Abstract
Work can be an important etiologic factor in the genesis of some mental disorders including cognitive disability. Occupational neuropsychology constitutes an intriguing new but neglected area of research and clinical practice which deals with the neurocognitive consequences of the work environment and work habits. Neuropsychological knowledge is fundamental to understand cognitive requirements of work competence. Work can impact sleep patterns and mental energy, which in turn can cause neuropsychological symptoms. This report presents relevant evidence to illustrate the relationship between work and cognitive dysfunction.
Keywords: occupational health, cognitive disorders, neuropsychology, executive functions, burnout
Abstract
O trabalho pode ser um importante fator etiológico na gênese de algumas doenças mentais, incluindo transtornos cognitivos. A neuropsicologia ocupacional constitui uma intrigante, mas negligenciada nova área de pesquisa e atuação clínica, que lida com as consequências neurocognitivas do ambiente e dos hábitos de trabalho. O conhecimento neuropsicológico é fundamental para se entender os pressupostos cognitivos da competência no trabalho. O trabalho pode afetar os padrões de sono e de energia mental, que por sua vez podem causar sintomas neuropsicológicos. Foi realizada uma apresentação de evidências relevantes para ilustrar a relação entre trabalho e disfunção cognitiva.
INTRODUCTION
Mental health problems have contributed increasingly to absenteeism at work and long-term disability,1 but the reverse can also occur: work can be an important etiologic factor in the genesis of some mental disorders including cognitive disability.2
Neuropsychology, covers a wide field of expertise and accumulated knowledge, can be subdivided into several subareas, including occupational neuropsychology which deals with the neurocognitive consequences of the work environment. Occupational neuropsychology encompasses and supports several medical specialties, including occupational psychiatry whose recent revival is due to the increasing number of mental disorders in work activities and new laws protecting employee health.3,4 In this latest approach, both occupational neuropsychology and occupational psychiatry are faced with a lack of concepts and current approaches, prompting us to study this issue.
The high incidence of mental disorders among workers - around 30% for minor mental disorders and 5%-10% for severe mental disorders5 coupled with the granting of social security benefits for those insured under Social Security ("psychoneuroses" ranks first among the causes of temporary disability and second and third among the causes of permanent disability6) show the importance of occupational psychiatry and occupational neuropsychology.3,4 The weight of statistics, plus the efforts of researchers, has led to legal recognition of the relationship between mental health and work, by Decree No. 3.048/99, from the Brazilian Ministry of Social Security, which deals with Work-Related Mental Disorders.
In this brief review, we present relevant evidence that illustrates and highlights the relationship between work and mental (including cognitive) function and dysfunction, contributing to structure knowledge in this field. Occupational neuropsychology is presented according to its most characteristic and structural aspects. In the present aggregated review of the literature, a search of the databases SciELO and MEDLINE was conducted using the key words "occupational health" "and" "cognition". The most representative articles on the subject were selected, dedicating special attention to the contributions of internationally renowned investigators. Reference books that were pertinent under the framework of this review article were also consulted.
WORK AND MENTAL HEALTH
The knowledge that work can cause illness is ancient. The very origin of the meaning of "work" alludes to its dual status as an activity that involves suffering and achievement, since the Latin word trapalium ("work") means a three-piece instrument which was used both as an agricultural tool and an instrument of torture. The theoretical understanding of the underlying health of workers is based on a body of knowledge from different disciplines such as Social Medicine, Public Health, Internal Medicine, Labor Medicine, Sociology, Social Epidemiology and Engineering among many others. This understanding, allied with the knowledge of the worker at their workstation and experiences of burden, represents a new approach to understanding the relationship between health and work and proposes a new practice of healthcare professionals and intervention in the workplace.7 Occupational diseases are easily recognized as an effect of en vironmental hazards to which workers are exposed, whereas the relationship between mental disorders and work are less recognized, i.e., the so-called psychosocial risks that include phenomena of a neurophysiological, cognitive, psychological, social and organizational nature.7,8
The multiple and complex aspects involved in the workplace, work environment, economic issues, work relationships and workers health, including mental health, define the need for government involvement in the provision of laws, economic incentives - policies, in short - to attenuate the effects of work distress on workers' mental health, as reported by some researchers.9
MENTAL CAPACITY AND WORK SUCCESS
There are several factors affecting the ability to work. Among these are the influence of age,9-11 shift work12 and particularly the presence of psychiatric symptoms.13 The success of labor is typically conceptualized in two main dimensions: an extrinsic dimension (objective) and an intrinsic dimension (subjective).14 The extrinsic dimension refers to measurable variables, such as salary, promotions, career and quality of work scrutinized by others. The intrinsic dimension refers to the self-assessment of levels of satisfaction related to job performance.15 This evaluation process is intrinsically related to the concept of work ability.16 According to some authors17 the "ability to work" is defined as the self-assessment the worker makes regarding their well-being now and in the near future and the ability to ensure their work in terms of its requirements, health and available psychological resources.
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION AND WORK
Over recent years in the neuropsychological field, there has been growing interest in the concept and assessment of executive cognitive functions and how these functions relate to health and adaptive behaviors, indispensable for personal, social and professional well-being.18 The concept includes a set of higher-order cognitive functions, which are instrumental in self-regulation of human behavior,19 such as abstract reasoning and problem solving;20 planning, organizing and self-monitoring of actions that target a specific goal;21 and inhibition of irrelevant behaviors as well as mental flexibility.19 In this sense, a lack of integrity or impairment of executive functions may impair effective work performance, including the ability to work.18
The relationship between work ability and executive cognitive functions, according to some authors,15 may be mediated or moderated by certain personality traits. According to other authors,22 high levels of neuroticism have a negative influence on the levels of decision-making and consequent injury at work, while high levels of extroversion and conscientiousness positively influence both executive functioning and the ability to work. The characterization of these variables, and understanding how they interrelate, is crucial for the implementation of techniques and preventive strategies to promote health among workers, contributing to an improvement in their ability to work and their executive functioning, favoring the development of a better quality of life.
TRIGGERING FACTORS IN THE PROCESS OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DYSFUNCTION AT WORK
Some authors23 have reported elements that act as triggers of the process of cognitive malfunctioning at work, namely: lack of time control at work (long hours, night or alternating shifts, etc.); requirement for high levels of attention and concentration to perform tasks; occupational exposure (especially to heavy metals and solvents). However, the establishment of a causal link with work is complex, since the morbidity process has an element of specificity and involves multiple aspects of the subject.
WORK, SLEEP DEPRIVATION AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE
Reduced sleep is a frequent effect of suboptimal work schedules or shift work. On the other hand, sleep deprivation results in significant impairments in cognitive and motor performance which increase for instance the risk of motor vehicle crashes and work-related injuries and fatal accidents.24 Empirical data from a study25 correlating accidents among interns and sleep deprivation on duties and night-shift work determined that each extended work shift scheduled during a month increased the monthly risk of a motor vehicle crash by 9.1 percent (95 percent confidence interval, 3.4 to 14.7 percent) and increased the monthly risk of a crash during the commute from work by 16.2 percent (95 percent confidence interval, 7.8 to 24.7 percent). On months in which interns worked five or more extended shifts, the risk of falling asleep at the wheel while driving or while stationary in traffic was significantly increased (odds ratios, 2.39 [95 percent confidence interval, 2.31 to 2.46] and 3.69 [95 percent confidence interval, 3.60 to 3.77], respectively).
Sleep and cognition are temporally regulated by a homeostatic process generating pressure for sleep as a function of sleep/wake history, and a circadian process generating pressure for wakefulness as a function of time of day. Under normal nocturnal sleep conditions, these two processes are aligned in such a manner as to provide optimal daytime performance and consolidated nighttime sleep. However, under conditions of sleep deprivation, shift work or trans-meridian travel, the two processes become misaligned, resulting in fatigue and cognitive deficits.25
Some authors26 investigated simple and high-order cognitive performance change after one night of sleep deprivation (SD) and recovery after 7 h of normal recovery sleep opportunity over three recovery days. According to these authors, performance on simple tasks such as addition or short-term memory, was not reduced after SD and was poorest on the baseline day, improving gradually thereafter. High-order cognitive performances were at their lowest on the post-vigil day and needed 2 recovery sleep opportunities to return to baseline levels.
Other authors report that short-term memory appears to decline after day and overnight shifts, based on data collected from a study involving attending emergency physicians before and after day and overnight shifts.27 Along the same line of investigation, Rollinson et al. studied interns working nights in an emergency department and showed a significant reduction in visual memory capacity during the night shift.28
WORK, FATIGUE AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE
For over a century, it has been recognized that long working hours can lead to a greater perception of fatigue by workers and to a reduction in worker efficiency. The role of fatigue in worker health and safety, measured in terms of accident and absenteeism rates, as a result of longer workdays has also been examined, and some authors have explored the effects of acute or short-term fatigue (i.e., over a prolonged workday) on job performance.27-30 Fatigue resulting from several days of a long work schedule (e.g. a compressed work week shift, sustained operations work, hospital on-call service by physicians and nurses) affects behavioral or psychological test performances.29
Prolonged cognitive load associated with some professions can cause fatigue, which, in turn, can dramatically impact cognitive functions by decreasing mental energy.30 Mental fatigue is reflected by a failure to complete mental tasks that require self-motivation and internal cues, in the absence of demonstrable cognitive failure or motor weakness. Lieberman31 suggests use of cognitive tests that assess vigilance, ability to sustain attention, and choice reaction time for the assessment of mental energy and fatigue. Some authors32 have shown that compared to a non-fatigued group, fatigued participants displayed more perseveration on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and showed prolonged planning time on the Tower of London task. Fatigue did not however, affect performance on a simple memory task. These authors concluded by highlighting compromised executive control under fatigue, which may explain the typical errors and sub-optimal performance often found in fatigued individuals.32
Burnout is an important concept relevant to the present issue. This refers to a work syndrome first described in physicians characterized as a unique affective multidimensional response to stress, the core components of which are emotional exhaustion, physical fatigue, and cognitive weariness.33 The phenomenon of burnout has a chronic nature and exhibits remarkable stability over time regardless of sample makeup, cultural context, and length of time of follow-up survey.34 Oosterholt et al.35 suggested that burnout leads to permanent cognitive deficits, that subjective burnout complaints reduce faster than deficits in cognitive test performance, or that cognitive deficits are a cause rather than a consequence of burnout.
At least two available instruments have been designed for assessing work burnout: the Copenhagen Burnout Inventory36 (CBI) and the Maslach Burnout Inventory.37 Despite the importance of the burnout phenomenon, few systematic investigation assessing work as a cause of burnout are available, although more attention has been dedicated to burnout as a consequence of disease. Systematic approaches to burnout as a consequence of work that go beyond the care of patients can shed light on the condition and help its prevention.
International commercial airline pilots may experience heightened fatigue due to irregular sleep schedules, long duty days, night flying, and multiple time zone changes. Some authors36 have investigated the relationship among commercial airline pilots' amount of sleep, subjective fatigue, and sustained attention before and after international flights using a 5 min PalmPilot-based psychomotor vigilance task and self-rating of level of fatigue using the Samn-Perelli Fatigue Checklist. A significant main effect of stage of flight was found for self-rated fatigue and mean response speed.
Proctor et al.29 examined whether increased overtime work among automotive workers predicted impairment in cognitive performance in the domains of attention and executive function. These researchers found that overtime work affects cognitive function as measured by neuropsychological testing, with impairments observed in tasks requiring attention and executive function skills.
Conclusions. Occupational neuropsychology constitutes an intriguing new but neglected area of research and clinical practice which deals with the neurocognitive consequences of the work environment and work habits. Neuropsychological knowledge is fundamental to understand cognitive requirements of work competence. Work can impact sleep patterns and mental energy, which in turn can cause neuropsychological symptoms. Recognition that increased overtime work affects cognitive function in certain functional areas and, by extension, may affect worker performance, has important implications for the health and safety of workers as well as for the economic management of the workplace. In addition, better understanding of neuropsychological implications of these work dysfunctions, and their relationships with work ability and workers' mental health, will allow assessment and preventive programs to be devised as well as more specific and efficient therapeutic tools for managing the mental distress triggered, exacerbated or caused by work.
Footnotes
Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Cornelius LR, van der Klink JJ, Groothoff JW, Brouwer S. Prognostic factors of long term disability due to mental disorders: a systematic review. J Occup Rehabil. 2011;21:259–274. doi: 10.1007/s10926-010-9261-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Khalil N, Morrow LA, Needleman H, Talbott EO, Wilson JW, Cauley JA. Association of cumulative lead and neurocognitive function in an occupational cohort. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:10–19. doi: 10.1037/a0013757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Camargo DAC, Guimarães LAM, Caetano D. Psiquiatria ocupacional I: aspectos históricos e conceituais J Bras. Psiquiatr. 2004;53:321–330. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Camargo DAC, Caetano D, Liliana AM. Psiquiatria ocupacional II: síndromes psiquiátricas orgânicas relacionadas ao trabalho. J Bras Psiquiatr. 2005;54:21–33. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Organização Mundial da Saúde . Relatório mundial da saúde. Saúde mental: nova concepção, nova esperança. Lisboa: OMS; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mendes AMB. Os novos paradigmas da organização do trabalho: implicação na saúde mental. Rev Bras de Saúde Ocup. 1997;23:55–60. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Baker DB, Landrigan PJ. Occupationally related disorders. Med Clin North Am. 1990;74:441–460. doi: 10.1016/s0025-7125(16)30572-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Landrigan PJ, Baker DB. The recognition and control of occupational disease. JAMA. 1991;266:676–680. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jané-Llopis E, Barry M, Hosman C, Patel V. What works in mental health promotion. Promotion Education. 2005;12(Supplement 2):9–25. doi: 10.1177/10253823050120020103x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Costa G, Sartori S. Ageing, working hours and work ability. Ergonomics. 2007;50:1914–1930. doi: 10.1080/00140130701676054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tuomi K, Ilmarinen J, Martikainen R. Aging, work, life-style and work ability among Finnish municipal workers in 1981-1992. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1997;23:58–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ilmarinen J. Aging workers. Occup Environ Med. 2001;58:546–552. doi: 10.1136/oem.58.8.546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rouch I, Wild P, Ansiau D, Marquié JC. Shiftwork experience, age and cognitive performance. Ergonomics. 2005;48:1282–1293. doi: 10.1080/00140130500241670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Elinson L, Houck P, Marcus S, Pincus H. Depression and the ability to work. Psychiatr Serv. 2004;5:29–34. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.55.1.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Eby L, Butts M, Lockwood A. Predictors of success in the area of the boundaryless career. J Organ Behav. 2003;24:689–708. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kern LK, Friedman HS, Martin LR, Reynolds CA, Luong G. Conscientiousness, career success, and longevity: a lifespan analysis. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37:154–156. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9095-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Marqueze EC, Voltz GP, Borges FN, Moreno CR. A 2-year follow-up study of work ability among college educators. Appl Ergon. 2008;39:640–645. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Silva CF, Silvério J, Nossa P, Rodrigues V, Pereira A, Queirós A. envelhecimento, ritmos biológicos e capacidade laboral - versão portuguesa do Work Ability Index. Psicolo Teor Inv Prat. 2000;2:329–339. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Williams PG, Thayer JT. Executive Functioning and Health: Introduction to the Special Series. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37:154–163. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9091-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jurado MB, Rosselli M. The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol Rev. 2007;17:213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16:17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sorel O, Pennequin V. Aging of the Planning process: The role of executive functioning. Brain Cog. 2008;66:196–201. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Denburg NL, Weller JA, Yamada TH, et al. Poor Decision Making Among Older Adults Is Related to Elevated Levels of Neuroticism. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37:164–172. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9094-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ramminger T. A saúde mental do trabalhador em saúde mental: um estudo com trabalhadores de um hospital psiquiátrico. Bol Saúde. 2002;16:111–124. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Luyster FS, Strollo PJ Jr, Zee PC, Walsh JK. Sleep: a health imperative. Sleep. 2012;35:727–734. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Barger LK, Cade BE, Ayas NT, Cronin JW, Rosner B, Speizer FE, Czeisler CA. Extended work shifts and the risk of motor vehicle crashes among interns. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:125–134. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Raslear TG, Hursh SR, Van Dongen HP. Predicting cognitive impairment and accident risk. Prog Brain Res. 2011;190:155–167. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53817-8.00010-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ikegami K, Ogyu S, Arakomo Y, Suzuki K, Mafune K, Hiro H, Nagata S. Recovery of cognitive performance and fatigue after one night of sleep deprivation. J Occup Health. 2009;51:412–422. doi: 10.1539/joh.l8127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Machi MS, Staum M, Callaway CW, Moore C, Jeong K, Suyama J, et al. The relationship between shift work, sleep, and cognition in career emergency physicians. Acad Emerg Med. 2012;19:85–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1553-2712.2011.01254.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rollinson DC, Rathlev NK, Moss M, Killiany R, Sassower KC, Auerbach S, et al. The effects of consecutive night shifts on neuropsychological performance of interns in the emergency department: a pilot study. Ann Emerg Med. 2003;41:400–406. doi: 10.1067/mem.2003.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Proctor SP, White RF, Robins TG, Echeverria D, Rocskay AZ. Effect of overtime work on cognitive function in automotive workers. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1996;22:124–132. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Yamaguti K, Kajimoto O, Kuratsune H, Watanabe Y. Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity. Behav Brain Funct. 2011;7:17–22. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lieberman HR. Cognitive methods for assessing mental energy. Nutr Neurosci. 2007;10:229–242. doi: 10.1080/10284150701722273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.van der Linden D, Frese M, Meijman TF. Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes: effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2003;113:45–65. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(02)00150-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schaufeli WB, Buunk BP. Burnout: an overview of 25 years of research and theorizing. In: Schabracq MJ, Winnubst JAM, Cooper CC, editors. The Handbook of Work and Health Psychology. 2nd ed. West Sussex, UK: Wiley; 2003. pp. 383–429. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Shirom A. Job-related burnout. In: Quick JC, Tetrick LE, editors. Handbook of Occupational Health Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003. pp. 245–265. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Oosterholt BG, Van der Linden D, Maes JH, Verbraak MJ, Kompier MA. Burned out cognition - cognitive functioning of burnout patients before and after a period with psychological treatment. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2012;38:358–369. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kristensen TS, Borrits M, Villdsen E, Christensen KB. The Copenhagen Burnout Inventory: a new tool for the assessment of burnout. Work Stress. 2005;19:192–207. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Green DE, Walkey FH, Taylor AJ. The three-factor structure of the Maslach Burnout Inventory: a multicultural, multinational confirmatory study. J Soc Behav Personal. 1991;6:453–472. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Petrilli RM, Roach GD, Dawson D, Lamond N. The sleep, subjective fatigue, and sustained attention of commercial airline pilots during an international pattern. Chronobiol Int. 2006;23:1357–1362. doi: 10.1080/07420520601085925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]