Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Sep 19.
Published in final edited form as: Child Dev Perspect. 2016 Aug 10;10(4):251–256. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12195

Promoting Healthy Child Development via a Two-Generation Translational Neuroscience Framework: The Filming Interactions to Nurture Development Video Coaching Program

Philip A Fisher 1, Tahl I Frenkel 2, Laura K Noll 1, Melanie Berry 1, Melissa Yockelson 1
PMCID: PMC5603284  NIHMSID: NIHMS846643  PMID: 28936231

Abstract

In this article, we focus on applying methods of translational neuroscience to two-generation, family-based interventions. In recent years, a small but growing body of evidence has documented the reversibility of some of the neurobiological effects of early adversity in the context of environmental early interventions. Some of these interventions are now being implemented at scale, which may help reduce disparities in the face of early life stress. Further progress may occur by extending these efforts to two-generation models that target caregivers’ capabilities to improve children’s outcomes. In this article, we describe the content and processes of the Filming Interactions to Nurture Development (FIND) video coaching intervention. We also discuss the two-generation, translational neuroscience framework on which FIND is based, and how similar approaches can be developed and scaled to mitigate the effects of adversity.

Keywords: parenting, video coaching, neuroscience


Historically, most research on the neurobiology of stress has focused on documenting the negative effects of early adversity on the developing brain and other stress-sensitive systems. This work has emphasized the effects of disrupted caregiving in infancy and early childhood, a time of particular vulnerability. For example, studies of rodents and nonhuman primates have investigated the effects of offspring being separated from mothers (1), being raised by peers (2), and being handled by researchers early in life (3). These stressors (and others) disrupt parental care when offspring depend on it, altering biobehavioral development. One of the main areas of investigation of this topic has been alterations in the functioning of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, a regulatory system that helps maintain homeostatic balance in the face of stressors. In addition, researchers have reported disruptions following early adversity in the functioning of the immune system, metabolic function, and numerous areas of the brain—with lifelong consequences for health and well-being (4). Similar neurobiological effects of early adversity have also been documented in humans for whom care was disrupted by neglect and traumatic stressors (5).

Research on the neurobiology of stress has generated attention from the media and stimulated public interest, leading to popularization of the concept of toxic stress (6). However, scientific knowledge in this area has rarely led to intervention strategies to mitigate the effects of early adversity. Relatedly, public-policy efforts and evidence-based programs designed to reduce population-level (or even community-level) disparities among individuals exposed to high adversity have produced only modest impacts. Indeed, consistent federal and state funding for social programs aimed at addressing these concerns, dating to the 1960s, has done little to mitigate the effects of toxic stress at the societal level, especially among those exposed to the greatest adversity (7).

In recent years, a small but growing body of evidence has documented that environmental early interventions can reverse some neurobiological effects of early adversity. Initial studies with animals (8) used enriched environment paradigms, providing enhanced cognitive and physical stimulation to rodents who were stressed prenatally. In subsequent research with humans, researchers documented that systematic changes in the environment, particularly in patterns of caregiving, could normalize some areas of brain function. For example, in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project, children who had been institutionalized and were randomly assigned to foster care in early childhood had improved cognitive function (9) and more typical levels of electrical activity in the brain (10). In other intervention studies, infants and preschoolers in foster care (11, 12) and economically disadvantaged children (13) had positive changes in salivary cortisol levels (the hormonal product of HPA axis activity). In other studies, when high-risk children participated in programs designed to promote self-regulation and readiness for school, their brain activity changed in regions associated with monitoring responses (e.g., the prefrontal cortex; 14, 15). Similarly, in studies using behavioral measures associated with key areas of brain functioning, children enrolled in school-readiness programs improved relative to their peers who did not participate in such programs (16, 17). Finally, in a recent study, adults who had taken part as children in a family-based intervention had significantly lower levels of blood-based markers of chronic inflammation, which are associated with risk for autoimmune and rheumatoid diseases (18).

These studies contribute to the literature from both a science and a policy perspective, telling us that the effects of early stress are not irreversible (19). Moreover, as the interventions in these studies show efficacy and move toward implementation at scale, they can incrementally reduce the effects of adversity on the health of those who are exposed to early life adversity. Nevertheless, among these studies, neurobiological measures have been used primarily with behavioral measures as indicators of children’s outcomes. Absent are theory-driven interventions that use translational neuroscience (20) to consider how family-based interventions might affect the underlying capabilities of care-givers, which, in turn, influence children’s outcomes. Such research is needed given the potential of two-generation models (i.e., those whose theory of change includes targeting specific processes in both parents and children) to improve outcomes for disadvantaged groups (7, 21).

Applying translational neuroscience to two-generation intervention models can move the field forward in four ways. First, it helps us identify underlying neural systems in caregivers that may mediate or moderate pathways among children’s early adversity, caregiving practices, and subsequent psychosocial adjustment. Second, it allows us to develop new intervention programs that can mitigate the effects of early adversity and determine whether changes in specific neurobiological systems in adults are the mechanisms by which interventions work. Third, it allows us to identify and change core capabilities (e.g., executive functioning) in adults that may underlie not only effective parenting but also family and personal goal setting, economic self-sufficiency, mental health, and other important outcomes. Fourth, it helps us identify common targets across different intervention strategies and programs, supporting the aggregation of knowledge in the field.

In the following section, we describe a scalable video coaching program designed to be used when children are infants and in early childhood, and that is based on a two-generational, translational neuroscience framework. We present an overview of the program and its distinguishing characteristics, describe its conceptual model, and discuss how the program is informed by an understanding of the core adult neurocognitive processes the intervention is designed to strengthen. We also address how the program can generate testable neuroscience hypotheses about caregivers’ capacities that take the bidirectional exchange between theory development and applied research beyond what could be accomplished by two-generation developmental science paradigms alone.

THE FILMING INTERACTIONS TO NURTURE DEVELOPMENT PROGRAM

Filming Interactions to Nurture Development (FIND) is a brief video coaching program for parents and other caregivers. FIND is designed to promote naturally occurring, developmentally supportive interactions between infants and young children and the significant adults in their lives. Drawing on emerging knowledge on the developmental neuroscience of early adversity (6), the intervention is rooted in microsocial interaction research at the Oregon Social Learning Center (22), as well as in the Marte Meo video coaching intervention, which has been implemented widely in Europe and elsewhere (23). FIND shares many features with attachment research that underscore the importance of reciprocal interactions (24, 25) and attachment-based interventions, such as the Video-Feedback Intervention to Promote Positive Parenting (26) and the Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-Up program (27), which use video coaching while enhancing developmentally supportive, responsive caregiving.

Like other two-generation video coaching programs, FIND targets interactions between children and their caregivers in the early years, when basic brain circuitry is being established for language, socioemotional, cognitive, and self-regulatory capacities. During these early years, the brain more than doubles in size, yet its maturation depends on experience. Developmental neuroscience suggests that experiences central to infants’ brain development are embedded in the relationship between infants and children and their caregivers; although infants are born genetically programmed to connect with caregivers, only within the context of attuned, reciprocal, and well-regulated interactions do genes have the possibility of being carried forth to promote sufficient brain growth (28).

Therefore, FIND focuses on enhancing patterns of infant–caregiver interaction known as “serve and return” interactions. From the perspective of behavioral research, these patterns comprise attuned, reciprocal, and well-regulated interactions, and from the perspective of developmental neuroscience, they promote optimal growth and development of the infant brain. The serve and return metaphor describes attentive, responsive care-giving in easy-to-understand terms for widespread dissemination. Children naturally serve when they initiate interaction through gaze, vocalization, and action; adults return the serve when they respond in developmentally supportive ways.

Consistent with meta-analyses that suggest that less is more when it comes to the length of video coaching interventions (29), FIND is brief, typically taking place over 10 weekly meetings that alternate between video recording sessions and coaching sessions. Between recording and coaching sessions, videos are edited (by a coach or editor) to show brief clips during which the caregiver and child are engaged in serve and return interactions. The edited film is designed to facilitate learning and optimize caregivers’ engagement; it features two still-frame photos (one at the beginning and one at the end) that flank three video clips. Each clip begins with a brief text description of the interaction and is viewed by the coach and the caregiver three times in immediate succession. During the second viewing, the coach provides a frame-by-frame analysis of the serve and return element. The video review is descriptive, but uses precise techniques and language that distinguish FIND from other video coaching programs. These techniques maximize the salience of the serve and return element to the caregiver, reduce reliance on professional and paraprofessional expertise, and facilitate fidelity of implementation. In addition, editing, coaching, and consultation can be done by one organization; alternatively, if an implementing organization lacks the resources or expertise to complete editing or consulting, these can be managed by a centralized FIND hub (typically based in a large social-service organization or university) where all videos can be uploaded to a server, edited by a centralized team, and returned to coaches in the field who may also receive consultation on the coaching process from the experts at the hub. This helps maximize the program’s scalability.

FIND’S CONCEPTUAL MODEL

The conceptual model for FIND specifies caregiver-based targets of intervention and outcomes in the caregiver and child (see Figure 1). It also details underlying neurocognitive capacities in the caregiver that are hypothesized to mediate the associations between targets and outcomes. Studies to evaluate the validity of this model through randomized clinical trials are under way.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Conceptual model for Filming Interactions to Nurture Development that depicts the associations among intervention targets, underlying neurocognitive capacities, and outcomes.

Note. In the outcomes listed, up arrows depict improvements in a domain and down arrows depict decreases in a domain.

TARGETS

The FIND intervention comprises five elements of serve and return, which are described in precise behavioral terms. An adult shares the child’s focus when he or she notices what the child is focused on (i.e., the child’s serve) and shows interest in that object, activity, or experience. The adult can then respond by supporting and encouraging or by naming. Supportive responses include offering help, offering comfort, or providing something the child needs. Encouragement consists of praise and acknowledgment. Naming occurs when the adult gives the child a word for what the child is seeing, doing, or feeling. Back and forth interaction is a natural extension of the serve and return process that takes place when the adult notices the serve, responds, and then waits for the child’s further initiations. This leads to a sustained, reciprocal interaction. The last element, endings and beginnings, happens when one back and forth interaction comes to an end and another begins.

OUTCOMES

Like other two-generational programs, FIND is hypothesized to decrease parenting-related stress and improve caregiving competence. However, the FIND program differs from more traditional interventions that teach parents what they should do differently or highlight areas they need to improve. In at-risk families, focusing on parenting deficits may evoke feelings of failure, thereby inducing or exacerbating mental health problems (e.g., symptoms of depression, substance use; 30). This may lead parents to disengage from or drop out of treatment, and it can have unintended negative effects, such as decreasing parents’ sense of competence. By contrast, consistent with other strengths-based models (31), FIND aims to increase caregivers’ beliefs that they are or can become good parents and that, despite any difficulties they experience, they have innate parenting capacities and know how to support their children.

We expect that when parents and caregivers take part in FIND, their parenting and well-being will improve, and that these improvements will co-occur with improvements in children’s outcomes across many domains, including more secure attachment, decreased problem behaviors, and more optimal early learning and school achievement. Responsive parenting has also been associated with improvements in children’s behavior (32). Thus, we expect that after participating in the FIND program, children will have lower rates of internalizing and externalizing behaviors. FIND is also designed to promote early learning and contribute to children’s early academic achievement (33). The emphasis on naming objects, people, and feelings that the child is oriented to is intended to directly improve children’s vocabulary development and increase their comprehension of verbal communication.

MEDIATORS

In the FIND conceptual model, targeted behavioral training is hypothesized to affect several cognitive capacities and associated neural substrates in caregivers. These in turn are thought to mediate a cascade of effects in the caregivers, the children, and the caregiver–child dyads. We also expect that FIND will modulate underlying capacities in caregivers, including executive function and mentalizing domains.

Executive function is related closely to emotion regulation (34), and both capacities are critical in developing and maintaining parenting practices (35). Moreover, cognitive control capacities are important for parents dealing with stressful contextual factors, such as low socioeconomic status and adverse events (36). Generally, low levels of maternal emotion and cognitive control capacity are associated with increased risk of maltreating children, while higher levels of maternal emotion and cognitive regulation are associated with more sensitive, involved parenting (37). Cognitive regulatory capacities are thought to underlie caregivers’ abilities to be perceptive, responsive, and flexible (38). Caregivers use these capacities extensively in interactions with their children to plan and flexibly change their behaviors when necessary, respond appropriately to children’s cues, and regulate their own emotions in the face of challenging behaviors by their children (38, 39).

Neuroscience research delineates the neural substrates of executive function and emotion regulation, pointing toward two complementary but interconnected neural systems: the ventral system and the dorsal system (see 40 for a review). In addition, researchers have begun to evaluate the neural circuitry of human parenting and parents’ responsiveness to infants’ affective cues (41, 42). The multimodal integration of data from behavioral and neuroscience research is central to the FIND conceptual model and may be necessary to advance the field beyond what could be accomplished with behavioral research alone. This is relevant for differentiating underlying neural substrates (e.g., underreactivity vs. overreactivity to children’s emotions) that produce identical behavior in caregivers (e.g., avoidance, child neglect).

With regard to behavioral and neuroscience research, care-giver behaviors targeted in the FIND intervention are hypothesized to modulate the following executive function and cognitive control capacities and their associated neural substrates:

  1. Attentional control: Caregivers are instructed to “notice” children’s cues and “share the children’s focus.” In doing so, caregivers practice deliberately, flexibly shifting attention toward the children in response to the children’s cues. The salience and attentional capture of children’s cues may be enhanced by reinforcement from the coach that emphasizes the importance of sensitivity to children’s cues. Positive interactions from successful sharing of attention are inherently rewarding and thus further reinforce heightened salience and attentional capture of infants’ and children’s cues.

  2. Self-monitoring: Through the FIND program, caregivers grow used to viewing themselves on videotape while interacting with their children. Caregivers are directed to reflect on their positive behavioral responses during the interaction, eliciting increased self-monitoring of their own behaviors during the interaction.

  3. Inhibitory control: Caregivers are instructed to wait for children’s cues (“serve”), allowing children to take the lead. After responding to children’s initial cues, caregivers are again instructed to wait for the following cue, allowing the children to reciprocate and facilitating an ongoing exchange with consecutive turns (“back and forth”). Waiting for children’s serve requires caregivers to practice inhibition while withholding prepotent response; such response inhibition may necessitate emotion regulation in the context of children’s distress.

Changes in each of these domains for parents are expected to be supported by underlying neural processes governing early stages of initial perceptual processing of their infant, and attentional engagement and in-depth cognitive evaluation of these stimuli. Neuroimaging studies indicate that several processes may come into play. First, increases in activity in adults’ limbic systems (from which emotions arise) may increase their perceptual sensitivity to emotional cues from the infants. Second, top-down cognitive control from areas of the brain involved in executive functioning (specifically the anterior cingulate cortex) may help parents regulate their own emotions more effectively, allowing increased ability to process infants’ cues and appraise their emotions (see 40 for a review).

FIND is also hypothesized to act via changes in theory of mind and reflective functioning, as caregivers come to perceive infant “serves” as cues. Both capabilities are domains of mentalizing, or the ability of an individual to hold the mind of others in mind, and attribute an underlying mental state and intentional stance to others’ behavior (43). Relevant neural substrates associated with mentalizing in relation to stimuli that signal intentions and intentional activity include regions of the temporoparietal junction.

CONCLUSIONS

The neurobiological effects of early adversity in infants and young children can be affected by changes in the caregiving environment. However, disparities in health and well-being continue at the societal level among groups exposed to high levels of adversity. Programs that use two-generation translational neuroscience frameworks, such as the FIND intervention we have described, can affect both science and policy. Specifically, they may hone interventions to be powerful yet cost effective and feasible to implement in settings with limited resources—taking the field beyond what could be accomplished with developmental science paradigms alone. Because the FIND intervention is being evaluated, we need to determine whether the conceptual model upon which it is built is empirically valid. As data from evaluation studies of FIND emerge, we also need to examine whether we can identify specific moderators of effectiveness in the individuals who take part in the intervention and in the contexts in which they exist. In doing so, the theory and practice of FIND can be adapted to increase the program’s impact and reach. In presenting the FIND program and its components, we hope to guide others as they develop two-generation interventions based on translational neuroscience.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge grant DA035763 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and grant HD075716 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, both to Philip A. Fisher.

References

  • 1.Levine S, Mody T. The long-term psychobiological consequences of intermittent postnatal separation in the squirrel monkey. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2003;27:83–89. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00011-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Shannon C, Champoux M, Suomi SJ. Rearing condition and plasma cortisol in rhesus monkey infants. American Journal of Primatology. 1998;46:311–321. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1998)46:4<311::AID-AJP3>3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Molecular Brain Research. 1993;18:195–200. doi: 10.1016/0169-328X(93)90189-V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Levine S. 1-Stress: An historical perspective. Techniques in the Behavioral and Neural Sciences. 2005;15:3–23. doi: 10.1016/S0921-0709(05)80004-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gunnar MR, Fisher PA. Bringing basic research on early experience and stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:651–677. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301:2252–2259. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Shonkoff JP, Fisher PA. Rethinking evidence-based practice and two-generation programs to create the future of early childhood policy. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:1635–1653. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Greenough WT, Black JE, Wallace CS. Experience and brain development. Child Development. 1987;58:539–559. doi: 10.2307/1130197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Smyke AT, Guthrie D. Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science. 2007;318:1937–1940. doi: 10.1126/science.1143921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Vanderwert RE, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. Normalization of EEG activity among previously institutionalized children placed into foster care: A 12-year follow-up of the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016;17:68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.201512.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bernard K, Dozier M, Bick J, Gordon MK. Intervening to enhance cortisol regulation among children at risk for neglect: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Development and Psychopathology. 2015;27:829–841. doi: 10.1017/S095457941400073x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M, Gunnar MR, Burraston BO. Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:892–905. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Brotman LM, Gouley KK, Huang KY, Kamboukos D, Fratto C, Pine DS. Effects of a psychosocial family-based preventive intervention on cortisol response to a social challenge in preschoolers at high risk for antisocial behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1172–1179. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bruce J, McDermott JM, Fisher PA, Fox NA. Using behavioral and electrophysiological measures to assess the effects of a preventive intervention: A preliminary study with preschool-aged foster children. Prevention Science. 2009;10:129–140. doi: 10.1007/s11121-008-0115-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Stevens C, Harn B, Chard DJ, Currin J, Parisi D, Neville H. Examining the role of attention and instruction in at-risk kindergarteners: Electrophysiological measures of selective auditory attention before and after an early literacy intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2013;46:73–86. doi: 10.1177/0022219411417877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science. 2007;318:1387–1388. doi: 10.1037/a0024657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Riggs NR, Greenberg MT, Kusché CA, Pentz MA. The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary school students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science. 2006;7:91–102. doi: 10.1007/s11121-005-0022-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Miller GE, Brody GH, Yu T, Chen E. A family-oriented psychosocial intervention reduces inflammation in low-SES African American youth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111:11287–11292. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1406578111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bruce J, Gunnar MR, Pears KC, Fisher PA. Early adverse care, stress neurobiology, and prevention science: Lessons learned. Prevention Science. 2013;14:247–256. doi: 10.1007/s11121-012-0354-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bryck RL, Fisher PA. Training the brain: Practical applications of neural plasticity from the intersection of cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology, and prevention science. American Psychologist. 2012;67:87–100. doi: 10.1037/a0024657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ramey CT, Ramey SL. Early intervention and early experience. American Psychologist. 1998;53:109. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Patterson GR, Reid JB, Dishion TJ. Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Vik K, Rohde R. Tiny moments of great importance: The MarteMeo method applied in the context of early mother–infant interaction and postnatal depression. Utilizing Daniel Stern’s theory of ‘schemas of being with’ in understanding empirical findings and developing a stringent Marte Meo methodology. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;19:77–89. doi: 10.1177/1359104512468286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Beebe B, Lachmann FM. The contribution of mother–infant mutual influence to the origins of self- and object representations. Psychoanalytic Psychology. 1988;5:305–337. doi: 10.1037/0736-9735.5.4.305. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tronick EZ, Cohn JF. Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Development. 1989;60:85–92. doi: 10.2307/1131074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, editors. Promoting positive parenting: An attachment-based intervention. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Dozier M, Peloso E, Lindhiem O, Gordon MK, Manni M, Sepulveda S, … Levine S. Developing evidence-based interventions for foster children: An example of a randomized clinical trial with infants and toddlers. Journal of Social Issues. 2006;62:767–785. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2006.00486.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Siegel DJ. Toward an interpersonal neurobiology of the developing mind: Attachment relationships, mindsight, and neural integration. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2001;22:67–94. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(200101/04)22:1<67::AID-IMHJ3>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Fukkink RG. Video feedback in widescreen: A meta-analysis of family programs. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28:904–916. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lapierre S. Mothering in the context of domestic violence: The pervasiveness of a deficit model of mothering. Child & Family Social Work. 2008;13:454–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2206.2008.00563.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kemp SP, Marcenko MO, Lyons SJ, Kruzich JM. Strength-based practice and parental engagement in child welfare services: An empirical examination. Children and Youth Services Review. 2014;47:27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2013.11.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Doan SN, Fuller-Rowell TE, Evans GW. Cumulative risk and adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems: The mediating roles of maternal responsiveness and self-regulation. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48:1529–1539. doi: 10.1037/a0027815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mistry RS, Benner AD, Biesanz JC, Clark SL, Howes C. Family and social risk, and parental investments during the early childhood years as predictors of low-income children’s school readiness outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2010;25:432–449. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Zelazo PD, Qu L, Kesek AC. Hot executive function: Emotion and the development of cognitive control. In: Calkins SD, Bell MA, editors. Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 97–111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Deater-Deckard K, Wang Z, Chen N, Bell MA. Maternal executive function, harsh parenting, and child conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53:1084–1091. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02582.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Deater-Deckard K. Family matters: Intergenerational and interpersonal processes of executive function and attentive behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2014;23:230–236. doi: 10.1177/0963721414531597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Crandall A, Deater-Deckard K, Riley AW. Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review. 2015;36:105–126. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kienhuis M, Rogers S, Giallo R, Matthews J, Treyvaud K. A proposed model for the impact of parental fatigue on parenting adaptability and child development. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 2010;28:392–402. doi: 10.1080/02646830903487383. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Barrett J, Fleming AS. Annual research review: All mothers are not created equal: Neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:368–397. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02306.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dennis TA, O’Toole LJ, DeCicco M. Emotion regulation from the perspective of developmental neuroscience: What, where, when and why. In: Barrett KC, Fox NA, Morgan GA, Fidler DJ, Daunhauer LA, editors. Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development: New directions and international perspectives. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2013. pp. 135–172. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Maupin AN, Hayes NJ, Mayes LC, Rutherford HJV. The application of electroencephalography to investigate the neural bases of parenting: A review. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2015;15:9–23. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2015.992735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Swain JE, Lorberbaum JP, Kose S, Strathearn L. Brain basis of early parent–infant interactions: Psychology, physiology, and in vivo functional neuroimaging studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:262–287. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01731.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist EL, Target M. Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. London, UK: H. Karnac (Books); 2006. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES