Abstract
BACKGROUND
Hypertension is a risk factor for cerebrovascular disease and cognitive impairment. Women who suffer hypertensive episodes during pregnancy report variable neurocognitive changes within the first decade following the affected pregnancy. However, long-term follow-up of these women into their post-menopausal years has not been conducted.
OBJECTIVE
The aim of this study was to examine whether women with a history of preeclampsia were at increased risk of cognitive decline 35–40 years after the affected pregnancy.
STUDY DESIGN
Women were identified and recruited through the medical linkage, population-based Rochester Epidemiologic Project. Forty women with a history of preeclampsia were age- and parity-matched to 40 women with a history of normotensive pregnancy. All women underwent comprehensive neuropsychological assessment and completed self-report inventories measuring mood, i.e., depression, anxiety, and other symptoms related to emotional state. Scores were compared between groups. In addition, individual cognitive scores were examined by neuropsychologists and a neurologist blinded to pregnancy status, and a clinical consensus diagnosis of normal, mild cognitive impairment, or dementia for each participant was conferred.
RESULTS
Age at time of consent did not differ between preeclampsia (59.2 years, range 50.9–71.5) and normotensive (59.6 years, range 52.1–72.2) groups, nor did time from index pregnancy (34.9 years, range 32.0–47.2 vs 34.5 years, range 32.0–46.4, respectively).There were no statistically significant differences in raw scores on tests of cognition and mood between women with histories of preeclampsia compared to women with histories of normotensive pregnancy. However, a consensus diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment or dementia trended towards greater frequency in women with histories of preeclampsia compared to those with normotensive pregnancies (20% versus 8%, p = 0.10) and affected more domains among the preeclampsia group (p = 0.03), most strongly related to executive dysfunction (d = 1.96) and verbal list learning impairment (d = 1.93).
CONCLUSIONS
These findings suggest a trend for women with a history of preeclampsia to exhibit more cognitive impairment later in life than those with a history of normotensive pregnancy. Furthermore, the pattern of cognitive changes is consistent with that observed with vascular disease/white matter pathology.
Keywords: Cardiovascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, cognition, dementia, hypertensive pregnancy, mild cognitive impairment, preeclampsia
Introduction
Hypertensive disorders of pregnancy, including preeclampsia, confer increased risk of cardiovascular disease (CVD) morbidity and mortality.1 Preeclampsia occurs in about 3% of pregnancies,2 and is associated with an approximate two-fold increased risk of CVD and cerebrovascular disease.3 Some risk factors associated with preeclampsia are shared with those for CVD and cognitive decline, including hypertension, metabolic syndrome, and insulin resistance.4–7
Women with a history of hypertensive pregnancy more frequently report subjective cognitive complaints and endorse more physical and psychological symptoms that negatively impact physical, social, and emotional well-being and quality of life than women with a history of normotensive pregnancy.8–10 In addition, reported changes in cognition associate with the severity of preeclampsia11 or post-traumatic stress symptoms of preeclampsia.12 However, studies examining the impact of hypertensive pregnancy on cognitive performance report mixed results. For example, no differences in sustained attention or executive functioning were reported in women in their late 30s to early 40s, up to a decade following an index pregnancy of preeclampsia or eclampsia, compared to age-matched women with normotensive pregnancies.13 In contrast, others have found slower motor speed,8 poorer attention,12 or poorer learning and memory11 in women with a history of hypertensive pregnancy. Long-term follow-up of these women into their post-menopausal years has not been conducted. Therefore, in the present study, cognition and mood were evaluated in women approximately 35 years after preeclamptic or normotensive pregnancy. We hypothesized that women with a history of preeclampsia (hPE) would be more likely than women with a history of normotensive pregnancy (hNTP) to have cognitive impairment and that the pattern would resemble changes characteristic of vascular/white matter disease.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Recruitment details have been previously reported.14 In brief, HICDA (Hospital Adaptation of the International Classification of Diseases Adapted) codes and the population-based Rochester Epidemiology Project medical records-linkage system15 were used to identify 40 women with hPE and 40 women with hNTP. Sample size was based on power calculation to detect differences in cardiovascular parameters. To be eligible for the study, a woman had to be a resident of Olmsted County, MN when delivering a baby from a pregnancy lasting >20 weeks (live birth or stillbirth) between January 1, 1976 and December 31, 1982, had to have had a documented clinical visit within the last 2 years to confirm that women did not have a cardiovascular or other neurological event that could potentially confound results, and had to live within 120 miles of Olmsted County in order to be available for in-person visits.
The medical records of all women were fully abstracted for demographic and clinical information. A potential exposure was confirmed as preeclampsia if a woman had at least one preeclamptic pregnancy between 1976–1982 that met the standard definition: 1) two or more blood pressure readings of a systolic blood pressure (SBP) > 140 mm Hg or a diastolic blood pressure (DBP) >90 mm Hg at least 4 hours apart after 20 weeks gestation and 2) new onset proteinuria, as defined by a urine dipstick 1+, or proteinuria ≥0.300 g /24 hours, or a protein/creatinine ratio equivalent to ≥ 0.300 g per 24 hours. Emergency room visits were not included. Women were sequentially contacted and recruited. Each of the 40 women with hPE was age and parity- matched to a woman with hNTP. All women were postmenopausal and index pregnancies were first pregnancies. Eligible women were sent a letter describing the study, given contact information, and if no response within two weeks, contacted by phone. Of the 77 women with confirmed hPE, 25 (32%) refused and 7 (9%) did not respond. Five (6%) were found to be ineligible after further screening. For women with hNTP, 104 women were contacted, 18 (17%) refused, 41 (39%) did not respond, and 5 (5%) wanted to participate but another matched control had already agreed.
As the primary focus of this study was to understand the potential mechanisms that place women with hPE at risk for subclinical CVD and cognitive decline, women were excluded with a medical-record confirmed clinical diagnosis of myocardial infarction, congestive heart failure, stroke, dementia, cancer (excluding non-melanoma skin cancer), autoimmune disease (e.g., multiple sclerosis, lupus), and neurological conditions (e.g., epilepsy). All protocols were approved by Mayo Clinic and Olmsted Medical Center Institutional Review Boards (PR10-005198-05) and all participants gave written informed consent.
Clinical Assessment of Cardiovascular Risk
Demographic and clinical data obtained from medical records and patient interviews at the time of the in-clinic assessment included age, body mass index (BMI), SBP, DBP, anti-hypertensive and-lipid lowering medications, and chart-abstracted and physician-confirmed diagnoses of hypertension, hyperlipidemia and diabetes mellitus. The diagnosis of hypertension was confirmed if a prior diagnosis and/or use of prescription anti-hypertensive medication were confirmed upon medical record review, or if a SBP≥ 140 mm Hg or DBP≥ 90 mm Hg was documented in the medical records on two separate occasions. The diagnosis of dyslipidemia was confirmed if one or more of the following criteria were met: use of lipid-lowering drugs or laboratory measurements revealing a total cholesterol ≥ 200 mg/dL, triglycerides ≥ 150 mg/dL, or HDL ≤ 50 mg/dL. Diabetes mellitus was diagnosed by either a HgA1c≥ 6.5% or a fasting glucose > 126 mg/dL, or a physician diagnosis in the past, with or without current glucose-lowering agents. Coronary artery calcification (CAC) is a measurement of the amount of calcium in the walls of the arteries that supply the heart muscle and is recorded in Agatston Units (AU). The higher the number, the greater the amount of plaque. CAC was evaluated by computed tomography and has been previously reported.14
Cognitive Assessment
Women underwent comprehensive cognitive testing administered by an experienced psychometrist under the supervision of a neuropsychologist. The 2.5-hour battery included standardized and validated tests of attention, working memory, psychomotor processing speed, executive functioning, language, perceptual processing, learning and memory administered in a fixed order. Women also completed self-report mood questionnaires one week prior to or at the same time as cognitive testing. (See Table 1 for complete description of cognitive and mood measures.)
Table 1.
Measures of cognition and mood
Attention | Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Digit Span measures auditory attention and working memory capacity. This test involves repetition of oral sequences of numbers of increasing length exactly as they are presented in the Forward condition and then in reverse in the Backward condition. Digit Span Forward measures simple auditory attention capacity and Digit Span Backward measures working memory capacity. Score is total number of forward and backward sequences repeated correctly. |
Trail Making Test-Part A measures visual attention and processing speed. It is a paper-and-pencil test requiring rapid connection of consecutively numbered circles. Score is time (in seconds) to complete the trial, with a higher score reflecting poorer performance. | |
| |
Working Memory | Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Letter-Number Sequencing measures auditory working memory. Letters and numbers of increasing length are orally presented in random order. Mental manipulation is required to rearrange the numbers and letters and then repeat in ascending order. Score is total number of correct sequences |
| |
Psychomotor Processing | Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Digit Symbol Coding measures visual scanning, motor persistence, sustained attention, response speed, and visuomotor coordination. It involves quickly transcribing symbols into blank spaces below numbers that correspond to symbol-number pairs presented in a key above. Score is the total number of symbols correctly transcribed within a specified amount of time. |
| |
Executive Functioning | Trail Making Test-Part B measures visuomotor speed, divided attention, and cognitive flexibility. It increases demands of Trail Making Test-Part A by requiring rapid connection of not only consecutively numbered circles but also lettered circles and alternating between them. Score is time (in seconds) to complete the trial, with a higher score reflecting poorer performance. |
| |
Language | Phonemic (C, F, L) and Semantic (animals, fruits, vegetables) Verbal Fluency measure the ability to produce fluent speech. It has also been used to assess the ability to think flexibly and to gauge how efficiently one uses search and retrieval strategies to organize thoughts. The objective is to rapidly generate as many words as possible within a certain amount of time that begin with specified letters and belong to specified categories. Scores are total number of words generated for the three letters and the three categories. |
Boston Naming Test measures the ease and accuracy of word retrieval and how intact semantic networks are. It also gives some indication of vocabulary level. This test requires providing the name for common objects presented in black and white drawings. Score is total number of objects correctly named. | |
| |
Visuospatial Processing | Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Picture Completion measures the ability to measure attention to visual detail. Pictures of common objects and scenes with a detail missing from them are presented, and the examinee is asked to identify what is missing. Score is total number of correct responses. |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Block Design measures visual problem-solving and understanding of part-to-whole relationships. It involves manual manipulation of 3-dimensional blocks to match 2-dimensional line drawings. Score is total number of correct designs. | |
| |
Learning and Memory | Auditory Verbal Learning Test measures learning efficiency, immediate memory span, sensitivity to interference with learning and recall, and rates of forgetting and retention. A list of unrelated words is serially presented. The object is to learn as many words as possible over repeated trials, recall them after being presented a distractor list, and recall them again after a 30-minute delay. Several scores are derived: The Learning score is the sum of the number of words recalled immediately on each trial, the Delay score is the number of words recalled after a long delay, and the % Retention score is the number of words on the delayed recall trial divided by the number of words recalled on the last learning trial. |
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Logical Memory I & II measure learning, recall, and retention of logically organized verbal information. Paragraph-length prose passages are read out loud to the examinee, who is to recall as much information from the stories as possible immediately following presentation and again after a 25- to 35-minute delay. Logical Memory I score is the total number of story details recalled immediately following presentation, Logical Memory II is the total number of story details recalled after a longer delay, and % Retention is the number of story details on delayed recall divided by immediate recall. | |
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Visual Reproduction I & II measure visual learning, memory, and retention of geometric designs. Cards with line drawings are presented for a brief amount of time, taken away, and then the examinee is asked to reproduce the designs as accurately as possible. After a 25- to 35-minute delay, the examinee is asked to again reproduce as many of the designs as possible, but this time without being shown the cards. Visual Reproduction I score is the total number of design details recalled immediately following presentation, Visual Reproduction II is the total number of design details recalled after a longer delay, and % Retention is the number of design details on delayed recall divided by immediate recall. | |
| |
Mood State | Beck Depression Inventory-II is a 21-item self-report scale measuring a range of affective, cognitive, and physiologic symptoms of depression (e.g., sense of failure, loss of interest, indecisiveness, appetite, libido). Each item contains four statements of graded severity expressing how a person might feel or think about the aspect of depression under consideration, with scores ranging from 0 for absence of problems in that area to 3 for the most severe level of that problem. The score is the sum of all of all statements endorsed, with a higher score representing greater severity. |
Beck Anxiety Inventory is a 21-item self-report scale measuring a range of subjective, physiologic, autonomic, and panic-related symptoms of anxiety (e.g., unable to relax, hands trembling, heart pounding or racing, fear of the worst happening). Each item contains four statements of graded severity expressing how a person might feel or think about the aspect of depression under consideration, with scores ranging from 0 for absence of problems in that area to 3 for the most severe level of that problem. The score is the sum of all of all statements endorsed, with a higher score representing greater severity. | |
Profile of Mood States is a 65-item self-report measure of six mood states: Tension-Anxiety, Depression-Dejection, Anger-Hostility, Vigor-Activity, Fatigue-Inertia, and Confusion-Bewilderment. It contains adjectives that describe how a person may be feeling (e.g., unhappy, relaxed, exhausted, rebellious, efficient), and individuals are asked to rate how much during the past week they have had that feeling. Scores range from 0 to 4, with 0 being not at all and 4 being extremely. A sub-scale score is derived for each of the six mood states, with a higher score representing greater difficulty in all but the Vigor-Activity state, where a higher score is better. |
Clinical Assessment of Cognitive Impairment
Three experienced neuropsychologists and a behavioral neurologist blinded to pregnancy status independently examined age-corrected scaled scores for each of the 80 women and assigned ratings of normal, single-domain mild cognitive impairment (MCI), multiple-domain MCI, or dementia. A consensus diagnosis was determined based on agreement of at least three raters.
Most diagnostic criteria require objective evidence of mild impairment (typically 1 to 1.5 standard deviations below the normative mean and a decline from baseline functioning) in one or more cognitive domains. For this study, incorporating published guidelines and criteria,16–18 MCI was defined as mild impairment on two or more measures within a single domain, or mild impairment on one or more measures within at least two domains. A diagnosis of dementia was considered if there were cognitive deficits ≥ 2 SD below the mean in two or more domains. Education was used to estimate baseline functioning.
Statistical Analyses
Demographic, clinical, and cognitive test data were summarized with descriptive statistics, including percentages for discrete variables and quartiles (median, 25th and 75th percentiles) for continuous variables. Bivariate comparisons between groups with hPE or hNTP were performed using the Chi-square test or Wilcoxon rank sum test, or with a Cochran-Armitage trend test for ordinal measures of cognitive impairment. To correct for multiple comparisons among all cognitive tests that were included, P-values were adjusted according to the method described by Benjamini and Hochberg.19 The magnitude of effect on measures of cognitive functioning was estimated with Cohen’s d standardized effect size.20 Convention suggests .2 is a small effect size, .5 moderate, and .8 large. All data were recorded and managed in a secure database (Medidata Rave, Medidata Solutions Inc.), and were analyzed using SAS statistical software (version 9.4, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).
Results
Demographics and current clinical characteristics by pregnancy status are presented in Table 2. Compared to women with hNTP, women with hPE had higher BMI (29.8 kg/m2 [IQR 25.9–33.7] vs 25.3 kg/m2 [IQR 23.1–32.0], p = 0.004), more CAC (0.0 AU [IQR 0.0–28.0] vs 0.0 AU [IQR 0.0–0.3], p = 0.007), and were also more frequently diagnosed with hypertension (60% vs 20%, p < 0.001). More women with hPE than with hNTP were taking medications that could potentially affect cognition (73% vs 40%, p = 0.003). In the hNTP group, none of the women with MCI were taking them. In the hPE group, three of the women with MCI were taking them but not the one with dementia.
Table 2.
Demographics and clinical characteristics by hypertensive pregnancy status
Variable | Normotensive (n=40) | Preeclampsia (n=40) | P-value |
---|---|---|---|
Age at study consent | 59.6 (56.2, 62.5) | 59.2 (56.3, 62.5) | 0.814 |
Age at 1st live birth | 24.0 (22.3, 26.3) | 24.5 (21.7, 25.8) | 0.931 |
Time since 1st live birth/index pregnancy (yrs) | 34.5 (33.6, 36.7) | 34.9 (32.9, 36.7) | 0.564 |
Parity | 3.0 (2.0, 3.0) | 3.0 (2.0, 3.0) | 0.967 |
Education: | 0.219 | ||
High school or less | 3 (8%) | 6 (15%) | |
Some college/tech/voc | 21 (53%) | 22 (55%) | |
College graduate or higher | 16 (40%) | 12 (30%) | |
BMI (kg/m2) | 25.3 (23.1, 32.0) | 29.8 (25.9, 33.7) | 0.023 |
Diastolic blood pressure (mm Hg) | 75.2 (69.7, 84.0) | 79.7 (69.3, 83.3) | 0.368 |
Systolic blood pressure (mm Hg) | 128.7 (116.5, 145.7) | 131.7 (119.7, 140.2) | 0.613 |
LDL cholesterol (mg/dL) | 123.0 (99.7, 136.4) | 106.1 (87.9, 124.3) | 0.087 |
HDL cholesterol (mg/dL) | 64.0 (50.5, 76.5) | 54.5 (41.0, 69.5) | 0.054 |
Triglycerides (mg/dL) | 97.5 (72.0, 123.5) | 108.0 (85.0, 163.0) | 0.078 |
Fasting glucose (mg/dL) | 95.5 (91.0, 101.5) | 98.0 (91.5, 109.5) | 0.151 |
Diabetes | 2 (5%) | 4 (10%) | 0.414 |
Albumin/creatinine ratio (mg/mmol) | 2.0 (0.00, 6.1) | 2.4 (0.00, 5.1) | 0.992 |
Past or current hormone therapy | 17 (43%) | 17 (43%) | 1.000 |
Ongoing hormone therapy | 8 (20%) | 6 (15%) | 0.556 |
17β-estradiol (pg/mL) | 3.5 (2.1, 9.5) | 5.4 (2.4, 7.8) | 0.495 |
Estrone (pg/mL) | 22.0 (14.0, 32.0) | 26.0 (14.5, 32.5) | 0.956 |
Coronary artery calcification: (AU) | 0.0 (0.0, 0.3) | 0.0 (0.0, 28.0) | 0.007 |
ApoE-4 polymorphism | 9 (23%) | 11 (28%) | 0.606 |
Hypertension | 8 (20%) | 24 (60%) | <.001 |
Medication with potential cognitive side effects | 16 (40%) | 29 (73%) | 0.003 |
Obstructive sleep apnea | 7 (18%) | 12 (30%) | 0.189 |
Family history of dementia | 13 (33%) | 12 (30%) | 0.809 |
Continuous variables are reported as median (25th, 75th percentiles) whereas discrete variables are presented as count (%).
AU = Agatston Unit; BMI = body mass index.
After correction for multiple comparisons, there were no significant differences in women with hNTP or hPE on any measure of cognition or mood. Test scores for each cognitive and mood variable are presented in Table 3. Cohen’s d for magnitude of effect between scores from women with hNTP or hPE indicated moderate effect sizes for Logical Memory I (d = 0.52) and Logical Memory II (d = 0.65), with lower scores in the hPE group. When examining clinical consensus diagnosis, in which impairment was rated on a scale from none to dementia and secondly as a count (observed range, 0–4) of affected domains (Table 4), there was a more widespread pattern of cognitive impairment in women with hPE compared to women with hNTP (p = 0.03). Using a binary measure in which all classifications of cognitive impairment are grouped together, there was a modest yet non-significant difference (p = 0.10) in the frequency of cognitive impairment among women with hPE (n=8; 20%) compared to women with hNTP (n=3; 8%). Further, two of the three women with hNTP had single-domain (memory) and one had multi-domain MCI, while all of the eight women with hPE and cognitive impairment had multiple domains affected, including seven with MCI and one with dementia. None of the women with cognitive impairment reported depression or anxiety sufficient to explain cognitive scores (i.e., none reported depression and one with hPE reported marginal mild anxiety). Comparing women with hPE with cognitive impairment to hPE without, the largest effect sizes were observed for Trail Making Test B—a test of cognitive speed and flexibility used to assess executive functioning (d = 1.96), followed by verbal learning (d = 1.93), visual memory (d = 1.87), and auditory attention (d = 1.69) (Table 5).
Table 3.
Cognitive and mood comparisons (raw scores) by hypertensive pregnancy status
Cognitive Domain | Variable |
Normotensive (n=40) |
Preeclampsia (n=40) |
P-value | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Attention | Trail Making Test A (time/secs)† | 24.0 (20.0, 28.0) | 25.5 (20.5, 31.0) | 0.643 | 0.39 |
Digit Span | 16.0 (13.5, 18.0) | 15.5 (13.5, 18.0) | 0.887 | 0.12 | |
| |||||
Working Memory | Letter-Number Sequencing | 10.0 (9.0, 11.0) | 9.0 (8.0, 11.0) | 0.778 | 0.07 |
| |||||
Psychomotor Speed | Digit Symbol Coding | 60.5 (55.0, 66.5) | 59.0 (52.0, 64.0) | 0.778 | 0.29 |
| |||||
Executive Functioning | Trail Making Test B (time/secs)† | 56.5 (51.5, 66.0) | 64.0 (50.5, 77.5) | 0.643 | 0.37 |
| |||||
Language | Letter Fluency | 39.0 (32.5, 46.0) | 41.0 (35.5, 45.5) | 0.887 | 0.00 |
Category Fluency | 52.0 (46.5, 60.5) | 50.5 (42.5, 61.0) | 0.778 | 0.21 | |
Boston Naming Test | 57.0 (55.5, 59.0) | 57.0 (55.0, 58.5) | 0.778 | 0.24 | |
| |||||
Visuospatial | Picture Completion | 15.0 (14.0, 16.0) | 15.0 (14.0, 16.0) | 0.981 | 0.07 |
Block Design | 27.0 (24.0, 35.5) | 27.5 (22.0, 32.0) | 0.778 | 0.21 | |
| |||||
Learning/Immediate Memory | AVLT Trials 1–5 | 52.5 (46.0, 59.0) | 50.0 (47.0, 55.5) | 0.738 | 0.30 |
Logical Memory I | 27.0 (23.0, 30.0) | 24.0 (20.0, 28.0) | 0.333 | 0.52 | |
Visual Reproduction I | 32.0 (29.0, 36.0) | 34.0 (31.0, 35.5) | 0.778 | 0.13 | |
| |||||
Delayed Memory | AVLT Delay | 10.5 (9.0, 12.5) | 10.5 (8.0, 12.0) | 0.778 | 0.22 |
AVLT % Retention | 83.0 (64.0, 100.0) | 84.0 (68.0, 93.0) | 0.915 | 0.11 | |
Logical Memory II | 23.0 (19.0, 26.5) | 19.0 (14.5, 23.0) | 0.161 | 0.65 | |
Logical Memory % Retention | 87.5 (76.5, 94.0) | 79.0 (66.0, 88.5) | 0.309 | 0.48 | |
Visual Reproduction II | 26.5 (24.0, 32.0) | 30.0 (27.0, 33.0) | 0.778 | 0.04 | |
Visual Reproduction % Ret. | 83.5 (75.5, 97.0) | 92.0 (79.0, 96.5) | 0.778 | 0.03 | |
| |||||
Mood | Beck Depression Inventory,† | 2.0 (0.0, 5.5) | 4.0 (1.0, 7.5) | 0.643 | 0.14 |
Beck Anxiety Inventory,† | 1.5 (0.0, 4.0) | 3.0 (1.0, 6.0) | 0.580 | 0.07 | |
POMS Anger,† | 0.0 (0.0, 1.0) | 0.0 (0.0, 2.5) | 0.643 | 0.23 | |
POMS Confusion† | 5.0 (4.0, 7.0) | 5.0 (4.0, 7.5) | 0.887 | 0.02 | |
POMS Depression Score,† | 1.0 (0.0, 2.5) | 1.0 (0.0, 5.0) | 0.778 | 0.02 | |
POMS Fatigue Score,† | 1.0 (0.0, 4.0) | 2.5 (0.0, 4.5) | 0.643 | 0.36 | |
POMS Tension Score,† | 2.0 (1.0, 4.5) | 2.0 (1.0, 4.5) | 0.981 | 0.09 | |
POMS Vigor Score | 18.5 (13.5, 23.5) | 16.5 (11.0, 21.5) | 0.643 | 0.32 |
Scores are presented as median (25th, 75th percentiles). No significant differences were found after correction for multiple comparisons.
Higher score is worse. Scores are total raw scores. AVLT = Auditory Verbal Learning Test; POMS = Profile of Mood States.
Table 4.
Consensus-based assessment of cognitive status
Variable |
Normotensive (n=40) N (%) |
Preeclampsia (n=40) N (%) |
P-value |
---|---|---|---|
Cognitive Impairment | 0.03~ | ||
None | 37 (93%) | 32 (80%) | |
MCI-single domain | 2 (5%) | 0 (0%) | |
MCI-multiple domains | 1 (3%) | 7 (18%) | |
Dementia | 0 (0%) | 1 (3%) | |
Number of Domains Affected | 0.03~ | ||
0 | 37 (93%) | 32 (80%) | |
1 | 2 (5%) | 0 (0%) | |
2 | 1 (3%) | 5 (13%) | |
3 | 0 (0%) | 1 (3%) | |
4 | 0 (0%) | 2 (5%) | |
| |||
MCI/Dementia | 0.10 | ||
No | 37 (93%) | 32 (80%) | |
Yes | 3 (8%) | 8 (20%) |
Measure of impairment analyzed as an ordinal variable with the Cochran-Armitage trend test.
MCI = mild cognitive impairment.
Table 5.
Cognitive and mood comparisons (raw scores) by cognitive status in women with preeclampsia
Cognitive Domain | Variable |
No Impairment (n=32) |
Impairment (n=8) |
P-value | Cohen’s d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Attention | Trail Making Test A (time/secs)† | 25.0 (20.5, 31.0) | 27.0 (21.0, 43.0) | 0.753 | 0.45 |
Digit Span | 17.0 (14.0, 19.0) | 11.0 (11.0, 14.0) | 0.016 | 1.69 | |
| |||||
Working Memory | Letter-Number Sequencing | 10.0 (8.5, 11.0) | 8.0 (6.5, 8.5) | 0.018 | 1.32 |
| |||||
Psychomotor Speed | Digit Symbol Coding | 59.0 (55.0, 65.5) | 54.5 (34.0, 64.0) | 0.487 | 0.83 |
| |||||
Executive Functioning | Trail Making Test B (time/secs)† | 59.5 (48.5, 72.0) | 101.0 (70.0, 157.0) | 0.018 | 1.96 |
| |||||
Language | Letter Fluency | 42.0 (36.5, 47.5) | 32.5 (23.0, 40.5) | 0.034 | 1.25 |
Category Fluency | 54.5 (48.0, 62.5) | 40.5 (31.0, 44.5) | 0.018 | 1.40 | |
Boston Naming Test | 57.0 (55.0, 59.0) | 54.5 (52.0, 58.0) | 0.243 | 0.72 | |
| |||||
Visuospatial | Picture Completion | 15.0 (14.0, 16.0) | 14.0 (12.0, 15.0) | 0.178 | 0.85 |
Block Design | 28.0 (25.0, 32.0) | 22.5 (16.0, 26.0) | 0.108 | 0.95 | |
| |||||
Learning/Immediate Memory | AVLT Trials 1–5 | 53.0 (47.0, 56.0) | 41.5 (29.5, 47.0) | 0.016 | 1.93 |
Logical Memory I | 25.0 (22.5, 29.0) | 18.5 (16.5, 23.0) | 0.019 | 1.40 | |
Visual Reproduction I | 34.5 (32.0, 36.0) | 31.5 (28.5, 34.0) | 0.134 | 0.69 | |
| |||||
Delayed Memory | AVLT Delay | 11.0 (9.0, 12.0) | 7.0 (4.0, 9.0) | 0.018 | 1.44 |
AVLT % Retention | 87.5 (69.0, 93.0) | 76.5 (46.0, 85.0) | 0.228 | 0.81 | |
Logical Memory II | 20.0 (15.0, 24.5) | 15.5 (10.0, 19.5) | 0.116 | 0.92 | |
Logical Memory % Retention | 79.0 (69.0, 88.5) | 78.0 (55.0, 89.0) | 0.897 | 0.26 | |
Visual Reproduction II | 30.0 (28.0, 33.5) | 16.0 (11.5, 27.0) | 0.026 | 1.87 | |
Visual Reproduction % Ret. | 93.0 (81.5, 96.5) | 64.5 (33.5, 91.0) | 0.134 | 1.72 | |
| |||||
Mood | Beck Depression Inventory,† | 4.0 (1.0, 6.5) | 3.5 (1.0, 9.0) | 0.933 | 0.08 |
Beck Anxiety Inventory,† | 3.0 (1.0, 6.0) | 3.5 (0.5, 6.0) | 0.922 | 0.15 | |
POMS Anger,† | 0.0 (0.0, 2.5) | 0.5 (0.0, 4.0) | 0.854 | 0.02 | |
POMS Confusion† | 5.0 (3.5, 7.5) | 7.0 (5.0, 7.5) | 0.409 | 0.39 | |
POMS Depression Score,† | 1.0 (0.0, 5.0) | 0.0 (0.0, 4.5) | 0.659 | 0.14 | |
POMS Fatigue Score,† | 2.0 (0.0, 4.5) | 3.0 (0.0, 8.0) | 0.895 | 0.33 | |
POMS Tension Score,† | 2.0 (1.0, 4.5) | 1.5 (0.5, 4.5) | 0.640 | 0.13 | |
POMS Vigor Score | 16.0 (11.0, 21.5) | 18.0 (13.5, 20.5) | 0.753 | 0.19 |
Scores are presented as median (25th, 75th percentiles).
Higher score is worse. Scores are total raw scores. Significant group differences are corrected for multiple comparisons. AVLT = Auditory Verbal Learning Test; POMS = Profile of Mood States.
In women with hPE, the amount of CAC was greater in those who had cognitive impairment versus those who did not (67.5 AU [IQR 6.0–180.0] vs 0.0 AU [IQR 0.0–25.0], p = 0.043). There was no significant difference in carriers of ApoE-4 polymorphism between hPE women with and without cognitive impairment nor was there any difference in use of hormone replacement therapy, BMI, frequency of hypertension, or use of medications with cognitive side effects (Table 6).
Table 6.
Demographics and clinical characteristics of women with a history of preeclampsia by cognitive status
Variable | No Impairment (n=32) | Impairment (n=8) | P-value |
---|---|---|---|
Age at study consent | 58.3 (55.6, 61.2) | 63.2 (59.3, 65.4) | 0.063 |
Age at 1st live birth | 24.4 (21.7, 25.2) | 26.4 (22.1, 29.9) | 0.166 |
Time since 1st live birth/index pregnancy (yrs) | 34.9 (32.9, 36.7) | 35.2 (32.7, 37.0) | 0.892 |
Parity | 3.0 (2.0, 3.5) | 2.0 (2.0, 2.5) | 0.055 |
Education: | 0.053 | ||
High school or less | 3 (9%) | 3 (38%) | |
Some college/tech/voc | 18 (56%) | 4 (50%) | |
College graduate or higher | 11 (34%) | 1 (13%) | |
BMI (kg/m2) | 29.4 (25.7, 33.3) | 32.5 (27.8, 34.1) | 0.398 |
Diastolic blood pressure (mm Hg) | 78.7 (71.0, 82.2) | 83.0 (66.5, 94.8) | 0.407 |
Systolic blood pressure (mm Hg) | 130.8 (119.7, 139.2) | 136.5 (124.3, 144.7) | 0.398 |
LDL cholesterol (mg/dL) | 106.1 (90.7, 124.3) | 93.1 (70.6, 131.1) | 0.447 |
HDL cholesterol (mg/dL) | 49.0 (37.5, 68.5) | 60.0 (48.5, 84.5) | 0.171 |
Triglycerides (mg/dL) | 116.0 (91.0, 178.0) | 97.5 (77.0, 114.5) | 0.223 |
Fasting glucose (mg/dL) | 101.0 (91.5, 112.0) | 97.5 (90.0, 98.5) | 0.457 |
Diabetes | 3 (9%) | 1 (13%) | 1.000 |
Albumin/creatinine ratio (mg/mmol) | 2.6 (0.0, 5.1) | 2.3 (0.0, 7.2) | 0.901 |
Past or current hormone therapy | 13 (41%) | 4 (50%) | 0.631 |
Ongoing hormone therapy | 5 (16%) | 1 (13%) | 1.000 |
17β-estradiol (pg/mL) | 5.5 (2.4, 8.0) | 4/0 (2.5, 6.4) | 0.447 |
Estrone (pg/mL) | 26.5 (15.0, 34.0) | 22.5 (8.0, 29.5) | 0.302 |
Coronary artery calcification: (AU) | 0.0 (0.0, 25.0) | 67.5 (6.0, 180.0) | 0.043 |
ApoE-4 polymorphism | 8 (25%) | 3 (38%) | 0.479 |
Hypertension | 18 (56%) | 6 (75%) | 0.333 |
Medication with potential cognitive side effects | 25 (78%) | 4 (50%) | 0.111 |
Obstructive sleep apnea | 8 (25%) | 4 (50%) | 0.168 |
Family History of Dementia | 8 (25%) | 4 (50%) | 0.168 |
Continuous variables are reported as median (25th, 75th percentiles) whereas discrete variables are presented as count (percentage).
AU = Agatston Unit; BMI = body mass index.
Comment
The first major finding from this study was a trend towards cognitive impairment being more frequently clinically diagnosed in women with hPE than in women with hNTP. Second, women with hPE showed a more diffuse range (i.e., multiple domains) of cognitive impairment, most prominently affecting abilities commonly ascribed to frontal subcortical brain processing, such as executive functioning, verbal learning, and attention.
Previous studies show that women with hPE exhibit variable neurocognitive changes in the first decade following the index pregnancy.8,13 Results of the present study extend the literature by providing long-term objective and clinical assessment of women with hPE 35 years after the affected pregnancy and are consistent with prior studies showing no differences in scores on objective cognitive measures between women with and without hPE. However, using a clinical approach where women were diagnosed with MCI or dementia according to standard criteria, more women with hPE than hNTP met such criteria. Differences in outcomes based on individual clinical characterization and effect sizes versus statistical group comparisons that obscure important individual differences may help explain disparate results in the literature, for example, frequent subjective cognitive complaints reported by women with hPE yet no objective psychometric evidence.
Factors known to increase brain vulnerability and associate with CVD and cognitive dysfunction include hypertension,4,5,21 diabetes,22 dyslipidemia,23 obesity and BMI,24,25 carotid intima-media thickness,26 CAC,27,28 and genetic polymorphisms (e.g., ApoE-4). 29,30 Many of these factors also share association with hypertensive pregnancy disorders.1,31–35 Our study is consistent with the literature in that women with hPE had higher BMI, higher CAC, and were more frequently hypertensive than women with hNTP. In the women with hPE, those with cognitive impairment had more CAC than those without cognitive impairment.
In preeclampsia, pathophysiological mechanisms associated with vascular disease may exist before, and persist long after, the affected pregnancy.36–39 It is unclear whether hypertensive pregnancies induce vascular changes or whether they further stimulate a cascade of already evolving changes. Vascular pathology is present in 29% to 41% of dementia cases that come to autopsy.40–42 Micro- and macro-vascular changes that disrupt blood flow integrity can cause structural and functional brain changes,43 which can lead to vascular cognitive impairment directly44 or indirectly by disrupting white matter pathways.45–47 White matter hyperintensities on imaging associate with decreased performance on tests of executive functioning and psychomotor speed and processing with a fair amount of consistency, and more variably to diminished learning, episodic memory, and visual memory and organization.48,49 Although most of these studies have focused on persons over the age of 65, findings were similar in a large cohort where the average age was 61 (range 34–88).48 This suggests that vascular changes associated with cognitive changes may not be purely age-related and lend support to changes observed in the current sample of younger women (age 59, range 52–72).
The association of white matter hyperintensities and decreased cognitive performance on tasks of executive functioning and psychomotor processing48,55 raises speculation that compromised white matter tracts result in the disruption of cortical-subcortical pathways that subserve these functions.47,54,59,66 Indeed, in women with hPE examined in their late 30s, approximately 5 years from index pregnancy, white matter lesions were greater compared to women with hNTP.50 In further support that hypertensive pregnancy is an independent predictor of change in cognition and brain structure, women with a history of hypertensive pregnancy disorders have been shown to have smaller brain volumes than women with normotensive pregnancies decades later, even after adjusting for traditional cardiovascular risk factors,51 in addition to increased risk of CAC.14 A recent study found no relationship between white matter hyperintensities, objective cognitive performance, or subjective cognitive complaints in formerly preeclamptic women 40 years of age 6 years after pregnancy,52 and we postulate that these women have not yet reached an age threshold that challenges cognitive reserve.
The fact that 25% of women with normal cognition and 38% with impaired cognition in our preeclampsia group were found to have an ApoE-4 polymorphism suggests that this gene may not independently increase risk of cognitive decline, but rather, interact with other underlying disease pathology, including cerebrovascular disease.53 Hormone replacement therapy, too, has been linked to negative cognitive function, and in particular, conjugated equine estrogen (CEE).54 In our study, we do not have information on type of hormone treatments, but current hormone levels (17β-estradiol and estrone) were measured in all women and did not differ between groups (see Tables 2 and 6).
A goal of the study was to assess the subclinical effect of hPE on cognitive functioning. A strength of our study is our extensive medical record data that allowed us to closely match women with and without hPE and to exclude women with potential neurologic confounding comorbidities.
Limitations of this study include the small sample size and potential response and selection biases. If women with hPE experience more cognitive decline and are more self-aware of cognitive inefficiencies, it may be that some were less likely to participate for fear of “confirming” their subjective experience of cognitive loss. In addition, a reported history of dementia was exclusionary. If this study captured a “more normal than not” sample, it might explain the lack of significant cognitive differences in comparisons of test scores between women with and without hPE. Results may also be biased towards “less physically healthy” women since women per inclusion criteria who did not have a documented clinical visit within the last two years, perhaps because they were healthier, were not contacted. Lastly, this study reflects Olmsted County, Minnesota, which is primarily white, non-Hispanic, and fairly highly educated. Higher levels of education and cognitive reserve55–57 have been associated with reduced risk of cognitive decline and dementia. Both groups were comparably educated, which could limit our ability to detect subtle cognitive changes. Women with hPE and cognitive impairment were less highly educated than those without cognitive impairment, although the sample size is too small for this to be a meaningful finding. In addition, 23 of the 40 hNTP and 26 of the 40 hPE women reported ethnicity, and all identified as white/Caucasian. Therefore, these findings may not generalize to women with lower education or of other races and ethnicities.
In summary, women with hPE more frequently have a higher BMI, hypertension, and metabolic dysregulation, exacerbating an increased risk of cardiovascular morbidity and mortality over the life course. Cardiovascular disease is associated with cognitive decline and dementia. Results of this study suggest a trend for women with a history of preeclampsia to exhibit more cognitive impairment 35 years later than women with similar demographics but who experienced a normotensive pregnancy. The mechanisms underlying this difference are unclear, but could be reflect common mechanisms contributing to CAC and brain changes, such as white matter lesions. White matter lesions are common even in the early years following index pregnancy, and the pattern of cognitive impairment observed in the present study appears to reflect disruption in frontal subcortical white matter tracts, although a larger sample size will be needed to delineate this more clearly. The broader implications are that many cardiovascular risk factors are modifiable, and lifestyle interventions, particularly in women with hPE, may help prevent cognitive impairment. OB/GYNs are often the only care provider women see regularly throughout their menopausal years, and as such are in a unique position to integrate pregnancy history into risk assessment for future neurologic morbidity, recognize often missed cognitive impairment, and initiate individualized prevention and treatment plans.
Condensation.
Cognitive impairment tends to be more common and more diffuse in women with a history of preeclampsia versus history of normotensive pregnancy later in life.
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge the participants for their interest, time, and dedication to our research.
Sources of Funding
This work was funded by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH; P50 AG44170, R01 AG034676), the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS, a component of the NIH; UL1 TR000135), and the Department of Surgery, Mayo Clinic and the Mayo Foundation. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NIH. Funding sources had no role in study design, analysis and interpretation of data, manuscript preparation, or decision to submit this article for publication.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors report no conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Wenger NK. Recognizing pregnancy-associated cardiovascular risk factors. Am J Cardiol. 2014;113(2):406–409. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2013.08.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Roberts JM, Hubel CA. Pregnancy: a screening test for later life cardiovascular disease. Womens Health Issues. 2010;20(5):304–307. doi: 10.1016/j.whi.2010.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Brown MC, Best KE, Pearce MS, Waugh J, Robson SC, Bell R. Cardiovascular disease risk in women with pre-eclampsia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Epidemiol. 2013;28(1):1–19. doi: 10.1007/s10654-013-9762-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2011;77(5):461–468. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227b227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Knopman DS, Roberts R. Vascular risk factors: imaging and neuropathologic correlates. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;20(3):699–709. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Rapp SR, Espeland MA, Shumaker SA, et al. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. Jama. 2003;289(20):2663–2672. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rawlings AM, Sharrett AR, Schneider AL, et al. Diabetes in midlife and cognitive change over 20 years: a cohort study. Ann Intern Med. 2014;161(11):785–793. doi: 10.7326/M14-0737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Postma IR, Bouma A, Ankersmit IF, Zeeman GG. Neurocognitive functioning following preeclampsia and eclampsia: a long-term follow-up study. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2014;211(1):37 e31, 39. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2014.01.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Postma IR, Groen H, Easterling TR, et al. The brain study: Cognition, quality of life and social functioning following preeclampsia; An observational study. Pregnancy Hypertens. 2013;3(4):227–234. doi: 10.1016/j.preghy.2013.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Roes EM, Raijmakers MT, Schoonenberg M, Wanner N, Peters WH, Steegers EA. Physical well-being in women with a history of severe preeclampsia. J Matern Fetal Neona. 2005;18(1):39–45. doi: 10.1080/14767050500127740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Brusse I, Duvekot J, Jongerling J, Steegers E, De Koning I. Impaired maternal cognitive functioning after pregnancies complicated by severe pre-eclampsia: a pilot case-control study. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. 2008;87(4):408–412. doi: 10.1080/00016340801915127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Baecke M, Spaanderman ME, van der Werf SP. Cognitive function after pre-eclampsia: an explorative study. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 2009;30(1):58–64. doi: 10.1080/01674820802546212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Postma IR, Wessel I, Aarnoudse JG, Zeeman GG. Neurocognitive functioning in women with a history of eclampsia: executive functioning and sustained attention. Am J Perinatol. 2010;27(9):685–690. doi: 10.1055/s-0030-1253099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.White WM, Mielke MM, Araoz PA, et al. A history of preeclampsia is associated with a risk for coronary artery calcification 3 decades later. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2016;214(4):519 e511–518. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2016.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.St Sauver JL, Grossardt BR, Yawn BP, et al. Data resource profile: the Rochester Epidemiology Project (REP) medical records-linkage system. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41(6):1614–1624. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging- Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B. 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gottesman RF, Schneider AL, Albert M, et al. Midlife hypertension and 20-year cognitive change: the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(10):1218–1227. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Biessels GJ, Staekenborg S, Brunner E, Brayne C, Scheltens P. Risk of dementia in diabetes mellitus: a systematic review. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5(1):64–74. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70284-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Solomon A, Kareholt I, Ngandu T, et al. Serum cholesterol changes after midlife and late-life cognition: twenty-one-year follow-up study. Neurology. 2007;68(10):751–756. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000256368.57375.b7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cournot M, Marquie JC, Ansiau D, et al. Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology. 2006;67(7):1208–1214. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000238082.13860.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Whitmer RA, Gunderson EP, Barrett-Connor E, Quesenberry CP, Jr, Yaffe K. Obesity in middle age and future risk of dementia: a 27 year longitudinal population based study. Bmj. 2005;330(7504):1360. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38446.466238.E0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wendell CR, Zonderman AB, Metter EJ, Najjar SS, Waldstein SR. Carotid intimal medial thickness predicts cognitive decline among adults without clinical vascular disease. Stroke. 2009;40(10):3180–3185. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.557280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Reis JP, Launer LJ, Terry JG, et al. Subclinical atherosclerotic calcification and cognitive functioning in middle-aged adults: the CARDIA study. Atherosclerosis. 2013;231(1):72–77. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2013.08.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Vidal JS, Sigurdsson S, Jonsdottir MK, et al. Coronary artery calcium, brain function and structure: the AGES-Reykjavik Study. Stroke. 2010;41(5):891–897. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.579581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, et al. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261(5123):921–923. doi: 10.1126/science.8346443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dube JB, Johansen CT, Robinson JF, Lindsay J, Hachinski V, Hegele RA. Genetic determinants of “cognitive impairment, no dementia”. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;33(3):831–840. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-121477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ahmed R, Dunford J, Mehran R, Robson S, Kunadian V. Pre-eclampsia and future cardiovascular risk among women: a review. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2014;63(18):1815–1822. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2014.02.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Breetveld NM, Ghossein-Doha C, van Kuijk S, et al. Cardiovascular disease risk is only elevated in hypertensive, formerly preeclamptic women. Bjog. 2015;122(8):1092–1100. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.13057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Drost JT, Arpaci G, Ottervanger JP, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors in women 10 years post early preeclampsia: the Preeclampsia Risk EValuation in FEMales study (PREVFEM) Eur J Prev Cardiol. 2012;19(5):1138–1144. doi: 10.1177/1741826711421079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.McDonald SD, Ray J, Teo K, et al. Measures of cardiovascular risk and subclinical atherosclerosis in a cohort of women with a remote history of preeclampsia. Atherosclerosis. 2013;229(1):234–239. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2013.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ray JG, Vermeulen MJ, Schull MJ, Redelmeier DA. Cardiovascular health after maternal placental syndromes (CHAMPS): population-based retrospective cohort study. Lancet. 2005;366(9499):1797–1803. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67726-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Amaral LM, Cunningham MW, Jr, Cornelius DC, LaMarca B. Preeclampsia: long-term consequences for vascular health. Vasc Health Risk Manag. 2015;11:403–415. doi: 10.2147/VHRM.S64798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Aykas F, Solak Y, Erden A, et al. Persistence of cardiovascular risk factors in women with previous preeclampsia: a long-term follow-up study. J Investig Med. 2015;63(4):641–645. doi: 10.1097/JIM.0000000000000189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Mangos GJ, Spaan JJ, Pirabhahar S, Brown MA. Markers of cardiovascular disease risk after hypertension in pregnancy. J Hypertens. 2012;30(2):351–358. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0b013e32834e5ac7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Savitz DA, Danilack VA, Elston B, Lipkind HS. Pregnancy-induced hypertension and diabetes and the risk of cardiovascular disease, stroke, and diabetes hospitalization in the year following delivery. Am J Epidemiol. 2014;180(1):41–44. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Holmes C, Cairns N, Lantos P, Mann A. Validity of current clinical criteria for Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174:45–50. doi: 10.1192/bjp.174.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Knopman DS, Parisi JE, Boeve BF, et al. Vascular dementia in a population-based autopsy study. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(4):569–575. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.4.569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lim A, Tsuang D, Kukull W, et al. Clinico-neuropathological correlation of Alzheimer’s disease in a community-based case series. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47(5):564–569. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb02571.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Honjo K, Black SE, Verhoeff NP. Alzheimer’s disease, cerebrovascular disease, and the beta-amyloid cascade. Can J Neurol Sci. 2012;39(6):712–728. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100015547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Arvanitakis Z, Leurgans SE, Barnes LL, Bennett DA, Schneider JA. Microinfarct pathology, dementia, and cognitive systems. Stroke. 2011;42(3):722–727. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.595082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Knopman DS, Griswold ME, Lirette ST, et al. Vascular imaging abnormalities and cognition: mediation by cortical volume in nondemented individuals: atherosclerosis risk in communities-neurocognitive study. Stroke. 2015;46(2):433–440. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.007847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lawrence AJ, Chung AW, Morris RG, Markus HS, Barrick TR. Structural network efficiency is associated with cognitive impairment in small-vessel disease. Neurology. 2014;83(4):304–311. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tuladhar AM, Reid AT, Shumskaya E, et al. Relationship between white matter hyperintensities, cortical thickness, and cognition. Stroke. 2015;46(2):425–432. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.007146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Au R, Massaro JM, Wolf PA, et al. Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: the Framingham Heart Study. Arch Neurol. 2006;63(2):246–250. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.2.246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Smith EE, Salat DH, Jeng J, et al. Correlations between MRI white matter lesion location and executive function and episodic memory. Neurology. 2011;76(17):1492–1499. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318217e7c8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Aukes AM, De Groot JC, Wiegman MJ, Aarnoudse JG, Sanwikarja GS, Zeeman GG. Long-term cerebral imaging after pre-eclampsia. Bjog. 2012;119(9):1117–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0528.2012.03406.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mielke MM, Milic NM, Weissgerber TL, et al. Impaired cognition and brain atrophy decades after hypertensive pregnancy disorders. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2016;9(1):S70–76. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.115.002461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Postma IR, Bouma A, de Groot JC, Aukes AM, Aarnoudse JG, Zeeman GG. Cerebral white matter lesions, subjective cognitive failures, and objective neurocognitive functioning: A follow-up study in women after hypertensive disorders of pregnancy. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2016;38(5):585–598. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1143453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Yu L, Boyle PA, Leurgans S, Schneider JA, Bennett DA. Disentangling the effects of age and APOE on neuropathology and late life cognitive decline. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(4):819–826. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Shumaker SA, Legault C, Kuller L, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. Jama. 2004;291(24):2947–2958. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Prince M, Acosta D, Ferri CP, et al. Dementia incidence and mortality in middle-income countries, and associations with indicators of cognitive reserve: a 10/66 Dementia Research Group population-based cohort study. Lancet. 2012;380(9836):50–58. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60399-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Stern Y, Gurland B, Tatemichi TK, Tang MX, Wilder D, Mayeux R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Jama. 1994;271(13):1004–1010. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, et al. Vascular and amyloid pathologies are independent predictors of cognitive decline in normal elderly. Brain. 2015;138(Pt 3):761–771. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]