Abstract
This review brings together current knowledge from tract tracing studies to update and reconsider those limbic connections initially highlighted by Papez for their presumed role in emotion. These connections link hippocampal and parahippocampal regions with the mammillary bodies, the anterior thalamic nuclei, and the cingulate gyrus, all structures now strongly implicated in memory functions. An additional goal of this review is to describe the routes taken by the various connections within this network. The original descriptions of these limbic connections saw their interconnecting pathways forming a serial circuit that began and finished in the hippocampal formation. It is now clear that with the exception of the mammillary bodies, these various sites are multiply interconnected with each other, including many reciprocal connections. In addition, these same connections are topographically organised, creating further subsystems. This complex pattern of connectivity helps explain the difficulty of interpreting the functional outcome of damage to any individual site within the network. For these same reasons, Papez’s initial concept of a loop beginning and ending in the hippocampal formation needs to be seen as a much more complex system of hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate connections. The functions of these multiple interactions might be better viewed as principally providing efferent information from the posterior medial temporal lobe. Both a subcortical diencephalic route (via the fornix) and a cortical cingulate route (via retrosplenial cortex) can be distinguished. These routes provide indirect pathways for hippocampal interactions with prefrontal cortex, with the preponderance of both sets of connections arising from the more posterior hippocampal regions. These multi-stage connections complement the direct hippocampal projections to prefrontal cortex, which principally arise from the anterior hippocampus, thereby creating longitudinal functional differences along the anterior–posterior plane of the hippocampus.
Keywords: Cingulate cortex, cingulum, fornix, hippocampus, mammillary bodies, Papez circuit, parahippocampal cortex, review, subiculum, thalamus
The limbic cortex, the limbic system, and the circuit of Papez
Just as the Latin word for border or edge (‘limbus’) has given us the term ‘limbo’ (that region bordering hell), so it has given us the term ‘limbic cortex’ for the cortex bordering the neocortical mantle (Broca, 1878; Da Silva et al., 1990; Pessoa and Hof, 2015). The ‘grand lobe limbique’ of Broca (1878) included the parahippocampal gyri, the underlying hippocampus, as well as the cingulate and subcallosal gyri. The subsequent notion that these structures and their interconnections play a vital role in emotion is often traced back to the work of James Papez. In ‘A proposed mechanism of emotion’, published 80 years ago, Papez (1937) tackled the daunting task of bringing together behavioural and anatomical knowledge to formulate a neuroscientific model of our emotions. With more than 3000 citations (Google Scholar), the remarkable impact of Papez’s ideas continues (Pessoa and Hof, 2015).
At the heart of Papez’s model was a set of serial connections linking the hippocampus with the hypothalamus, thalamus, cingulate cortex, and back again to the hippocampus (Figure 1). The resulting circuit was thought to support and sustain emotions. While this model involved the limbic cortex of Broca, it also included key, subcortical connections within the diencephalon. Building on Papez’s ideas, Paul MacLean (1949, 1952) introduced the term ‘limbic system’. This term referred to the set of structures highlighted by Papez, but included other sites, such as the amygdala. It is MacLean’s concept of a ‘limbic system’ that has stuck, despite its many shortcomings (Isaacson, 1992; Kötter and Meyer, 1992; Roxo et al., 2011). The significance of this concept is seen in the way that limbic system connections are now regarded as vital for emotion, memory, personality, and navigation. At the same time, disruptions to these connections have been linked with numerous disorders including schizophrenia, autism, depression, obsessive-compulsive disorders, amnesia, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease (Aggleton and Brown, 1999; Dalgleish, 2004; Small et al., 2011).
The purpose of this review is to re-examine those connections initially described by Papez. These core limbic connections, which are typically placed within a larger limbic system (Catani et al., 2013; Livingston and Escobar, 1971; Rolls, 2015), have particular importance for memory and spatial functions (Aggleton and Brown, 1999; Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012; Rolls, 2015; Vann et al., 2009a). Over time, this particular set of connections has been given a variety of names including Papez circuit (Teuber, 1955; Van der Horst, 1951), the Delay and Brion circuit (after Delay and Brion, 1969), the medial limbic system (Livingston and Escobar, 1971), the extended hippocampal system (Aggleton and Brown, 1999), the posterior medial temporal system (Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012), the hippocampal–diencephalic network and the parahippocampal–retrosplenial network (Catani et al., 2013), and the hippocampal limbic system (Rolls, 2015). None of these titles is ideal so we will use the term ‘hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network’, which reflects the key components but does not give a special weighting to just one structure.
This set of limbic connections is often thought to begin in the hippocampus (Shah et al., 2012). This notion reflects Papez’s (1937) original proposal that
“The central emotive process of cortical origin may then be conceived as being built up in the hippocampal formation and as being transferred to the mammillary body and thence through the anterior thalamic nuclei to the cortex of the gyrus cingula.” (p. 91)
Papez’s ideas were further strengthened when later tract tracing studies in animals confirmed that the direct hippocampal projections to the mammillary bodies (via the fornix) are solely efferent, as are the projections from the mammillary bodies to the anterior thalamic nuclei (via the mammillothalamic tract). These discoveries encouraged the idea of a return hippocampal loop that sequentially involved the diencephalon and cingulate cortex.
The following sections describe this hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network in rat, macaque (rhesus and cynomolgus monkeys), and human brains. Where possible, the routes taken by the various connections are described to help explain the effects of tract disconnections. It emerges that while the connections comprising Papez original ‘circuit’ exist as substantial pathways, there are also additional, parallel connections, as well as return projections. Together, these connections create a more complex limbic network than that often described. It should finally be added that the individual structures within this network all have numerous, additional connections beyond these limbic pathways, but these extra connections are not the focus of this review.
The rat hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network
Throughout this review, the term ‘hippocampus’ includes the subiculum. Adjacent to the subiculum, the postsubiculum is treated as a distinct area (Van Groen and Wyss, 1990c), even though it can be regarded as part of the presubiculum (Van Strien et al., 2009). The rat hippocampus has a ventral (‘temporal’) and dorsal (‘septal’) division (Figure 2). The rat ventral and dorsal hippocampus are, respectively, homologous with the primate anterior and posterior hippocampus (Strange et al., 2014). In addition to its long axis, the hippocampus has a medial–lateral axis, in which the ‘proximal’ subiculum borders CA1 while the ‘distal’ subiculum borders the presubiculum (Figure 2). The ‘parahippocampal region’ consists of the presubiculum, postsubiculum, parasubiculum, entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex (areas 35 and 36), as well as areas TH and TF (designated postrhinal cortex in the rat; Witter and Wouterlood, 2002). The connections within this network will be described in the sequence given by Papez (1937), but with new additions along the way.
Hippocampus to mammillary bodies
These projections do not arise from the hippocampal CA fields. Rather, it is the subiculum, along with the presubiculum and postsubiculum, that provides the direct hippocampal efferents to the mammillary bodies (Allen and Hopkins, 1989; Meibach and Siegel, 1975; Swanson and Cowan, 1977; Wright et al., 2010). These subicular efferents all join the fornix before descending in the postcommissural fornix (i.e. the division of the fornix that descends behind the anterior commissure), with some fibres crossing in the columns of the fornix to reach the mammillary bodies in the opposite hemisphere.
The hippocampal projections to the mammillary bodies are topographically organised. The inputs to the medial mammillary nucleus arise from the mid-cell layer across the proximal–distal plane of the subiculum (Christiansen et al., 2016b; Naber and Witter, 1998), while the projections to the lateral mammillary nucleus arise from the postsubiculum and presubiculum (Van Groen and Wyss, 1990b, 1990c). Projections to the posterior mammillary nucleus also arise from the presubiculum (Meibach and Siegel, 1977). While the dorsal subiculum projects to dorsal parts of the medial mammillary nucleus, the ventral subiculum projects to ventral parts of the same nucleus, that is, there is a horizontal topography across the medial mammillary nucleus with respect to its hippocampal (subicular) inputs (Hopkins, 2005; Meibach and Siegel, 1975). There are, however, no direct return projections from the mammillary bodies to the hippocampus.
Mammillary bodies to the anterior thalamic nuclei
The next step in this core limbic subsystem consists of the unidirectional projections from the mammillary bodies to the anterior thalamic nuclei, via the mammillothalamic tract (Figure 3). There is little evidence for interneurons in the rat mammillary bodies (Allen and Hopkins, 1988; Seki and Zyo, 1984) and it is likely that almost every mammillary body neuron contributes to this thalamic projection (Powell et al., 1957). Few, if any, mammillothalamic projections reach the laterodorsal thalamic nucleus. Thus, even though this thalamic nucleus shares many connections with the anterior thalamic nuclei, it has a separate status within this network.
The mammillary body projections consist of ipsilateral efferents from the medial mammillary nucleus to the anteroventral and anteromedial thalamic nuclei, contrasting with bilateral efferents from the lateral mammillary nucleus to the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus (Figure 3). The most midline portion of the mammillary bodies (pars medianus) projects to the most midline part of the anterior thalamic nuclei (the interoanteromedial nucleus). In general, more medial parts of the medial mammillary nucleus terminate in the anteromedial thalamic nucleus, while more lateral parts of the medial mammillary nucleus terminate in the anteroventral thalamic nucleus (Shibata, 1992). In addition, the posterior mammillary nucleus projects to a dorsal medial part of the anteroventral thalamic nucleus (Shibata, 1992). Consequently, the anterior thalamic projections from the mammillary bodies are organised in a plane largely orthogonal to the pattern of horizontal mammillary terminations from the subiculum (Hopkins, 2005). One result is that both ventral and dorsal subicular inputs to the mammillary bodies may indirectly influence the same anterior thalamic area.
Contrary to initial depictions of this limbic circuitry (Figure 1), there are dense, direct projections to the anterior thalamic nuclei from the subiculum, presubiculum, postsubiculum, and parasubiculum (Meibach and Siegel, 1977; Swanson and Cowan, 1977; Van Groen and Wyss, 1990b, 1990c). The subiculum efferents to the anteromedial nucleus rely on the fornix (Figure 3), as do the large majority of subiculum inputs to the anteroventral nucleus (Dillingham et al., 2015a). Some projections from the distal subiculum and presubiculum, however, take a parallel (nonfornical) route via the internal capsule before terminating in the dorsolateral part of the anteroventral nucleus (Dillingham et al., 2015a). Likewise, many of the hippocampal efferents to the anterodorsal nucleus, which predominantly arise from the postsubiculum and parasubiculum, project via the internal capsule (Dillingham et al., 2015a; Van Groen and Wyss, 1990b, 1990c).
The hippocampal cells giving rise to the anterior thalamic nuclei and mammillary body projections are largely segregated by their respective depths within the subiculum (Christiansen et al., 2016b; Wright et al., 2010). In addition, the proximal subiculum preferentially projects to the anteromedial nucleus, while the distal subiculum and the adjacent presubiculum preferentially project to the anteroventral thalamic nucleus (Christiansen et al., 2016b; Naber and Witter, 1998; Van Groen and Wyss, 1990b; Wright et al., 2013). These direct anterior thalamic inputs predominantly arise from the dorsal hippocampus. In contrast, the mammillary body inputs arise from both the dorsal and ventral hippocampus (Christiansen et al., 2016b).
Anterior thalamic nuclei to cingulate cortex
The remaining connections, which Papez regarded as unidirectional (Figure 1), are now all known to be reciprocal (Figure 4). These bidirectional connections are found between the anterior thalamic nuclei and the cingulate cortices, between the cingulate cortices and the parahippocampal region, and between the parahippocampal region and the hippocampus. In addition, there are bidirectional connections between the anterior thalamic nuclei and both the parahippocampal region and the hippocampus (Shibata, 1993). This pattern of reciprocity adds greater complexity to Papez’s initial concept of a serial circuit linking these limbic sites (Figure 4).
The first reciprocal connections to be described are between the anterior thalamic nuclei and the cingulate cortex. The rat cingulate cortex contains two major divisions, the anterior cingulate cortex (principally composed of area 24) and the posterior cingulate or, more accurately, the retrosplenial cortex (areas 29 and 30). (The term retrosplenial is more accurate as the rat brain lacks posterior cingulate areas 23 and 31, which are present in primate brains.) While multiple designations exist for the subregions within retrosplenial cortex (see Jones and Witter, 2007), we have divided granular area 29 into subregions Rga and Rgb, while the dysgranular area 30 is designated Rdg (see Van Groen and Wyss, 1990a, 1992, 2003). Within the cingulate cortex, the retrosplenial cortex has the more extensive interconnections with the anterior thalamic nuclei (Shibata, 1993), as well as appreciably denser connections with hippocampal and parahippocampal regions.
Only restricted parts of the anteromedial nucleus project to the anterior cingulate cortex (Shibata, 1993), with return projections from the same cortical area terminating in the anteromedial and anteroventral thalamic nuclei (Shibata and Naito, 2005; Wright et al., 2013). In addition, reciprocal connections with prelimbic cortex are essentially restricted to the anteromedial nucleus (Mathiasen et al., 2017; Shibata and Naito, 2005). In contrast, almost all parts of the anterior thalamic nuclei appear to project to the retrosplenial cortex (areas 29, 30), with topographical associations between a particular thalamic nucleus (and subregion) and a particular retrosplenial region (Shibata, 1993, 1998; Shibata and Kato, 1993; Van Groen and Wyss, 1990a, 1992, 2003). Both Rga and Rgb are reciprocally connected with the anteroventral nucleus while the dysgranular cortex (Rdg) has reciprocal connections with the anteromedial nucleus. Meanwhile, the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus projects to Rga and Rgb, receiving light return inputs from Rgb (Van Groen and Wyss, 1990a, 2003). The projections from the anteroventral nucleus principally terminate in layer I of Rgb while those from the anterodorsal nucleus terminate in deep II/III, as well as layer I of Rgb (Shibata, 1993; Van Groen and Wyss, 2003). The projections from the anteromedial nucleus principally terminate in layers I and V of Rdg (Shibata, 1993; Van Groen and Wyss, 1992).
The direct outputs from the anterior thalamic nuclei to the cingulate cortex are almost entirely ipsilateral, with just a small population of anteroventral cells appearing to cross to the contralateral retrosplenial cortex (Mathiasen et al., 2017). The route taken by the anterior thalamic projections often involves the cingulum bundle (Domesick, 1970), with many anterior thalamic fibres passing rostrally and then dorsally (through the striatum) before joining the bundle. Other anterior thalamic fibres emerge laterally from the anterior thalamic nuclei to reach the internal capsule, then turn dorsally to cross through the corpus callosum, and so join the cingulum more directly. Meanwhile, the efferents from the retrosplenial cortex to the anterior thalamic nuclei, which arise from layer VI (Mathiasen et al., 2017; Sripanidkulchai and Wyss 1987), reach both the ipsilateral and contralateral anterior thalamic nuclei (Mathiasen et al., 2017). The more direct route is favoured by these retrosplenial projections, that is, around the lateral ventricle, briefly joining the internal capsule, before cutting across the dorsal thalamus to reach the anterior thalamic nuclei (Shibata, 1998; Van Groen and Wyss, 1992). A small number of fibres from Rga and Rgb may reach the anterior thalamic nuclei via the fornix (Shibata, 1998).
Cingulate cortex to the parahippocampal region and hippocampus
The retrosplenial cortex has many projections to the parahippocampal region (Figure 4), thereby completing the notional circuit (Jones and Witter, 2007; Sugar et al., 2011). Both the granular and dysgranular retrosplenial cortices densely innervate the postsubiculum and presubiculum, as well as projecting to the medial and lateral entorhinal cortices (Jones and Witter, 2007). These connections involve the cingulum. There are also a few direct retrosplenial projections to the subiculum, which arise from Rgb (Sugar et al., 2011). In contrast, the anterior cingulate cortex has more restricted projections, which terminate in the perirhinal cortex and lateral entorhinal cortex (Jones and Witter, 2007). Some of these anterior cingulate efferents do not join the cingulum (Jones and Witter, 2007).
The presubiculum and postsubiculum have dense projections to the entorhinal cortices (Van Groen and Wyss, 1990b, 1990c), thereby completing an additional pathway back to the hippocampus. Entorhinal projections terminate in either the dentate gyrus and CA3 (via the perforant pathway) or CA1 and the subiculum (via the temporoammonic pathway). These connections (Figure 5) are largely segregated by the lamina of their origin within the entorhinal cortex (layer II to the dentate gyrus and CA3, layer III to CA1 and subiculum). Many reviews have detailed the numerous parahippocampal–hippocampal interconnections (see Furtak et al., 2007; Van Strien et al., 2009).
Completing the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network
While the core connections described by Papez (Figure 1) exist in the rat, many other connections add to its complexity. Some projections seemingly bypass one or more stages (Figures 3–6). A striking example, already described, concerns the dense, direct projections from the subiculum to the anterior thalamic nuclei. Other examples include the direct projections from the anterior thalamic nuclei to caudal hippocampal (subiculum) and parahippocampal areas. Dense projections from both the anteroventral and anterodorsal thalamic nuclei focus on the presubiculum, parasubiculum, and postsubiculum, with the anteroventral nucleus also innervating the caudal subiculum (Shibata, 1993). These projections are thought to take a very similar route, passing forward and then upwards from the thalamus to join the cingulum, before travelling caudally, while some of those from the anterodorsal nucleus may take a direct route that does not involve the cingulum (Shibata, 1993; Shibata and Kato, 1993; Van Groen and Wyss, 1990b, 1990c). Meanwhile, the anteromedial nucleus has light projections to the ventral subiculum, but more appreciable projections to the perirhinal and entorhinal (medial and lateral) cortices (Shibata, 1993). The anterodorsal thalamic nucleus also projects to the entorhinal cortices (Shibata, 1993). Other connections include efferents from the medial entorhinal cortex to the anteroventral thalamic nucleus (Shibata, 1996), which involve the internal capsule, that is, they take a nonfornical route (Figure 3).
As already noted, there are connections that project in the opposite direction to that portrayed in depictions of Papez’s original circuit. One example, already discussed, concerns the dense projections from the cingulate cortices to the anterior thalamic nuclei (Figures 4 and 6). The significance of these projections is highlighted by a viral tracing study showing that a major pathway from the retrosplenial cortex to the dorsal hippocampus is via the anterior thalamic nuclei (Prasad and Chudasama, 2013). There are also many projections from the subiculum and parahippocampal regions to the retrosplenial cortices. It is principally the distal subiculum that projects directly to area 29 (Rga and Rgb; Honda and Ishizuka, 2015), where fibres terminate in layers I, II, and III. The postsubiculum also projects to Rgb, with terminations in layers I and III–V (Van Groen and Wyss, 2003). There are also light projections from CA1 to area 29 (Jones and Witter, 2007). Finally, there is a very light, direct pathway from the granular retrosplenial cortex to the medial mammillary bodies, which appears to join the postcommissural fornix (Van Groen and Wyss, 2003).
Figure 6 depicts a simplified, but updated, hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network for the rat. Arguably, the most striking feature is how almost all the structures project to more than one site within the network. The mammillary bodies provide the sole exception, as they only project to the anterior thalamic nuclei (Figure 6). An integral feature is how the various connections within the circuit are topographically organised, creating parallel pathways that presumably reflect multiple functions (Aggleton et al., 2010; Vann and Aggleton, 2002; Vertes et al., 2004).
The monkey hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network
Hippocampus to mammillary bodies
The network connections in macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta and Macaca fascicularis) are very similar to those described for the rat brain (Figures 7 and 8). Once again, the hippocampal projections to the mammillary bodies originate from the subiculum (Aggleton et al., 2005). These subicular projections (Figure 7), which arise from pyramidal cells, join the body of the fornix and then descend in the postcommissural fornix, where they make up approximately one half of its fibres (Powell et al., 1957). The neurons projecting to the medial mammillary nucleus are most numerous in the distal and posterior subiculum (Christiansen et al., 2016b). As in the rat, there is evidence of a horizontal topography in their terminations, as the anterior subiculum projects to more ventral aspects of the medial mammillary nucleus while the posterior subiculum projects more dorsally (Aggleton et al., 2005). In addition, light projections from the presubiculum reach the medial and lateral mammillary nuclei, while the entorhinal cortex projects to the medial mammillary nucleus (Aggleton et al., 2005).
Mammillary bodies to the anterior thalamic nuclei
The very dense mammillary body projections to the anterior thalamus are organised such that neurons in the dorsal medial mammillary nucleus project to the anteromedial thalamic nucleus while the remainder of the medial mammillary nucleus projects to the anteroventral thalamic nucleus (Vann et al., 2007; Xiao and Barbas, 2002). The lateral mammillary nucleus projects to the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus, although it may also provide light inputs to other anterior thalamic nuclei (Vann et al., 2007). While the lateral mammillary inputs to the anterodorsal nucleus are bilateral, the medial mammillary projections to the other anterior thalamic nuclei remain ipsilateral. It is again thought that almost every neuron in the primate mammillary bodies contributes to the anterior thalamic projections (Powell et al., 1957; Xiao and Barbas, 2002).
As in the rat, the indirect route from the hippocampus to the anterior thalamic nuclei via the mammillary bodies is reinforced by numerous, direct projections from the hippocampus to the anteromedial and anteroventral thalamic nuclei, with lighter inputs to the anterodorsal nucleus (Figure 7). These hippocampal projections arise from the deepest cell layer of the subiculum, ensuring that they are segregated from the hippocampal projections to the mammillary bodies, which are found in the middle layer (Aggleton et al., 1986; Christiansen et al., 2016b). The projections to the anterior thalamus predominantly arise from the distal subiculum before joining the fornix (Christiansen et al., 2016b). While the hippocampal inputs to the anteromedial thalamic nucleus are bilateral, those to the anteroventral nucleus and the anterodorsal nucleus essentially remain ipsilateral (Aggleton et al., 1986). The presubiculum provides light inputs to the anteroventral and anteromedial thalamic nuclei, with even lighter inputs arising from the parasubiculum (Saunders et al., 2005; Xiao and Barbas, 2002). In addition, there are light projections from the entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex to the anterior thalamic nuclei (especially to the anteroventral nucleus), some of which join the fornix (Saunders et al., 2005; Xiao and Barbas, 2002).
Anterior thalamic nuclei to cingulate gyrus
The dense anterior thalamic projections to the cingulate cortices (Figure 8) are most concentrated in the posterior cingulate region (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980; Shibata and Yukie, 2009; Vogt et al., 1979, 1987). This region in the macaque comprises posterior cingulate areas 23 and 31, as well as retrosplenial areas 29 and 30. Both the anteroventral and anteromedial nuclei project to ventral area 23, with lighter projections to dorsal area 23 (Morris et al., 1999; Shibata and Yukie, 2009; Vogt et al., 1987). These projections to area 23 principally arise from the anteromedial nucleus (Vogt et al., 1987). Meanwhile, the anteroventral nucleus provides most of the projections to area 30, while those to area 29 are from both the anteroventral and anterodorsal thalamic nuclei (Shibata and Yukie, 2009; Vogt et al., 1987).
There are dense, return projections from layer VI of the posterior cingulate region to the anterior thalamic nuclei (Aggleton et al., 2014). Area 23 projects to both the anteromedial and anteroventral thalamic nuclei, alongside lighter projections from area 31 to the same thalamic nuclei (Aggleton et al., 2014; Shibata and Yukie, 2009). The retrosplenial efferents from areas 29 and 30 principally target the anteroventral thalamic nucleus, with lighter inputs to the anteromedial nucleus. It is presumed, but not certain, that area 29 also projects to the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus (Shibata and Yukie, 2009). The inputs to the anteromedial nucleus from areas from 23 and 30 include crossed connections from the other hemisphere (Aggleton et al., 2014; Shibata and Yukie, 2009).
The anterior thalamic–posterior cingulate connections have a reciprocal organisation (Figure 8). While areas 29 and 30 especially interact with the anteroventral nucleus, the anteromedial nucleus is especially connected with area 23 (and areas 24, 25, and 32). Many of the anterior thalamic projections to the posterior cingulate region leave the thalamus laterally, before passing around the caudate nucleus in the internal capsule to join and cross the cingulum. The reciprocal projections from the posterior cingulate region to the thalamus appear to take essentially the same route (Mufson and Pandya, 1984).
The anterior cingulate cortex receives fewer inputs from the anterior thalamic nuclei than its posterior counterpart (Vertes et al., 2015; Vogt et al., 1987). Area 24, which forms much of this region, receives light inputs from the anteromedial nucleus (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980; Shibata and Yukie, 2009; Vogt et al., 1987). The same thalamic nucleus also gives rise to modest projections to medial frontal areas 32 and 25, which are also part of the anterior cingulate region (Shibata and Yukie, 2009; Vogt et al., 1987). These thalamic projections to anterior areas 24, 25, and 32 leave the thalamus rostrally to pass the anterior limb of the internal capsule before joining and crossing the cingulum (Mufson and Pandya, 1984). Other anterior thalamic projections to area 24 cross the dorsal thalamus to skirt around the caudate nucleus before turning medially to join and cross the cingulum. The projections to areas 25 and 32 are of additional note as these same areas receive many of the direct hippocampal (CA1, subiculum) projections to prefrontal cortex (Aggleton et al., 2015).
There are also return projections from the anterior cingulate region to the anterior thalamic nuclei (Figure 8). While the densest thalamic projections from area 24 terminate in the medial dorsal thalamic nucleus, there are some projections to the anteromedial nucleus (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980; Shibata and Yukie, 2009). Likewise, areas 25 and 32 project to the anteromedial thalamic nucleus (Xiao and Barbas, 2002), but for all three of these anterior cingulate areas (24, 25, and 32) it is the reciprocal connections with the medial dorsal thalamic nucleus that are the most dense (Shibata and Yukie, 2009).
Cingulate cortex to the parahippocampal region and hippocampus
Numerous inputs arise from across retrosplenial areas 29 and 30, as well as ventral 23 in the posterior cingulate region, to reach the parahippocampal region (Yukie and Shibata, 2009). Many of these projections terminate in the presubiculum and parasubiculum, with relatively few fibres innervating the subiculum (Kobayashi and Amaral, 2007; Morris et al., 1999). The presubiculum projects to the entorhinal cortex, thereby completing the circuit (Figure 8). In addition, there are dense, direct projections from the retrosplenial cortex and ventral area 23 to entorhinal cortex, as well as to areas TH and TF of the parahippocampal region (Insausti et al., 1987a; Kobayashi and Amaral, 2007; Pandya et al., 1981; Suzuki and Amaral, 1994). Dorsal area 23 has more restricted projections, focussed on area TF (Kobayashi and Amaral, 2007).
The anterior cingulate region (areas 24, 25, and 32) has light, reciprocal connections with much of the parahippocampal region (Insausti et al., 1987a; Pandya et al., 1981; Suzuki and Amaral, 1994; Yukie and Shibata, 2009). There are, for example, light interconnections between the perirhinal cortex and ventral area 24, as well as with area 32. In addition, both the entorhinal cortex and areas TH/TF have modest, reciprocal connections (not depicted in figures) with areas 24, 25, and 32 (Yukie and Shibata, 2009).
Completing the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network
To help clarify the situation, Figure 9 depicts a simplified version of the updated hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network, highlighting its principal connections. Based on the pattern of thalamic connections, it can be seen that within the cingulate gyrus, the retrosplenial cortex forms a particularly important link in this limbic subsystem (Figures 8 and 9). Consistent with this distinction, the direct projections from the hippocampus to the cingulate gyrus are largely restricted to the retrosplenial cortex. Dense inputs to the retrosplenial cortex arise from the subiculum, as well as from the presubiculum, and parasubiculum, (Aggleton et al., 2015; Kobayashi and Amaral, 2003; Morris et al., 1999; Parvizi et al., 2006; Vogt et al., 1987). The subiculum preferentially innervates area 29 (layers I and III), with less dense projections reaching area 30 (layer III; Aggleton et al., 2012; Kobayashi and Amaral, 2003). The anterior subiculum targets more ventral retrosplenial areas while the posterior subiculum has denser projections to mid and dorsal retrosplenial cortex, as well as a light input to area 23 (layer III). This pattern is matched by the return projections from retrosplenial cortex, for example, the most ventral parts of 29/30 project to the anterior hippocampus (presubiculum; Kobayashi and Amaral, 2007). In contrast, there are only very limited subiculum inputs to area 24 in the anterior cingulate region, although there are more inputs from the subiculum to areas 25 and 32, which reach these sites via the fornix (Aggleton et al., 2015).
The retrosplenial cortex also receives direct inputs from the parahippocampal region. Areas TH and TF provide appreciable projections to areas 29 and 30, with lighter efferents to area 23 (Kobayashi and Amaral, 2003; Lavenex et al., 2002; Morris et al., 1999). Additional retrosplenial inputs arise from the caudal entorhinal cortex, which focus on area 29 (Aggleton et al., 2012; Kobayashi and Amaral 2003; Morris et al., 1999). A feature of the subiculum projections to the retrosplenial cortex is that they do not join the cingulum, rather they cross directly through the presubiculum to reach the posterior cingulate cortices directly (Aggleton et al., 2012).
As in other species, the anterior thalamic nuclei have direct projections to the hippocampal formation (Amaral and Cowan, 1980). These light thalamic projections, which join the caudal cingulum (Mufson and Pandya, 1984), arise from all three major anterior thalamic nuclei and terminate in the region of the subiculum (Amaral and Cowan, 1980). The anterior thalamic nuclei do not, however, appear to project directly to the entorhinal cortex (Insausti et al., 1987b).
Monkey versus rat
Although the core connections in the monkey hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network are, in many respects, very similar to those in rodents, there are some differences. Unsurprisingly, most of these differences reflect the connections of the cingulate cortices. These changes partly arise from the additional areas present in the primate brain (areas 23 and 31), while other areas (25 and 32) do not have precise counterparts in the rat brain (Vogt and Paxinos, 2014). Other challenges arise from the frequent failure to distinguish a midcingulate cortex in rodents which is evident in primates (Vogt, 2009) and can be identified in rodents (Vogt and Paxinos, 2014). Nevertheless, in both rats and macaque monkeys, it is the retrosplenial cortex that forms the principal cingulate node within the hippocampal limbic network.
In the rat brain, there is a clear cingulate–thalamic demarcation as the retrosplenial cortex (areas 29 and 30) is interconnected with the anterior thalamic nuclei but not with the medial dorsal thalamic nucleus (Van Groen and Wyss, 1990a, 1992, 2003). In the monkey, this same distinction remains but is not so marked, as the retrosplenial cortex now has light interconnections with the medial dorsal thalamic nucleus (Aggleton et al., 2014; Vogt et al., 1987). There are also additional connections involving the monkey posterior cingulate region, area 23, which is more clearly connected with both the anterior thalamic nuclei and the medial dorsal nucleus (Aggleton et al., 2014; Shibata and Yukie, 2009; Vogt et al., 1987). Likewise, the anterior cingulate cortex is interconnected with both the anterior thalamic nuclei and the medial dorsal nucleus in rats and macaque monkeys (Shibata, 1993; Shibata and Naito, 2005; Shibata and Yukie, 2009). Furthermore, in both species, these anterior cingulate connections are focussed on the anteromedial thalamic nucleus.
One species difference involves the topographic organisation of the subiculum neurons that project to the mammillary bodies and the anterior thalamic nuclei (Christiansen et al., 2016b). In the rat, this organisation is partly based on the proximal–distal plane, but in the monkey this separation is achieved by laminar position only. Finally, it might be supposed that the emergence of additional prefrontal connections would create very different network relationships across species. In fact, this difference is not so marked as may be imagined as the rat prelimbic cortex has many corresponding connections with the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network as those seen with primate medial frontal areas, for example, with the anteromedial thalamic nucleus and with the hippocampus (CA1 and subiculum).
The human hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network
The inability to use axonal tract tracers means that our knowledge of this system in the human brain remains superficial. One approach has been to use fibre dissection techniques. In this way, the tracts from the hippocampus to the mammillary bodies (postcommissural fornix), from the mammillary bodies to the anterior thalamus (mammillothalamic tract), from the anterior thalamus to the cingulate cortices (anterior thalamic radiations and cingulum), and, finally, from the cingulate cortices to the parahippocampal region (posterior cingulum) can be visualised (Shah et al., 2012). One interesting finding is that the majority of fibres in the body of the fornix join the postcommissural fornix (Shah et al., 2012), emphasising the likely importance of the connections highlighted by Papez, as the postcommissural pathway principally terminates in the mammillary bodies and the anterior thalamic nuclei. The fibre dissection technique is, however, limited as it cannot confirm the direction of a set of fibres and fails to reveal diffuse pathways.
Some of the same limitations apply to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques such as diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and diffusion spectrum imaging (DSI). Nevertheless, with these techniques, it is possible to reconstruct pathways such as the fornix (including the postcommissural fornix), the mammillothalamic tract, and the cingulum (Catani et al., 2013; Christiansen et al., 2016a; Granziera et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2013; Kwon et al., 2010; Wei et al., 2017), with the added ability to look for changes in axonal properties based on pathology or experience (see section ‘Anatomy and function’). It has also been possible to show that as in macaques, the retrosplenial cortex is the main cingulate site for direct hippocampal connections (Wei et al., 2017). With the exception of the mammillothalamic tract, there is, however, the added problem for these imaging methods (and microdissection) that all these tracts connect multiple sites, that is, their fibres are not restricted to the hippocampal limbic network. For this reason, the mammillothalamic tract is the only pathway devoted to Papez’s part of the limbic system.
Re-connecting the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network
From Figures 3 to 9, it is immediately evident that the connectivity in these pathways is more complex than often described. While we cannot be certain of the fine details in the human brain, a great many features are shared in both the rat and macaque brain, suggesting that they are also present in our brains. One of these common features, the bypassing of stages within the serial circuit described by Papez (e.g. the direct hippocampal projections to the anterior thalamic nuclei), reinforces the concept of an integrated system as it strengthens the close interactions between serial sites. This concept is further strengthened by the many reciprocal connections within the network. The many bypassing connections do, however, pose questions about the computational value of combined direct and indirect projections between the same sites, a feature that occurs repeatedly within this limbic subsystem. This feature is arguably most striking in the anterior thalamic nuclei, which receive dense direct hippocampal inputs alongside dense, indirect inputs (hippocampus → mammillary body → anterior thalamic nuclei; hippocampus → retrosplenial cortex → anterior thalamic nuclei).
A further feature of the network is the presence of clear topographies within every pathway. These topographies imply parallel functions set within the same broad set of connections (Aggleton et al., 2010). One well-established example concerns the head-direction system, which aids navigation (Taube, 2007). In the rodent, cells signalling this direction information are especially prevalent in the lateral mammillary nucleus, the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus, the lateral dorsal thalamic nucleus, the retrosplenial cortex, and the postsubiculum. Consequently, there is a head-direction subsystem set within Papez’s original circuit (Vann and Aggleton, 2004). Other functional divisions are presumably reflected by the topographic differences in the connections of the anteromedial and anteroventral thalamic nuclei (Figures 3 and 4; Aggleton et al., 2010). One example concerns the relative switch in influence between thalamic–frontal interactions (anteromedial nucleus) and thalamic–hippocampal interactions (anteroventral nucleus). It should be added that these topographic divisions extend to subregions within a nucleus (Shibata, 1992; Shibata and Kato, 1993).
A potentially important issue concerns the distinction between the anterior cingulate and posterior cingulate regions. These two regions differ in the strength and breadth of interconnections with the anterior thalamic nuclei, the hippocampus, and parahippocampal region (Kobayashi and Amaral, 2003, 2007; Shibata and Yukie, 2009; Vertes et al., 2015; Yukie and Shibata, 2009) making the posterior cingulate region (especially retrosplenial cortex) much more closely tied to the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network. In contrast, the much greater levels of interaction between the anterior cingulate region and the amygdala indicate that this cortical area is better seen as part of a different limbic subsystem more involved in emotion (Catani et al., 2013; Dalgleish, 2004; Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012; Rolls, 2015). This different system has been called the ‘basolateral limbic system’ (Livingston and Escobar, 1971). It is, however, important to appreciate that the anterior cingulate–posterior cingulate distinction is not absolute as both regions are reciprocally interconnected and they are both connected with the anterior thalamic nuclei and the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980). Consequently, the cingulate cortices have a potentially important role in providing cross-talk between these two major limbic subsystems (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980; Livingston and Escobar, 1971; Rolls, 2015). In addition, the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and amygdala are all reciprocally connected (Aggleton, 1986; Saunders et al., 1988) providing further interplay between these putative systems.
Anatomy and function
For both historic reasons and to reflect current research priorities, there has been a natural tendency to emphasise the hippocampus within the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network. This emphasis has been reinforced by the realisation that the network’s connections are required for memory (see below). For this same reason, the hippocampus is still often seen as both the principal start and finish points for many of the connections within this limbic subsystem (Aggleton and Brown, 1999; Rolls, 2015). An unfortunate consequence of this viewpoint is that it tends to diminish the perceived importance of the individual steps around the ‘circuit’ beyond the hippocampus, as a return loop implies that these additional stages are not always needed. In fact, this hippocampal focus, while understandable, has no particular anatomical claim. Indeed, an alternate way to consider this network is to regard the connections as principally a set of direct and indirect projections from the medial temporal lobe to the anterior thalamus and cingulate cortices, one function of which will be to engage additional cingulate and prefrontal areas. Consequently, the connections might better be considered as parallel projections emanating from hippocampal and parahippocampal regions, creating a medial temporal lobe efferent system, rather than a circuit (Figure 10), despite the many return connections. When re-cast in this way, it is striking how many of the connections within this limbic subsystem are also components of the ‘default mode network’ (Greicius et al., 2003; Raichle, 2015).
This review helps explain one reason why it has proved difficult to appreciate the contributions of structures beyond the hippocampus in this network. The difficulty arises from the complexity of their connections. The presence of parallel, bypassing projections (Figures 6 and 9) will typically protect against the full effects of damage to an individual site beyond the hippocampus, as only incomplete disconnections can occur. One apparent exception is provided by the anterior thalamic nuclei, which receive convergent inputs from the hippocampus, mammillary bodies, and cingulate cortices. While the lack of clinical conditions that selectively target the anterior thalamic nuclei makes it difficult to test this prediction in humans (but see Harding et al., 2000), support comes from lesion studies in rats. Such experiments have repeatedly confirmed that these thalamic nuclei are vital for hippocampal-dependent learning (Henry et al., 2004; Warburton et al., 2001). Furthermore, anterior thalamic lesions are typically more disruptive than the corresponding lesions in the mammillary bodies (Aggleton et al., 1991, 1995a) and can, sometimes, be more disruptive than fornix lesions (Warburton and Aggleton, 1998). These findings presumably reflect the array of direct and indirect connections within this network that converge on the anterior thalamic nuclei. For opposite reasons, it is supposed that the impact of retrosplenial cortex lesions in rats can often be mitigated by the many parallel hippocampal pathways that can bypass this area (Nelson et al., 2015).
Memory and amnesia
The current concepts on the nature of the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network are strongly affected by the realisation that its interconnections are vital for normal episodic memory (Aggleton and Brown, 1999; Catani et al., 2013; Rolls, 2015). The switch from emotion (Papez, 1937) to memory followed the description of temporal lobe amnesic cases, including patient, H.M. (Delay and Brion, 1969; Scoville and Milner, 1957; Spiers et al., 2001). Complementary evidence came from neuropathological studies of the amnesic Korsakoff’s syndrome, which has repeatedly implicated the mammillary bodies and, thereby, the anterior thalamic nuclei in diencephalic amnesia (Delay and Brion, 1969; Harding et al., 2000; Kopelman, 1995; Victor et al., 1989). Further support comes from studies of amnesic patients with thalamic vascular accidents and those with colloid cysts in the third ventricle (Carlesimo et al., 2011; Tsivilis et al., 2008; Van der Werf et al., 2003). Critically, it has also been shown that fornix damage is sufficient to cause anterograde amnesia (Aggleton et al., 2000; Gaffan et al., 1991; Gaffan and Gaffan, 1991; McMackin et al., 1995), thereby linking together hippocampal and medial diencephalic sites for episodic memory (Aggleton et al., 2000; Rudebeck et al., 2009; Tsivilis et al., 2008; Vann et al., 2009b).
Combining these neuropsychological findings led to the proposal that all the sites and connections interlinking the hippocampal formation with the medial diencephalon and posterior cingulate cortices work together to support memory in an ‘extended hippocampal system’ (Aggleton and Brown, 1999, 2006). An apparent problem, however, is that cingulum damage does not appear to be sufficient to induce amnesia in humans (Ballantine et al., 1987; Feldman et al., 2001; Turner, 1973), despite the considerable involvement of this tract for connections between the cingulate cortices and the parahippocampal region (and, hence, the hippocampus). Likewise, posterior cingulum bundle lesions in rats often have only mild effects on spatial memory tasks that are highly sensitive to hippocampal damage (Aggleton et al., 1995b; Neave et al., 1996). While techniques such as DTI have helped to reveal the likely importance of the cingulum bundle for some aspects of cognition, including cognitive control, there remains a failure to find memory functions that resemble those of the fornix (Hollocks et al., 2015; Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2012; Turner, 1973; see also Aggleton et al., 1995b; Neave et al., 1996).
Part of the explanation for these negative findings is anatomical. The idea of a hippocampal-based return circuit ignores how the connections in this limbic subsystem are reciprocal. Furthermore, as described above, the dense hippocampal (subiculum) projections to retrosplenial cortex do not join the cingulum (Aggleton et al., 2012). For these reasons, it is less likely that cingulum bundle damage would be sufficient to induce amnesia. That said, there is much evidence that the retrosplenial cortex is important for multiple aspects of memory (Maguire, 2001; Vann et al., 2009a), including the existence of ‘retrosplenial amnesia’ (Valenstein et al., 1987). This account implies that many retrosplenial connections important for memory do not rely on the cingulum, examples of which include the projections from the hippocampus to retrosplenial cortex and some of those between the retrosplenial cortex and the anterior thalamic nuclei (Aggleton et al., 2012, 2014; Mufson and Pandya, 1984). Another part of the explanation is conceptual. As already noted, there is an understandable tendency to see the hippocampus as the centre of a circuit around which information flows, but with little alteration in the information itself. This misconception leads to the false idea that cingulum bundle damage and fornix damage will have similar effects on memory, notwithstanding the fact that both pathways also contain numerous fibres involved in other connections.
Despite these limitations, the realisation that individual components of the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network have key roles in episodic memory is proving increasingly insightful when trying to understand the functional neuropathology of disorders such as amnesic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease (Aggleton et al., 2016; Nestor et al., 2003; Tan et al., 2013; Vogt et al., 2009). Over recent decades, it has often been assumed that hippocampal and parahippocampal dysfunctions are responsible for the memory loss in these neurological conditions. In fact, there is growing evidence that structures in Papez’s original circuit beyond the parahippocampal and hippocampal regions also show pathological changes and activity abnormalities during the prodromal stages of these same disorders (Aggleton et al., 2016; Minoshima et al., 1997; Nestor et al., 2003). Consequently, to understand the origins of the memory loss in conditions such as mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease, it will be necessary to broaden our perspectives to incorporate anterior thalamic and retrosplenial sites (Aggleton et al., 2016; Hornberger et al., 2012). An important aspect of this realignment is the growing realisation that sites such as the mammillary bodies and anterior thalamic nuclei make contributions to learning and memory that are not solely dependent on their hippocampal inputs (Dillingham et al., 2015b; Wright et al., 2015).
Unlike other structures within the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network, damage to the anterior cingulate region does not produce an anterograde amnesia, despite recording data revealing long-term mnemonic functions (Xiang and Brown, 2004). Instead, there is considerable imaging evidence, in particular, that this region and its connections have key roles in multiple functions, such as cognitive control and schema usage (Fernández, 2017; Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2012; Shenhav et al., 2013; Van Kesteren et al., 2013; Weible, 2013) that impact on memory. At the same time, the anterior cingulate region and its connections remain strongly implicated in emotional processes (Dalgleish, 2004; Etkin et al., 2011, 2015; Fan et al., 2011).
Emotion and psychiatry
Despite the increased emphasis on memory in recent decades, the notion that the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network (Papez, 1937) is vital for emotion never fully disappeared. From the 1950s to the 1980s, ideas about the limbic system remained centred on its likely role in emotion and its presumed imbalance in psychiatric conditions (Kelly, 1973; Livingston and Escobar, 1971). This preserved emphasis partly reflected how the term ‘limbic system’ had been broadened to include areas such as the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Reflecting this trend, it was proposed that that the limbic system should be subdivided (Livingston and Escobar, 1971; Rolls, 2015). One subsystem was a ‘medial limbic’ circuit (Livingston and Escobar, 1971), which particularly emphasised anterior thalamic–posterior cingulate–hippocampal connections, that is, the connections highlighted in this review. This ‘medial limbic’ circuit was contrasted with an amygdala-based ‘basolateral circuit’ that included the anterior cingulate region, the two circuits jointly contributing to affect and learnt emotion (Dalgleish, 2004; Livingston and Escobar, 1971).
Within such conceptual frameworks, surgeons targeted sites such as the anterior cingulate cortex for obsessional (obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD)) and affective disorders (Feldman et al., 2001; Lewin, 1961). Likewise, the cingulum bundle has been selectively damaged as a means to combat a variety of severe refractory psychiatric illnesses, including depression, anxiety, OCDs, and schizophrenia (Ballantine et al., 1987; Feldman et al., 2001). The literature fails to clarify or explain the specific resultant effects on cognition and behaviour and suggests simply that the disconnection of limbic structures from the forebrain disrupts the behavioural expression of internal emotional states (Ballantine et al., 1967). Nonetheless, it is noteworthy that memory deficits are not normally reported with such procedures. Another target, especially for OCD, has been the region of the anterior capsule at the anterior limb of the internal capsule (Feldman et al., 2001; Mashour et al., 2005). Such surgeries would be expected to disconnect thalamic–frontal pathways, including those anterior thalamic efferents that reach the cingulum in this way. At the same time, posterior cingulectomy (the removal of the posterior cingulate gyri) has been used for psychiatric conditions (Turner, 1973). The reports emphasise changes in emotion rather than memory (Turner, 1973). A feature of these various surgeries is that in different ways, they disrupt aspects of both the ‘medial’ (hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate) and ‘basolateral’ limbic circuits.
In recent years, there has been renewed interest in the hippocampus and emotion, partly from growing evidence that hippocampal dysfunctions contribute to conditions such as schizophrenia, anxiety disorders, and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Small et al., 2011). This interest has been fuelled by the discovery of functional changes along the long axis of the hippocampus, which partly reflect changing relative contributions to emotion and memory (O’Mara et al., 2001; Poppenk et al., 2013; Small et al., 2011). In particular, it is supposed that the functions of the anterior hippocampus are biased towards emotional states, including anxiety, while the posterior hippocampus is more critical for mnemonic functions (Aggleton, 2012; McHugh et al., 2004; O’Mara et al., 2001; Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012; Small et al., 2011). This framework is reflected in hippocampal connectivity as the anterior hippocampus is particularly linked to sites such as the amygdala, nucleus accumbens, medial, and orbital prefrontal cortex, while the posterior hippocampus is more densely connected with sites closely linked to episodic memory, including the mammillary bodies and retrosplenial cortex (Aggleton, 2012). For this reason, the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network appears to particularly engage the posterior hippocampus.
A related issue is that many of the hippocampal projections most associated with emotion do not, in fact, join those connections highlighted by Papez (Clarke et al., 2015; Small et al., 2011), as there are direct hippocampal projections to sites such as the frontal cortex, amygdala, and ventral striatum (Aggleton, 1986; Aggleton et al., 2015; Friedman et al., 2002). These particular hippocampal connections have been especially linked to conditions such as PTSD and schizophrenia (Sigurdsson and Duvarci, 2015; Small et al., 2011). Even so, animal studies reveal contributions from the anterior thalamic nuclei and retrosplenial cortex to fear conditioning (Célérier et al., 2000; Gabriel, 1991; Gabriel et al., 1991; Keene and Bucci, 2008), suggesting that Papez’s connections retain a contributory role in emotional conditions such as anxiety.
Summary
In conclusion, we can state that the concept of a serial limbic circuit for emotion, first promoted by Papez (1937), is misleading with respect to both information flow and function. It remains the case that the connections originally described by Papez do exist. Indeed, it could be argued that with respect to the mammillary bodies, the anterior thalamic nuclei, and the retrosplenial cortex, these interconnections may well be the most dominant with respect to their respective functions. At the same time, Papez could not appreciate the weight of reciprocal connections between some of the structures, as well as the number of additional connections that jump the nodes in his circuit. This network, which appears more critical for learning and memory than emotion, involves complex topographies that reflect multiple subsystems. Furthermore, the predominant pattern of information flow need not be circular, as initially supposed. Instead, many of the connections can be seen as providing parallel efferents from the medial temporal lobes, where the subiculum has a particularly important role.
Despite all these complexities, the structures initially highlighted by Papez still retain a special status. One unifying example is that theta-rhythm appears to resonate throughout these same sites, consistent with a circuit (Vertes et al., 2001, 2004, 2015). Such neuronal activity potentially plays an important role in mnemonic processes. Another example is the way in which sites throughout the hippocampal–diencephalic–cingulate network contain head-direction cells (Taube, 2007). Furthermore, when trying to understand the relationships between conditions such as temporal lobe amnesia and diencephalic amnesia, or when trying to unravel the neuropathologies underlying prodromal states in dementia, the importance of these same structures and their interlinking pathways comes to the fore. Consequently, we still need a more comprehensive appreciation of the group of connections initially described 80 years ago by Papez, combined with the specific need to uncover far more about the details of these same connections in the human brain.
Acknowledgments
E.J.B. and L.K. are equal first authors.
Footnotes
Declaration of conflicting interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The Wellcome Trust (grant # 103722/Z14/Z) funded some of the rodent anatomical studies that contributed to this review.
References
- Aggleton JP. (1986) A description of the amygdalo-hippocampal interconnections in the macaque monkey. Experimental Brain Research 64(3): 515–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP. (2012) Multiple anatomical systems embedded within the primate medial temporal lobe: Implications for hippocampal function. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36(7): 1579–1596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Brown MW. (1999) Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(3): 425–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Brown MW. (2006) Interleaving brain systems for episodic and recognition memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(10): 455–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Desimone R, Mishkin M. (1986) The origin, course, and termination of the hippocampothalamic projections in the macaque. Journal of Comparative Neurology 243(3): 409–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Keith AB, Sahgal A. (1991) Both fornix and anterior thalamic, but not mammillary, lesions disrupt delayed non-matching-to-position memory in rats. Behavioural Brain Research 44(2): 151–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, McMackin D, Carpenter K, et al. (2000) Differential cognitive effects of colloid cysts in the third ventricle that spare or compromise the fornix. Brain 123(4): 800–815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Neave N, Nagle S, et al. (1995. a) A comparison of the effects of anterior thalamic, mamillary body and fornix lesions on reinforced spatial alternation. Behavioural Brain Research 68(1): 91–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Neave N, Nagle S, et al. (1995. b) A comparison of the effects of medial prefrontal, cingulate cortex, and cingulum bundle lesions on tests of spatial memory: Evidence of a double dissociation between frontal and cingulum bundle contributions. The Journal of Neuroscience 15(11): 7270–7281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, O’Mara SM, Vann SD, et al. (2010) Hippocampal-anterior thalamic pathways for memory: Uncovering a network of direct and indirect actions. European Journal of Neuroscience 31(12): 2292–2307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Pralus A, Nelson AJ, et al. (2016) Thalamic pathology and memory loss in early Alzheimer’s disease: Moving the focus from the medial temporal lobe to Papez circuit. Brain 139: 1877–1890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Saunders RC, Wright NF, et al. (2014) The origin of projections from the posterior cingulate and retrosplenial cortices to the anterior, medial dorsal and laterodorsal thalamic nuclei of macaque monkeys. European Journal of Neuroscience 39(1): 107–123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Vann SD, Saunders RC. (2005) Projections from the hippocampal region to the mammillary bodies in macaque monkeys. European Journal of Neuroscience 22(10): 2519–2530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Wright NF, Rosene DL, et al. (2015) Complementary patterns of direct amygdala and hippocampal projections to the macaque prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 25(11): 4351–4373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton JP, Wright NF, Vann SD, et al. (2012) Medial temporal lobe projections to the retrosplenial cortex of the macaque monkey. Hippocampus 22(9): 1883–1900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen GV, Hopkins DA. (1988) Mamillary body in the rat: A cytoarchitectonic, Golgi, and ultrastructural study. Journal of Comparative Neurology 275(1): 39–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen GV, Hopkins DA. (1989) Mamillary body in the rat: Topography and synaptology of projections from the subicular complex, prefrontal cortex, and midbrain tegmentum. Journal of Comparative Neurology 286(3): 311–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amaral DG, Cowan WM. (1980) Subcortical afferents to the hippocampal formation in the monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology 189(4): 573–591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baleydier C, Mauguiere F. (1980) The duality of the cingulate gyrus in monkey. Neuroanatomical study and functional hypothesis. Brain 103(3): 525–554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballantine HT, Jr, Bouckoms AJ, Thomas EK, et al. (1987) Treatment of psychiatric illness by stereotactic cingulotomy. Biological Psychiatry 22(7): 807–819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballantine HT, Jr, Cassidy WL, Flanagan NB, et al. (1967) Stereotaxic anterior cingulotomy for neuropsychiatric illness and intractable pain. Journal of Neurosurgery 26(5): 488–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broca P. (1878) Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales: Le grand lobe limbique et la scissure limbique dans la série des mammifères (Revue d’Anthropologie series 2). Paris, pp. 385–498. [Google Scholar]
- Carlesimo GA, Lombardi MG, Caltagirone C. (2011) Vascular thalamic amnesia: A reappraisal. Neuropsychologia 49(5): 777–789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catani M, Dell’Acqua F, De Schotten MT. (2013) A revised limbic system model for memory, emotion and behaviour. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37(8): 1724–1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Célérier A, Ognard R, Decorte L, et al. (2000) Deficits of spatial and non-spatial memory and of auditory fear conditioning following anterior thalamic lesions in mice: Comparison with chronic alcohol consumption. European Journal of Neuroscience 12(7): 2575–2584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christiansen K, Aggleton JP, Parker GD, et al. (2016. a) The status of the precommissural and postcommissural fornix in normal ageing and mild cognitive impairment: An MRI tractography study. Neuroimage 130(0): 35–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christiansen K, Dillingham CM, Wright NF, et al. (2016. b) Complementary subicular pathways to the anterior thalamic nuclei and mammillary bodies in the rat and macaque monkey brain. European Journal of Neuroscience 43(8): 1044–1061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke HF, Horst NK, Roberts AC. (2015) Regional inactivations of primate ventral prefrontal cortex reveal two distinct mechanisms underlying negative bias in decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112(13): 4176–4181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Da Silva FL, Witter MP, Boeijinga PH, et al. (1990) Anatomic organization and physiology of the limbic cortex. Physiological Reviews 70(2): 453–511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalgleish T. (2004) The emotional brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5(7): 583–589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delay J, Brion S. (1969) Le syndrome de Korsakoff. Paris: Masson. [Google Scholar]
- Dillingham CM, Erichsen JT, O’Mara SM, et al. (2015. a) Fornical and nonfornical projections from the rat hippocampal formation to the anterior thalamic nuclei. Hippocampus 25(9): 977–992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dillingham CM, Frizzati A, Nelson AJ, et al. (2015. b) How do mammillary body inputs contribute to anterior thalamic function? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 54(0): 108–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domesick VB. (1970) The fasciculus cinguli in the rat. Brain Research 20(1): 19–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin A, Büchel C, Gross JJ. (2015) The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16(11): 693–700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. (2011) Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(2): 85–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, et al. (2011) Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35(3): 903–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman RP, Alterman RL, Goodrich JT. (2001) Contemporary psychosurgery and a look to the future. Journal of Neurosurgery 95(6): 944–956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández G. (2017) The medial prefrontal cortex is a critical hub in the declarative memory system. In: Axmacher N, Rasch B. (eds) Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation. Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 45–56. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman DP, Aggleton JP, Saunders RC. (2002) Comparison of hippocampal, amygdala, and perirhinal projections to the nucleus accumbens: Combined anterograde and retrograde tracing study in the Macaque brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 450(4): 345–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furtak SC, Wei SM, Agster KL, et al. (2007) Functional neuroanatomy of the parahippocampal region in the rat: The perirhinal and postrhinal cortices. Hippocampus 17(9): 709–722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabriel M. (1991) Functions of anterior and posterior cingulate cortex during avoidance learning in rabbits. Progress in Brain Research 85(0): 467–483. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabriel M, Vogt BA, Kubota Y, et al. (1991) Training-stage related neuronal plasticity in limbic thalamus and cingulate cortex during learning: A possible key to mnemonic retrieval. Behavioural Brain Research 46(2): 175–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaffan D, Gaffan EA. (1991) Amnesia in man following transection of the fornix. A review. Brain 114(6): 2611–2618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaffan EA, Gaffan D, Hodges JR. (1991) Amnesia following damage to the left fornix and to other sites. A comparative study. Brain 114(3): 1297–1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granziera C, Hadjikhani N, Arzy S, et al. (2011) In-vivo magnetic resonance imaging of the structural core of the Papez circuit in humans. Neuroreport 22(5): 227–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, et al. (2003) Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100(1): 253–258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harding A, Halliday G, Caine D, et al. (2000) Degeneration of anterior thalamic nuclei differentiates alcoholics with amnesia. Brain 123(1): 141–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry J, Petrides M, St-Laurent M, et al. (2004) Spatial conditional associative learning: Effects of thalamo-hippocampal disconnection in rats. Neuroreport 15(15): 2427–2431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollocks MJ, Lawrence AJ, Brookes RL, et al. (2015) Differential relationships between apathy and depression with white matter microstructural changes and functional outcomes. Brain 138(12): 3803–3815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honda Y, Ishizuka N. (2015) Topographic distribution of cortical projection cells in the rat subiculum. Neuroscience Research 92(0): 1–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins DA. (2005) Neuroanatomy of head direction cell circuits. In: Wiener SI, Taube JS. (eds) Head Direction Cells and the Neural Mechanisms of Spatial Orientation. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 17–44. [Google Scholar]
- Hornberger M, Wong S, Tan R, et al. (2012) In vivo and post-mortem memory circuit integrity in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 135(10): 3015–3025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insausti R, Amaral DG, Cowan WM. (1987. a) The entorhinal cortex of the monkey: II. Cortical afferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology 264(3): 356–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insausti R, Amaral DG, Cowan WM. (1987. b) The entorhinal cortex of the monkey: III. Subcortical afferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology 264(3): 396–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacson RL. (1992) A fuzzy limbic system. Behavioural Brain Research 52(2): 129–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones BF, Witter MP. (2007) Cingulate cortex projections to the parahippocampal region and hippocampal formation in the rat. Hippocampus 17(10): 957–976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones DK, Christiansen KF, Chapman RJ, et al. (2013) Distinct subdivisions of the cingulum bundle revealed by diffusion MRI fibre tracking: Implications for neuropsychological investigations. Neuropsychologia 51(1): 67–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keene CS, Bucci DJ. (2008) Neurotoxic lesions of retrosplenial cortex disrupt signaled and unsignaled contextual fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience 122(5): 1070–1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly D. (1973) Psychosurgery and the limbic system. Postgraduate Medical Journal 49(578): 825–833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi Y, Amaral DG. (2003) Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: II. Cortical afferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology 466(1): 48–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi Y, Amaral DG. (2007) Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: III. Cortical efferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology 502(5): 810–833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman MD. (1995) The Korsakoff syndrome. The British Journal of Psychiatry 166(2): 154–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kötter R, Meyer N. (1992) The limbic system: A review of its empirical foundation. Behavioural Brain Research 52(2): 105–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwon HG, Hong JH, Jang SH. (2010) Mammillothalamic tract in human brain: Diffusion tensor tractography study. Neuroscience Letters 481(1): 51–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavenex P, Suzuki WA, Amaral DG. (2002) Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Projections to the neocortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology 447(4): 394–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewin W. (1961) Observations on selective leucotomy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 24(1): 37–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livingston KE, Escobar A. (1971) Anatomical bias of the limbic system concept. A proposed reorientation. Archives of Neurology 24(1): 17–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHugh SB, Deacon RM, Rawlins JN, et al. (2004) Amygdala and ventral hippocampus contribute differentially to mechanisms of fear and anxiety. Behavioral Neuroscience 118(1): 63–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean PD. (1949) Psychosomatic disease and the visceral brain; recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic Medicine 11(6): 338–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean PD. (1952) Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain). Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 4(4): 407–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMackin D, Cockburn J, Anslow P, et al. (1995) Correlation of fornix damage with memory impairment in six cases of colloid cyst removal. Acta Neurochirurgica 135(1–2): 12–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire E. (2001) The retrosplenial contribution to human navigation: A review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 42(3): 225–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mashour GA, Walker EE, Martuza RL. (2005) Psychosurgery: Past, present, and future. Brain Research Reviews 48(3): 409–419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathiasen ML, Dillingham CM, Kinnavane L, et al. (2017) Asymmetric cross-hemispheric connections link the rat anterior thalamic nuclei with the cortex and hippocampal formation. Neuroscience 349(0): 128–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meibach RC, Siegel A. (1975) The origin of fornix fibers which project to the mammillary bodies in the rat: A horseradish peroxidase study. Brain Research 88(3): 508–512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meibach RC, Siegel A. (1977) Thalamic projections of the hippocampal formation: Evidence for an alternate pathway involving the internal capsule. Brain Research 134(1): 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metzler-Baddeley C, Jones DK, Steventon J, et al. (2012) Cingulum microstructure predicts cognitive control in older age and mild cognitive impairment. The Journal of Neuroscience 32(49): 17612–17619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minoshima S, Giordani B, Berent S, et al. (1997) Metabolic reduction in the posterior cingulate cortex in very early Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Neurology 42(1): 85–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris R, Petrides M, Pandya DN. (1999) Architecture and connections of retrosplenial area 30 in the rhesus monkey ( Macaca mulatta). European Journal of Neuroscience 11(7): 2506–2518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mufson EJ, Pandya DN. (1984) Some observations on the course and composition of the cingulum bundle in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology 225(1): 31–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naber PA, Witter MP. (1998) Subicular efferents are organized mostly as parallel projections: A double-labeling, retrograde-tracing study in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 393(3): 284–297. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neave N, Nagle S, Sahgal A, et al. (1996) The effects of discrete cingulum bundle lesions in the rat on the acquisition and performance of two tests of spatial working memory. Behavioural Brain Research 80(1): 75–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson AJ, Powell AL, Holmes JD, et al. (2015) What does spatial alternation tell us about retrosplenial cortex function? Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9(0): 126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestor PJ, Fryer TD, Smielewski P, et al. (2003) Limbic hypometabolism in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Annals of Neurology 54(3): 343–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Mara SM, Commins S, Anderson M, et al. (2001) The subiculum: A review of form, physiology and function. Progress in Neurobiology 64(2): 129–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pandya DN, Van Hoesen GW, Mesulam MM. (1981) Efferent connections of the cingulate gyrus in the rhesus monkey. Experimental Brain Research 42(3–4): 319–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papez JW. (1937) A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 38(4): 725–743. [Google Scholar]
- Parvizi J, Van Hoesen GW, Buckwalter J, et al. (2006) Neural connections of the posteromedial cortex in the macaque. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103(5): 1563–1568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessoa L, Hof PR. (2015) From Paul Broca’s great limbic lobe to the limbic system. Journal of Comparative Neurology 523(17): 2495–2500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poppenk J, Evensmoen HR, Moscovitch M, et al. (2013) Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(5): 230–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell TP, Guillery RW, Cowan WM. (1957) A quantitative study of the fornixmamillo-thalamic system. Journal of Anatomy 91(4): 419–437. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prasad JA, Chudasama Y. (2013) Viral tracing identifies parallel disynaptic pathways to the hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience 33(19): 8494–8503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME. (2015) The brain’s default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience 38(0): 433–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranganath C, Ritchey M. (2012) Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13(10): 713–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET. (2015) Limbic systems for emotion and for memory, but no single limbic system. Cortex 62(0): 119–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roxo MR, Franceschini PR, Zubaran C, et al. (2011) The limbic system conception and its historical evolution. The Scientific World Journal 11(0): 2427–2440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudebeck SR, Scholz J, Millington R, et al. (2009) Fornix microstructure correlates with recollection but not familiarity memory. The Journal of Neuroscience 29(47): 14987–14992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders RC, Mishkin M, Aggleton JP. (2005) Projections from the entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, presubiculum, and parasubiculum to the medial thalamus in macaque monkeys: Identifying different pathways using disconnection techniques. Experimental Brain Research 167(1): 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders RC, Rosene DL, Van Hoesen GW. (1988) Comparison of the efferents of the amygdala and the hippocampal formation in the rhesus monkey: II. Reciprocal and non-reciprocal connections. Journal of Comparative Neurology 271(2): 185–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scoville WB, Milner B. (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 20(1): 11–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seki M, Zyo K. (1984) Anterior thalamic afferents from the mamillary body and the limbic cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 229(2): 242–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shah A, Jhawar SS, Goel A. (2012) Analysis of the anatomy of the Papez circuit and adjoining limbic system by fiber dissection techniques. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience 19(2): 289–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. (2013) The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79(2): 217–240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibata H. (1992) Topographic organization of subcortical projections to the anterior thalamic nuclei in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 323(1): 117–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibata H. (1993) Efferent projections from the anterior thalamic nuclei to the cingulate cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 330(4): 533–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibata H. (1996) Direct projections from the entorhinal area to the anteroventral and laterodorsal thalamic nuclei in the rat. Neuroscience Research 26(1): 83–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibata H. (1998) Organization of projections of rat retrosplenial cortex to the anterior thalamic nuclei. European Journal of Neuroscience 10(10): 3210–3219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibata H, Kato A. (1993) Topographic relationship between anteromedial thalamic nucleus neurons and their cortical terminal fields in the rat. Neuroscience Research 17(1): 63–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibata H, Naito J. (2005) Organization of anterior cingulate and frontal cortical projections to the anterior and laterodorsal thalamic nuclei in the rat. Brain Research 1059(1): 93–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibata H, Yukie M. (2009) Thalamocingulate connections in the monkey. In: Vogt BA. (ed.) Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 95–111. [Google Scholar]
- Sigurdsson T, Duvarci S. (2015) Hippocampal-prefrontal interactions in cognition, behavior and psychiatric disease. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 9(0): 190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small SA, Schobel SA, Buxton RB, et al. (2011) A pathophysiological framework of hippocampal dysfunction in ageing and disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12(10): 585–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spiers HJ, Maguire EA, Burgess N. (2001) Hippocampal amnesia. Neurocase 7(5): 357–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sripanidkulchai K, Wyss JM. (1987) The laminar organization of efferent neuronal cell bodies in the retrosplenial granular cortex. Brain Research 406(1): 255–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strange BA, Witter MP, Lein ES, et al. (2014) Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 15(10): 655–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugar J, Witter MP, van Strien N, et al. (2011) The retrosplenial cortex: Intrinsic connectivity and connections with the (para)hippocampal region in the rat. An interactive connectome. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics 5(0): 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki WL, Amaral DG. (1994) Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: Cortical afferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology 350(4): 497–533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson LW, Cowan WM. (1977) An autoradiographic study of the organization of the efferent connections of the hippocampal formation in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 172(1): 49–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan RH, Wong S, Hodges JR, et al. (2013) Retrosplenial cortex (BA 29) volumes in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 35(3–4): 177–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taube JS. (2007) The head direction signal: Origins and sensory-motor integration. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30(0): 181–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teuber HL. (1955) Physiological psychology Annual Review of Psychology 6(1): 267–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsivilis D, Vann SD, Denby C, et al. (2008) A disproportionate role for the fornix and mammillary bodies in recall versus recognition memory. Nature Neuroscience 11(7): 834–842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner E. (1973) Custom psychosurgery. Postgraduate Medical Journal 49(578): 834–844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valenstein E, Bowers D, Verfaellie M, et al. (1987) Retrosplenial amnesia. Brain 110(6): 1631–1646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Horst L. (1951) Disorders in Papez’ circuit from multiple glioblastoma. Bruxelles Médical 31(10): 513–527. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Werf YD, Scheltens P, Lindeboom J, et al. (2003) Deficits of memory, executive functioning and attention following infarction in the thalamus; a study of 22 cases with localised lesions. Neuropsychologia 41(10): 1330–1344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Groen T, Wyss JM. (1990. a) Connections of the retrosplenial granular a cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 300(4): 593–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Groen T, Wyss JM. (1990. b) The connections of presubiculum and parasubiculum in the rat. Brain Research 518(1): 227–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Groen T, Wyss JM. (1990. c) The postsubicular cortex in the rat: Characterization of the fourth region of the subicular cortex and its connections. Brain Research 529(1): 165–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Groen T, Wyss JM. (1992) Connections of the retrosplenial dysgranular cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 315(2): 200–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Groen T, Wyss JM. (2003) Connections of the retrosplenial granular b cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 463(3): 249–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Kesteren MT, Beul SF, Takashima A, et al. (2013) Differential roles for medial prefrontal and medial temporal cortices in schema-dependent encoding: From congruent to incongruent. Neuropsychologia 51(12): 2352–2359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Strien NM, Cappaert NL, Witter MP. (2009) The anatomy of memory: An interactive overview of the parahippocampal–hippocampal network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10(4): 272–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann SD, Aggleton JP, Maguire EA. (2009. a) What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10(11): 792–802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann SD, Aggleton JP. (2002) Extensive cytotoxic lesions of the rat retrosplenial cortex reveal consistent deficits on tasks that tax allocentric spatial memory. Behavioral Neuroscience 116(1): 85–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann SD, Aggleton JP. (2004) The mammillary bodies: Two memory systems in one? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5(1): 35–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann SD, Saunders RC, Aggleton JP. (2007) Distinct, parallel pathways link the medial mammillary bodies to the anterior thalamus in macaque monkeys. European Journal of Neuroscience 26(6): 1575–1586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann SD, Tsivilis D, Denby CE, et al. (2009. b) Impaired recollection but spared familiarity in patients with extended hippocampal system damage revealed by 3 convergent methods. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(13): 5442–5447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP, Albo Z, Di Prisco GV. (2001) Theta-rhythmically firing neurons in the anterior thalamus: Implications for mnemonic functions of Papez’s circuit. Neuroscience 104(3): 619–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP, Hoover WB, Di Prisco GV. (2004) Theta rhythm of the hippocampus: Subcortical control and functional significance. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3(3): 173–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP, Linlkey SB, Groenewegen HJ, et al. (2015) Thalamus. In: Paxinos G. (ed.) The Rat Nervous System (4th edn). Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 335–390. [Google Scholar]
- Victor M, Adams RD, Collins GH. (1989) The Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome and Related Neurologic Disorders Due to Alcoholism and Malnutrition. Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis Company. [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA. (2009) Regions and subregions of the cingulate cortex. In: Vogt BA. (ed.) Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 3–30. [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Paxinos G. (2014) Cytoarchitecture of mouse and rat cingulate cortex with human homologies. Brain Structure and Function 219(1): 185–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Pandya DN, Rosene DL. (1987) Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: I. Cytoarchitecture and thalamic afferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology 262(2): 256–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Rosene DL, Pandya DN. (1979) Thalamic and cortical afferents differentiate anterior from posterior cingulate cortex in the monkey. Science 204(4389): 205–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Vogt LJ, Perl DP, et al. (2009) Cingulate neuropathology in anterior and posterior cortical atrophies in Alzheimer’s disease. In: Vogt BA. (ed.) Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 763–799. [Google Scholar]
- Warburton EC, Aggleton JP. (1998) Differential deficits in the Morris water maze following cytotoxic lesions of the anterior thalamus and fornix transection. Behavioural Brain Research 98(1): 27–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warburton EC, Baird A, Morgan A, et al. (2001) The conjoint importance of the hippocampus and anterior thalamic nuclei for allocentric spatial learning: Evidence from a disconnection study in the rat. The Journal of Neuroscience 21(18): 7323–7330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wei PH, Mao ZQ, Cong F, et al. (2017) In vivo visualization of connections among revised Papez circuit hubs using full q-space diffusion spectrum imaging tractography. Neuroscience 2017; 357(0): 400–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weible AP. (2013) Remembering to attend: The anterior cingulate cortex and remote memory. Behavioural Brain Research 245(0): 63–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witter MP, Wouterlood FG. (2002) The Parahippocampal Region: Organization and Role in Cognitive Function. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wright NF, Erichsen JT, Vann SD, et al. (2010) Parallel but separate inputs from limbic cortices to the mammillary bodies and anterior thalamic nuclei in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 518(12): 2334–2354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright NF, Vann SD, Aggleton JP, et al. (2015) A critical role for the anterior thalamus in directing attention to task-relevant stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience 35(14): 5480–5488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright NF, Vann SD, Erichsen JT, et al. (2013) Segregation of parallel inputs to the anteromedial and anteroventral thalamic nuclei of the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 521(13): 2966–2986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiang JZ, Brown MW. (2004) Neuronal responses related to long-term recognition memory processes in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 42(5): 817–829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao D, Barbas H. (2002) Pathways for emotions and memory: II. Afferent input to the anterior thalamic nuclei from prefrontal, temporal, hypothalamic areas and the basal ganglia in the rhesus monkey. Thalamus & Related Systems 2(1): 33–48. [Google Scholar]
- Yukie M, Shibata H. (2009) Temporocingulate interactions in the monkey. In: Vogt BA. (ed.) Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 145–162. [Google Scholar]