Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017 Sep;2(6):464–475. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.06.002

An altered scaffold for information processing: Cognitive control development in adolescents with autism

Marjorie Solomon a,b,c, Jeremy Hogeveen a,b, Lauren Libero a,b, Christine Nordahl a,b
PMCID: PMC5600519  NIHMSID: NIHMS886527  PMID: 28924621

Abstract

We investigated how cognitive neuroscientific studies during the last decade have advanced understanding of cognitive control from adolescence to young adulthood in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). To do so, we conducted a selective review of the larger structural, resting state, and diffusion imaging studies of brain regions and networks related to cognitive control that have been conducted since 2007 in individuals with ASD and typical development (TYP) ages 10 to 30 years that examined how these regions and networks support behavioral and task-based fMRI performance on tasks assessing cognitive control during this period. Longitudinal structural studies reveal overgrowth of the anterior cingulate (ACC) and slower white matter development in the parietal cortex in adolescents with ASD versus TYP. Cross-sectional studies of the salience, executive control and default mode resting state functional connectivity networks, which mediate cognitive control, demonstrate patterns of connectivity that differ from TYP through adolescence. Finally, white matter tracts underlying these control-related brain regions continue to show reduced diffusion properties compared to TYP. It is thus not surprising that cognitive control tasks performance improves less during adolescence in ASD versus TYP. This review illustrates that a cognitive neuroscientific approach produces insights about the mechanisms of persistent cognitive control deficits in individuals with ASD from adolescence into young adulthood not apparent with neuropsychological methods alone, and draws attention to the great need for longitudinal studies of this period in those with ASD. Further investigation of ACC and fronto-parietal neural circuits may help specify pathophysiology and treatment options.

Keywords: executive functions, cognitive control, autism, neuroimaging, adolescent development, young adulthood


Cognitive control – the maintenance of situational context and inhibition of prepotent responding to permit the goal-directed behavior [1] – is a key component in the NIMH Research Domain Criteria project (RDoC; [2]). As specified by RDoC, cognitive control consists of three broad components: working memory or cue/context maintenance; inhibition of prepotent response tendencies; and set shifting, task switching or cognitive flexibility.

In perhaps the most influential theory of the neural mechanisms governing cognitive control, Miller & Cohen [3] proposed that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is specialized for the representation and maintenance of situational context that provides top-down biasing to facilitate information flow from relevant neural systems. This model has provided a foundation for the development of testable mechanistic hypotheses using cognitive neuroscientific versus clinical neuropsychological measurements [4]. Hypotheses then can be verified using functional magnetic resonance imaging studies (fMRI). These studies have localized control processes to the dorsolateral prefrontal (DLPFC), ventrolateral prefrontal (VLPFC), anterior PFC (aPFC), anterior cingulate (ACC), and parietal cortices [5, 6], and have generated insights about how control is evoked in response to ACC-generated conflict signals [7] and timing differences in neural circuits recruitment [8]; and basal ganglia/PFC interactions that guide reward-driven learning [9].

In typical development (TYP), adolescence is considered a critical period for the development of mature thought and action [10]. This has not been well-examined in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). To help fill this gap in understanding, this manuscript selectively reviews structural, resting state, diffusion, and task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of brain regions and networks sub-serving cognitive control in individuals with ASD. Peer-reviewed and published scientific papers written in English from 2007–2017 were identified through a computerized literature search using Google Scholar and PubMed. Search terms used across all studies included autism, cognitive control, adolescence, young adulthood, and MRI. Other terms including brain structure, default mode network, salience network, executive control network, diffusion, DTI, cue/context maintenance, response inhibition, set shifting, and task switching also were used in their respective sections. To be included, studies also had to have a mean of >10 participants per group (e.g. a two-group study where one group had 9 participants and the other had 11 participants was included). Given the relative lack of longitudinal studies of ASD during the adolescence to young adulthood period, many comparisons are of cross-sectional studies, although greater weight is given to existing longitudinal studies when drawing inferences about findings.

Components of the Scaffold

Structural neuroimaging of cognitive control related brain regions

Cortical gray matter volume, increases during childhood, peaks around puberty and then begins to decline across adolescence and adulthood [9]. Frontal and parietal regions reach peak volume around 12 years of age in males, and about a year earlier in females [11]. Increases and decreases in volume pre and post-adolescents are steeper for parietal lobe than for frontal lobe. In TYP, decreases in volume within the cognitive control network during adolescence and adulthood are associated with increased performance on tasks of executive functioning and emotion identification [12].

Several cross-sectional structural MRI studies have evaluated global cortical gray and white matter differences between adolescents with ASD compared to age matched TYPs (see [13]). Since 2007, there has been one study that utilized whole brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM; a technique involving comparing differences in brain anatomy using volumetric MRI scans across groups) that identified volume increases in both the DLPFC and superior and interior parietal lobule, as well as decreased white matter volume across all cerebral lobes in those with ASD versus TYP [14]. Differences in the folding of the cerebral cortex (gyrification), also have been reported in the frontal lobe and intraparietal sulcus in individuals with ASD versus TYP [15].

More recently, evidence from two longitudinal studies suggest there are between group differences in the structure of cognitive control-related brain regions. Hua et al. [16] evaluated youth with ASD from late childhood into adolescence. They report abnormal overgrowth versus the pruning typically found during adolescence, in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and slower white matter development in the parietal lobe in those with ASD versus TYP. Wallace et al. [17] evaluated cortical thickness (a brain morphometric measure used to assess the combined thickness of the layers of the cerebral cortex) and surface area in slightly older adolescents with ASD beginning at 17 and then again at 19 years. They observed accelerated cortical thinning in superior parietal cortex in ASD relative to TYP. Table 1 presents the two cross-sectional structural, VBM and cortical folding studies and two longitudinal studies of white and gray matter and cortical thickness development since 2017. While older studies are ambiguous, two recent longitudinal studies suggest that the rate of development in key cognitive control regions is implicated in ASD. However, no studies include both males and females with ASD. See Figure 1A for a depiction of brain regions implicated in structural studies.

Table 1.

Summary of Structural Neuroimaging Studies in ASD.

STUDY N Mean Age (SD) Method Finding(s)
Bonilha et al. 2008 [14] ASD: 12
TYP: 16
ASD: 12.4 ± 4
TYP: 13.2 ± 5
Whole-brain VBM ASD > TYP: DLPFC and parietal gray matter
ASD < TYP: frontal and parietal white matter
Nordahl et al 2007 [15] ASDa : 15
TYP: 29
ASD: 12.3 (3.2)
TYP: 11.8 (2.6)
Cortical folding ASD > TYP: intraparietal sulcal depth
Hua et al 2013 [16] ASD: 13
TYP: 7
ASD: 12.0 ± 2.3
TYP: 12.3 ± 2.4
Two MRI time points;
2.9 ± 0.9 year interval
Longitudinal brain growth ASD < TYP: white matter growth in parietal lobe
ASD > TYP: gray matter expansion in ACC
Wallace et al 2015 [17] ASD: 17
TYP: 18
ASD: 17.4 (2.4)
TYP: 17.5 (1.5)
Two MRI time points; 1.7 (0.8) year interval
Longitudinal cortical thickness and surface area ASD > TYP: Cortical thinning in superior parietal cortex
a

These individuals were diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome per DSM-IV criteria

Abbreviations used: ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TYP: typical development; ACC: anterior cingulate cortex; GI: Gyrification Index.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Glass brain schematic of the neural circuits underlying cognitive control difficulties in ASD. (A) Outline of the brain structures that have demonstrated developmental anomalies in individuals with autism spectrum disorders, including the anterior cingulate cortex [yellow; [19]] and superior parietal cortex [yellow/white; [20]]. (B) Depiction of the intrinsic functional brain networks that have demonstrated aberrant functional connectivity profiles in ASD [default mode network, dark green; salience network, green; executive control network, bright green; [23]]. (C) White matter tracts involved in cognitive control that have demonstrated alterations in ASD [corpus callosum, dark red; cingulum bundle; red; superior longitudinal fasciculus, bright red; [142]].

Resting-state functional neuroimaging studies of cognitive control in ASD

Resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) examines the correlation in BOLD signal between brain regions during periods of quiet rest. This is referred to as functional connectivity (FC) [18]. A reliable set of rsfMRI networks [19] emerges during adolescence, when there is a strengthening of FC within networks (integration) and decreased FC between networks (segregation). This culminates in the establishment of a mature intrinsic functional brain architecture in young adulthood [2022]. Atypical connectivity between three specific intrinsic functional networks – the default mode (DMN), salience (SN), and executive control networks (ECN) [23]–is central to disorders involving cognitive control impairments, and are the focus of this selective review.

Most rsfMRI studies in ASD have examined the default mode network, which includes the posterior cingulate cortex/retrosplenial cortex and medial prefrontal cortex, and which demonstrates robust FC at rest and during self-referential processing [Figure 1B; [24]]. Children with ASD demonstrate aberrant FC within the DMN, and greater FC between the DMN and other intrinsic functional networks [2527] than those with TYP. During adolescence DMN connectivity does not increase to the same extent in ASD relative to TYP [28, 29], and remains decreased in young adults with ASD [3033]. Collectively, these data suggest that DMN integration during adolescence may be reduced in individuals with ASD.

The second network implicated in cognitive control impairments is the salience network (SN). The SN is centered around the anterior insula (AI) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Figure 1B), and plays a role in detecting stimuli of high relevance [34, 35]. FC within the SN is increased in adults relative to children, suggesting this circuit develops during adolescence [36]. Whereas the SN demonstrates overconnectivity in children with ASD [37, 38], the SN is under-connected in adolescents with ASD relative to those with TYP [3840], suggesting that it demonstrates a flatter developmental trajectory in ASD. This may lead to enduring differences in salience processing within this network in adulthood in those with ASD [41].

One hypothesized role of the SN is to prioritize stimuli for processing by the ECN—the third network implicated in cognitive control. The ECN is anchored in the DLPFC and parietal cortex [23, 34]. Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of TYP have suggested that ECN FC increases during adolescence and young adulthood, whereas FC between the ECN and other networks decreases [20, 42, 43]. Recently, Elton et al. [26] analyzed ECN connectivity in 90 ASD and 95 TYP youth ages 6.5–18.7 years. Youth with ASD showed underconnectivity within prefrontal sectors of the ECN relative to TYP. In this study there also was a positive association between connectivity of the ECN and the DMN and scores on a dimensional ASD symptom measure [26, 44]. Abbott and colleagues [45] found similar FC anomalies within the ECN and increased connectivity between the ECN and DMN in youth ages 8–17 with ASD relative to TYP. As the DMN and ECN underlie “task-negative” and “task-positive” information processing, respectively, these findings suggest that the typical pattern of segregation between these networks across adolescence may not be present in ASD. See Table 2 for studies of the three networks in ASD since 2007. Notably, there have been no large-scale longitudinal rsfMRI studies in ASD, and hypotheses derived herein are based on cross-sectional comparisons.

Table 2.

Summary of resting state fMRI (rsfMRI) of intrinsic functional brain networks in ASD.

STUDY N Mean Age (SD) Motion Correction Finding(s)
Default-mode network (DMN)
Kennedy & Courchesne, 2008 [32] ASD: 12
TYP: 12
ASD: 26.5 (12.8)
TYP: 27.5 (10.9)
Scrubbing Underconnectivity
Monk et al., 2009 [33] ASD: 12
TYP: 12
ASD: 26 (5.93)
TYP: 27 (6.1)
Standard Mixed (PCC-frontal under-, PCC-temporal overconnectivity)
Weng et al., 2009 [30] ASD: 16
TYP: 15
ASD: 13–17
TYP: 13–18
Standard Underconnectivity; Note: no mean/SD age provided
Assaf et al., 2010 [31] ASD: 15
TYP: 15
ASD: 15.7 (3.0)
TYP: 17.1 (3.6)
ICA Underconnectivity
Wiggins et al., 2011 [29] ASD: 39
TYP: 41
ASD: 14.0 (2.1)
TYP: 15.3 (2.4)
Standard TYP have greater increases in DMN connectivity with age
Lynch et al., 2013 [27] ASD: 20
TYP: 19
ASD: 10.0 (1.6)
TYP: 9.9 (1.6)
Scrubbing Overconnectivity
Uddin et al., 2014 [36] ASD: 17
TYP: 17
ASD: 9.9 (0.4)
TYP: 9.8 (0.4)
Standard Reduced segregation between rest and task in ASD
Washington et al., 2014 [28] ASD: 24
TYP: 24
ASD: 10.9 (2.3)
TYP: 10.1 (3.2)
Scrubbing TYP have greater increases in DMN connectivity with age
Elton et al., 2016 [26] ASD: 90
TYP: 95
ASD: 13.1 (3.3)
TYP: 13.2 (3.1)
Standard Overconnectivity
Salience network (SN)
Ebisch et al., 2010 [40] ASD: 14
TYP: 15
ASD: 15.8 (1.9)
TYP: 16.0 (1.6)
Standard Mixed (AI under- and overconn. to different seeds)
Uddin et al., 2013 [37] ASD: 20
TYP: 20
ASD: 10.0 (1.6)
TYP: 10.0 (1.6)
Scrubbing Overconnectivity predictive of restricted and repetitive behavior
Eilam-Stock et al., 2014 [41] ASD: 14
TYP: 13
ASD: 26.1 (6.5)
TYP: 27.1 (8.2)
Standard Low correlation between arousal and SN activity in ASD
Elton et al., 2016 [26] ASD: 90
TYP: 95
ASD: 13.1 (3.3)
TYP: 13.2 (3.1)
Standard Mixed (ACC overconnectivity, frontal underconnectivity)
Executive control network (ECN)
Abbott et al., 2015 [45] ASD: 37
TYP: 38
ASD: 13.9 (2.6)
TYP: 13.0 (2.6)
Scrubbing Mixed (Right ECN over- and left ECN underconnectivity)
Elton et al., 2016 [26] ASD: 90
TYP: 95
ASD: 13.1 (3.3)
TYP: 13.2 (3.1)
Standard Underconnectivity

Given its potential to introduce systematic between-group variance in functional connectivity (FC) measures, subject motion is one of the most important methodological considerations in rsfMRI studies in ASD (Power et al., 2012; Van Dijk et al., 2012). “Standard” motion correction refers to some combination of removing subjects with excessive motion and standard image realignment, “scrubbing” refers to the use of temporal masks to censor the influence of high-motion data points within a rsfMRI session, and independent component analysis (ICA) refers to the decomposition of the rsfMRI signal to identify and control for spatial components that represent motion-related artifact. List of abbreviations: Anterior insula (AI), autism spectrum disorders (ASD), default-mode network (DMN), executive control network (ECN), functional connectivity (FC), independent component analysis (ICA), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), salience network (SN), standard deviation (SD), and typical development (TYP).

Diffusion MRI studies of cognitive control in ASD

Three white matter tracts – the corpus callosum, cingulum bundle, and superior longitudinal fasciculus – underlie brain regions involved in cognitive control. See Figure 1(C). These tracts play a role in processing speed and complex cognition [46], attention and working memory [47], motor behavior, spatial attention, language, and response inhibition [48, 49], respectively. Atypical development of these three tracts in early childhood leads to continued alterations in diffusion properties in adolescents and adults with ASD [5053].

The corpus callosum is most implicated in ASD. Structural MRI studies have consistently reported reduced size of the corpus callosum, in ASD relative to TYP. The front end or genu of the corpus callosum, and its most anterior portion (the forceps minor), radiates across the lateral and medial sides of the PFC, connecting the PFC and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [54].

Alterations in white matter diffusion specific to the genu and forceps minor have been reported cross-sectionally in adolescents and young adults with ASD compared to TYP [5560], as well as in the white matter tracts connecting to the OFC specifically [61]. In addition, one longitudinal study found that the developmental trajectory of diffusion properties of the genu was altered during childhood in individuals with ASD compared to TYP, leading to persisting atypicalities in the white matter into adolescence and adulthood [50]. The few studies that relate alterations in diffusion with behavioral measurements have found that fractional anisotropy (FA) – a measurement of the directional flow of cerebrospinal fluid thought to reflect fiber density, axonal diameter, and myelination – of the corpus callosum was correlated with performance IQ with a medium effect size and that FA of the genu was associated with processing speed with a large effect size [55].

The second white matter tract implicated in cognitive control – the cingulum bundle –stretches from anterior temporal gyrus to orbitofrontal cortex and runs within the cingulate gyrus and over the top of the corpus callosum [54]. Cross-sectional studies have identified consistent alterations in the diffusion properties of the cingulum bundle in both adolescents and young adults with ASD [56, 57, 59, 6165]. In addition, one study reported a significant negative relationship between FA of the cingulum bundle and scores on a parent-report measure of executive functions, such that lower FA in cingulum predicted greater executive function deficits in participants with ASD with a large effect size [65]. The final tract involved in cognitive control – the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) – extends from the inferior fontal gyrus (IFG) to the superior temporal gyrus (STG) and temporoparietal junction (TPJ), terminating close to Broca’s Area, Wernicke’s Area, precentral gyrus and the supramarginal gyrus [48, 6668]. For this tract too, a number of previous cross-sectional studies have found reductions in diffusion properties in those with ASD during both adolescence and young adulthood [56, 59, 62, 64, 6971]. See Figure 1C, and Table 3 for a review of twenty cross-sectional studies and one accelerated longitudinal study comparing the diffusion properties of these tracts between adolescents and young adults with ASD and TYP. Although we must rely on findings from diffusion MRI studies in ASD that have been almost entirely cross-sectional by design, it appears that alterations of diffusion properties within the three tracts associated with cognitive control are present during adolescence and persist into early adulthood.

Table 3.

Summary of relevant diffusion-weighted imaging studies in ASD.

STUDY N Mean Age Method Tract Findings
Alexander et al., 2007 [55] ASD: 43
TYP: 34
ASD: 16.23 (6.70)
TYP: 16.44 (5.97)
ROI Corpus Callosum Reduced FA and AD, increased MD and RD
FA correlated with processing speed
Jou et al., 2011 [56] ASD: 15
TYP: 8
ASD: 10.9 (3.7)
TYP: 11.5 (2.6)
TBSS Corpus Callosum Reduced FA
Cingulum Reduced FA
SLF Reduced FA
Brito et al., 2009 [57] ASD: 8
TYP: 8
ASD: 9.53 (1.83)
TYP: 9.57 (1.36)
ROI Corpus Callosum Reduced FA
Shukla et al., 2010 [58] ASD: 26
TYP: 24
ASD: 12.7 (0.6)
TYP: 13.0 (0.6)
VBA/ROI Corpus Callosum Reduced FA, increased RD
Shukla et al., 2011 [59] ASD: 26
TYP: 24
ASD: 12.8 (0.6)
TYP: 13.0 (0.6)
TBSS Corpus Callosum Reduced FA, increased RD
Cingulum Reduced FA, increased MD and RD
SLF Reduced FA, increased MD and RD
Libero et al., 2015 [60] ASD :19
TYP: 18
ASD: 27.1 (1.38)
TYP: 24.6 (1.22)
Tractography/ROI Corpus Callosum Reduced FA, increased RD
Ameis et al., 2011 [69] ASD: 19
TYP: 16
ASD: 12.4 (3.1)
TYP: 12.3 (3.6)
TBSS/ROI Corpus Callosum Increased MD and RD
Jou et al. 2011 [62] ASD: 10
TYP: 10
ASD: 13.06 (3.85)
TYP: 13.94 (4.23)
Tractography/VBA Corpus Callosum Reduced FA
Cingulum Reduced FA
SLF Reduced FA
Pardini et al., 2009 [61] ASD: 10
TYP: 19
ASD: 19.7 (2.83)
TYP: 19.9 (2.64)
Tractography/VBA Cingulum Reduced FA
Orbitofrontal cortex network Reduced FA
Travers et al., 2015 [50] ASD: 100
TYP: 56
ASD: 18.3 (8.5)
TYP: 18.9 (7.8)
ROI Corpus Callosum Reduced FA, age effects (under 10 yrs.)
Pugliese et al., 2009 [63] ASD: 24
TYP: 42
ASD: 23.3 (12.4)
TYP: 25.3 (10.3)
Tractography/ROI Cingulum Increased MD, greater number of streamlines
Lee et al., 2009 [90] ASD: 43
TYP: 34
ASD: 16.23 (6.70)
TYP: 16.44 (5.97)
VBA/T-SPOON Corpus Callosum Reduced FA, increased MD
Noriuchi et al., 2010 [64] ASD: 7
TYP: 7
ASD: 13.96 (2.68)
TYP: 13.36 (2.74)
VBA Cingulum Reduced FA and AD
SLF Reduced FA and AD
Thakkar et al., 2008 [91] ASD :12
TYP: 14
ASD: 30 (11)
TYP: 27 (8)
VBA Cingulum Reduced FA
Ikuta et al., 2014 [65] ASD: 21
TYP: 21
ASD: 18.1 (2.7)
TYP: 18.2 (2.9)
Tractography/ROI Cingulum Reduced FA
Ameis et al., 2013 [51] ASD: 19
TYP: 16
ASD: 12.4 (3.1)
TYP: 12.3 (3.6)
ROI Cingulum Reduced FA, increased MD, RD, and AD
Poustka et al., 2012 [70] ASD: 18
TYP: 18
ASD: 9.7 (2.1)
TYP: 9.7 (1.9)
ROI Corpus Callosum Reduced FA
SLF Reduced FA
Libero et al., 2016 [71] ASD: 42
TYP: 44
ASD: 19.9 (1.27)
TYP: 20.1 (1.21)
Tractography/ROI SLF Reduced FA, increased RD

Abbreviations used: ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TYP: typical development; ROI: region (or tract) of interest; TBSS: tract-based spatial statistics; VBA: voxel-based analyses; SLF: superior longitudinal fasciculus; FA: fractional anisotropy; RD: radial diffusivity; MD: mean diffusivity; AD: axial diffusivity.

Behavioral and neuroimaging studies of cognitive control in ASD

Neuropsychological studies illustrate that executive control deficits are among the most common impairments found in individiuals with ASD [72]. Neuropsychological and cognitive neuroscientific studies find impairments in the three RDoC domains of cognitive/executive control including: working memory or cue/context maintenance [73, 74], inhibition of prepotent response tendencies or response inhibition [7577] and set shifting or task switching or cognitive flexibility [78]. However, shifting deficits have been questioned [79], and may only be present if participants must choose to switch tasks [8082]. Despite the typical maturation of cognitive control from adolescence to young adulthood, studies of those with ASD generally show persisting lag through this period [83] or isolated development of a subset of component processes [75, 84], or limited improvement within the context of persistent delay [85].

Since the advent of fMRI during the decade of the brain (1990–1999), there have been a growing number of task-based fMRI studies of cognitive control in ASD from adolescence to young adulthood. Studies since 2007 are reviewed below.

Cue/context maintenance

Solomon et al. [86] used event-related fMRI to assess holding a cue in mind when preparing to overcome a prepotent response tendency in 12–18 year olds with ASD (n=22) and TYP (n=23). TYP versus ASD recruited significantly more aPFC and parietal (BA 7 and BA 40) regions for correct trials requiring overcoming a prepotent response tendency. ASD also exhibited reduced FC and network integration compared to TYP that was associated, with symptoms of attention deficits (medium effect size). An additional cross-sectional followup study in an overlapping larger sample investigated the development of control through early (ages 12–15) and late (ages 16–18) adolescents [87]. Older ASD and TYP showed reduced activation in sensory and premotor areas relative to younger ones. However, older individuals with ASD showed reduced left parietal recruitment relative to TYP. FC analyses showed that the older ASD group exhibited increased functional connectivity strength between the VLPFC and the ACC, bilaterally. This was interpreted as a signature of cognitive control that was less planful/proactive and more last-minute and reactive. This FC strength was associated with task performance in ASD with a medium effect size, whereas DLPFC and parietal cortex FC was related to task performance in TYP with a large effect size. Vogan et al. [88] examined performance on a simple color matching one-back task. TYP activated regions of the PFC, while ASD activated posterior regions of the brain. TYP recruited more of the PFC and parietal cortex as load increased, while ASD did not. In sum, based on the few studies of context maintenance to date, adolescents with ASD appear to recruit the PFC and parietal cortex less than those with TYP. One study suggested that TYP appear to become more planful (proactive), while those with ASD remain more reactive, and show increasing PFC/ACC FC.

Cognitive control of response inhibition

Response inhibition in ASD has been studied using both go/no-go and saccadic eye movement paradigms which require the participant to look towards a target. Kana, Keller, Minshew, & Just [89] manipulated working memory load in a go/no-go paradigm in young adults with ASD and TYP, and examined recruitment in ROIs in the ACC, PFC, and parietal cortex. The ASD group showed poorer memory performance in greater load conditions than TYP with less recruitment of the PFC, ACC, and insula. In the most difficult condition, the ASD group showed reduced recruitment of the ACC and precuneus, but greater recruitment of premotor regions. A factor analysis of FC showed poorer integration of the inhibitory and control networks in ASD. However, writing about FC in a younger group of adolescents, Lee et al. [90] conducted a left and right IFG seed-based FC study of a similar go/no-go task and found no group FC differences.

Thakkar et al. [91] used rapid even-related fMRI and diffusion imaging of an anti-saccade task in which participants were required to make a saccadic eye movement away from a target, rather than towards it (an anti-saccade), to investigate response inhibition deficits, ACC dysfunction, and restricted interests and repetitive behaviors (RBs) in young adults with ASD versus TYP. The ASD group made more anti-saccade errors; responded more quickly on correct trials; showed reduced discrimination in rostral ACC between error and correct trials and reduced FA in white matter underlying the ACC. Recruitment on correct trials and reduced FA were associated with RBs with a large effect size. Agam et al. [92] then examined ACC regions recruited during anti-saccades versus pro-saccades in the same young adult participants, using an ROI based approach. TYP showed greater bilateral frontal eye field (FEF) and dorsal ACC (dACC) recruitment than ASD. Greater activation in the dACC predicted fewer errors across both groups. In those with ASD, greater recruitment in the dACC during inhibition predicted faster anti-saccades. Activation in both the left and right dACC predicted RBs. Finally, Padmanabhan et al. [93] used fMRI to extend Luna et al.’s [85] behavioral study of cognitive control using an anti-saccade task. The groups did not differ in pro-saccade performance. However, the ASD group showed less BA 7 recruitment during task preparation, but greater activation of this region during anti-saccade performance. Taken together, go/no-go and eye movement studies again suggest that integrated recruitment of the ACC and parietal cortices is critical to cognitive control of response inhibition, although there may be no group differences in FC of these regions with the PFC in adolescents. The presence of RBs may be associated with the functioning of the ACC, but the relation remains complex and difficult to interpret.

Cognitive control of set shifting/task switching

Shaffritz, Dichter, Baranek, & Belger [94] used an event-related target detection task to examine the association between set shifting and RBs. There were no group differences in the ability to shift versus maintain responding to the target. TYP exhibited greater recruitment of the DLPFC, IPS, and basal ganglia during target, and target shift versus target maintain trials. RBs were negatively associated with ACC activation to targets with a large effect size. Using a different task, Yerys et al. [95] employed event-related fMRI to examine a model of set shifting involving simple versus more complex rules in children and adolescents with ASD and TYP. The ASD group performed more poorly and was slower than TYP. There was no group by trial type interaction for the lowest level dimension shifts. ASD versus TYP recruited more ACC, superior frontal gyrus, frontal pole and right IFG, in the switch versus stay condition. Authors interpreted this as a sign that ASD needed stronger recruitment of task-relevant brain regions to complete the task when task performance was the same for both groups. In conclusion set shifting appears to rely on similar brain regions as other components of cognitive control, and that group differences may emerge with age. Inconsistent study findings may also derive from different levels of task difficulty. As shown in Table 4, during the past decade there have been only 10 task-based fMRI studies (3 of cue/context maintenance, 5 of response inhibition, and 2 of switching) meeting our criteria. Studies were small, and utilized diverse paradigms, study designs, and thresholding. Furthermore, none were longitudinal or examined sex differences.

Table 4.

Summary of Task-Based fMRI Studies in ASD.

Study N, Mean Age & IQ Paradigm Design Features Thresholding & Methods Major Findings
Kana et al., 2007 [89] n=12 (TYP)
n=12 (ASD)
Age TYP=23
Age ASD=27
IQ ASD=110
IQ TYP=117
Response inhibition under 3 load types. Blocked design fMRI
Time series-based
FC of all ROIs from within group maps. Factor analysis of connectivity of these ROIs.
Uncorrected height threshold of p=.005 cluster size=10 ASD with reduced activation in inhibition and executive control regions. In harder condition, this is greater premotor recruitment in ASD. Factor analysis shows less integration of networks in ASD.
Thakkar et al., 2008 [91] n=14 (TYP)fMRI
n=12 (TYP) DTI
n=10 (ASD)fMRI
n=10 (ASD)DTI
Age TYP=27
Age ASD=30
VIQ ASD=116
VIQ TYP=114
Saccadic paradigm with eye tracking Event-related fMRI DTI.
Correlations of RBs with ACC fMRI activation and FA.
Multiple comparison correction w/Monte Carlo similation for p<.05 probability that cluster found in masked ACC ROIs. ASD < ACC recruitment for error and correct trials, driven by ASD group’s greater ACC response on correct trials. Less differentiated responding in medial superior frontal gyrus.
DTI shows reduced FA in white matter underlying ACC, fronto-polar cortex, parietal cortex, precentral gyrus. RBs associated with correct trials and reduced FA.
Agam, Joseph, Barton, & Manoach, 2010 [92] n=14 (TYP) fMRI
n=11 (ASD)fMRI
Age ASD=30
ASD=117 VIQ
TYP=114 VIQ
Saccadic paradigm with eye tracking Event-related fMRI
ROIs in FEF dACC
Multiple comparison correction w/Monte Carlo similation for p<.05 probability that cluster found in masked ACC ROIs. ASD with reduced inhibition related recruitment of FEF dACC, driven by reduced dfferentiation on anti-saccades in TYP. In ASD&TYP, rACC predicts lower anti-saccade errors. In ASD this region predicts lower RT. Greater FEF activation predicts RBs in ASD. TYP with greater FC. In ASD, lACC activation correlates with RBs.
Shafritz, Dichter, Baranek, & Belger (2008) [94] n=14 (TYP)
n=15 (ASD)
Age TYP=24
Age ASD=22
IQ ASD=103
IQ TYP=111
Target detection paradigm involving rule shifts. Event-related fMRI
ROIs correlated with RBs.
Displayed at p<.001, Multiple comparison correct w/Forman method and cluster size of 10. On targets, TYPs recruit BA9, 46, 6(PFC) BA 24, 32 (ACC) PPC, BG, thalamus, & cerebellum. In ASD there is no significant recrutiment in same regions or equal engagment across target and stay.
Between group comparisons for target trials: DLPFC, IPS, BG show > activation in TYPs. ACC and lIPS activation is negatively associated with RBs.
Lee et al. (2009) [90] n=12 (TYP)
n=12 (ASD)
Mean TYP=16
Mean ASD=15
ASD=107
TYP=113
Go/No-Go task Block design fMRI
FC of all ROIs from within group maps using method of Kana et al. 2007.
Seeds in r and l IFC.
Correlations with ADI-R scales.
Whole brain analysis used to generate ROIs with height threshold of t=2.52 and p<.05. No FC differences for l and r IFC seeds.
Solomon et al. (2009) [86] n=23 (TYP)
n=22 (ASD)
Age TYP=11
Age ASD=10
IQ ASD=113
IQ TYP=115
Preparing to Overcome Prepotency (POP) Task. Simple stimulus response incompatibility task. Event-related fMRI.
FC analysis. Factor analysis of network integration.
Correlation with ADHD sx & errors.
Voxel-wise FDR corrected for p<.05 t=2.5, cluster size=10
Masked for control
related regions.
ASD with reduced recruitment of BA 10, bilaterally, l BA 6, BA 7 bilaterally, lBA 40, driven by red trials.
PFC, ACC, parietal FC reduced in ASD.
ASD with less network integration.
For TYP, BA 9 to BA 7 FC related to red error rates. For ASD, ADHD scores.
Vogan et al. (2014) [88] n=17 (TYP)
n=19 (ASD)
Age TYP=11
Age ASD=11
IQ ASD=109
IQ TYP=115
One-back working memory task with color matching of different difficulty levels. Block design fMRI Cluster corrected at t=2.3, p<.05 ROIs selected from between group maps. Within group maps show differences wTYP activating BA 37, 7, 5, 45, 9; ASD BA 19, 37. Both groups see decreases in BA 23/32 &10 with increased load. No between group differences overall. TYP shows > activation in BA 7, 9, 6 with increasing load.
Padmanabhan et al. (2015) [93] n=9 TYP adolescents & n=14 TYP adults
n=11 ASD adolescents & n= 8 ASD adults
TYP Adol.=14yr
TYP Adult=23
ASD Adol.=15
ASD Adult=25
TYP/Adol.=110
TYP/Adult=110
ASD/Adol.=106
ASD/Adult=99
Saccadic paradigm with eye tracking. Examine prep trials; anti-saccade trials; pro-saccade trials.
ROIs extracted from within group maps in areas known to be involved in occulomotor control-FEF, SEF, ACC DLPFC, IPS, putamen.
Monte Carlo simul’tn with AlphaSim shows 23 voxel clusters with a single voxel threshold of p<.0001 required for p<.05. 12 mm ROIs from peaks & used as a mask. Within group maps with no differences.
Cortical eye fields (FEF, SEF, and PEF) & putamen, thalamus, & caudate recruited Anti-saccade Trials: Within group maps are similar and include: FEF, SEF, DLPFC, IPL.
Both groups show increased SES & lFEF activation with age. ASD with greater left precuneus activation vs. TYP. L putamen w adol with ASD>TYP adol & TYP wASD. TYP > ASD in r IPL activation with age.
Yerys et al. (2015) [95] Mean TYP=11
Mean ASD=11
ASD=114
TYP=119
Set shifting paradigm Focus on lowest level response reconfiguration. Event-related fMRI.
Correlations with accuracy, RT, switch
costs, ADOS scores.
Cluster corrected at t=2.6, p<.05 No group differences in switch + stay trials vs. fixation. ASD with greater activation on switch vs. stay in ACC, l MFG, r IFG. No significant correlations between activation & task performance or ADOS scores.

Abbreviations used: ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TYP: typical development; vs.: versus; FC: functional connectivity; RT: reaction time; ACC: anterior cingulate cortex; DTI: diffusion tensor imaging; FA: fractional anisotropy; w/: with; ROI: region of interest; RB: repetitive behavior; d ACC: dorsal ACC; FEF: frontal eye fields; l:left;r: right; PPC: posterior parietal cortex; BG: basal ganglia; DLPFC: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; IPS: inferior parietal sulcus; FDR: false discovery rate; MFG: middle frontal gyrus; IPL: inferior parietal lobule; MTG: medial temporal gyrus; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; adol.:adolescents; ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Discussion

This review of structural, rsfMRI, and diffusion imaging studies converged in suggesting that the scaffold supporting cognitive control in adolescents with ASD transitioning to young adulthood is altered. Longitudinal structural studies suggest there is enlargement of the ACC and excess white matter and gray and white matter pruning in the parietal cortex during this period [16]. This is accompanied by over and under-connectivity within the DMN and other brain networks important for cognitive control (SN, ECN) compared to that found in TYP. Finally, ASD shown early developing, persistent, and widespread diffusion-related differences compared to TYP in the white matter tracts supporting control. Given that a different and compromised neural scaffold restricts the functioning of control processes in adolescents and young adults with ASD, it is not surprising that their performance on executive control and task-based fMRI paradigms across multiple component processes develops less during adolescence than TYP. More specifically, adolescents with ASD appear to recruit the PFC and parietal cortex less than those with TYP when keeping context in mind, inhibiting prepotent responses, and engaging in more difficult forms of set shifting.

This conclusion offers several potential etiological and treatment related leads that could be explored in future studies. First, based on structural findings of its potential increased size [16] and fMRI studies documenting its atypical recruitment and potential relationship to RBs [89, 91, 92, 94], the ACC may represent a node that alters the functioning of the impaired cognitive control network. This assertion is consistent with early theoretical work suggesting that inefficient functioning the dorsal medial PFC system produces social orienting deficits found in young children with ASD [96]. The work of our group, which finds that as adolescence progresses those with ASD show increasing PFC/ACC FC while those with TYP show decreasing FC between these brain regions, also is consistent with the contention that the mechanisms of cognitive control operate differently in those with ASD. Also supportive of the importance of the ACC in ASD, are recent rsMRI FC studies of the SN, which includes the ACC. One such study showed that functioning of this brain region has extremely high and unique power in predicting the development of ASD traits and adaptive functioning from late adolescence to early adulthood [97]. Furthermore, several dimensional psychopathology studies illustrate that the ACC is implicated in ASD-like symptoms across multiple neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [98], and repetitive behaviors in obsessive compulsive disorder [99].

This review also highlighted the potential role of the parietal cortex and related impairments in the functioning of fronto-parietal neural circuits in both structural neuroimaging and task-based fMRI [86, 87, 89] studies. The parietal cortex has been implicated in the storage of spatial information in working memory [100], rule representation, and attention allocation [101]. Parietal cortex also is thought to help establish and maintain context [102]. Given the ubiquity of executive functions deficits to neurodevelopmental disorders, investigations of atypical fronto-parietal functioning, holds the potential to provide a more trans-diagnostic perspective on neuropsychiatric disorders. The association between fronto-parietal FC deficits and symptoms of attention and hyperactivity in adolescents with ASD constitutes a promising lead, although it is not likely that RDoC will trully help carve nature at its joints, and there still will likely be multiple mechanisms underlying each symptom domain [103, 104].

It was challenging to complete a comprehensive and integrative review. Although the neuroimaging field has seen the evolution of increasingly rigorous best-practice standards [105], these have not been fully implemented in ASD research. As illustrated by Tables 1, 2, 3, and 4, ASD neuroimaging studies typically are small (< 20 participants/group), which is alarming given the recent suggestion that 50–100 participants/group may be necessary to detect subtle effects in fMRI [106]. Subject motion also can both confound task-based fMRI analyses and alter rsfMRI connectivity metrics [107109]. While data scrubbing [110] techniques have been developed, they only have been used routinely for the past 5 years. The use of overly liberal thresholds, which still are routinely employed in ASD fMRI research, also has drawn criticism [111]. In addition to methodological challenges related to imaging, studies of cognitive control in ASD often employ heterogeneous neuroimaging tasks with different levels of discriminating power/difficulty, making it virtually impossible to compare and interpret their results. Finally, we can only truly understand development if we complete more longitudinal studies that carefully examine sex differences.

Despite revealing an altered scaffold that produces decreased cognitive control development in adolescence, this review is hopeful. The use of minimally invasive neuroimaging methodologies has greatly advanced our capacity for understanding the neural systems underpinning ASD-specific symptoms and strengths. The field should move towards developing adequately powered and standardized cognitive control paradigms that can be used in large longitudinal studies across a wide range of ages and cognitive ability levels, which is important given the dearth of studies in those with intellectual disability who also experience control impairments [112]. Until these studies have been conducted, meta-analyses can help bridge the gap in better understanding the mechanisms of the development of cognitive control in ASD – a goal that is highly consistent with the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) view that the best way to advance treatment development is through research that furthers understanding of the function of neural circuits and how to manipulate them.

Acknowledgments

Support: During this work, Dr. Solomon was supported by R01MH106518 and R01MH103284; Dr. Nordahl was supported by R01MH104438.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflict of Interest and Disclosures: Drs. Solomon, Hogeveen, Libero, and Nordahl report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Botvinick MM, et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108(3):624–52. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Insel TR. The NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Project: precision medicine for psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(4):395–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.MacDonald AW, 3rd, Carter CS. Cognitive experimental approaches to investigating impaired cognition in schizophrenia: a paradigm shift. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24(7):873–82. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.7.873.8386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Niendam TA, et al. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012;12(2):241–68. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Badre D, D’Esposito M. Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10(9):659–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn2667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8(12):539–546. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Braver TS. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(2):106–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Frank MJ, Claus ED. Anatomy of a decision: Striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. Psychological Review. 2006;113(2):300–26. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Crone EA. Executive functions in adolescence: inferences from brain and behavior. Dev Sci. 2009;12(6):825–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00918.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Giedd JN, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10):861–3. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Breukelaar IA, et al. Cognitive control network anatomy correlates with neurocognitive behavior: A longitudinal study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016 doi: 10.1002/hbm.23401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ismail MM, et al. Studying Autism Spectrum Disorder with Structural and Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging: A Survey. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:211. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bonilha L, et al. Gray and white matter imbalance–typical structural abnormality underlying classic autism? Brain Dev. 2008;30(6):396–401. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2007.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Nordahl CW, et al. Cortical folding abnormalities in autism revealed by surface-based morphometry. J Neurosci. 2007;27(43):11725–35. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0777-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hua X, et al. Brain growth rate abnormalities visualized in adolescents with autism. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(2):425–36. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wallace GL, et al. Longitudinal cortical development during adolescence and young adulthood in autism spectrum disorder: increased cortical thinning but comparable surface area changes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(6):464–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Biswal B, et al. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1995;34(4):537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Yeo BTT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fair DA, et al. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104(33):13507–13512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705843104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Menon V. Developmental pathways to functional brain networks: emerging principles. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(12):627–40. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Stevens MC. The contributions of resting state and task-based functional connectivity studies to our understanding of adolescent brain network maturation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;70:13–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Menon V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(10):483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Raichle ME. The brain’s default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2015;38:433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Yerys BE, et al. Default mode network segregation and social deficits in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from non-medicated children. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;9:223–32. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Elton A, et al. Neural Connectivity Evidence for a Categorical-Dimensional Hybrid Model of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;80(2):120–128. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lynch CJ, et al. Default mode network in childhood autism: Posteromedial cortex heterogeneity and relationship with social deficits. Biological Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):212–219. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Washington SD, et al. Dysmaturation of the default mode network in autism. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35(4):1284–1296. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wiggins JL, et al. Using a self-organizing map algorithm to detect age-related changes in functional connectivity during rest in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research. 2011;1380:187–197. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Weng SJ, et al. Alterations of resting state functional connectivity in the default network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research. 2009;1313:202–214. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Assaf M, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. NeuroImage. 2010;53(1):247–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kennedy DP, Courchesne E. The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1877–1885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Monk CS, et al. Abnormalities of intrinsic functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage. 2009;47(2):764–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014;16(1):55–61. doi: 10.1038/nrn3857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Seeley WW, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(9):2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Uddin LQ, et al. Dynamic reconfiguration of structural and functional connectivity across core neurocognitive brain networks with development. J Neurosci. 2011;31(50):18578–89. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4465-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Uddin LQ, et al. Salience network–based classification and prediction of symptom severity in children With autism. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(8):869–879. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nomi JS, Uddin LQ. Developmental changes in large-scale network connectivity in autism. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2015;7:732–741. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Di Martino A, et al. The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(6):659–67. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ebisch SJH, et al. Altered intrinsic functional connectivity of anterior and posterior insula regions in high-functioning participants with autism spectrum disorder. Human Brain Mapping. 2011;32(7):1013–1028. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Eilam-Stock T, et al. Abnormal autonomic and associated brain activities during rest in autism spectrum disorder. Brain. 2014;137(1):153–171. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fair DA, et al. Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Computational Biology. 2009;5(5):14–23. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Sherman LE, et al. Development of the default mode and central executive networks across early adolescence: a longitudinal study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014;10:148–59. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Constantino JN, Gruber CP. The social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Abbott AE, et al. Patterns of atypical functional connectivity and behavioral links in autism differ between default, salience, and executive networks. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26(10):4034–4045. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Demopoulos C, et al. Corpus callosum in cognitive and sensory processing: insights into autism. Future Neurology. 2015;10(2):147–160. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Takahashi M, et al. White matter microstructure of the cingulum and cerebellar peduncle is related to sustained attention and working memory: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroscience letters. 2010;477(2):72–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Makris N, et al. Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15(6):854–869. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Urger SE, et al. The Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus in Typically Developing Children and Adolescents: Diffusion Tensor Imaging and Neuropsychological Correlates. J Child Neurol. 2014 doi: 10.1177/0883073813520503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Travers BG, et al. Atypical development of white matter microstructure of the corpus callosum in males with autism: a longitudinal investigation. Molecular Autism. 2015;6(1):1–15. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0001-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ameis SH, et al. Altered cingulum bundle microstructure in autism spectrum disorder. Acta neuropsychiatrica. 2013;25(05):275–282. doi: 10.1017/neu.2013.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Solso S, et al. Diffusion Tensor Imaging Provides Evidence of Possible Axonal Overconnectivity in Frontal Lobes in Autism Spectrum Disorder Toddlers. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;79(8):676–684. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ouyang M, et al. Atypical age-dependent effects of autism on white matter microstructure in children of 2–7 years. Human brain mapping. 2016;37(2):819–832. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Catani M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex. 2008;44(8):1105–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Alexander AL, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum in Autism. Neuroimage. 2007;34(1):61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Jou R, et al. Structural neural phenotype of autism: preliminary evidence from a diffusion tensor imaging study using tract-based spatial statistics. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2011;32(9):1607–1613. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A2558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Brito AR, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging findings in school-aged autistic children. Journal of Neuroimaging. 2009;19(4):337–343. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2009.00366.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Shukla DK, et al. White matter compromise of callosal and subcortical fiber tracts in children with autism spectrum disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49 doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Shukla DK, Keehn B, Müller RA. Tract-specific analyses of diffusion tensor imaging show widespread white matter compromise in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52(3):286–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02342.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Libero LE, et al. Multimodal neuroimaging based classification of autism spectrum disorder using anatomical, neurochemical, and white matter correlates. Cortex. 2015;66:46–59. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Pardini M, et al. White matter reduced streamline coherence in young men with autism and mental retardation. Eur J Neurol. 2009;16(11):1185–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02699.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jou RJ, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in autism spectrum disorders: preliminary evidence of abnormal neural connectivity. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011;45 doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.534069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Pugliese L, et al. The anatomy of extended limbic pathways in Asperger syndrome: a preliminary diffusion tensor imaging tractography study. Neuroimage. 2009;47(2):427–434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Noriuchi M, et al. Altered white matter fractional anisotropy and social impairment in children with autism spectrum disorder. Brain research. 2010;1362:141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Ikuta T, et al. Abnormal cingulum bundle development in autism: a probabilistic tractography study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2014;221(1):63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bernal B, Altman N. The connectivity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus: a tractography DTI study. Magnetic resonance imaging. 2010;28(2):217–225. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2009.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Catani M, Jones DK. Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of neurology. 2005;57(1):8–16. doi: 10.1002/ana.20319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wakana S, et al. Fiber Tract–based Atlas of Human White Matter Anatomy 1. Radiology. 2004;230(1):77–87. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2301021640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Ameis SH, et al. Impaired structural connectivity of socio-emotional circuits in autism spectrum disorders: a diffusion tensor imaging study. PLoS One. 2011;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Poustka L, et al. Fronto-temporal disconnectivity and symptom severity in children with autism spectrum disorder. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2012;13(4):269–280. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.591824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Libero LE, et al. White Matter Diffusion of Major Fiber Tracts Implicated in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain Connectivity. 2016 doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Hill EL. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(1):26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bennetto L, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Dev. 1996;67(4):1816–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Williams DL, et al. Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(6):747–56. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0021-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Christ SE, et al. Evidence for selective inhibitory impairment in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology. 2011;25(6):690–701. doi: 10.1037/a0024256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Geurts HM, et al. How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines Special Issue: Annual research review. 2004;45(4):836–854. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00276.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Solomon M, et al. Cognitive control in autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2008;26(2):239–47. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2007.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Ozonoff S, et al. Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with autistic disorder: evidence from the Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(2):139–50. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022605.81989.cc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Geurts HM, Corbett B, Solomon M. The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(2):74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Poljac E, Bekkering H. A review of intentional and cognitive control in autism. Front Psychol. 2012;3:436. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Poljac E, et al. Understanding Behavioural Rigidity in Autism Spectrum Conditions: The Role of Intentional Control. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(3):714–727. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-3010-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Poljac E, Poljac E, Yeung N. Cognitive control of intentions for voluntary actions in individuals with a high level of autistic traits. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(12):2523–33. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1509-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Rosenthal M, et al. Impairments in real-world executive function increase from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology. 2013;27(1):13–8. doi: 10.1037/a0031299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Geurts HM, van den Bergh SF, Ruzzano L. Prepotent Response Inhibition and Interference Control in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Two Meta-Analyses. Autism Res. 2014 doi: 10.1002/aur.1369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Luna B, et al. Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61(4):474–81. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Solomon M, et al. The neural substrates of cognitive control deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(12):2515–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Solomon M, et al. The development of the neural substrates of cognitive control in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Biological psychiatry. 2014;76(5):412–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Vogan VM, et al. The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load. J Neurodev Disord. 2014;6(1):19. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-6-19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Kana RK, et al. Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;62(3):198–206. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Lee PS, et al. Functional connectivity of the inferior frontal cortex changes with age in children with autism spectrum disorders: a fcMRI study of response inhibition. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(8):1787–94. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Thakkar KN, et al. Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Brain. 2008;131(9):2464–2478. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Agam Y, et al. Reduced cognitive control of response inhibition by the anterior cingulate cortex in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage. 2010;52(1):336–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Padmanabhan A, et al. Developmental changes in brain function underlying inhibitory control in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2015;8(2):123–35. doi: 10.1002/aur.1398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Shafritz KM, et al. The neural circuitry mediating shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(10):974–80. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Yerys BE, et al. Neural Correlates of Set-Shifting in Children With Autism. Autism Res. 2015;8(4):386–97. doi: 10.1002/aur.1454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Mundy P. Annotation: The neural basis of social impairments in autism: the role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2003;44(6):793–809. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Plitt M, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity predicts longitudinal change in autistic traits and adaptive functioning in autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(48):E6699–706. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510098112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Rommelse N, Buitelaar JK, Hartman CA. Structural brain imaging correlates of ASD and ADHD across the lifespan: a hypothesis-generating review on developmental ASD-ADHD subtypes. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2016 doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1651-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Carlisi CO, et al. Comparative Multimodal Meta-analysis of Structural and Functional Brain Abnormalities in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Wager TD, Smith EE. Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3(4):20. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.4.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Brass M, von Cramon DY. The Role of the Frontal Cortex in Task Preparation. Cerebral Cortex. 2002;12(9):908–914. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.9.908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Sanefuji M, et al. Double-dissociation between the mechanism leading to impulsivity and inattention in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A resting-state functional connectivity study. Cortex. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Elton A, et al. Neural Connectivity Evidence for a Categorical-Dimensional Hybrid Model of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80(2):120–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Ragland JD, Solomon M. Categorical Dimensions of Social Impairment and Disrupted Functional Connectivity in Autism Spectrum Disorders: When Does Continuous Become Discrete? Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80(2):90–1. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Poldrack RA, et al. Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017 doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Carter CS, Lesh TA, Barch DM. Threshold, Power, and Sample Sizes in Clinical Neuroimaging. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2016;1(2):99–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Nair A, et al. Impact of methodological variables on functional connectivity findings in autism spectrum disorders. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(8):4035–48. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Power JD, et al. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2142–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):431–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Hahamy A, Behrmann M, Malach R. The idiosyncratic brain: distortion of spontaneous connectivity patterns in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(2):302–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.3919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(28):7900–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Panerai S, et al. Executive functions and adaptive behaviour in autism spectrum disorders with and without intellectual disability. Psychiatry journal. 2014;2014 doi: 10.1155/2014/941809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES