Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Oct 15.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry. 2017 Mar 28;82(8):570–577. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.02.1183

Association Between Interleukin-6 and Striatal Prediction-Error Signals Following Acute Stress in Healthy Female Participants

Michael T Treadway 1,*, Roee Admon 2,*, Amanda R Arulpragasam 1, Malavika Mehta 2, Samuel Douglas 2, Gordana Vitaliano 3, David P Olson 3, Jessica A Cooper 1, Diego A Pizzagalli 2,3
PMCID: PMC5610086  NIHMSID: NIHMS876553  PMID: 28506437

Abstract

Background

Stress is widely known to alter behavioral responses to rewards and punishments. It is believed that stress may precipitate these changes through modulation of corticostriatal circuitry involved in reinforcement learning and motivation, although the intervening mechanisms remain unclear. One candidate is inflammation, which can rapidly increase following stress, and can disrupt dopamine-dependent reward pathways.

Methods

Here, in a sample of 88 healthy female participants, we first assessed the effect of an acute laboratory stress paradigm on levels of plasma interleukin-6 (IL-6), a cytokine known to be both responsive to stress and elevated in depression. In a second laboratory session, we examined the effects of a second laboratory stress paradigm on reward prediction error (RPE) signaling in the ventral striatum.

Results

We show that individual differences in stress-induced increases in IL-6 (session 1) were associated with decreased ventral striatal RPE signaling during reinforcement learning (session 2), though there was no main effect of stress on RPE. Further, changes in IL-6 following stress predicted intra-individual variability in perceived stress during a 4-month follow-up period.

Conclusions

Taken together, these data identify a novel link between IL-6 and striatal reward prediction errors during reinforcement learning in the context of acute psychological stress, as well as future appraisal of stressful life events.

Keywords: Stress, inflammation, interleukin-6, ventral striatum, reinforcement learning, reward prediction error

Introduction

Stress is a major risk factor for psychiatric disorders (13), though its effects are not fully understood. Stress exposure can initiate a neuroendocrine cascade that modulates how individuals perceive and respond to rewarding or threatening cues in their environment (411). Specifically, it has been shown that stress may reduce acquisition of reward-related information (8, 12, 13) as well as disrupt normal reinforcer devaluation (9, 14, 15), two phenomena that are commonly observed in stress-related psychiatric disorders (1620).

In animal models, substantial research has suggested that stress may induce these behavioral changes to rewarding stimuli via effects on the mesocorticolimbic dopamine (DA) system. Acute stressors transiently increase DA release in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) while also promoting longer-term DAergic increases in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) (10, 2123). Interestingly, more recent studies have suggested that stress may have selective effects on DAergic responses to reward receipt (24), raising the possibility that behavioral changes to reinforcers may be mediated by the effects of stress on DAergic reward prediction error (RPE) signaling, a core mechanism of reinforcement learning (25, 26).

While early research on the relationship between stress, DAergic function, and subsequent behavioral changes focused on the role of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) Axis (27), recent work has increasingly recognized an important role for stress-induced immune responses (2831). As with glucocorticoids, pro-inflammatory cytokines such as interleukin-6 (IL-6), interleukin1 (IL-1), and tumor-necrosis factor alpha (TNF-alpha) can be stimulated by acute stress exposure (3234). Behaviorally, acute administration of these pro-inflammatory cytokines has been shown to reduce sensitivity to rewards while augmenting sensitivity to punishment (35), a pattern that is consistent with evolutionary models (31) and matches the effects of acute stress (12, 13, 36) (though see also (6, 37, 38)). Importantly, only IL-6 has been reliably shown in meta-analyses to be both elevated in depression (3942) as well as sensitive to laboratory measures of acute stress (33), and is increasingly recognized as a playing an important role in mood disorders (43).

A growing body of evidence suggests that DA and cytokines may influence each other through multiple pathways. Both acute and chronic treatment with cytokine inducers–including direct administration of IL-6–has been shown to disrupt DA synaptic availability and synthesis in rodents (4446), non-human primates (47, 48), and humans (49). Similarly, in human functional neuroimaging studies, both chronic and acute administration of cytokine-inducers has been associated with blunted ventral striatal responses to reward anticipation (49, 50), prediction-error signaling during reinforcement learning (35), and novelty-driven activity in the DAergic midbrain (51). Alternatively, however, there is also growing evidence that DA signaling may modulate cytokine responses. DA receptors have been identified on numerous components of the innate immune system (52), including lymphocytes and T-Cells. These studies have largely suggested that DA acts to inhibit the actions of activated T-Cells. In particular, dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) knockout mice show a remarkable anti-inflammatory response, suggesting that DA signaling may be primarily anti-inflammatory in nature (53, 54), though not in all cases (52).

Given these potentially bi-directional pathways between inflammation on DA signaling pathways, we predicted that stress-induced increases in inflammatory cytokines would be associated with a reduction in DA-dependent RPE signals during reinforcement learning. To date, however, no study has tested the relationship between stress-induced IL-6 and stress-related changes in striatal prediction error signaling and whether these mechanisms predict future levels of stress appraisal.

In the present study, we sought to address this question through a combination of laboratory stress challenges, plasma measures of IL-6, and functional neuroimaging in a sample of 88 healthy female participants assessed across two study visits (Figure 1). Only women were investigated owing to elevated prevalence of depression in females (55), as well as significant sex differences in psychological and hormonal responses to stress (56) that could substantially reduce our power to detect individual differences. During the first session, participants were exposed to the Maastricht Acute Stress Task (MAST; (57)), a robust laboratory stress paradigm, while blood was sampled intravenously. During the second session, participants completed a functional neuroimaging session that included functional runs of a reinforcement learning task (58) interleaved with blocks of a well-validated neuroimaging stress-paradigm, the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST) (59). Effects of both stressors on mood were assessed using visual analog mood scales (VAMS) (60). We hypothesized that larger increases in IL-6 following stress (as assessed in the first behavioral session) would predict a greater blunting of RPE signals during stress (as assessed in the second session). After these laboratory visits, participants were followed for a period of four months to assess self-reported stressful experiences in daily life. For these assessments, we predicted that greater biological responses to laboratory stressors would predict self-reported stressful experiences during the follow-up period.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic diagram illustrating the design of study sessions 1 and 2. A Overall flow of participants through the study. B. During session 1, participants first completed a SCID and other screening measures (see methods) a baseline blood draw, and then completed the MAST laboratory stress challenge. Following the MAST, two other blood draws were collected. C. During session 2, participants completed an fMRI scanning session in which they had to complete blocks of a reinforcement learning (RL) task that were interleaved between easy and hard (stressful) blocks of the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST). For each of the 3 stress conditions (Pre-Stress, During Stress, Post-stress) runs of the MIST and RL were completed twice.

Methods and Materials

Participants and Study Description

A total of 88 healthy female participants were included in this study. For details on participant eligibility criteria, please see Supplemental Methods. All recruitment and testing procedures were approved by the Partners Institutional Review Board. The study was comprised of two laboratory visits followed by a 4-month period of self-report questions administered online every two weeks. Details of study procedures can be found in the Supplemental Methods. Subject demographic characteristics are summarized in Supplemental Table S1.

Session 1 – MAST Laboratory Stressor

To induce stress during the first session, participants completed the Maastricht Acute Stress Test (MAST; (57)). The MAST is a laboratory stress paradigm that combines alternating periods of well-validated stress-inducing procedures including a cold pressor and performance of serial subtraction in front of evaluators. For details of the MAST administration, please see Supplemental Methods.

Session 1: Sample collection and Analysis

To assess IL-6 responses, plasma samples were drawn intravenously at −10 minutes (before stressor), +45 minutes following stressor and +90 minutes following stressor. To assess salivary cortisol, saliva samples were collected at six time-points: −110 minutes (before stressor), −30 minutes, immediately before stressor, +20 minutes following stressor, +35 minutes, and +80 minutes. For details of collection and analysis, please see Supplemental Methods.

Session 2: Laboratory Stressor

For the session 2’s laboratory stressor (Figure 1), which was performed during an fMRI scan, we used a modified version of the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST; (59)), a widely used and well-validated stress-paradigm. Briefly, this task requires participants to solve arithmetic problems while their performance is publicly evaluated. For details of the MIST administration, please see Supplemental Methods. To assess salivary cortisol during session 2, saliva samples were collected at four time-points: Prior to entry into the scanner, 3 minutes prior to onset of stress blocks, +25 minutes after the onset of the stress blocks, +40 minutes after the onset of the stress blocks.

Reinforcement Learning Task

To assess RPE signals participants were asked to complete a well-validated instrumental conditioning paradigm (58). Details of the task are presented in the Supplemental Methods). Briefly, subjects were instructed to choose between two visual stimuli displayed on a screen. Each of the stimuli in the pairs was associated with either an 80% or 20% probability of a given outcome (gain: win $1 or $0; loss: lose $1 or $0; neutral: look at grey square or nothing. There were a total of 6 RL runs across the experiment, with 2 runs for each stress condition (“Pre-Stress”, “During-Stress”, “Post-Stress”).

Primary analysis focused on a parametric modulation (pmod) contrast for RPE signals extracted from an anatomically defined NAcc mask. For details on the computational model, neuroimaging acquisition, processing and ROI analysis please see Supplemental Materials.

Follow-up Period

To examine the ecological validity of biological responses to laboratory stressors, all participants were asked to complete online self-report questionnaires every two-weeks for a 4-month follow-up period. Our primary measures of interest was the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; (62)), which was used to assess ongoing perceptions of stress in daily life. We examined both mean-level of perceived stress as well as variability over time. To assess variability, we calculated mean sum of squared differences (MSSD), a standard metric used to capture variability in symptom experience (63).

Results

Session 1: Effects of Acute Stress on Plasma IL-6 and Salivary Cortisol

Using a 3 (Timepoints) repeated measures ANOVA, we found that the MAST induced a significant increase in plasma IL-6 (F(1.43,92) = 17.89, p = 8.0 × 10−6; partial η2 = 0.28) (Figure 2). This effect remained highly significant when controlling for menstrual cycle phase (70% follicular; 30% luteal) (F(1.43,90) = 16.77, p = 1.6 × 10−5; partial η2 = 0.27), and there was no Timepoints × Menstrual Cycle Phase interaction (F(1.43,90) = 0.89, p = 0.384). There was, however, a main effect of cycle phase such that participants in the luteal phase had lower levels of IL-6 than those in the follicular phase (F(1,45) = 5.24, p = 0.027; partial η2 = 0.10). Given prior studies (64), we also examined whether BMI was associated with change in IL-6, but did not find association between BMI and change in IL-6 following stress (see Supplemental Table S2). Baseline PSS scores were also unrelated to change in IL-6 levels (Spearman r = 0.10, p = 0.466), though we did observe baseline associations with the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, (see Supplemental Materials).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Change in plasma IL-6 levels (raw values) following the Maastricht Acute Stress Task (MAST). Error bars represent standard error.

Additionally, using a 6 (Timepoints) repeated measures ANOVA, we found that the MAST produced a significant increase in salivary cortisol (F(2.34,182.38.) = 27.87, p = 1.5 × 10−12), with a strong quadratic effect (F(1,78) = 33.14, p = 1.62 × 10−7) (see Supplemental Figure S1).

Session 1: Effects of Acute Stress on Mood and Relationships to IL-6

Using an 8 (Timepoints) × 5 (Questions) repeated-measures ANOVA, we found that the MAST stressor during session 1 induced a significant overall change in mood (F(3.28,553) = 70.78, p = 1.78 × 10−35), with the expected quadratic effect (F(1,79) = 125.05, p = 5.98 × 10−18) showing an increase in negative mood following the stressor (Figure 3A). This quadratic effect remained significant when controlling for Menstrual Cycle Phase (F(1,77) = 30.56, p = 4.26 × 10−7), and there was no interaction between this quadratic effect and Menstrual Cycle Phase (F(1,77) = 0.064, p = 0.801). For each individual VAMS question, quadratic effects revealed that participants felt immediately following the MAST less happy (F(1,80) = 113.84, p = 4.87 × 10−17), relaxed (F(1,80) = 98.01, p = 1.51 × 10−15), friendly (F(1,80) = 114.65, p = 4.11 × 10−17), sociable (F(1,80) = 66.79, p = 3.71 × 10−12), and quick witted (F(1,80) = 67.08, p = 3.71 × 10−12).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Stress manipulations increase negative affect. A. In session 1, the Maastricht Acute Stress Task (MAST) induced a significant increase in negative affect across all 5 VAMS questions (Happy-Sad, Relaxed-Tense, Friendly-Hostile, Sociable-Withdrawn, Quick Witted-Mentally Slow). B. Similarly, in session 2, the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST) also induced a significant decrease in mood across all 5 VAMS questions. All VAMS items are scored such that higher scores indicate more negative affect.

There were no relationships between change in IL-6 levels in response to the MAST and change in mood ratings as assessed by any of the five VAMS questions: (happy: Spearman r = 0.06, p = 0.663; relaxed: Spearman r = 0.13, p =0.345; friendly: Spearman r = .10, p = 0.473; sociable: Spearman r = .001, p = 0.992; quick witted: Spearman r = 0.02, p = 0.868).

Session 2: Effects of Acute Stress on Mood and Salivary Cortisol

Using a 5 (Timepoints) × 5 (Questions) repeated-measures ANOVA, we found that the MIST stressor during session 2 also induced a significant overall decrease in mood (F(2.05,260) = 50.65, p = 2.46 × 10−17) with a quadratic effect (F(1,65) = 67.85, p = 1.10 × 10−11) (Figure 3B). This quadratic effect remained significant when controlling for Menstrual Cycle Phase (F(1,61) = 7.28, p = 0.009), and there was no interaction between this quadratic effect and Menstrual Cycle Phase (F(1,61) = 0.003, p = 0.960). Specifically, participants reported feeling immediately following the MIST less happy (F(1,65) = 46.60, p = 3.51 × 10−9), relaxed (F(1,65) = 39.75, p = 2.88 × 10−8), friendly (F(1,65) = 62.92, p = 3.85 × 10−11), sociable (F(1,65) = 36.48, p = 8.27 × 10−8), and quick witted (F(1,65) = 24.56, p = 5.0 × 10−6). In addition to these main effects, individual differences in mood responses to stress were significantly correlated between the MAST (Session 1) and MIST (Session 2) stressors for all 5 questions (happy: Pearson r = 0.48, p = 0.0002; relaxed: r =0.31, p = 0.019; friendly: r = 0.35, p = 0.007; sociable: r = 0.38, p = 0.004; quick-witted: r = 0.46, p = 0.0004).

For salivary cortisol, a 3 (Timepoints) Repeated Measures ANOVA revealed no main effect of the MIST stressor on cortisol (F(1.71,116.06) = 21.31, p = 0.437) (See Supplemental Figure S1). This null result was driven by the absence of a positive cortisol response in approximately half of the participants, which is consistent with other studies using the MIST (59, 65). Importantly however, %change in cortisol from pre-stress to post stress during session 1 was positively correlated with %change in cortisol from pre-stress to post-stress during session 2 (Pearson r = 0.40, p = 0.006).

Session 2: Effects of Acute Stress on Behavioral Performance

A 2 (Valence: win/loss) × 3 (Stress Condition: Pre-, During-, Post-Stress) ×2 (Run Number) repeated measures ANOVA with Menstrual Cycle Phase included as a between-groups variable revealed a main effect of the Stress Condition such that performance accuracy increased over the course of the experiment (F(2,106) = 3.30, p = 0.041). There was no main effect of Valence (win/loss) (F(1,53) = 2.5, p = 0.120) nor Stress Condition × Valence interaction (F(2,106) = 1.60, p = 0.21), though follow-up t-tests did reveal a significant improvement in performance on loss trials during stress as opposed to pre-stress (t(62) = 2.96, p = 0.004), with no change in accuracy for win-trials (t(62) = 0.20, p = 0.842).

There was no main effect of Menstrual Cycle Phase, nor any interactions with Menstrual Cycle Phase and Stress Condition, though there was a significant interaction between Menstrual Cycle Phase and Valence (F(1,52) = 7.94, p = 0.007) such that women in the luteal phase showed a greater overall accuracy for win trials relative to loss trials, while women in the follicular phase showed little difference between the two.

Session 2: Prediction Error Signaling

Averaging across all RL sessions, we observed a main effect of positive RPE signals in the NAcc using a small volume correction (SVC) with a bilateral NAcc anatomical mask drawn from the Harvard-Oxford probabilistic atlas (SVC Left NAcc: × = −6, y = 10, z = −6, t = 5.25, pFWE = 0.0005; SVC Right NAcc: × = 8, y = 6, z = −4, t = 4.69, pFWE = 0.003) (Figure 4A). For negative RPE, a whole-brain analysis revealed significant activity in bilateral anterior insula and areas of dorsal anterior cingulate and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (for a full list of regions identified by RPE contrasts, please see Supplemental Table S3). There was no main effect (linear or quadratic) of the MIST stress manipulation on the magnitude of positive or negative RPE signals. Consistent with prior studies (58, 66), the strength of positive RPE signals in the NAcc was positively associated with performance accuracy across win and loss trials accuracy (see Supplemental Materials).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Positive and Negative RPE signals during reinforcement learning and relationship to stress-induced change in IL-6. A. Model-based PE signals averaged across all three stress conditions (Pre-, During- and Post-stress) and found to predict activity in ventral striatum (SVC; positive RPE) and bilateral insula/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (negative RPE). All reported regions were corrected for multiple-comparisons. Activation patterns are shown using an uncorrected height threshold of t > 2.5 for visualization purposes. B. Association between stress-induced change in Left (top) and Right (bottom) NAcc Positive RPE beta weight (RPE contrast Pre Stress – During Stress) and change in plasma IL-6 following stress. Note: Extracted values for right and left nucleus accumbens (NAcc) ROI were defined anatomically to avoid statistical non-independence (see Methods). Note that for L NAcc, one subject was a univariate outlier (Z = 4.38), but the association with change in IL-6 was unchanged when including (r = −0.39, p = 0.019) or excluding this subject (r = −0.42, p = 0.014)

Session 2: Stress-induced change in RPE signals and IL-6 (assessed in session 1)

Using extracted RPE betaweights from an anatomically defined NAcc ROI, we examined the relationships between change in IL-6 during stress (assessed in session 1) and change in NAcc RPE betaweights following stress (assessed in session 2). We observed an inverse relationship such that larger increases in IL-6 following stress at times 2 and 3 were associated with larger decreases in NAcc PE beta weights following stress (see Table 1 and Figure 4B). This effect was strongest in the left NAcc for the comparison of Pre-Stress > Post-Stress RPE signals. Importantly, the association between IL-6 and RPE remained when controlling for change in cortisol (β = −0.60, t = −3.54, p = 0.002). This targeted ROI analysis was also followed by a whole-brain analysis for both positive and negative RPE contrasts, but no region showed a significant association after controlling for multiple comparisons. There were no significant associations with baseline IL-6 and NAcc RPE across the Pre, During and Post-Stress time points, though these associations were not significantly different from the correlations observed using difference scores (see Supplemental Table S4).

Table 1.

Spearman correlations between stress-induced change in IL-6 and change in striatal RPE signals

Log IL-6 Increase
Time 1 to Time 2
Log IL-6 Increase
Time 1 to Time 3
Change in L Nacc RPE from Pre-Stress to During-Stress −0.08 −0.10
Change in R Nacc RPE from Pre-Stress to During-Stress −0.21 −0.34*
Change in L Nacc RPE from Pre-Stress to Post-Stress −0.39** −0.39*
Change in R Nacc RPE from Pre-Stress to Post-Stress 0.04 −0.16

Nacc: Nucleus accumbens

**

p < 0.01

*

p < 0.05

Follow-up Data

To assess how well inflammatory responses to a laboratory stressor predicted perceived stress over the 4-month follow-up period, we examined associations between stress-induced IL-6 levels and mean PSS scores as well as mean sum of squared differences (MSSD) in PSS scores. The latter is a commonly used measure of symptom variability over time (63). There was no relationship between stress-induced change in IL-6 response and average PSS score over the 4-month time period. However, for participants followed for at least 1 month with available IL-6 data (N = 47), greater change in IL-6 following stress predicted heightened variability of perceived stress (r = 0.39, p = 0.007; Supplemental Figure S2). We detected a similar effect for participants followed for at least 2 months (N = 44, r = 0.37, p = 0.014), 3 months (N = 40, r = 0.46, p = 0.003), and for participants completing the full 4 months of follow-up data (N = 31, r = 0.48, p = 0.007).

To demonstrate these relationships were not driven solely due to the effects of mood during the MAST, multiple regression analyses were conducted to evaluate the relationship between change in IL-6 following stress and variability of perceived stress when controlling for changes in mood ratings. When controlling for VAMS rating changes, stress-induced change in IL-6 predicted perceived stress variability more strongly (β = 0.60, t = 4.53, p = 0.00005). As an additional control, we examined whether this association remained present when controlling for baseline PSS scores, and findings were confirmed (β = −0.60, t = −3.54, p = 0.002). Finally, we additionally examined whether changes in RPE signals were similarly predictive of PSS variability, but did not observe a significant relationship for either left (r = −0.20, p = 0.146) or right (r = 0.03, p = 0.829) NAcc ROIs.

Discussion

In this study we observed that stress-induced IL-6 was significantly predictive of subsequent stress-induced changes in NAcc RPE signals during reinforcement learning. In addition, stress-induced change in IL-6 predicted variability of perceived stress in daily life over the ensuing four months even when controlling for stress-induced changes in mood. To our knowledge, this is the first study with a prospective component to link IL-6 and striatal RPE responses to stress, suggesting that individual differences in immune responses to stress may be a marker of vulnerability for stress-related effects on reward processes.

The relationship between cytokines and DA signaling is complex, and prior work suggests possible bi-directional pathways that may account for our observed relationships. One possibility is that acute increases in IL-6 may suppress striatal DA, thereby disrupting RPE signals (67). Evidence for such rapid (< 30 minutes) effects of systemic IL-6 injections on striatal DA has been found in several rodent microdialysis studies (44, 45). Moreover, such effects appear somewhat specific to striatal DA levels, and have not been detected in other regions (e.g., (68)). This interpretation is also consistent with prior work in humans showing that acute administration of cytokine inducers leads to blunted ventral striatal activity following reward cues (50), RPE signals (35), and midbrain responses to novelty (51). Similarly, chronic exposure to cytokine induces has been shown to reduce DA availability and synthesis in primates (47, 49). One caveat to this interpretation is the timing of IL-6 changes. While a statistically significant increase was observed within 30-minutes of the MAST, the magnitude of the increase was small. It is unclear whether this small increase would be sufficient to have a major effect on striatal DA. Moreover, the MIST was a less potent stressor. Consequently, it may the that the relationship is better conceptualized as a marker of individual differences in immune-striatal interactions as compared to a casual description of the direct effects of increased IL-6 on striatal function.

An alternative possibility, however, is that lower levels of DA may influence cytokine responses to stress. As noted in the introduction, DA receptors have been identified on a variety of cells within the innate immune system, including T-cells and lymphocytes (52), and may regulate immune responses in the body and brain at multiple levels. Consequently, the observed relationship may be driven by the effects of stress-induced DA release on cytokine signaling. Additionally, it should be emphasized that while our analyses focused on the association between change in IL-6 and change in RPE following stress, these results should not be taken to suggest that baseline levels in either case are necessarily unrelated.

In addition to the association between inflammatory responses to stress and RPE signals, we also observed that the magnitude of IL-6 increases following stress were predictive of variability in perceived stress during a 4-month follow-up period, but not overall mean level of perceived stress. Initially, we had hypothesized that both mean and variability on PSS might be related to IL-6 responses. One explanation for this discrepancy from our hypotheses is that mean level of stress may be more determined by the presence or absence of external stressors than variability. Importantly, we found that this relationship was robust, and remained significant even when controlling for sample attrition, baseline PSS scores, and stress-induced change in mood, thereby helping extend the ecological validity of our laboratory-based stress paradigms as a means to probe neurobiological responses to stress. Variability of symptom and risk factor expression is increasingly recognized as an important marker of psychological disorders (6972), and our data suggest that variability–rather than mean level–may be a critical factor.

An important potential caveat to our findings is the lack of concurrent assessment for all measures, particularly given the absence of main effect of the MIST stressor (session 2) on striatal RPE signals or salivary cortisol. This raises the possibility that the second stress manipulation (MIST) was not as effective as the first one (MAST), and could limit the interpretability of the Pre- vs. Post- stress change in RPE signals. Specifically, it is possible that changes in RPE signals were not due to stress, given the weakness of the MIST stressor and the use of a fixed-order design, which was chosen to maximize power for individual differences analysis. Arguing against this point is the fact that there were clear increases in negative affect, and individual differences in both salivary cortisol and mood reactivity to the MAST (session 1) and MIST (session 2) stressors were correlated, suggesting that while the session 2 stressor had a less potent effect overall, the examination of individual differences across the two session is still valid (64).

There are several other limitations worth noting. First, our sample included female participants only. This was done to limit sex-based heterogeneity in hormonal response to stress but it is unclear whether the current findings will extend to males. While possible sex differences is a critical question, the inclusion of both genders would likely have significantly reduced our statistical power for identifying individual differences. Additionally, our study design required multiple stress sessions, which may have produced some degree of habituation. Still, we observed clear affective responses to both stressors (Figure 3), and we likely reduced habituation by using two different stress manipulations. Additionally, caution is warranted in attributing the observed changes in RL performance accuracy to the stress manipulation due to the lack of a no-stress control group for the neuroimaging session. We also note that for collection of plasma samples, we used an intravenous catheter, which may have itself stimulated some degree of IL-6 production (73). That said, this effect has generally only been observed over longer time periods (e.g., > 3 hours) than were required for the current study (74). Additionally, we note that while IL-6 is generally conceptualized as being pro-inflammatory (43) it is important to note that it can also be, anti-inflammatory depending on the target (43, 75, 76).

In sum, we found that stress-induced changes in IL-6 levels were associated with both striatal reward prediction errors during reinforcement learning as well as stress sensitivity during a 4-month follow-up period. These data have important implications for understanding the relationships between stress, IL-6, and their impact on reward-related corticostriatal circuitry.

Supplementary Material

1
Download video file (5.8MB, mov)
2
Download video file (1.6MB, mov)
3
Download video file (2.6MB, mov)
4

Acknowledgments

This work was support by the National Institutes of Mental Health (R01 and R37 MH068376, MH068376-09S1 to DAP). MTT was supported by K99/R00MH102355 and R01MH108605. RA was supported a Brain and Behavior Research Foundation Young Investigator award, respectively. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors also gratefully acknowledge support from Gary Bradwin, Dan Cole, Nancy Brooks, Dave Crowley, Laurie Scott, Randy Auerbach and Christian Webb as well as the staff of the Laboratory for Affective and Translational Neuroscience at McLean Hospital.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Financial Disclosures

Over the past 3 years, MTT has served as a paid consultant to Avanir Pharmaceuticals, NeuroCog, BlackThorn Pharmaceuticals and the Boston Consulting Group. He has also received honoraria and royalties related to his writing. Over the past 3 years, Dr. Pizzagalli has received consulting fees from Akili Interactive Labs, BlackThorn Therapeutics, Pfizer, and PositScience for activities unrelated to the current research. No funding or sponsorship was provided by these companies for the current work, and all views expressed herein are solely those of the authors. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Hammen C. Stress and depression. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:293–319. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kessler RC. The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annu Rev Psychol. 1997;48:191–214. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sinha R. Chronic stress, drug use, and vulnerability to addiction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1141:105–130. doi: 10.1196/annals.1441.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Shafiei N, Gray M, Viau V, Floresco SB. Acute Stress Induces Selective Alterations in Cost/Benefit Decision-Making. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012 doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Arnsten AF. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:410–422. doi: 10.1038/nrn2648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ, Allen JJ. Social stress reactivity alters reward and punishment learning. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;6:311–320. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mather M, Lighthall NR. Both Risk and Reward are Processed Differently in Decisions Made Under Stress. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21:36–41. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, Ratner KG, Jahn AL. Increased perceived stress is associated with blunted hedonic capacity: potential implications for depression research. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:2742–2753. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. J Neurosci. 2009;29:7191–7198. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0979-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. The mesoaccumbens dopamine in coping with stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;36:79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hollon NG, Burgeno LM, Phillips PE. Stress effects on the neural substrates of motivated behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1405–1412. doi: 10.1038/nn.4114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bogdan R, Santesso DL, Fagerness J, Perlis RH, Pizzagalli DA. Corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor type 1 (CRHR1) genetic variation and stress interact to influence reward learning. J Neurosci. 2011;31:13246–13254. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2661-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA. Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1147–1154. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lemmens SG, Rutters F, Born JM, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Stress augments food ‘wanting’ and energy intake in visceral overweight subjects in the absence of hunger. Physiol Behav. 2011;103:157–163. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dias-Ferreira E, Sousa JC, Melo I, Morgado P, Mesquita AR, Cerqueira JJ, et al. Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science. 2009;325:621–625. doi: 10.1126/science.1171203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Huys QJ, Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, Dayan P. Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: a behavioural meta-analysis. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2013;3:12. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Voon V, Derbyshire K, Rück C, Irvine M, Worbe Y, Enander J, et al. Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:345–352. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gillan CM, Robbins TW. Goal-directed learning and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130475. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Chen C, Takahashi T, Nakagawa S, Inoue T, Kusumi I. Reinforcement learning in depression: a review of computational research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:247–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vrieze E, Pizzagalli DA, Demyttenaere K, Hompes T, Sienaert P, de Boer P, et al. Reduced reward learning predicts outcome in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Puglisi-Allegra S, Cestari V, Cabib S, Castellano C. Strain-dependent effects of post-training cocaine or nomifensine on memory storage involve both D1 and D2 dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994;115:157–162. doi: 10.1007/BF02244766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Deutch AY, Lee MC, Gillham MH, Cameron DA, Goldstein M, Iadarola MJ. Stress selectively increases fos protein in dopamine neurons innervating the prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1991;1:273–292. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.4.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lemos JC, Wanat MJ, Smith JS, Reyes BAS, Hollon NG, Van Bockstaele EJ, et al. Severe stress switches CRF action in the nucleus accumbens from appetitive to aversive. Nature. 2012;490:402–406. doi: 10.1038/nature11436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wanat MJ, Bonci A, Phillips PE. CRF acts in the midbrain to attenuate accumbens dopamine release to rewards but not their predictors. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:383–385. doi: 10.1038/nn.3335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Schultz WDP, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Schultz W. Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data. Physiol Rev. 2015;95:853–951. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00023.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. 2007;87:873–904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Irwin MR, Cole SW. Reciprocal regulation of the neural and innate immune systems. Nature Reviews Immunology. 2011;11:625–632. doi: 10.1038/nri3042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Slavich GM, Irwin MR. From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: A social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychol Bull. 2014;140:774. doi: 10.1037/a0035302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bower JE, Ganz PA, Aziz N, Olmstead R, Irwin MR, Cole SW. Inflammatory responses to psychological stress in fatigued breast cancer survivors: relationship to glucocorticoids. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 2007;21:251–258. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Miller AH, Raison CL. The role of inflammation in depression: from evolutionary imperative to modern treatment target. Nature Reviews Immunology. 2016;16:22–34. doi: 10.1038/nri.2015.5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Doyle WJ, Miller GE, Frank E, Rabin BS, et al. Chronic stress, glucocorticoid receptor resistance, inflammation, and disease risk. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 109:5995–5999. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1118355109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Steptoe A, Hamer M, Chida Y. The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating inflammatory factors in humans: a review and meta-analysis. Brain Behav Immun. 2007;21:901–912. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Miller AH, Maletic V, Raison CL. Inflammation and its discontents: the role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:732–741. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Harrison NA, Voon V, Cercignani M, Cooper EA, Pessiglione M, Critchley HD. A neurocomputational account of how inflammation enhances sensitivity to punishments versus rewards. Biol Psychiatry. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Berghorst LH, Bogdan R, Frank MJ, Pizzagalli DA. Acute stress selectively reduces reward sensitivity. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013:7. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lighthall NR, Gorlick MA, Schoeke A, Frank MJ, Mather M. Stress Modulates Reinforcement Learning in Younger and Older Adults. Psychol Aging. 2012 doi: 10.1037/a0029823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Lighthall NR, Mather M, Gorlick MA. Acute stress increases sex differences in risk seeking in the balloon analogue risk task. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e6002. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Zorrilla EP, Luborsky L, McKay JR, Rosenthal R, Houldin A, Tax A, et al. The relationship of depression and stressors to immunological assays: a meta-analytic review. Brain Behav Immun. 2001;15:199–226. doi: 10.1006/brbi.2000.0597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dowlati Y, Herrmann N, Swardfager W, Liu H, Sham L, Reim EK, et al. A meta-analysis of cytokines in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:446–457. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Goldsmith D, Rapaport M, Miller B. A meta-analysis of blood cytokine network alterations in psychiatric patients: comparisons between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Haapakoski R, Mathieu J, Ebmeier KP, Alenius H, Kivimäki M. Cumulative meta-analysis of interleukins 6 and 1β, tumour necrosis factor α and C-reactive protein in patients with major depressive disorder. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 2015;49:206–215. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hodes GE, Ménard C, Russo SJ. Integrating Interleukin-6 into depression diagnosis and treatment. Neurobiology of Stress. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Yohn SE, Arif Y, Haley A, Tripodi G, Baqi Y, Müller CE, et al. Effort-related motivational effects of the pro-inflammatory cytokine interleukin-6: pharmacological and neurochemical characterization. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2016;233:3575–3586. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4392-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Song C, Merali Z, Anisman H. Variations of nucleus accumbens dopamine and serotonin following systemic interleukin-1, interleukin-2 or interleukin-6 treatment. Neuroscience. 1999;88:823–836. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00271-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Qin L, Wu X, Block ML, Liu Y, Breese GR, Hong JS, et al. Systemic LPS causes chronic neuroinflammation and progressive neurodegeneration. Glia. 2007;55:453–462. doi: 10.1002/glia.20467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Felger JC, Mun J, Kimmel HL, Nye JA, Drake DF, Hernandez CR, et al. Chronic interferon-α decreases dopamine 2 receptor binding and striatal dopamine release in association with anhedonia-like behavior in nonhuman primates. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:2179–2187. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Felger JC, Alagbe O, Hu F, Mook D, Freeman AA, Sanchez MM, et al. Effects of interferon-alpha on rhesus monkeys: a nonhuman primate model of cytokine-induced depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1324–1333. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Capuron L, Pagnoni G, Drake DF, Woolwine BJ, Spivey JR, Crowe RJ, et al. Dopaminergic mechanisms of reduced Basal Ganglia responses to hedonic reward during interferon alfa administration. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:1044–1053. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Eisenberger NI, Berkman ET, Inagaki TK, Rameson LT, Mashal NM, Irwin MR. Inflammation-induced anhedonia: endotoxin reduces ventral striatum responses to reward. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:748–754. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Harrison NA, Cercignani M, Voon V, Critchley HD. Effects of inflammation on hippocampus and substantia nigra responses to novelty in healthy human participants. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:831–838. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sarkar C, Basu B, Chakroborty D, Dasgupta PS, Basu S. The immunoregulatory role of dopamine: an update. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 2010;24:525–528. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Shao W, Zhang S-z, Tang M, Zhang X-h, Zhou Z, Yin Y-q, et al. Suppression of neuroinflammation by astrocytic dopamine D2 receptors via [agr] B-crystallin. Nature. 2013;494:90–94. doi: 10.1038/nature11748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Yan Y, Jiang W, Liu L, Wang X, Ding C, Tian Z, et al. Dopamine Controls Systemic Inflammation through Inhibition of NLRP3 Inflammasome. Cell. 2015;160:62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.11.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Koretz D, Merikangas KR, et al. The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) JAMA. 2003;289:3095–3105. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.23.3095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kirschbaum C, Wüst S, Hellhammer D. Consistent sex differences in cortisol responses to psychological stress. Psychosom Med. 1992;54:648–657. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199211000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Smeets T, Cornelisse S, Quaedflieg CW, Meyer T, Jelicic M, Merckelbach H. Introducing the Maastricht Acute Stress Test (MAST): A quick and non-invasive approach to elicit robust autonomic and glucocorticoid stress responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1998–2008. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006;442:1042–1045. doi: 10.1038/nature05051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Dedovic K, Renwick R, Mahani NK, Engert V, Lupien SJ, Pruessner JC. The Montreal Imaging Stress Task: using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2005;30:319–325. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Stern RA, Arruda JE, Hooper CR, Wolfner GD, Morey CE. Visual analogue mood scales to measure internal mood state in neurologically impaired patients: description and initial validity evidence. Aphasiology. 1997;11:59–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structred Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders - Patient Edition (With Psychotic Screen) Biometrics Research Department 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Solhan MB, Trull TJ, Jahng S, Wood PK. Clinical assessment of affective instability: comparing EMA indices, questionnaire reports, and retrospective recall. Psychol Assess. 2009;21:425–436. doi: 10.1037/a0016869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Park HS, Park JY, Yu R. Relationship of obesity and visceral adiposity with serum concentrations of CRP, TNF-α and IL-6. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2005;69:29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.diabres.2004.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Dedovic K, Rexroth M, Wolff E, Duchesne A, Scherling C, Beaudry T, et al. Neural correlates of processing stressful information: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Res. 2009;1293:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Schönberg T, Daw ND, Joel D, O’Doherty JP. Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:12860–12867. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2496-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Felger JC, Treadway MT. Inflammation effects on motivation and motor activity: role of dopamine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:216–241. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Brebner K, Hayley S, Zacharko R, Merali Z, Anisman H. Synergistic effects of interleukin-1β, interleukin-6, and tumor necrosis factor-α: central monoamine, corticosterone, and behavioral variations. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:566–580. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00166-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Treadway MT, Leonard CV. Isolating biomarkers for symptomatic states: considering symptom–substrate chronometry. Mol Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Bringmann LF, Vissers N, Wichers M, Geschwind N, Kuppens P, Peeters F, et al. A network approach to psychopathology: new insights into clinical longitudinal data. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e60188. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.van de Leemput IA, Wichers M, Cramer AO, Borsboom D, Tuerlinckx F, Kuppens P, et al. Critical slowing down as early warning for the onset and termination of depression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111:87–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312114110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer U. Ambulatory assessment. Annual review of clinical psychology. 2013;9:151. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Haack M, Kraus T, Schuld A, Dalal M, Koethe D, Pollmächer T. Diurnal variations of interleukin-6 plasma levels are confounded by blood drawing procedures. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27:921–931. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00006-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Gudmundsson A, Ershler WB, Goodman B, Lent SJ, Barczi S, Carnes M. Serum concentrations of interleukin-6 are increased when sampled through an indwelling venous catheter. Clin Chem. 1997;43:2199–2201. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Wolf J, Rose-John S, Garbers C. Interleukin-6 and its receptors: a highly regulated and dynamic system. Cytokine. 2014;70:11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2014.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Petersen A, Pedersen B. The role of IL-6 in mediating the anti inflammatory. J Physiol Pharmacol. 2006;57:43–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1
Download video file (5.8MB, mov)
2
Download video file (1.6MB, mov)
3
Download video file (2.6MB, mov)
4

RESOURCES