Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2017 Jul 18;106(9):1438–1444. doi: 10.1111/apa.13936

Defining pain in newborns: need for a uniform taxonomy?

Kanwaljeet J S Anand 1,
PMCID: PMC5601230  PMID: 28556311

Abstract

A framework for defining pain terms such as acute, persistent, prolonged or chronic pain to newborns was derived from the scientific literature on neonatal pain assessments, previous attempts to define chronic pain and the clinical and neurophysiological features of neonatal pain. This novel framework incorporates the temporal features, localising characteristics, and secondary effects of the pain experienced, as well as the behavioural and physiological response patterns of newborns.

Conclusion

Although not evidence‐based, this framework provides an initial starting point for defining commonly used neonatal pain terms. It will require future revision/refinement based on the accumulating evidence for non‐acute pain.

Keywords: Chronic pain, Pain assessment, Pain in neonate, Persistent pain, Prolonged pain


Key notes.

  • Neonatal pain assessments are focused mostly on acute pain, whereas prolonged, persistent or chronic pain are relatively ignored; without clear definitions, these terms are often used interchangeably for newborns.

  • An initial framework for defining neonatal pain terms is presented, derived from characteristics of the pain experienced, as well as behavioural and physiological response patterns of newborns.

  • Explicit definitions of pain terms may facilitate future neonatal pain research and management.

graphic file with name APA-106-1438-g001.jpg

‘Ideas need to be fruitful; they do not have to be right. And, curiously enough, the two do not necessarily go together.’ 1 Peter W. Nathan, MD, FRCP (1914–2002).

A scientific rationale for pain and its effects in human newborns were first presented thirty years ago 2. Multidisciplinary efforts have since fuelled significant progress in neonatal pain 3, exploring its underlying mechanisms 4, 5, describing its epidemiology in clinical settings 6, 7, defining its impact on the brain and subsequent development 8, 9 or devising clinical assessment and management approaches 10, 11. Despite this progress, defining and identifying pain in newborns remains a major challenge. Descriptors such as acute, persistent, prolonged or chronic pain are often used interchangeably for newborns, without clear definitions for these terms. Explicit definitions may help reduce confusion and controversy among clinicians, improve assessment and management and inform study designs in neonatal pain research.

The International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) defined pain as ‘an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage’ (IASP Committee on Taxonomy, 1969, updated in 1994 and 2002)12. This definition requires patients to describe their pain, by default establishing the primacy of self‐report as a ‘gold standard’. Although widely accepted across all healthcare professions and biomedical disciplines, this definition lacks applicability to non‐verbal populations 13, 14 and ignores the cognitive and social dimensions of pain 15. Indeed, pain in newborns was often discounted until the IASP Committee on Taxonomy added a note clarifying that, ‘The inability to communicate verbally does not negate the possibility that an individual is experiencing pain’ 16.

The question of conscious pain perception in the early preterm newborn (or foetus) has been hotly debated 17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, mainly because of its social, ethical and legal implications 23, 24, 25, 26. Consciousness was widely believed to reside in the cerebral cortex, thus putatively being absent or rudimentary in those without functional thalamocortical connections 20, 26, although mechanisms underlying the subcortical control of consciousness 27, 28, 29 and functionality of the subplate zone 30, 31, 32, 33 appear to challenge that default. Attempting to set forth criteria for early human consciousness would create the difficulties of ‘measuring’ consciousness and the conundrums of trying to prove or disprove whether consciousness is present at different stages of development 34, 35. For the purpose of this review, it is presumed that all viable newborns are capable of consciously perceiving and responding to pain 13, 14, 36, 37.

Given the absence of self‐report, pain assessment in newborns is challenging, particularly among ventilated preterm infants with a limited behavioural repertoire. Although numerous pain assessment methods have been devised, validated and implemented in clinical care 38, 39, most are focused on the acute, episodic pain resulting from clinically essential, frequently performed invasive procedures. Hartley et al. recently presented an EEG‐based measure of nociceptive brain activity evoked by acute noxious stimulation and reduced by a topical anaesthetic 40. This too applies only to acute pain, requires specialised expertise, equipment and analytic capabilities and has a relatively low sensitivity (57%, 64%) and specificity (65%, 68%) to be clinically useful 40.

The need to differentiate acute from prolonged pain was first proposed at the 8th World Congress on Pain 41, and an expert panel later recognised the ability of newborns to experience prolonged/chronic pain 42. To the clinician‐researcher, acutely painful events in newborns clearly appeared to cause pain‐related distress and could be standardised for research. Clinical examples of prolonged or persistent pain were harder to study—they defied quantification, occurred less frequently, and did not elicit reproducible responses in newborns 43, 44. Not surprisingly, therefore, only 10% of newborns in neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) received daily clinical assessments for prolonged, continuous pain 11.

Attempts to define chronic pain in the neonatal context have contributed greatly to our current understanding of pain in infancy 45, 46. A few methods to assess the intensity of prolonged/chronic pain were devised and validated (Table 1), but given the absence of clear definitions, other aspects specific for chronic pain (duration, periodicity, character or secondary effects) have not been addressed. Despite these gaps, clinicians are using therapies normally reserved for chronic pain in newborns without any clear indications 47, 48, 49, 50, or assessment of short‐term and long‐term risk/benefit ratios. Most clinicians can easily identify examples of persistent pain following tissue injury (circumcision, other post‐operative pain) or inflammation (necrotizing enterocolitis, pyelonephritis), as well as examples of chronic pain (osteogenesis imperfecta, epidermolysis bullosa), but a consensus for developing the taxonomy of pain terms specifically for newborn infants remains elusive 45, 46, 51.

Table 1.

Previous studies on persistent or chronic pain in newborns

Authors, Year Krechel & Bildner, 1995 86 Debillon et al., 2001 93 Boyle et al., 2006 43 Hummel et al., 2008 87, 94 van Dijk et al., 2009 95 Lundqvist et al., 2014 96 van Ganzewinkel et al., 2014 45
Study design Observational study Staff survey, observational study Staff survey, within an ongoing RCT Observational study Observational study Observational studies, survey Delphi survey, three rounds
Number of subjects 24 76 22 46 286 86 294
Number of observations 1382 76 89 72 3600 246 525
Age group(s) 32–60 weeks PCA 25–36 weeks GA 23–32 weeks GA 23–40 weeks GA 24–42 weeks GA 23–29 weeks GA N/A
Male/Female 10/14 N/A N/A 21/25 174/112 N/A N/A
Pain assessed Post‐operative Preterm Mechanical ventilation Ventilated or post‐operative Acute procedural Preterm and sick term Post‐operative or mechanical ventilation
Comparators Objective Pain Scale None NEOPAIN PIPP Numeric Rating Scale None Likert scale
Assessment method CRIES Pain Scale EDIN scale none N‐PASS COMFORTneo scale ALPS‐Neo none
Stimulus studied Various surgical procedures (VPS placement to PDA closure) Mechanical ventilation, NEC, surgical closure of PDA Mechanical ventilation Mechanical ventilation, various surgical procedures Not defined Not defined Not defined (conditions associated with chronic pain)
Parameters/Findings Facial expression (grimace) Facial activity Facial expressions Facial expression Facial tension Facial expression
Requires oxygen for SpO2 < 95% Body movements Infant activity levels Extremities, muscle tone Body movement Hand/foot activity Increased energy consumption
Sleepless Quality of sleep Posture/quality of movements Behaviour state (body) Muscle tone Tone of extremities Hyperalgesia/altered pain perception
Increased vital signs (BP and HR) Response to nursing Response to handling Changes in vital signs (HR, RR, BP, SpO2) Calmness/agitation Level of activity Hyperresponsive to all interactions or procedures
Crying Consolability Ventilator dyssynchrony Crying/irritability Respiratory response/crying Breathing pattern Recurrent or long‐lasting pain
alertness no proximate event or procedure
Validity Convergent: R s = 0.73, p < 0.0001
Discriminant: scores decreased 3.0 pre‐ vs. post‐analgesia, p < 0.0001
Discriminant: scores decreased 4.4 (0.4) pre‐ vs. post‐analgesia, p < 0.0001 N/A Convergent: Rs = 0.83, p < 0.0001
Discriminant: scores decreased 3.05 pre‐ vs. post‐analgesia, p < 0.0001
Convergent: r = 0.83, p < 0.0001;
Discriminant: scores decreased 6.9 pre‐ vs. post‐analgesia, p < 0.001;
Sensitivity 81% Specificity 90%
N/A N/A
Reliability Inter‐rater: Rs=0.72, p < 0.0001 Inter‐rater: weighted κ coefficients = 0.59–0.74 (0.69) N/A Inter‐rater: pain scale ICC 0.95‐0.97, p < 0.001; sedation scale ICC 0.9–0.95, p < 0.0001 Inter‐rater: weighted κ coefficients = 0.65–0.97 (0.79) Inter‐rater: ICC 0.91 (0.61–0.82 for items) N/A
Internal consistency N/A Cronbach's α coefficient = 0.92 N/A Cronbach's α coefficient = 0.89 Cronbach's α coefficient = 0.88 Cronbach's α coefficient = 0.95 N/A

ALPS‐Neo = Astrid Lindgren's Children's Hospital Pain Scale; BP = Blood pressure; CRIES = Crying, Requires oxygen, Increased vital signs, Expression, Sleepless; EDIN = Échelle Douleur Inconfort Nouveau‐Né; GA = Gestational age; HR = Heart rate; ICC = Intraclass correlation coefficient; NEC = Necrotizing enterocolitis; N‐PASS = Neonatal Pain, Agitation and Sedation Scale; N/A = not available); PCA = Post‐conceptional age; PDA = Patent ductus arteriosus; R s = Spearman rank correlation coefficient; r = Pearson moment correlation coefficient; RCT = Randomised controlled trial; RR = Respiratory rate; SpO2 = Peripheral oxygen saturation.

For adults, various professional societies define acute pain as that associated with tissue injury, whereas chronic pain is defined as pain that extends beyond the period of tissue healing, with levels of pathology insufficient to explain the presence and/or extent of pain. Pain signals may remain active for months or years, causing a ‘persistent pain that disrupts sleep and normal living, ceases to have protective functions, and instead degrades health and functional capability’12, 52, 53. Turk and Okifuji differentiated acute and chronic pain using criteria for duration and pathology, short‐lasting pain with high physical pathology reflects acute pain, whereas prolonged durations with low pathology represent chronic pain 54. However, most chronic pain conditions in adults represent an interplay between significant nociceptive inputs and psychosocial/cognitive factors 55. The ‘expected healing period’ for defining transitions from acute to chronic pain is variably pegged at one, three or six months 12, 52, 53, 54, 56.

Such time‐points clearly exclude newborn infants who have not lived long enough to experience chronic pain, whereas the examples for chronic pain commonly cited by clinicians (e.g. epidermolysis bullosa) usually portend some kind of ongoing tissue pathology 45, 46. Also, diseases associated with prolonged pain in newborns (e.g. necrotizing enterocolitis) may have variable and undefined durations of tissue pathology. An empirical approach may be justified therefore, for defining the pain terms commonly used in neonatal care. Putative definitions for acute, prolonged, persistent or chronic pain must be explicit and relevant to the transient newborn period; they must represent the types of pain being experienced, independent of their aetiology or management.

Limited evidence supports management of chronic or persistent pain in neonates, so why do definitions matter at all? We argue that defining an infant's pain would justify a bedside clinician's level of concern, focus their attention towards specific assessment methods and allow them to weigh the risks/benefits of appropriate interventions. Pain definitions will also stimulate further advances to: understand the epidemiology of neonatal pain, investigate the underlying mechanisms at different levels of neurologic maturity, identify biomarkers/patterns for psychophysical or molecular phenotyping, recognise genetic, epigenetic or other factors that place infants at high risk for poor outcomes or long‐term complications and lastly, develop targeted therapies for specific types of non‐acute pain 15, 54. Most clinical trials chose their subjects based on a few selected clinical characteristics, which may or may not match individual newborns with the therapies uniquely suited for their pain. Thus, inclusion criteria incorporating explicit pain definitions may improve homogeneity in clinical trials. As an initial starting point for defining the different pain terms used for newborns (Table 2), we should consider the following:

Table 2.

Suggested starting point for defining the pain terms used for neonatal pain

Pain term Onset Duration Charactera Primary hyperalgesia
Acute episodic Immediate 0–120b minutes Sharp, well‐localised Present, mild, short‐lasting
Acute recurrent Immediate variable Sharp, well‐localised Present, moderate or severe
Prolongedc Rapid, may be gradual One hour to 24b hours Sharp, diffusely localised Present, moderate or severe
Persistentc Rapid or gradual, cumulative one to seven days Dull/sharp, diffusely localised Present, moderate or severe
Chronic Usually gradual Eight days or longer Dull, diffusely localised May be present or absent, mild if present
Pain term Secondary hyperalgesia Allodynia Behavioural phenotype Physiological phenotype
Acute episodic Probably absent Probably absent Strongly reactive and reflexive High peak, sympathetic activation
Acute recurrent Present, mild or moderate Probably absent Weakly reactive or reflexive Prolonged peak, sympathetic activation
Prolongedc Mild or absent Probably absent Strongly reactive on stimulation High plateau, sympathetic activation
Persistentc Present, mild or moderate May be present, mild/moderate Hyperreactive initially, later hyporeactive Normal or low sympathetic activation
Chronic Present, moderate or severe May be present, moderate/severe Hyporeactive more often, could also be hyperreactive Normal or suppressed sympathetic drive
a

Based on descriptions in adult patients, but may be discerned by a careful physical examination.

b

Some infants with increased sensitivity to pain may have a slower decay of the acute pain following an invasive procedure, thus justifying some overlap in the durations of acute episodic pain and prolonged pain.

c

Continuous pain may be characterised as either ‘prolonged’ or ‘persistent’.

Temporal features

Any painful experience is defined by its onset and duration, exemplifying the salient differences between acute and non‐acute pain. Acute pain occurs immediately with the onset of tissue injury or stimulation of an inflamed area, and it usually lasts for the duration of the stimulus or for brief periods thereafter (some infants experience a slower decay of pain compared to others). However, the durations assigned for acute, prolonged, persistent or chronic pain are arbitrary at best. In adults, some experts classify pain lasting longer than one month as chronic pain, whereas others consider pain as chronic only if it lasts for longer than three or six months 12, 52, 53, 54. Similarly, variable criteria are used for children 56, 57. Given the temporal characteristics of painful conditions in newborns, the length of the neonatal period, as well as time‐courses for developing long‐term effects of pain, tolerance to analgesic drugs or other systemic effects, we posit that pain lasting longer than seven days be considered as chronic pain in newborns. This should prompt further diagnostic efforts, re‐evaluation of current analgesic strategies, use of alternative therapies and longer‐term plans for preventing disability, promoting rehabilitation and restoring function.

Character of pain

For obvious reasons, precise descriptors cannot be chosen for the character of pain (e.g. burning, piercing and shooting) that newborns experience, but clinicians may attempt to discern how well it is localised, or whether it is associated with clear boundaries or not. In the developing nervous system, two features characterise neonatal pain processing: (i) the immature peripheral and central nervous systems are biologically primed towards lower thresholds for activation, excitation and transmission of nociceptive stimuli as compared to older ages; this feature is further accentuated in preterm infants 5, 51; (ii) dorsal horn neurons in the spinal cord have large, overlapping cutaneous receptive fields; stimulation of these receptive fields heightens nociceptive signalling and can evoke a long‐lasting excitability within the spinal cord 58, 59, 60. Indeed, inhibitory signalling in the spinal cord is weak or absent in newborns and develops gradually during infancy 61, 62. These features are likely to promote poorer localisation of pain in newborns, while also heightening its secondary effects.

Secondary effects

Tissue injury or inflammation leads to secondary effects such as hyperalgesia (increased pain to a stimulus that is normally painful) and allodynia (pain due to stimuli that do not normally provoke pain). Primary hyperalgesia localises to the area of tissue damage, whereas secondary hyperalgesia occurs in normal areas remote from the site of tissue damage. Despite their biological plausibility 5, 61, 63, 64, limited clinical evidence supports these phenomena in human newborns. Fitzgerald et al. reported primary hyperalgesia following heel lances in newborns and its reversal with topical anaesthetic cream 65, whereas Taddio et al. reported secondary hyperalgesia to venipuncture in one‐day‐old newborns of diabetic mothers, who had received multiple heel lances for monitoring blood glucose levels 66. Similarly, Andrews et al. reported signs of visceral and somatic hyperalgesia in infants undergoing abdominal surgery 67, 68. Allodynia has not been investigated in neonates with prolonged or persistent pain, although it may be more likely in infants with neurologic impairment 47, 48, 49, 50 or in those experiencing opioid withdrawal 69, 70. A developmental allodynia appears to exist in preterm neonates 71, 72, 73, 74, 75 (but not term neonates 76), manifesting as similar responses to non‐noxious and noxious stimuli. Standardised tests for allodynia need to be developed and performed in newborns with persistent or chronic pain.

Response patterns

The physiological and behavioural responses to acute pain are well characterised in newborns and used for pain assessments 38. Assessment methods developed from models of prolonged or chronic pain also show considerable overlap in the parameters chosen (Table 1), and some of these are different from acute pain 77. In older children, chronic pain is often associated with fatigue, insomnia, impaired cognition or executive function, physical disabilities and mood disturbances 56, 57, 78. These may be absent or difficult to assess in newborns, particularly among those receiving neonatal intensive care 45, 46, 51. Behavioural responses generally manifest as ‘distress’38, 79, varying in severity and incorporating facial expressions 80, gross body movements 81, 82 and subtle movement of hands, fingers or toes 81, 83. Physiological responses are incorporated into most assessment scales for acute pain, measuring increased sympathetic activity 38 (and lower parasympathetic tone? 84, 85). Although scales such as CRIES 86 and N‐PASS 87 do include changes in vital signs, it is arguable whether neonates facing acute procedural pain versus chronic pain will show similar changes in vital signs. An increased sympathetic drive may not occur in chronic or persistent pain. Heart rate variability, for example, increases during acute pain but is diminished in response to persistent or chronic pain 88, 89.

Could the spectrum of rehabilitative interventions used for adult chronic pain be analogous to the behavioural and environmental interventions advocated for newborn care? These include everything from relationship‐based models of nursing to management of temperature, light, sound, and circadian rhythms, kangaroo care, sensorial saturation and other interventions 90. As with adults in chronic pain, many drug‐based interventions may have unproven benefits and potential harms in newborns. Because of their greater potential for short‐term and long‐term adverse effects in infants 91, 92, we should consider the importance of investigating behavioural and environmental interventions for infant chronic pain as possibly safer than drug therapies 47, 48, 49, 50. Although future research will determine novel ways for assessing acute versus non‐acute pain in newborns, an empirical framework is proposed to help define various types of neonatal pain. Putative criteria may evolve from this framework, eventually leading to more accurate methods for studying the diverse types of pain experienced by human newborns.

Conflict of Interest

The author has no conflicts of interest related to this article.

References

  • 1. Nathan PW. The gate‐control theory of pain. A critical review. Brain 1976; 99: 123–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Anand KJS, Hickey PR. Pain and its effects in the human neonate and fetus. N Engl J Med 1987; 317: 1321–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Walker SM. Neonatal pain. Paediatr Anaesth 2014; 24: 39–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Fitzgerald M. The development of nociceptive circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005; 6: 507–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Walker SM, Beggs S, Baccei ML. Persistent changes in peripheral and spinal nociceptive processing after early tissue injury. Exp Neurol 2016; 275(Pt 2): 253–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Carbajal R, Rousset A, Danan C, Coquery S, Nolent P, Ducrocq S, et al. Epidemiology and treatment of painful procedures in neonates in intensive care units. JAMA 2008; 300: 60–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Lago P, Boccuzzo G, Garetti E, Pirelli A, Pieragostini L, Merazzi D, et al. Pain management during invasive procedures at Italian NICUs: Has anything changed in the last five years? J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2013; 26: 303–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Schwaller F, Fitzgerald M. The consequences of pain in early life: injury‐induced plasticity in developing pain pathways. Eur J Neurosci 2014; 39: 344–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Vinall J, Grunau RE. Impact of repeated procedural pain‐related stress in infants born very preterm. Pediatr Res 2014; 75: 584–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Carbajal R, Eriksson M, Courtois E, Boyle E, Avila‐Alvarez A, Andersen RD, et al. Sedation and analgesia practices in neonatal intensive care units (EUROPAIN): results from a prospective cohort study. Lancet Respir Med 2015; 3: 796–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Anand KJS, Eriksson M, Boyle EM, Avila‐Alvarez A, Andersen RD, Sarafidis K, et al. Assessment of continuous pain in newborns admitted to NICUs in 18 European countries. Acta Paediatr 2017; 106: 1248–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Merskey H, Bogduk N. Task force on taxonomy: classification of chronic pain: descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definition of pain terms, 2nd Edition. Seattle, WA: IASP Press, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Anand KJS, Craig KD. New perspectives on the definition of pain. Pain 1996; 67: 3–6; discussion 209‐11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Anand KJS, Rovnaghi C, Walden M, Churchill J. Consciousness, behavior, and clinical impact of the definition of pain. Pain Forum 1999; 8: 64–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Williams AC, Craig KD. Updating the definition of pain. Pain 2016; 157: 2420–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. IASP . Pain terms, a current list with definitions and notes on usage. IASP task force on taxonomy. Seattle, WA: IASP Press, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Anand KJS, Maze M. Fetuses, fentanyl, and the stress response: signals from the beginnings of pain? Anesthesiology 2001; 95: 823–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Bellieni CV. Pain assessment in human fetus and infants. AAPS J 2012; 14: 456–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Lagercrantz H, Changeux JP. The emergence of human consciousness: from fetal to neonatal life. Pediatr Res 2009; 65: 255–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Lee SJ, Ralston HJ, Drey EA, Partridge JC, Rosen MA. Fetal pain: a systematic multidisciplinary review of the evidence. JAMA 2005; 294: 947–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Reissland N, Francis B, Mason J. Can healthy fetuses show facial expressions of “pain” or “distress”? PLoS ONE 2013; 8: e65530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Derbyshire SW. Foetal pain? Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol 2010; 24: 647–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Williams C. Framing the fetus in medical work: rituals and practices. Soc Sci Med 2005; 60: 2085–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Cohen IG, Sayeed S. Fetal pain, abortion, viability, and the constitution. J Law Med Ethics 2011; 39: 235–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Brugger EC. The problem of fetal pain and abortion: toward an ethical consensus for appropriate behavior. Kennedy Inst Ethics J 2012; 22: 263–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Lagercrantz H. The emergence of consciousness: science and ethics. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med 2014; 19: 300–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Anand KJS. Fetal pain? Pain Clin Updates 2006; XIV: 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Merker B. Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behav Brain Sci 2007; 30: 63–81; discussion ‐134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Aleman B, Merker B. Consciousness without cortex: a hydranencephaly family survey. Acta Paediatr 2014; 103: 1057–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Perkins L, Hughes E, Srinivasan L, Allsop J, Glover A, Kumar S, et al. Exploring cortical subplate evolution using magnetic resonance imaging of the fetal brain. Dev Neurosci 2008; 30: 211–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Moore AR, Zhou WL, Jakovcevski I, Zecevic N, Antic SD. Spontaneous electrical activity in the human fetal cortex in vitro. J Neurosci 2011; 31: 2391–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Kostovic I, Jovanov‐Milosevic N, Rados M, Sedmak G, Benjak V, Kostovic‐Srzentic M, et al. Perinatal and early postnatal reorganization of the subplate and related cellular compartments in the human cerebral wall as revealed by histological and MRI approaches. Brain Struct Funct 2014; 219: 231–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Gatti MG, Becucci E, Fargnoli F, Fagioli M, Aden U, Buonocore G. Functional maturation of neocortex: a base of viability. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2012; 25(Suppl 1): 101–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Hudson AJ. Consciousness: physiological dependence on rapid memory access. Front Biosci (Landmark Ed) 2009; 14: 2779–800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Goupil L, Kouider S. Behavioral and neural indices of metacognitive sensitivity in preverbal infants. Curr Biol 2016; 26: 3038–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Bartocci M, Bergqvist LL, Lagercrantz H, Anand KJS. Pain activates cortical areas in the preterm newborn brain. Pain 2006; 122: 109–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Bartocci M, Anand KJS, Lagercrantz H. Response to David Bowsher's comment: the hump from cerebral neurovascular events to the subjective experience of pain in neonates. Pain 2006; 126: 320–2.16934926 [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Hatfield LA, Ely EA. Measurement of acute pain in infants: a review of behavioral and physiological variables. Biol Res Nurs 2015; 17: 100–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Stevens B, Gibbins S. Clinical utility and clinical significance in the assessment and management of pain in vulnerable infants. Clin Perinatol 2002; 29: 459–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Hartley C, Duff EP, Green G, Mellado GS, Worley A, Rogers R, et al. Nociceptive brain activity as a measure of analgesic efficacy in infants. Sci Transl Med 2017; 9: pii: eaah6122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Anand KJS. Long‐term effects of pain in neonates and infants In: Jensen TS, Turner JA, Wiesenfeld‐Hallin Z, editors. Proceedings of the 8th World congress on pain. Seattle: IASP Press, 1997: 881–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Anand KJS, Aranda JV, Berde CB, Buckman S, Capparelli EV, Carlo W, et al. Summary proceedings from the neonatal pain‐control group. Pediatrics 2006; 117: S9–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Boyle EM, Freer Y, Wong CM, McIntosh N, Anand KJS. Assessment of persistent pain or distress and adequacy of analgesia in preterm ventilated infants. Pain 2006; 124: 87–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Fitzgerald M. What do we really know about newborn infant pain? Exp Physiol 2015; 100: 1451–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. van Ganzewinkel CJ, Anand KJS, Kramer BW, Andriessen P. Chronic pain in the newborn: toward a definition. Clin J Pain 2014; 30: 970–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Pillai Riddell RR, Stevens BJ, McKeever P, Gibbins S, Asztalos L, Katz J, et al. Chronic pain in hospitalized infants: health professionals’ perspectives. J Pain 2009; 10: 1217–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Allegaert K, Naulaers G. Gabapentin as part of multimodal analgesia in a newborn with epidermolysis bullosa. Paediatr Anaesth 2010; 20: 972–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Behm MO, Kearns GL. Treatment of pain with gabapentin in a neonate. Pediatrics 2001; 108: 482–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Haney AL, Garner SS, Cox TH. Gabapentin therapy for pain and irritability in a neurologically impaired infant. Pharmacotherapy 2009; 29: 997–1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Hauer J, Mackey D. Treatment with gabapentin associated with resolution of apnea in two infants with neurologic impairment. J Palliat Med 2013; 16: 455–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. DiLorenzo M, Pillai Riddell R, Holsti L. Beyond acute pain: Understanding chronic pain in Infancy. Children Basel 2016; 3: pii: E26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Jovey RD. Pain pathways and pathophysiology In: Jovey RD, editor. Managing pain: the Canadian healthcare professional's reference. Toronto, ON: Healthcare & Financial Publishing, Rogers Media, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Berry PH, Covington EC, Dahl JD, Katz JA, Miaskowski C. Pain: current understanding of assessment, management, and treatments. Washington, DC: Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations (JCAHO); National Pharmaceutical Council, Inc., 2001: 1–105. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Turk DC, Okifuji A. Pain terms and taxonomies of pain In: Fishman FM, Ballantyne JC, Rathmell JP, editors. Bonica's management of pain, 4th Edition. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Zwingmann J, Hagelschuer P, Langenmair E, Bode G, Herget G, Sudkamp NP, et al. Lower health‐related quality of life in polytrauma patients: long‐term follow‐up after over 5 years. Medicine (Baltimore) 2016; 95: e3515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Mclpine L, McGrath PJ. Chronic and recurrent pain in children, in (eds). Mahwah, NJ, LEA. 1999, pp 529–549 In: Block AR, Kremer EF, Fernandez E, editors. Handbook of pain syndromes. New York, NY: LEA Publishers, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Palermo T, Eccleston C, Goldschneider K, McGinn KL, Sethna NF, Schechter N, et al. Assessment and management of children with chronic pain, a position statement from the American Pain Society. Chicago, IL: American Pain Society, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Jackman A, Fitzgerald M. Development of peripheral hindlimb and central spinal cord innervation by subpopulations of dorsal root ganglion cells in the embryonic rat. J Comp Neurol 2000; 418: 281–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Torsney C, Fitzgerald M. Age‐dependent effects of peripheral inflammation on the electrophysiological properties of neonatal rat dorsal horn neurons. J Neurophysiol 2002; 87: 1311–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Andrews K, Fitzgerald M. The cutaneous withdrawal reflex in human neonates: sensitization, receptive fields, and the effects of contralateral stimulation. Pain 1994; 56: 95–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Beggs S. Long‐term consequences of neonatal injury. Can J Psychiatry 2015; 60: 176–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Baccei ML, Fitzgerald M. Development of GABAergic and glycinergic transmission in the neonatal rat dorsal horn. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 4749–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Ririe DG, Liu B, Clayton B, Tong C, Eisenach JC. Electrophysiologic characteristics of large neurons in dorsal root ganglia during development and after hind paw incision in the rat. Anesthesiology 2008; 109: 111–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Ruda MA, Ling QD, Hohmann AG, Peng YB, Tachibana T. Altered nociceptive neuronal circuits after neonatal peripheral inflammation. Science 2000; 289: 628–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Fitzgerald M, Millard C, McIntosh N. Cutaneous hypersensitivity following peripheral tissue damage in newborn infants and its reversal with topical anaesthesia. Pain 1989; 39: 31–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Taddio A, Shah V, Gilbert‐MacLeod C, Katz J. Conditioning and hyperalgesia in newborns exposed to repeated heel lances. JAMA 2002; 288: 857–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Andrews K, Fitzgerald M. Wound sensitivity as a measure of analgesic effects following surgery in human neonates and infants. Pain 2002; 99: 185–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Andrews KA, Desai D, Dhillon HK, Wilcox DT, Fitzgerald M. Abdominal sensitivity in the first year of life: comparison of infants with and without prenatally diagnosed unilateral hydronephrosis. Pain 2002; 100: 35–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Sweitzer SM, Allen CP, Zissen MH, Kendig JJ. Mechanical allodynia and thermal hyperalgesia upon acute opioid withdrawal in the neonatal rat. Pain 2004; 110: 269–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Zissen MH, Zhang G, McKelvy A, Propst JT, Kendig JJ, Sweitzer SM. Tolerance, opioid‐induced allodynia and withdrawal associated allodynia in infant and young rats. Neuroscience 2007; 144: 247–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Slater R, Fabrizi L, Worley A, Meek J, Boyd S, Fitzgerald M. Premature infants display increased noxious‐evoked neuronal activity in the brain compared to healthy age‐matched term‐born infants. NeuroImage 2010; 52: 583–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Fabrizi L, Slater R, Worley A, Meek J, Boyd S, Olhede S, et al. A shift in sensory processing that enables the developing human brain to discriminate touch from pain. Curr Biol 2011; 21: 1552–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Cornelissen L, Fabrizi L, Patten D, Worley A, Meek J, Boyd S, et al. Postnatal temporal, spatial and modality tuning of nociceptive cutaneous flexion reflexes in human infants. PLoS ONE 2013; 8: e76470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Goksan S, Hartley C, Emery F, Cockrill N, Poorun R, Moultrie F, et al. fMRI reveals neural activity overlap between adult and infant pain. eLife 2015; 4: e06356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Holsti L, Grunau RE, Oberlander TF, Whitfield MF. Prior pain induces heightened motor responses during clustered care in preterm infants in the NICU. Early Hum Dev 2005; 81: 293–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Verriotis M, Fabrizi L, Lee A, Cooper RJ, Fitzgerald M, Meek J. Mapping cortical responses to somatosensory stimuli in human infants with simultaneous near‐infrared spectroscopy and event‐related potential recording. eNeuro 2016; 3: pii: eNeuro.0026‐16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Hadjistavropoulos HD, Craig KD, Grunau RE, Whitfield MF. Judging pain in infants: behavioural, contextual, and developmental determinants. Pain 1997; 73: 319–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Kashikar‐Zuck S, Carle A, Barnett K, Goldschneider KR, Sherry DD, Mara CA, et al. Longitudinal evaluation of patient‐reported outcomes measurement information systems measures in pediatric chronic pain. Pain 2016; 157: 339–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Valitalo PA, van Dijk M, Krekels EH, Gibbins S, Simons SH, Tibboel D, et al. Pain and distress caused by endotracheal suctioning in neonates is better quantified by behavioural than physiological items: a comparison based on item response theory modelling. Pain 2016; 157: 1611–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Heiderich TM, Leslie AT, Guinsburg R. Neonatal procedural pain can be assessed by computer software that has good sensitivity and specificity to detect facial movements. Acta Paediatr 2015; 104: e63–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Morison SJ, Holsti L, Grunau RE, Whitfield MF, Oberlander TF, Chan HW, et al. Are there developmentally distinct motor indicators of pain in preterm infants? Early Hum Dev 2003; 72: 131–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Holsti L, Grunau RE, Oberlander TF, Osiovich H. Is it painful or not? Discriminant validity of the Behavioral Indicators of Infant Pain (BIIP) scale. Clin J Pain 2008; 24: 83–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Grunau RE, Holsti L, Whitfield MF, Ling E. Are twitches, startles, and body movements pain indicators in extremely low birth weight infants? Clin J Pain 2000; 16: 37–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Weissman A, Aranovitch M, Blazer S, Zimmer EZ. Heel‐lancing in newborns: behavioral and spectral analysis assessment of pain control methods. Pediatrics 2009; 124: e921–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Franck LS, Boyce WT, Gregory GA, Jemerin J, Levine J, Miaskowski C. Plasma norepinephrine levels, vagal tone index, and flexor reflex threshold in premature neonates receiving intravenous morphine during the postoperative period: a pilot study. Clin J Pain 2000; 16: 95–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Krechel SW, Bildner J. CRIES: a new neonatal postoperative pain measurement score. Initial testing of validity and reliability. Paediatr Anaesth 1995; 5: 53–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Hummel P, Puchalski M, Creech SD, Weiss MG. Clinical reliability and validity of the N‐PASS: neonatal pain, agitation and sedation scale with prolonged pain. J Perinatol 2008; 28: 55–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. de Jesus JA, Tristao RM, Storm H, da Rocha AF, Campos D Jr. Heart rate, oxygen saturation, and skin conductance: a comparison study of acute pain in Brazilian newborns. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2011; 2011: 1875–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. De Jonckheere J, Rakza T, Logier R, Jeanne M, Jounwaz R, Storme L. Heart rate variability analysis for newborn infants prolonged pain assessment. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2011; 2011: 7747–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Anand KJS, Hall RW. Love, pain, and intensive care. Pediatrics 2008; 121: 825–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Anand KJS, Hall RW, Desai N, Shephard B, Bergqvist LL, Young TE, et al. Effects of morphine analgesia in ventilated preterm neonates: primary outcomes from the NEOPAIN randomised trial. Lancet 2004; 363: 1673–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Duerden EG, Guo T, Dodbiba L, Chakravarty MM, Chau V, Poskitt KJ, et al. Midazolam dose correlates with abnormal hippocampal growth and neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants. Ann Neurol 2016; 79: 548–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Debillon T, Zupan V, Ravault N, Magny JF, Dehan M. Development and initial validation of the EDIN scale, a new tool for assessing prolonged pain in preterm infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2001; 85: F36–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Hummel P, Lawlor‐Klean P, Weiss MG. Validity and reliability of the N‐PASS assessment tool with acute pain. J Perinatol 2010; 30: 474–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. van Dijk M, Roofthooft DW, Anand KJS, Guldemond F, de Graaf J, Simons S, et al. Taking up the challenge of measuring prolonged pain in (premature) neonates: the COMFORTneo scale seems promising. Clin J Pain 2009; 25: 607–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Lundqvist P, Kleberg A, Edberg AK, Larsson BA, Hellstrom‐Westas L, Norman E. Development and psychometric properties of the Swedish ALPS‐Neo pain and stress assessment scale for newborn infants. Acta Paediatr 2014; 103: 833–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Acta Paediatrica (Oslo, Norway : 1992) are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES