negative Reinforcement. Psychological Review. 2004;111:33–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braddock KH, Dillard JP, Voigt DC, Stephenson MT, Sopory P, Anderson JW. Impulsivity partially mediates the relationship between BIS/BAS and risky health behaviors. Journal of personality. 2011;79:793–810. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00699.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bravo AJ, Anthenien AM, Prince MA, Pearson MR. Marijuana protective behavioral strategies as a moderator of the effects of risk/protective factors on marijuana-related outcomes. Addictive Behaviors. 2017;69:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.01.007. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldeira KM, Arria AM, O’Grady KE, Vincent KB, Wish ED. The occurrence of cannabis use disorders and other cannabis-related problems among first-year college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33:397–411. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron AC, Trivedi PK. Regression Analysis of Count Data. Cambridge University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Carlson SR, Pritchard AA, Dominelli RM. Externalizing behavior, the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior scale and Reward and Punishment Sensitivity. Personality & Individual Differences. 2013;54:202–207. [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS. Impulse and constraint: Perspectives from personality psychology, convergence with theory in other areas, and potential for integration. Personality and social psychology review. 2005;9:312–333. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0904_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caseras X, Àvila C, Torrubia R. The measurement of individual differences in Behavioural Inhibition and Behavioural Activation Systems: a comparison of personality scales. Personality & Individual Differences. 2003;34:999. [Google Scholar]
- Colder CR, Hawk LW, Lengua LJ, Wiezcorek W, Eiden RD, Read JP. Trajectories of Reinforcement Sensitivity During Adolescence and Risk for Substance Use. Journal of Research on Adolescence (Wiley-Blackwell) 2013;23:345–356. doi: 10.1111/jora.12001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collado A, Felton JW, MacPherson L, Lejuez CW. Longitudinal trajectories of sensation seeking, risk taking propensity, and impulsivity across early to middle adolescence. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39:1580–1588. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins SE, Spelman PJ. Associations of descriptive and reflective injunctive norms with risky college drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2013;27:1175–1181. doi: 10.1037/a0032828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corr PJ. Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;28:317–332. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corr PJ. Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST): Introduction. In: Corr J, editor. The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 1–43. [Google Scholar]
- Corr PJ. Approach and avoidance behaviour: Multiple systems and their interactions. Emotion Review. 2013;5:285–290. [Google Scholar]
- Corr PJ, Cooper AJ. The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality Questionnaire (RST-PQ): Development and validation. Psychological assessment. 2016;28:1427. doi: 10.1037/pas0000273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corr PJ, McNaughton N. Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. In: Corr PJ, editor. The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 155–187. [Google Scholar]
- Costa P, McCrae R. Toward a new generation of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. The five factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives. Hrsg.: JS Wiggins. New York. 1996:51–87. [Google Scholar]
- Daruna JH, Barnes PA. A neurodevelopmental view of impulsivity. In: McCown WG, Johnson JL, Shure MB, editors. The impulsive client: Theory, research, and treatment. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 1993. pp. 23–37. [Google Scholar]
- Dawe S, Loxton NJ. The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;28:343–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day AM, Metrik J, Spillane NS, Kahler CW. Working memory and impulsivity predict marijuana-related problems among frequent users. Drug And Alcohol Dependence. 2013;131:171–174. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.12.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeYoung CG, Gray JR. Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual differences in affect, behavior, and cognition. In: Corr PJ, Matthews G, editors. The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2009. pp. 323–346. [Google Scholar]
- Dissabandara LO, Loxton NJ, Dias SR, Dodd PR, Daglish M, Stadlin A. Dependent heroin use and associated risky behaviour: The role of rash impulsiveness and reward sensitivity. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39:71–76. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dvorak RD, Day AM. Marijuana and self-regulation: Examining likelihood and intensity of use and problems. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39:709–712. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erdle S, Rushton JP. The general factor of personality, BIS-BAS, expectancies of reward and punishment, self-esteem, and positive and negative affect. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;48:762–766. [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck SB, Eysenck HJ. The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1977;16:57–68. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1977.tb01003.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck SB, Pearson PR, Easting G, Allsopp JF. Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Personality and Individual Differences. 1985;6:613–619. [Google Scholar]
- Franken IHA, Muris P. BIS/BAS personality characteristics and college students’ substance use. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;40:1497–1503. [Google Scholar]
- Gaher RM, Hahn AM, Shishido H, Simons JS, Gaster S. Associations between sensitivity to punishment, sensitivity to reward, and gambling. Addictive behaviors. 2015;42:180–184. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray J, McNaughton N. The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford University Press; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Gray JA. The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. New York, NY, US: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Gray JA. The neuropsychology of emotion and personality. In: Stahl SM, Iversen SD, Goodman EC, Stahl SM, Iversen SD, Goodman EC, editors. Cognitive neurochemistry. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 1987. pp. 171–190. [Google Scholar]
- Harvey PD, Greenberg BR, Serper MR. The affective lability scales: development, reliability, and validity. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1989;45:786–793. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198909)45:5<786::aid-jclp2270450515>3.0.co;2-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heym N, Ferguson E, Lawrence C. An evaluation of the relationship between Gray’s revised RST and Eysenck’s PEN: Distinguishing BIS and FFFS in Carver and White’s BIS/BAS scales. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45:709–715. [Google Scholar]
- Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
- Jahng S, Solhan MB, Tomko RL, Wood PK, Piasecki TM, Trull TJ. Affect and alcohol use: an ecological momentary assessment study of outpatients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2011;120:572. doi: 10.1037/a0024686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, Miech RA. Monitoring the Future: National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2014. Volume II: college students and adults ages 19–55. National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) 2015 [Google Scholar]
- Kendall PC, Williams CL. Assessing the cognitive and behavioral components of children’s self-management. In: Karoly P, Kanfer FH, editors. Self-management and behavior change. New York: Pergamon Press; 1982. pp. 240–284. [Google Scholar]
- Klein A, Moosbrugger H. Maximum likelihood estimation of latent interaction effects with the LMS method. Psychometrika. 2000;65:457–474. [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3. New York, NY US: Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Knyazev GG. Behavioural activation as predictor of substance use: mediating and moderating role of attitudes and social relationships. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2004;75:309–321. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:217–238. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krupić D, Corr PJ, Ručević S, Križanić V, Gračanin A. Five reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST) of personality questionnaires: Comparison, validity and generalization. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016a;97:19–24. [Google Scholar]
- Krupić D, Gračanin A, Corr PJ. The evolution of the Behavioural Approach System (BAS): Cooperative and competitive resource acquisition strategies. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016b;94:223–227. [Google Scholar]
- Kuvaas NJ, Dvorak RD, Pearson MR, Lamis DA, Sargent EM. Self-regulation and alcohol use involvement: A latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39:146–152. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lejuez C, Magidson JF, Mitchell SH, Sinha R, Stevens MC, De Wit H. Behavioral and biological indicators of impulsivity in the development of alcohol use, problems, and disorders. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2010;34:1334–1345. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01217.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman MD. Social Cognitive Neuroscience: A Review of Core Processes. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:259–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Little TD, Cunningham WA, Shahar G, Widaman KF. To Parcel or Not to Parcel: Exploring the Question, Weighing the Merits. Structural Equation Modeling. 2002;9:151–173. [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J. Confidence Limits for the Indirect Effect: Distribution of the Product and Resampling Methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2004;39:99–128. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markarian SA, Pickett SM, Deveson DF, Kanona BB. A model of BIS/BAS sensitivity, emotion regulation difficulties, and depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms in relation to sleep quality. Psychiatry Research. 2013;210:281–286. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maslowsky J, Jager J, Hemken D. Estimating and interpreting latent variable interactions: A tutorial for applying the latent moderated structural equations method. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2015;39:87–96. doi: 10.1177/0165025414552301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy DE, Curtin JJ, Piper ME, Baker TB. Negative reinforcement: Possible clinical implications of an integrative model. In: Kassel JD, editor. Substance abuse and emotion. Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 15–42. [Google Scholar]
- Metcalfe J, Mischel W. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review. 1999;106:3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén BO, Muthén LK. Mplus User’s Guide. Seventh. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor RM, Colder CR. Predicting alcohol patterns in first-year college students through motivational systems and reasons for drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19:10. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor RM, Colder CR, Hawk LW., Jr Confirmatory factor analysis of the Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences. 2004;37:985–1002. [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor R, Stewart S, Marlatt G. BIS risk for negative reinforcement alcohol expectancies: Role of anxious mood and impulsivity; Paper presented at the New York 2009a Symposium presentation at the 43rd annual convention of the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies; NY. 2009. Nov, [Google Scholar]
- Oliver MNI, Simons JS. The affective lability scales: Development of a short-form measure. Personality & Individual Differences. 2004;37:1279–1288. [Google Scholar]
- Pardo Y, Aguilar R, Molinuevo B, Torrubia R. Alcohol use as a behavioural sign of disinhibition: Evidence from JA Gray’s model of personality. Addictive behaviors. 2007;32:2398–2403. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson MR, Liese BS, Dvorak RD, Team MOS. College student marijuana involvement: perceptions, use, and consequences across 11 college campuses. Addictive Behaviors. 2017;66:83–89. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen ER, Hummer JF, Rinker DV, Traylor ZK, Neighbors C. Measuring Protective Behavioral Strategies for Marijuana Use Among Young Adults. Journal Of Studies On Alcohol And Drugs. 2016;77:441–450. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2016.77.441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plutchik R, Van Praag HM. The nature of impulsivity: Definitions, ontology, genetics, and relations to aggression 1995 [Google Scholar]
- Settles RE, Fischer S, Cyders MA, Combs JL, Gunn RL, Smith GT. Negative urgency: A personality predictor of externalizing behavior characterized by neuroticism, low conscientiousness, and disagreeableness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:160–172. doi: 10.1037/a0024948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Arens AM. Moderating effects of sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward on associations between marijuana effect expectancies and use. Psychol Addict Behav. 2007;21:409–414. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.3.409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Carey KB. Risk and vulnerability for marijuana use problems: The role of affect dysregulation. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2002;16:72–75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Carey KB. An affective and cognitive model of marijuana and alcohol problems. Addictive Behaviors. 2006;31:1578–1592. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Carey KB, Wills TA. Alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms: A multidimensional model of common and specific etiology. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2009a;23:415–427. doi: 10.1037/a0016003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Dvorak RD, Batien BD. Methamphetamine use in a rural college population: associations with marijuana use, sensitivity to punishment, and sensitivity to reward. Psychol Addict Behav. 2008;22:444–449. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.3.444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Dvorak RD, Lau-Barraco C. Behavioral inhibition and activation systems: Differences in substance use expectancy organization and activation in memory. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2009b;23:315–328. doi: 10.1037/a0015834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Gaher RM, Correia CJ, Hansen CL, Christopher MS. An affective-motivational model of marijuana and alcohol problems among college students. Psycholology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005a;19:326–334. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.3.326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Gaher RM, Oliver MNI, Bush JA, Palmer MA. An Experience Sampling Study of Associations between Affect and Alcohol Use and Problems among College Students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2005b:459–469. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Neal DJ, Gaher RM. Risk for marijuana-related problems among college students: an application of zero-inflated negative binomial regression. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2006;32:41–53. doi: 10.1080/00952990500328539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Oliver MN, Gaher RM, Ebel G, Brummels P. Methamphetamine and alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms: associations with affect lability and impulsivity in a rural treatment population. Addictive behaviors. 2005c;30:1370–1381. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Wills TA, Neal DJ. The many faces of affect: A multilevel model of drinking frequency/quantity and alcohol dependence symptoms among young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2014;123:676–694. doi: 10.1037/a0036926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smillie LD, Pickering AD, Jackson CJ. The new reinforcement sensitivity theory: Implications for personality measurement. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2006;10:320–335. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, Cornelius JR, Pajer K, Vanyukov M, Clark D. Neurobehavioral Disinhibition in Childhood Predicts Early Age at Onset of Substance Use Disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1078. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torrubia R, Avila C, Moltó J, Caseras X. The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;31:837–862. [Google Scholar]
- Tull MT, Gratz KL, Latzman RD, Kimbrel NA, Lejuez CW. Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory and emotion regulation difficulties: A multimodal investigation. Personality & Individual Differences. 2010;49:989–994. [Google Scholar]
- Valero S, Daigre C, Rodríguez-Cintas L, Barral C, Gomà-i-Freixanet M, Ferrer M, Roncero C. Neuroticism and impulsivity: Their hierarchical organization in the personality characterization of drug-dependent patients from a decision tree learning perspective. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2014;55:1227–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdejo-García A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voigt DC, Dillard JP, Braddock KH, Anderson JW, Sopory P, Stephenson MT. BIS/BAS scales and their relationship to risky health behaviours. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47:89–93. [Google Scholar]
- Wardell JD, O’Connor RM, Read JP, Colder CR. Behavioral approach system moderates the prospective association between the behavioral inhibition system and alcohol outcomes in college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2011;72:1028–1036. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.1028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:669–689. [Google Scholar]
- Williams C, Adams S, Stephens R, Roffman R. Varied methods of assessing marijuana use and related problems: Validity analyses; Paper presented at the 34th Annual Convention of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy; New Orleans. 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Bantum EOC, Pokhrel P, Maddock JE, Ainette MG, Morehouse E, Fenster B. A dual-process model of early substance use: Tests in two diverse populations of adolescents. Health Psychology. 2013;32:533–542. doi: 10.1037/a0027634. supp (Supplemental) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Cleary S, Filer M, Shinar O, Mariani J, Spera K. Temperament related to early-onset substance use: test of a developmental model. Prevention Science: The Official Journal of the Society For Prevention Research. 2001;2:145–163. doi: 10.1023/a:1011558807062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Pokhrel P, Morehouse E, Fenster B. Behavioral and emotional regulation and adolescent substance use problems: A test of moderation effects in a dual-process model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011;25:279–292. doi: 10.1037/a0022870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Sandy JM, Yaeger AM. Moderators of the relation between substance use level and problems: Test of a self-regulation model in middle adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:3–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Simons JS, Manayan O, Robinson MK. Emotion regulation and substance use disorders in adolescents. In: Essau CA, Leblanc S, Ollendick TA, editors. Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology in Children and Adolescents. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Walker C, Mendoza D, Ainette MG. Behavioral and emotional self-control: relations to substance use in samples of middle and high school students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2006;20:265. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.3.265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wray TB, Simons JS, Dvorak RD. Alcohol-related infractions among college students: Associations with subsequent drinking as a function of sensitivity to punishment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011;25:352–357. doi: 10.1037/a0023614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]