Skip to main content
Dementia & Neuropsychologia logoLink to Dementia & Neuropsychologia
. 2011 Jan-Mar;5(1):8–16. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642011DN05010003

Effects of stress hormones on the brain and cognition: Evidence from normal to pathological aging

Efeitos dos hormônios do estresse no cérebro e cognição: evidências do envelhecimento normal ao patológico

Juliana Nery de Souza-Talarico 1,, Marie-France Marin 2, Shireen Sindi 3, Sonia J Lupien 4
PMCID: PMC5619133  PMID: 29213714

Abstract

Several studies have demonstrated a wide cognitive variability among aged individuals. One factor thought to be associated with this heterogeneity is exposure to chronic stress throughout life. Animal and human evidence demonstrates that glucocorticoids (GCs), the main class of stress hormones, are strongly linked to memory performance whereby elevated GC levels are associated with memory performance decline in both normal and pathological cognitive aging. Accordingly, it is believed that GCs may increase the brain’s vulnerability to the effects of internal and external insults, and thus may play a role in the development of age-related cognitive disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD). The aim of this review article was to investigate the effects of GCs on normal and pathological cognitive aging by showing how these hormones interact with different brain structures involved in cognitive abilities, subsequently worsen memory performance, and increase the risk for developing dementia.

Keywords: glucocorticoids, memory, aging, Alzheimer’s disease


It is well-documented in the scientific literature that older adults show significant variability in terms of cognitive performance. Consequently, some researchers became particularly interested in understanding the various factors that could contribute to this differential aging phenomenon. Differing patterns of glucocorticoids (GCs) secretion have also been reported in older adults, with some individuals attaining very high levels of GCs, while others maintain moderate levels. Hence, it has been proposed that the cognitive variability observed in older adults could be explained by exposure to elevated levels of GCs throughout the life span.

Glucocorticoids are a major class of stress hormones released by activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. When an organism is exposed to a stressful situation, the HPA axis is activated. This cascade is first initiated by the release of corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) from the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. This leads to the secretion of adrenocorticotropin hormone (ACTH) from the pituitary and the release of GCs (mainly corticosterone in animals and cortisol in humans) from the adrenal glands then ensues. It is important to note that basal GC secretion follows a circadian rhythm characterized by a peak reached approximately 30 to 60 minutes after awakening followed by a progressive decline throughout the day.

Following an increase in GCs, the organism needs to return to a homeostatic basal state. In order to do so, GCs cross the blood-brain-barrier exploiting their liposoluble properties, and bind to the pituitary and the hypothalamic regions to exert negative feedback. Importantly, other brain structures are rich in GC receptors. There are two types of GC receptor: mineralocorticoid receptors (MR or Type I) and glucocorticoid receptors (GR or Type II). They differ from each other with respect to their affinity and their distribution throughout the various brain structures. GCs bind with a much higher affinity to MRs than GRs.1 This means that in the morning period, GCs occupy more than 90% of MRs, but only 10% of GRs. However, when facing a stressor and/or during the circadian peak of GC secretion, MRs are saturated and approximately 70% of the GRs are occupied.2 Moreover, the two categories of GC receptors differ with regards to their distribution in the brain. In fact, the MRs are exclusively present in the limbic system whereas GRs are present in both subcortical and cortical structures, with a preferential distribution in the prefrontal cortex.3

Briefly, GC receptors are mainly found in the following regions: the amygdala, prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Given that the amygdala is very important for processing emotional information, the prefrontal cortex is involved in executive functions, and that the hippocampus is well-known for its role in learning and memory, this represented a good rationale for scientists to further investigate the role of stress and stress hormones on the different cognitive functions subserved by these brain regions. Interestingly, the effects of GCs on cognitive performance are quite variable and depend on many factors, one of them being the duration of exposure to high levels of GCs. Thus, acute effects of GCs on memory are quite distinct from chronic effects.

GCs and memory: acute effects

Acute variations in GC levels, induced either endogenously in response to a stressor or exogenously with pharmacological protocols, profoundly impact memory performance. These effects are differential and depend on multiple factors, such as time of day and the population being studied. Notwithstanding these two modulators, one of the most important factors that needs to be taken into account is the memory process (e.g. consolidation vs. retrieval) being studied.

GCs and memory consolidation – Memory consolidation refers to the process whereby newly acquired information is transferred from an initially unstable state in the short-term memory system into a stable state in the long-term memory system.4 This process of memory consolidation can be modulated (enhanced or impaired) by different manipulations administered proximally to the time of encoding, which demonstrates the unstable nature of the memory trace early in the consolidation process. Additionally, GCs have the capacity to modulate this consolidation process. In general, an elevation in GC levels enhances memory consolidation.5,6 Moreover, it is important to note that very low levels of GCs (induced pharmacologically by the administration of metyrapone which blocks the synthesis of GCs) can impair the consolidation of both neutral and emotional information.7

GCs and memory retrieval – Memory retrieval refers to the notion of remembering memory traces that are already consolidated. Interestingly, the impact of GCs on memory retrieval is the opposite from that observed on the consolidation process, whereby an elevation in GC levels can impair the process of memory retrieval.8-10 In other words, when GC levels are elevated, the capacity to retrieve a previously consolidated memory trace is reduced.10

However, it would be wrong to assume that the relationship between GCs and memory retrieval is linear. Studies have demonstrated that low GC levels, induced pharmacologically, are also harmful for delayed memory recall.11 These findings are in line with the proposed inverted-U shape relationship between circulating levels of GCs and memory performance.12 In this formulation, both very low and very high levels of GCs are detrimental for memory performance, whereas moderate levels result in optimal performance. This can be explained by the occupancy of GC receptors and has been termed the GC Receptor Balance Hypothesis. Specifically, it proposes that when the MR/GR ratio is high (low or moderate levels of GCs, thus high occupation of MRs but low occupation of GRs), memory performance is enhanced. However, when the MR/GR ratio is low (high levels of GCs, thus high occupation of MRs and GRs), memory performance is impaired. Supporting this hypothesis, a recent finding demonstrated an association between impaired free recall and low glucocorticoids levels induced by administration of cortisol synthesis inhibitor. However, recognition remained unaffected.104

GCs and working memory – For many years, the effects of GCs on memory were thought to be restricted to declarative memory, which is subserved by the hippocampus. In 2000, a study on primates demonstrated the presence of GC receptors in the prefrontal cortex (mainly Type II receptors), which led scientists to hypothesize that an increase in GC levels could have an impact on cognitive functions which are dependent on the frontal lobes. Various neuropsychological studies have demonstrated the role of the prefrontal cortex in working memory, the process allowing an individual to maintain a limited amount of information online for a short amount of time.13

With this in mind, the effects of GCs on working memory have been investigated in different studies. For example, it has been shown that hydrocortisone administration had detrimental effects on working memory in young healthy adults.14 Interestingly enough, declarative memory was also measured in the same study and was unaffected by the hydrocortisone administration. This demonstrates not only that working memory is impaired by an elevation in GC levels but also that this cognitive function is more sensitive to GC variations compared to declarative memory. Other studies using psychosocial stressor instead of hydrocortisone administration have also reported impairments in working memory functions.15

Clearly, acute modulation of GC levels could have significant effects on different memory processes. Moreover, as mentioned previously, older adults as a population demonstrate broad variability in terms of cognitive performance.101,102 Based on animal studies supporting the relationship between elevated levels of GCs and memory performance in aged rodents, some researchers have started to question whether the cognitive impairments observed in a certain portion of older adults might be explained by their long-term exposition to elevated levels of GCs.

Aging and GCs

Older adults demonstrate great variability in HPA axis activity and in its impact on cognitive performance. Research findings on stress and aging are inconclusive as to whether basal GC levels increase during the aging process. Whereas some studies on older adults yield evidence for elevated basal levels of cortisol,16,17 others have shown lower basal levels,18,19 and some evidence suggests that cortisol levels remain stable in healthy older subjects.20,21 Additionally, circadian rhythm among older adults also tends be characterized by an advanced phase.16 This finding revealed that the diurnal rhythmicity of cortisol secretion is preserved in old age, but the relative amplitude was dampened, and the timing of the circadian elevation was advanced.16 Further, a study on the circadian rhythm cortisol profile in a large community dwelling population showed that those with a raised cortisol profile tended to be older than those with normative curves.103 Different sampling methods as well as times at which samples were collected may partly explain the diverse results obtained thus far. Moreover, inter-individual differences play an important role in determining cortisol levels. Lupien et al. (1996) conducted a longitudinal study where a sample of healthy older adults had cortisol levels measured on an annual basis. Their findings demonstrated that basal cortisol levels could be categorized into three sub-groups.22 One group showed an annual increase and had high current levels (Increasing / High group), another had an annual increase with moderate current levels (Increasing / Moderate group), while the final group presented with an annual decrease and moderate current cortisol levels (Decreasing / Moderate group). This evidence suggests that HPA-axis functioning displays vast inter-individual variation among older adults. Interestingly, the group with high current levels and an annual increase showed impaired cognitive performance compared to the other groups. Higher levels of basal GCs may not be a feature of normal aging but instead serve as a marker for cognitive impairment and pathological aging.23

Perhaps aging does not have an impact on the regulation of basal HPA functioning, but instead may have an impact on HPA reactivity. Older adults show an altered HPA recovery response to both psychological and pharmacological challenges.24,25 Evidence suggests that older individuals tend to show decreased responsiveness to HPA negative feedback inhibition, as demonstrated by a blunted suppression of plasma ACTH in response to ACTH challenge.24,26 Insufficient sensitivity to cortisol feedback inhibition is associated with memory impairments.27 Moreover, prolonged increases in levels of cortisol reactivity were observed in response to a driving simulation test.28 Consistently, some studies have indicated that older adults show large cortisol reactivity in response to a psychosocial stressor (which involved a public speaking task in a laboratory setting),29 yet conversely other studies have found a decreased response.30 More recent findings failed to detect a correlation between age and cortisol reactivity in response to a psychosocial stress task.31 Such results may provide further evidence for the inter-individual variation among older populations. Notably, socio-economic characteristics such as education level and social economic status may also contribute to contradictory findings. Recently, educational level has been reported as a factor that may modulate cortisol reactivity to psychosocial stress in aging.100 The cited study demonstrated that elderly participants with a low educational level showed greater specific stress response to the Trier Social Stress Test.100

Aging, GCs, memory and the hippocampus – In aged rodents, chronic stress and high levels of basal GC were associated with impaired cognitive performance on hippocampal-dependent tasks, as well as decreased hippocampal volume, hippocampal neuronal loss and dendritic atrophy.32-39 Interestingly, prospective reports of high levels of chronic stress over a period of 20-years were associated with hippocampal atrophy and reduced orbitofrontal cortex grey matter.40 Impairments have been observed in hippocampal dependent tasks including spatial memory.33,35 Chronically high levels of GC attenuate neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus region of the hippocampus, a brain region that continues to generate neurons throughout the adult lifespan.41 Intriguingly, if GC secretion is decreased from midlife onwards, increased neurogenesis and preserved spatial memory functioning is subsequently observed in aging.42 This evidence suggests that neuronal plasticity may play an important role in maintaining cognitive functioning during aging.

When middle-aged rats are administered high levels of GC for extended periods of time, the resulting deficits in memory performance are similar to those found in aged rats with high basal GC levels.43 Conversely, memory impairments and hippocampal atrophy are not present when cortisol levels are maintained at low levels (either through surgical andrenalectomy or pharmacologic methods).43 Consistent discoveries have formed the foundations of the ‘glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis',44 which postulates that exposure to elevated levels of GC for extended periods of time can have a cumulative impact, which in turn increases the risk for memory impairments and hippocampal atrophy. According to this hypothesis, hippocampus dysfunction disrupts the normal negative feedback to the HPA axis, which may result in hypercortisolemia. Prolonged hypercortisolemia may also lead to hippocampal dysfunction, potentially creating a vicious circle.44 Based on more recent evidence regarding potential underlying mechanisms of this relationship, this hypothesis is currently referred to as the ‘neurotoxicity hypothesis’.

As previously stated, cognitive performance and hippocampal functioning both show significant inter-individual variation among older adults.45,46 Findings from a recent study demonstrated that hippocampal volume also shows a considerable degree of variability among older adults.47 Reduced hippocampal volume may be a marker of cognitive decline as opposed to a natural consequence of healthy aging.48

Consistently, prospective studies have demonstrated that older adults with increased levels of cortisol presented deficits in memory performance compared to groups with stable cortisol levels.49,50 More significant increases in cortisol reactivity to a psychosocial stressor were associated with deficits in declarative memory.51 Similarly, longitudinal data has shown elevated levels of cortisol to be correlated with impaired memory performance and reduced hippocampal volume in a sample of healthy older adults.52 These findings are consistent with the neurotoxicity framework previously described.

One factor that may partially contribute to the high variability in cognitive performance among elderly subjects is the testing environment that may itself be inherently stressful. When measuring cognitive performance among older individuals, the testing environment is a critical factor to take into account. It has been demonstrated that older adults are more reactive to the environment in which they are tested compared to young adults.53,54 Older adults show higher levels of cortisol at the time of their arrival at the laboratory to perform tests of cognitive functioning.53 However, after memory testing and performance of a psychosocial stress task, older adults’ cortisol levels no longer differ from young adults, elucidating their heightened sensitivity to the testing environment. Moreover, when older adults perform cognitive tasks that emphasize the memory component, they show impaired memory performance compared to young adults. Yet when the task instructions (for the same tasks) are altered to decrease the emphasis on the memory, performance between older and young adults becomes quite similar.55,56 It is possible that having cognitive capacities tested is a stressful task for older adults due to their concerns about symptoms of dementia. Clearly, a stressful testing environment for cognitive testing may play an important role in predicting stress-induced memory impairments observed among older adults. Stereotypes in aging and memory loss may also have an important role to play in influencing older adults’ beliefs regarding their cognitive capacities.57 Evidently, the testing environment consists of a variety of components that need to be adjusted for older adults in order to prevent stress-induced deficits in memory performance.

The evidence on the relationship between glucocorticoids and memory decline allied to hippocampal atrophy has aroused the interest of different research groups in investigating the involvement of stress hormones in pathological cognitive impairment among the elderly.

Acute and chronic stress in Alzheimer dementia-type

The neurotoxicity hypothesis previously described has given rise to the investigation of the relationship between stress hormones and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), a neurodegenerative disorder clinically characterized by progressive cognitive and functional impairments. In line with this hypothesis, it has been proposed that the hippocampal atrophy reported in AD subjects may lead to negative feedback caused by HPA axis dysfunction that produces high levels of cortisol. Such dysregulation may be detrimental to hippocampal neurons and thereby compromise memory performance.2,44 Accordingly, transgenic rodent models of AD have demonstrated increased levels of corticosterone under acute and chronic stress situations.58-60 In humans, recent findings have demonstrated a significant association between decreased visuospatial memory, decreased hippocampal CA1 volume (hippocampal region which expresses high levels of GR receptors) and abnormal negative feedback in the HPA axis among patients with mild to moderate AD.61 Further, several studies have shown that subjects with AD61-70 and Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), a state between normal aging and dementia,71 have higher basal cortisol levels than healthy elderly individuals.70,72,73 However, longitudinal findings failed to demonstrate significant differences in cortisol levels between AD and healthy controls.66 Notably, biological specimens as well as time and methods of sampling may contribute to contradictory findings. Recently, seasonal variations in cortisol levels have been reported as a factor influencing GC concentrations in MCI and AD subjects.70 The study reveals that controlling for seasonal effects on basal salivary cortisol levels, participants with MCI and AD secreted higher cortisol concentrations compared to healthy elderly controls.70 In addition to the neurotoxicity hypothesis as the central mechanism that induces GCs elevation, exposure to stressful events has also been associated with increased AD development. Epidemiological evidence has shown that older adults who are more prone to distress are 2.7 times more likely to develop AD compared to those who are not prone to distress.74 Moreover, high levels of perceived stress have been associated with elevated cortisol levels in MCI subjects.75

As shown in normal cognitive aging, recent animal and human evidence has demonstrated a relationship between GCs and memory performance in pathological cognitive conditions. In experimental models of AD, both acute and chronic exposure to stress was followed by further declines in cognitive function.76,77 Similarly, increased levels of basal plasma cortisol were inversely related to cognitive performance in both MCI68,78 and AD patients.86 Moreover, a positive association between high cortisol levels and disease progression was reported in AD subjects.69

Despite the evidence of a relationship between cortisol levels and cognitive impairment, the role of GCs in the neuropathology of AD remains elusive. In fact, beyond the association between elevated cortisol levels and AD, recent studies have shown that GCs exacerbate AD pathogenesis and may worsen cognitive deficits.79

The neuropathology of AD is characterized by extracellular plaque formation by β-amyloid (Aβ) deposition and intracellular neurofibrillary tangles by aggregates of protein tau hyperphosphorylation in the cortex and hippocampus.80 Alterations in hippocampus plasticity and neurogenesis have also been reported in AD subjects.81,82

Besides previous animal studies showing that accumulations of Aβ in the hippocampus precede increases in corticosterone,58,60,83 elevations in GCs have also been associated with both accelerated Aβ production and decreased Aβ degradation in AD.84 Taken together, these findings support the hypothesis that GCs may play a role in AD neuropathology. Accordingly, increase in Aβ production can be pharmacologically reversed after administration of metyrapone in animals submitted to chronic restraint stress leading to a decrease in Aβ deposition.84 Interestingly, better spatial working memory was also observed after metyrapone administration in unstressed rodent models of AD,83 implying that GCs may also contribute to the cognitive impairments exhibited by individuals with dementia.

Increased corticosterone levels induced by chronic immobilization stress or by acute administration of dexamethasone are associated with both accelerated Aβ plaque formation and tau protein phosphorylation, once again showing a relationship between AD neuropathology biomarkers and stress hormones.59,84 Additionally, recent evidence links GCs, Aβ formation and neuronal apoptosis after chronic administration of high doses of dexamethasone in mice models of AD.85

Although evidence suggests a relationship between GCs and AD neuropathology biomarkers, the exact mechanisms involved in this association are unclear. It is known that oxidative stress (imbalance between reactive oxygen species production and antioxidant defenses) and hippocampal dysfunction can be observed in both stressed and AD participants. This could represent a potential pathway in which stress hormones play a role in AD pathogenesis.

In agreement with this, chronic restraint stress, sleep deprivation and social isolation are associated with markers of oxidative stress in the brain.86-88 In fact, GCs have been proposed to be a marker of susceptibility to oxidative brain damage since rodents exposed to acute stress have shown accumulation of oxidative/nitrosative and pro-inflammatory mediators in the brain while showing less anti-inflammatory protection.89 Some studies have shown that Aβ deposition induces formation of reactive oxygen species leading to lipid peroxidation and protein oxidation90,91 in MCI and AD subjects. Oxidative stress markers have been shown to precede increases in AD neuropathological hallmarks,92 suggesting that oxidative stress may contribute to AD development. Concordantly, previous associations between AD pathology and common oxidized brain proteins in MCI subjects, early AD (EAD) and late-stage AD91,93 have been substantiated. The presence of the epsilon4 allele of apolipoprotein E (APOE), a risk factor for sporadic AD, has also been associated with nitrite oxidative stress markers.94 Interestingly, recent findings have shown that increases in lipid peroxidation were three-fold greater in stressed AD mice compared to unstressed controls, and these elevations in oxidative stress markers decrease after pharmacological blocking of corticosterone.84

Regarding hippocampal plasticity, chronic adverse stress may lead to increased apoptosis of newly generated neurons in the hippocampus, a group of cells believed to be associated with hippocampus plasticity and neurogenesis.95 Further, elevated GC levels were found to lead to atrophy of the CA3 apical dendrites region of the hippocampus in rodents.96 Intriguingly, a similar reduction in dendritic spine density is observed in AD,97 and previous studies have shown a relationship between hippocampal atrophy and elevated GC levels in animals and humans.98,99 This association among stress hormones, hippocampal cell loss and plasticity reveal another common finding observed in chronic stress and AD pathology. In line with these findings, it has been proposed that the GCs released in response to a psychological stressor trigger oxidative stress markers, which increase the susceptibility of the brain to the damaging effects of pathological aging.

Conclusion

The literature has extensively shown that elderly subjects are widely exposed to adverse and challenging situations in their daily routine and are consequently also exposed to stress hormones and their effects on cognition. In addition to age-induced brain modifications, these individuals carry the effects of stress hormones cumulated throughout life, which may increase their susceptibility to pathological cognitive impairment. Although the exact mechanisms by which stress hormones may contribute to dementia development are not fully understood, the literature has consistently shown a strong association between GCs and biomarkers of AD pathogenesis during aging. Hence, psychoneuroendocrine evaluation with appropriate methods allied to neuropsychological assessment may represent an additional factor to be taken into account in the AD screening. In addition, given that psychological stress can be managed to a certain extent, learning how to decrease stress levels using evidence-based stress management strategies may be beneficial for older adults. Findings in elderly individuals may set the stage for more complex models that can help prevent the development of pathological cognitive impairment.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge Ms. Nathalie Wan and Mr. Robert-Paul Juster who kindly assisted with the manuscript review.

Footnotes

Disclosure: The authors report no conflits of interest.

References

  • 1.Reul JM, de Kloet ER. Two receptor systems for corticosterone in rat brain: microdistribution and differential occupation. Endocrinology. 1985;117:2505–2511. doi: 10.1210/endo-117-6-2505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joels M. Stress and cognition: are corticosteroids good or bad guys? Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:422–426. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01438-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Diorio D, Viau V, Meaney MJ. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (cingulate gyrus) in the regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3839–3847. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-09-03839.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McGh JL. Memory: a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287(5451):248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Beckner VE, Tucker DM, Delville Y, Mohr DC. Stress facilitates consolidation of verbal memory for a film but does not affect retrieval. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:518–527. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schwabe L, Bohringer A, Chatterjee M, Schachinger H. Effects of pre-learning stress on memory for neutral, positive and negative words: different roles of cortisol and autonomic arousal. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;90:44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Maheu FS, Joober R, Beaulieu S, Lupien SJ. Differential effects of adrenergic and corticosteroid hormonal systems on human short- and long-term larative memory for emotionally arousing material. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:420–428. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Buchanan TW, Tranel D. Stress and emotional memory retrieval: effects of sex and cortisol response. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;89:134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Adolphs R. Impaired memory retrieval correlates with individual differences in cortisol response but not autonomic response. Learn Mem. 2006;13:382–387. doi: 10.1101/lm.206306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Het S, Ramlow G, Wolf OT. A meta-analytic review of the effects of acute cortisol administration on human memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lupien SJ, Wilkinson CW, Briere S, Menard C, Ying Ng, Kin NM, Nair NP. The modulatory effects of corticosteroids on cognition: studies in young human populations. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27:401–416. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00061-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lupien SJ, McEwen BS. The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: integration of animal and human model studies. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1997;24:1–27. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00004-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:417–423. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lupien SJ, Gillin CJ, Her RL. Working memory is more sensitive than larative memory to the acute effects of corticosteroids: a dose-response study in humans. Behav Neurosci. 1999;113:420–430. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.113.3.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Oei NY, Everaerd WT, Elzinga BM, van Well S, Bermond B. Psychosocial stress impairs working memory at high loads: an association with cortisol levels and memory retrieval. Stress. 2006;9:133–141. doi: 10.1080/10253890600965773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Van Cauter E, Leproult R, Kupfer DJ. Effects of gender and age on the levels and circadian rhythmicity of plasma cortisol. J Clin Endocr Metab. 1996;81:2468–2473. doi: 10.1210/jcem.81.7.8675562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Touitou Y, Sulon J, Bogdan A, et al. Adrenal circadian system in young and elderly human subjects: a comparative study. J Endocrinol. 1982;93:201–210. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.0930201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sharma M, Palacios-Bois J, Schwartz G, et al. Circadian rhythms of melatonin and cortisol in aging. Biol Psychiatry. 1989;25:305–319. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(89)90178-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Drafta D, Schindler AE, Stroe E, Neacsu E. Age-related changes of plasma steroids in normal adult males. J Steroid Biochem. 1982;17:683–687. doi: 10.1016/0022-4731(82)90571-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sherman B, Wysham C, Pfohl B. Age-related changes in the circadian rhythm of plasma cortisol in man. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1985;61:439–443. doi: 10.1210/jcem-61-3-439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Waltman C, Blackman MR, Chrousos GP, Riemann C, Harman SM. Spontaneous and glucocorticoid-inhibited adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol secretion are similar in healthy young and old men. J Clin Endocr Metab. 1991;73:495–502. doi: 10.1210/jcem-73-3-495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lupien S, Lecours AR, Schwartz G, et al. Longitudinal study of basal cortisol levels in healthy elderly subjects: evidence for subgroups. Neurobiol Aging. 1996;17:95–105. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)02005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lee BK, Glass TA, McAtee MJ, et al. Associations of salivary cortisol with cognitive function in the Baltimore memory study. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2007;64:810–818. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wilkinson CW, Peskind ER, Raskind MA. Reased hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis sensitivity to cortisol feedback inhibition in human aging. Neuroendocrinology. 1997;65:79–90. doi: 10.1159/000127167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Otte C, Yassouridis A, Jahn H, et al. Mineralocorticoid receptor-mediated inhibition of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in aged humans. J Gerontol Biol. 2003;58:900–905. doi: 10.1093/gerona/58.10.b900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Boscaro M, Paoletta A, Scarpa E, et al. Age-related changes in glucocorticoid fast feedback inhibition of adrenocorticotropin in man. J Clin Endocr Metab. 1998;83:1380–1383. doi: 10.1210/jcem.83.4.4745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.O'Brien JT, Schweitzer I, Ames D, Tuckwell V, Mastwyk M. Cortisol suppression by dexamethasone in the healthy elderly: effects of age, dexamethasone levels, and cognitive function. Biol Psychiatry. 1994;36:389–394. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)91214-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Seeman TE, Berkman LF, Gulanski BI, et al. Self-esteem and neuroendocrine response to challenge: MacArthur studies of successful aging. J Psychosom Res. 1995;39:69–84. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)00076-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gotthardt U, Schweiger U, Fahrenberg J, Lauer CJ, Holsboer F, Heuser I. Cortisol, ACTH, and cardiovascular response to a cognitive challenge paradigm in aging and depression. Am J Physiol. 1995;268:R865–R873. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1995.268.4.R865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nicolson N, Storms C, Ponds R, Sulon J. Salivary cortisol levels and stress reactivity in human aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1997;52:M68–M75. doi: 10.1093/gerona/52a.2.m68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kudielka BM, Schmidt-Reinwald AK, Hellhammer DH, Schurmeyer T, Kirschbaum C. Psychosocial stress and HPA functioning: no evidence for a reduced resilience in healthy elderly men. Stress. 2000;3:229–240. doi: 10.3109/10253890009001127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Landfield PW, Baskin RK, Pitler TA. Brain aging correlates: retardation by hormonal-pharmacological treatments. Science. 1981;214(4520):581–584. doi: 10.1126/science.6270791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sandi C, Davies HA, Cordero MI, Rodriguez JJ, Popov VI, Stewart MG. Rapid reversal of stress induced loss of synapses in CA3 of rat hippocampus following water maze training. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:2447–2456. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02675.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sandi C. Stress, cognitive impairment and cell adhesion molecules. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:917–930. doi: 10.1038/nrn1555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Borcel E, Perez-Alvarez L, Herrero AI, et al. Chronic stress in adulthood followed by intermittent stress impairs spatial memory and the survival of newborn hippocampal cells in aging animals: prevention by FGL, a peptide mimetic of neural cell adhesion molecule. Behav Pharmacol. 2008;19:41–49. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282f3fca9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Issa AM, Rowe W, Gauthier S, Meaney MJ. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity in aged, cognitively impaired and cognitively unimpaired rats. J Neurosci. 1990;10:3247–3254. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-10-03247.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, Mcewen BS. Prolonged glucocorticoid exposure reduces hippocampal neuron number - implications for aging. J Neurosci. 1985;5:1222–1227. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-05-01222.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kerr DS, Campbell LW, Applegate MD, Brodish A, Landfield PW. Chronic stress-induced acceleration of electrophysiologic and morphometric biomarkers of hippocampal aging. J Neurosci. 1991;11:1316–1324. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-05-01316.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Woolley CS, Gould E, Mcewen BS. Exposure to excess glucocorticoids alters dendritic morphology of adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Brain Res. 1990;531:225–231. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90778-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Gianaros PJ, Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Greer PJ, Kuller LH, Matthews KA. Prospective reports of chronic life stress predict reased grey matter volume in the hippocampus. Neuroimage. 2007;35:795–803. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Gould E, Tanapat P. Stress and hippocampal neurogenesis. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1472–1479. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00247-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Montaron MF, Drapeau E, Dupret D, et al. Lifelong corticosterone level determines age-related line in neurogenesis and memory. Neurob Aging. 2006;27:645–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Landfield PW, Waymire JC, Lynch G. Hippocampal aging and adrenocorticoids: quantitative correlations. Science. 1978;202(4372):1098–1102. doi: 10.1126/science.715460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. The neuroendocrinology of stress and aging: the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Endocr Rev. 1986;7:284–301. doi: 10.1210/edrv-7-3-284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Christensen H. What cognitive changes can be expected with normal ageing? Aust Nz J Psychiat. 2001;35:768–775. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00966.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Nyberg L, Persson J, Nilsson LG. Individual differences in memory enhancement by encoding enactment: relationships to adult age and biological factors. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26:835–839. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00074-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lupien SJ, Evans A, Lord C, et al. Hippocampal volume is as variable in young as in older adults: Implications for the notion of hippocampal atrophy in humans. Neuroimage. 2007;34:479–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Burgmans S, van Boxtel MPJ, Vuurman EFPM, et al. The prevalence of cortical gray matter atrophy may be overestimated in the healthy aging brain. Neuropsychol. 2009;23:541–550. doi: 10.1037/a0016161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lupien S, Lecours AR, Lussier I, Schwartz G, Nair NPV, Meaney MJ. Basal cortisol-levels and cognitive deficits in human aging. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2893–2903. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02893.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kalmijn S, Launer LJ, Stolk RP, et al. A prospective study on cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate, and cognitive function in the elderly. J Clin Endocr Metab. 1998;83:3487–3492. doi: 10.1210/jcem.83.10.5164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lupien SJ, Gaudreau S, Tchiteya BM, et al. Stress-induced larative memory impairment in healthy elderly subjects: relationship to cortisol reactivity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;82:2070–2075. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.7.4075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lupien SJ, de Leon M, de Santi S, et al. Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:69–73. doi: 10.1038/271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lupien SJ, Maheu F, Tu M, Fiocco A, Schramek TE. The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain Cogn. 2007;65:209–237. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wolf OT, Convit A, McHugh PF, et al. Cortisol differentially affects memory in young and elderly men. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:1002–1011. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.115.5.1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hasher L, Zacks RT, Rahhal TA. Timing, instructions, and inhibitory control: some missing factors in the age and memory debate. Gerontology. 1999;45:355–357. doi: 10.1159/000022121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Rahhal TA, Colcombe SJ, Hasher L. Instructional manipulations and age differences in memory: now you see them, now you don't. Psychol Aging. 2001;16:697–706. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.16.4.697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lupien SJ, Wan N. Successful ageing: from cell to self. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1449):1413–1426. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pedersen W, Culmsee C, Ziegler D, Herman J, Mattson M. Aberrant stress response associated with severe hypoglycemia in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. J Mol Neurosci. 1999;13:159–165. doi: 10.1385/JMN:13:1-2:159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Green K, Billings L, Roozendaal B, McGh J, LaFerla F. Glucocorticoids increase amyloid-beta and tau pathology in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9047–9056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2797-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Dong H, Yuede C, Yoo H, et al. Corticosterone and related receptor expression are associated with increased beta-amyloid plaques in isolated Tg2576 mice. Neuroscience. 2008;155:154–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Elgh E, Lindqvist Astot A, Fagerlund M, Eriksson S, Olsson T, Näsman B. Cognitive dysfunction, hippocampal atrophy and glucocorticoid feedback in Alzheimer's disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Davis K, Davis B, Greenwald B, et al. Cortisol and Alzheimer's disease, I: Basal studies. Am J Psychiatry. 1986;143:300–305. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.3.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dodt C, Dittmann J, Hruby J, et al. Different regulation of adrenocorticotropin and cortisol secretion in young, mentally healthy elderly and patients with senile dementia of Alzheimer's type. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1991;72:272–276. doi: 10.1210/jcem-72-2-272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.O'Brien J, Ames D, Schweitzer I, Colman P, Desmond P, Tress B. Clinical and magnetic resonance imaging correlates of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function in depression and Alzheimer's disease. Br J Psychiatry. 1996;168:679–687. doi: 10.1192/bjp.168.6.679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hartmann A, Veldhuis J, Deuschle M, Standhardt H, Heuser I. Twenty-four hour cortisol release profiles in patients with Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease compared to normal controls: ultradian secretory pulsatility and diurnal variation. Neurobiol Aging. 1997;18:285–289. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(97)80309-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Swanwick G, Kirby M, Bruce I, et al. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: lack of association between longitudinal and cross-sectional findings. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:286–289. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.2.286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Umegaki H, Ikari H, Nakahata H, et al. Plasma cortisol levels in elderly female subjects with Alzheimer's disease: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Brain Res. 2000;881:241–243. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02847-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wolf O, Convit A, Thorn E, de Leon M. Salivary cortisol day profiles in elderly with mild cognitive impairment. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27:777–789. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00079-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Csernansky J, Dong H, Fagan A, et al. Plasma cortisol and progression of dementia in subjects with Alzheimer-type dementia. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:2164–2169. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.12.2164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Arsenault-Lapierre G, Chertkow H, Lupien S. Seasonal effects on cortisol secretion in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:1051–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Petersen R, Stevens J, Ganguli M, Tangalos E, Cummings J, DeKosky S. Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review): report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2001;56:1133–1142. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.9.1133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lind K, Edman A, Nordlund A, Olsson T, Wallin A. Increased saliva cortisol awakening response in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24:389–395. doi: 10.1159/000109938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Popp J, Schaper K, Kölsch H, et al. CSF cortisol in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:498–500. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wilson R, Arnold S, Schneider J, Kelly J, Tang Y, Bennett D. Chronic psychological distress and risk of Alzheimer's disease in old age. Neuroepidemiology. 2006;27:143–153. doi: 10.1159/000095761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Souza-Talarico JN. Chonic stress is associated with emotional coping style in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009:10–10. doi: 10.1159/000259696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Dong H, Goico B, Martin M, Csernansky C, Bertchume A, Csernansky J. Modulation of hippocampal cell proliferation, memory, and amyloid plaque deposition in APPsw (Tg2576) mutant mice by isolation stress. Neuroscience. 2004;127:601–609. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.05.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Jeong Y, Park C, Yoo J, et al. Chronic stress accelerates learning and memory impairments and increases amyloid deposition in APPV717I-CT100 transgenic mice, an Alzheimer's disease model. FASEB J. 2006;20:729–731. doi: 10.1096/fj.05-4265fje. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Souza-Talarico J, Chaves E, Lupien S, Nitrini R, Caramelli P. Relationship between cortisol levels and memory performance may be modulated by the presence or absence of cognitive impairment: evidence from healthy elderly, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease subjects. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;19:839–848. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Rothman S, Mattson M. Adverse stress, hippocampal networks, and Alzheimer's disease. Neuromolecular Med. 2010;12:56–70. doi: 10.1007/s12017-009-8107-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Mattson M. Pathways towards and away from Alzheimer's disease. Nature. 2004;430(7000):631–639. doi: 10.1038/nature02621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Scheff S, Price D, Schmitt F, Mufson E. Hippocampal synaptic loss in early Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2006 Oct;27:1372–1384. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Scheff S, Price D, Schmitt F, DeKosky S, Mufson E. Synaptic alterations in CA1 in mild Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;68:1501–1508. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000260698.46517.8f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Pedersen W, McMillan P, Kulstad J, Leverenz J, Craft S, Haynatzki G. Rosiglitazone attenuates learning and memory deficits in Tg2576 Alzheimer mice. Exp Neurol. 2006;199:265–273. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2006.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Lee K, Kim J, Seo J, et al. Behavioral stress accelerates plaque pathogenesis in the brain of Tg2576 mice via generation of metabolic oxidative stress. J Neurochem. 2009;108:165–175. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05769.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Li W, Li W, Yao Y, et al. Glucocorticoids increase impairments in learning and memory due to elevated amyloid precursor protein expression and neuronal apoptosis in 12-month old mice. Eur J Pharmacol. 2010;628:108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.11.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Pajovic S, Pejic S, Stojiljkovic V, Gavrilovic L, Dronjak S, Kanazir D. Alterations in hippocampal antioxidant enzyme activities and sympatho-adrenomedullary system of rats in response to different stress models. Physiol Res. 2006;55:453–460. doi: 10.33549/physiolres.930807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Singh A, Kumar A. Protective effect of alprazolam against sleep deprivation-induced behavior alterations and oxidative damage in mice. Neurosci Res. 2008;60:372–379. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Zafir A, Banu N. Modulation of in vivo oxidative status by exogenous corticosterone and restraint stress in rats. Stress. 2009;12:167–177. doi: 10.1080/10253890802234168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Pérez-Nievas B, García-Bueno B, Caso J, Menchén L, Leza J. Corticosterone as a marker of susceptibility to oxidative/nitrosative cerebral damage after stress exposure in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:703–711. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Goodman Y, Mattson M. Secreted forms of beta-amyloid precursor protein protect hippocampal neurons against amyloid beta-peptide-induced oxidative injury. Exp Neurol. 1994;128:1–12. doi: 10.1006/exnr.1994.1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Sultana R, Butterfield D. Oxidatively modified, mitochondria-relevant brain proteins in subjects with Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Bioenerg Biomembr. 2009;41:441–446. doi: 10.1007/s10863-009-9241-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Nunomura A, Perry G, Aliev G, et al. Oxidative damage is the earliest event in Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2001;60:759–767. doi: 10.1093/jnen/60.8.759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Sultana R, Butterfield D. Role of oxidative stress in the progression of Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;19:341–353. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Marcourakis T, Bahia V, Kawamoto E, et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype is related to nitric oxide production in platelets. Cell Biochem Funct. 2008;26:852–858. doi: 10.1002/cbf.1516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Simon M, Czéh B, Fuchs E. Age-dependent susceptibility of adult hippocampal cell proliferation to chronic psychosocial stress. Brain Res. 2005;1049:244–248. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Watanabe Y, Gould E, Cameron H, Daniels D, McEwen B. Phenytoin prevents stress- and corticosterone-induced atrophy of CA3 pyramidal neurons. Hippocampus. 1992;2:431–435. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Jacobsen J, Wu C, Redwine J, et al. Early-onset behavioral and synaptic deficits in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:5161–5166. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600948103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Lupien S, de Leon M, de Santi S, et al. Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:69–73. doi: 10.1038/271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Donohue H, Gabbott P, Davies H, et al. Chronic restraint stress induces changes in synapse morphology in stratum lacunosum-moleculare CA1 rat hippocampus: a stereological and three-dimensional ultrastructural study. Neuroscience. 2006;140:597–606. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.02.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Fiocco AJ, Joober R, Lupien SJ. Education modulates cortisol reactivity to the Trier Social Stress Test in middle-aged adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:1158–1163. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Valdois S, Joanette Y, Poissant A, Ska B, Dehaut F. Heterogeneity in the cognitive profile of normal elderly. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1990;12:587–596. doi: 10.1080/01688639008401003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Ylikoski R, Ylikoski A, Keskivaara P, Tilvis R, Sulkava R, Erkintti T. Heterogeneity of cognitive profiles in aging: successful aging, normal aging, and individuals at risk for cognitive line. Eur J Neurology. 1999;6:645–652. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.1999.660645.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Kumari M, Badrick E, Sacker A, Kirschbaum C, Marmot M, Chandola T. Identifying patterns in cortisol secretion in an older population. Findings from the Whitehall II study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:1091–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Rimmele U, Meier F, Lange T, Born J. Suppressing the morning rise in cortisol impairs free recall. Learn Mem. 2010;17:186–190. doi: 10.1101/lm.1728510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dementia & Neuropsychologia are provided here courtesy of Academia Brasileira de Neurologia

RESOURCES