Skip to main content
Dementia & Neuropsychologia logoLink to Dementia & Neuropsychologia
. 2015 Apr-Jun;9(2):149–155. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642015DN92000009

The most frequently used tests for assessing executive functions in aging

OS TESTES NEUROPSICOLÓGICOS MAIS UTILIZADOS PARA AVALIAR FUNÇÕES EXECUTIVAS NO ENVELHECIMENTO

Camila de Assis Faria 1,, Heloisa Veiga Dias Alves 3, Helenice Charchat-Fichman 2
PMCID: PMC5619353  PMID: 29213956

Abstract

There are numerous neuropsychological tests for assessing executive functions in aging, which vary according to the different domains assessed.

Objective

To present a systematic review of the most frequently used instruments for assessing executive functions in older adults with different educational levels in clinical and experimental research.

Methods

We searched for articles published in the last five years, using the PubMed database with the following terms: "neuropsychological tests", "executive functions", and "mild cognitive impairment". There was no language restriction.

Results

25 articles fulfilled all the inclusion criteria. The seven neuropsychological tests most frequently used to evaluate executive functions in aging were:

  • [1] Trail Making Test (TMT) Form B;

  • [2] Verbal Fluency Test (VFT) - F, A and S;

  • [3] VFT Animals category;

  • [4] Clock Drawing Test (CDT);

  • [5] Digits Forward and Backward subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III);

  • [6] Stroop Test; and

  • [7] Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variants. The domains of executive functions most frequently assessed were: mental flexibility, verbal fluency, planning, working memory, and inhibitory control.

Conclusion

The study identified the tests and domains of executive functions most frequently used in the last five years by research groups worldwide to evaluate older adults. These results can direct future research and help build evaluation protocols for assessing executive functions, taking into account the different educational levels and socio-demographic profiles of older adults in Brazil.

Keywords: review, mild cognitive impairment, dementia, older adults, executive functions, neuropsychological tests

INTRODUCTION

EFs are complex cognitive abilities that enable the identification of goals, mental planning, behavior organization, and planning actions to achieve these goals.2,3 In addition, EFs impact affective-emotional, motivational, and social skills.3,4 EFs are especially important in older adults for performing and troubleshooting routine tasks, from the most simple to the most complex. Executive functions comprise the six domains described below. Planning refers to the identification of a sequence of actions required to achieve a goal. Efficient planning includes thinking about alternatives and choosing the most effective one.4 Working memory is defined as a system of temporary storage and manipulation of information. This system is activated during the process of learning, language comprehension, reasoning, and production of one's conscience.4 Mental flexibility refers to the ability of alternating between mental sets or tasks, and changing strategies within the same task.5 Inhibitory control refers to the inhibition of a prepotent response, which facilitates the choice of an adequate response and avoids errors.4 Verbal fluency is the ability to generate an appropriate strategy for word searching.6 Processing speed refers to the time required to process a specific item of information.6 Neural substrates of these domains are mainly located in the prefrontal cortex of the brain, and are connected to several other brain areas and the central nervous system as a whole.3

Executive dysfunction leads to significant limitations in the daily routines of elderly, such as impact on functionality, i.e., in the performance of activities of daily living (ADLs), which reduces autonomy and quality of life.7,8 Deficits in EF found in older patients include inflexibility of thought, reduced working memory processing, difficulty in solving problems, decreased behavioral initiation, perseveration errors, inadequate planning strategies, and disinhibition.1,8 Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is considered an intermediate state of cognitive functioning between the changes seen in healthy aging and those typically found in dementia. MCI criteria include self- or informant-reported cognitive complaints, objective cognitive impairment, preserved Independence in functional abilities and no dementia.9,10 Executive dysfunction in aging may appear in MCI or may be an early sign of dementia.1

Executive dysfunction in aging can be measured objectively with neuropsychological tests.11 There are several executive functions tests available, which vary according to the domains assessed. In general, a neuropsychological test predominantly assesses one of the EF domains. The wide variability of tests, the need for effective measures for early diagnosis of dementia, and the impact of executive dysfunction on ADLs, autonomy, and quality of life of older adults justify the need to identify which instruments are most used to assess executive functions in aging. This aim of this study was to present a systematic review of the instruments most frequently used to assess executive functions in older adults with different educational levels, in clinical and experimental research.

METHODS

A systematic review was conducted on July 30th, 2014 using the PubMed database and combining the search terms "neuropsychological tests", "executive functions", and "mild cognitive impairment", with no language restriction. The study included articles that fulfilled the following criteria:

  • [1] all participants aged over 50 years;

  • [2] healthy older adults, MCI or dementia;

  • [3] neuropsychological tests used to assess executive functions;

  • [4] published in the last 5 years; and

  • [5] cross-sectional or longitudinal studies.

The studies that fulfilled the following criteria were excluded:

  • [1] review studies;

  • [2] studies with psychiatric or neurological patients, except for dementia and MCI. To assess which executive functions tests were most commonly used, a percentage of usage of over 20% was adopted as the counting criterion, considering only the articles that met the inclusion and exclusion criteria.

RESULTS

Seventy-four articles published between July 2009 and July 2014 were found in Pubmed. Of these, only 25 fulfilled all the inclusion criteria and 49 articles were excluded according to the exclusion criteria.

Seven tests of executive function were used in more than 20% of the selected articles:

  • [1] Trail Making Test (TMT) Form B;

  • [2] Verbal Fluency Test (VFT) - F, A and S;

  • [3] VFT Animals category;

  • [4] Clock Drawing Test (CDT);

  • [5] Digits Forward and Backward subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III);

  • [6] Stroop Test; and

  • [7] Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variations. The EF domains most frequently studied by the researchers were mental flexibility, verbal fluency, planning, working memory, inhibitory control, and processing speed.

Table 1 describes the relationship between the most used neuropsychological tests and their cognitive domains.

Table 1.

The seven executive function tests most frequently used in aging research

Names of EF tests Predominant domain Task description
Trail Making Test (TMT) Form B Mental flexibility Connect 13 numbers and 12 letters alternately and as quickly as possible.9-10
Verbal Fluency Test (VFT)
F, A and S
Verbal fluency The test entails saying as many words a possible beginning with F, A and S in 1 minute. Participants cannot use proper nouns or use a stem word with different endings.11-12
Verbal Fluency Test (VFT)
Animals category
Verbal fluency The entails saying as many animal names, as quickly as possible, in 1 minute, with no restrictions on first letter or any other characteristics.11,13-14
Clock Drawing Test (CDT) Planning Draw a clock with all the numbers and pointers marking a particular time.13,15-19
Digits Forward and Backward subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) Working memory In the Digits Forward subtest, the participant must repeat the numbers dictated by the examiner in the same order. In the Digits Backward subtest, the patient must repeat the same numbers in reverse order.13,20
Stroop Test Inhibitory control The test consists of three conditions: in the first condition, the subject must say, as quickly as possible, the names of the colors that are arranged on a card. In the second condition, the subject must say the names of the colors that the words “all”, “today”, etc. are printed in. In the third condition, the participant has to name the colors that the words “yellow”, “red”, etc. are printed in.21-22
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variants Mental flexibility The test consists of cards that need to be classified according to color, shape or number categories. The categorization rules change every time 10 (out of a maximum of 128) response cards have been sorted correctly. WCST variants are versions with fewer cards.23

Table 2 presents all the executive function tests used in the 25 articles, with their respective frequencies and percentage of usage. Table 2 also shows the number of studies by domain evaluated.

Table 2.

Frequency of use of executive function tests in aging research.

Executive function tests Frequency
of test use
Percentage use, considering the 25 articles (%) EF Domain Study references (some studies used more than one test for the same domain) Total studies evaluating the domain
Trail Making Test (TMT) Form B 9 36 mental
flexibility
1, 29-42, 47 15
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variants 6 24
Rule Shift Cards Test (BADS) 2 8
Trail Making Test (TMT) Oral 1 4
Intra-Extra Dimensional Set Shifting (IED) of the CANTAB 1 4
VFT - F, A and S 7 28 verbal
fluency
21-23, 32-33, 36, 38, 40, 42-46 13
VFT Animals category 6 24
VFT Fruits category 3 12
VFT semantics without specifying category 2 8
VFT “S” 1 4
VFT Vegetables category 1 4
VFT Supermarket category 1 4
VFT “A” of the EXIT-25 1 4
Clock Drawing Test (CDT) 6 24 Planning 1, 21-23, 31, 33, 40, 43, 45-49 13
Rey Complex Figure Copy 3 12
Action Program Test (BADS) 3 12
Key Search Test (BADS) 3 12
Zoo Map Test (BADS) 3 12
Tower of London 2 8
Setting Clock Test 1 4
Verbal Clock Test 1 4
Block Design subtest (WAIS-R) 1 4
Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices 1 4
Digits Forward and Backward subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) 6 24 working
memory
21-22, 29-31, 33, 35-39, 42, 49 13
Codes subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) 5 20
Arithmetic subtest (WAIS-R) 1 4
Number-Letter Sequencing subtest (WAIS-III) 1 4
Mind Control (WMS-R) 2 8
Clock Reading Test 1 4
Updating Test 1 4
SWM Strategy (CANTAB) 1 4
Cognitive Estimation Test 1 4
PaSMO 1 4
Stroop test 6 24 inhibitory
control
1, 22, 29-30, 38-41, 43-45, 47, 49 13
Rule Shift Cards Test (BADS) 2 8
Inhibitory control subtest (FAB) 2 8
D-KEFS Interference Inhibition Test 1 4
Inhibitory control subtest (EXIT-25) 1 4
Hayling test 1 4
Codes subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) 5 20 processing
speed
30, 33, 35-37, 49 6
Simple reaction time (SRT) test (CANTAB) 1 4

VFT: Verbal Fluency Test; BADS: Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome.

Few studies chose to use only one EF test, considering only one domain, and most studies combined two or more EF domains, as follows:

  • [1] Only one EF domain: 4 studies; of these, three were interested in the psychometric properties or validation of a specific test and one study opted for a mental flexibility test as the single measure of executive function;

  • [2] Combining two EF domains: five studies; of these, four chose one or more verbal fluency tests plus one inhibitory control test, planning test or mental flexibility test; only one study combined tests assessing mental flexibility and inhibitory control;

  • [3] Combining three EF domains: nine studies;

  • [4] Combining four EF domains: six studies;

  • [5] Combining five EF domains: only one study; all domains except for inhibitory control were included in this study.

These results, together with the references of the studies, are shown in Table 3.

Table 3.

Frequency of studies by number of domains used to evaluate executive functions in aging research.

Number of EF domains
evaluated in study
Domain
(number of studies)
Study references
1 planning (3) or mental flexibility (1) 1, 34, 47-48
2 verbal fluency (4), mental flexibility (2), planning (2) and inhibitory control (2) 23, 32, 41, 44, 46
3 working memory (7), mental flexibility (6), verbal fluency (4), planning (4),
inhibitory control (4) and processing speed (2)
21, 29, 31, 35, 37, 39, 42-43,45
4 working memory (5), mental flexibility (4), inhibitory control (4), verbal fluency (4), processing speed (3) and planning (3) 22, 30, 36, 38, 40, 49
5 mental flexibility, verbal fluency, planning, working memory, processing speed 33

DISCUSSION

The systematic review presented here identified that the tests most frequently used to assess executive functions in aging research in the last five years were:

  • [1] Trail Making Test Form B,

  • [2] Verbal Fluency Test - F, A and S,

  • [3] VFT Animals category,

  • [4] Clock Drawing Test,

  • [5] Digits subtests (WAIS-R or WAIS-III),

  • [6] Stroop Test, and

  • [7] Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and its variants (Table 1).

The EF domains predominantly evaluated were:

  • [1] mental flexibility (15 studies),

  • [2] verbal fluency (13 studies),

  • [3] planning (13 studies),

  • [4] working memory (13 studies), and

  • [5] inhibitory control (13 studies), as shown in Table 2.

Processing speed was evaluated in 6 of the 25 studies (Table 2). The assessment of processing speed is of utmost importance in aging because it is one of the first processes to decline in the early stages of dementia.12 However, processing speed was assessed in only six studies. Most of the selected articles favored the use of tests involving the most complex processes of executive functions that require greater action control.

As seen in Table 3, none of the 25 studies selected evaluated all six domains and only one study combined EF tests covering five domains.33 This can be explained by the attempt to reduce the time spent on a long neuropsychological assessment. In other words, if many neuropsychological tests were used to evaluate EF domains, in addition to tests assessing other cognitive functions, data collection would take too long, resulting in high dropout rates and making it difficult to conduct research with a large sample.41,46 However, important EF data is lost when some domains are not covered in a neuropsychological assessment.

There is no established criterion in the literature with respect to the optimal number of tests needed to evaluate executive functions, but, according to the results of the present review, combining 3 to 4 EF domains in comprehensive batteries was the most frequent procedure adopted.21,22, 29,31,35-40,42,43,45,49 The choice of three or more combinations selected by the majority of studies in this review21,22,29-31,33,35-40,42,43,45,49 (Table 3) is because EFs are a multidimensional construct, i.e. comprise a series of interrelated skills and high-level cognitive processing and recruit several domains in parallel, such as mental flexibility, planning, verbal fluency, inhibitory control, processing speed, and working memory.3,4 Researchers seem to avoid using only one or two EF measures in order to ensure that most of the EF domains are covered. A few studies23,32,41,44,46 (Table 3) restricted the EF assessment to two tests, usually combining a flexibility, planning or inhibitory control test with a verbal fluency test, which is quick, easy-to-apply and sensitive for discriminating people with dementia, MCI and healthy older adults.13

As seen in Table 2, the TMT Form B and WCST and its variants were the most frequently selected tests for assessing mental flexibility. The TMT Form B was used in nine studies (from a total of 14 studies assessing flexibility) whereas the WCST or one of its variants were used in six studies. The TMT Form B is predominantly a mental flexibility measure.14,50 The individual must switch their focus of attention repeatedly between two sequences (numerical and alphabetical). Mental engagement, motor dexterity, and working memory are also recruited during the test.14,50 The WCST and its variants also assess mental flexibility.51 The subject must migrate from one classification rule to another during the test. Selective attention and impulsivity are also evaluated in this test.51 The fact that the TMT Form B has a simpler and faster application procedure than the WCST and its variants justifies its selection by most studies.

Other tests commonly used to evaluate executive functions in aging were the VFT - F, A and S and the VFT Animals category. These tests were mainly chosen for the evaluation of verbal fluency. Seven studies that assessed this domain chose the VFT - F, A and while six chose the VFT Animals category. The VFT - F, A and S evaluate verbal fluency and entail an active search for specific information in memory.17 The VFT Animals category predominantly evaluates verbal fluency.19 During execution of the task, there is an active search for information falling into a certain category. Language and semantic memory are also evaluated in this test.19

Another frequently used task was the CDT, which was chosen by six studies out of a total of 13 that evaluated planning. Planning is the main cognitive process recruited during the CDT, since the circle should be large enough to accommodate the 12 numbers of the clock, which in turn should be equally distributed and correctly positioned. In addition, the hands must mark the requested time, with the minute hand greater than the hour hand. Visual constructive skills are also evaluated in this test.18 CDT is simple and widely used in cognitive aging research and for cognitive impairment screening in geriatric services.18

A working memory measure, the Digits subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) was the most frequently used, having been chosen by six out of thirteen studies. This test encompasses two tasks, forward and the backward. A detailed description of the test can be found in Table 1. The Digits forward subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) evaluates information storage by verbal working memory. A string of digits is dictated by the examiner and the participant has to repeat it immediately after. The Digits backward subtest (WAIS-R or WAIS-III) measures verbal content processing in working memory.

Finally, the Stroop test was the most frequently chosen test (6 out of 11 studies) for evaluating inhibitory control. It assesses the subject's ability to inhibit an automatic behavior (reading a word) and perform a controlled behavior (saying the color the word is printed in).

Executive functions undergo changes during the normal aging process, that is, performance on EF tests is expected to deteriorate with increasing age.14,15,17-20,25-27 Decline in EF during the aging process can be mild or severe. Older people with a significant decline in executive functions fall within the Cognitive Impairment Mild (MCI) diagnostic category and may progress to dementia.52 Executive dysfunction in aging is related to a reduced ability to perform activities of daily living, which creates dependency, loss of autonomy, and reduced quality of life,7 justifying the importance of investigating the functioning of EF in the elderly population and the main tests discussed in the present review.

Besides the age effect, EF tests are affected by education, that is, the lower the subject's educational level, the worse the test performance.14,15,17-20,25-28 The Brazilian older population has a higher educational variability than older people in developed countries.53 The relationship between education and cognition is widely acknowledged in the literature. Foss et al.54 conducted a study showing that educational level is strongly correlated with subtests of executive functions and other cognitive functions (attention, visual constructive skills, language, memory, and global cognition score). The sample consisted of 502 Brazilian older adults without cognitive deficits. The results indicated that illiterate older adults performed significantly worse than participants with 1 year or more of formal education. Participants with 1 or 2 years of education had a significantly worse performance on all subtests than participants with 3 or 4 years of education which, in turn, performed worse than the group with 11 or more years of formal education. Groups with 5-10 years of education showed no differences in performance.

There is evidence in the literature that the performance of executive functions in older adults is also influenced by socioeconomic status.52 Charchat-Fichman et al.52 mapped the neuropsychological profile of a sample of 88 elderly people without dementia at a geriatric outpatient clinic of a public hospital in the city of Rio de Janeiro. Patients with traumatic brain injury, stroke, Parkinson's disease, and other neurological or psychiatric disorders previously diagnosed by the medical team, were excluded. Charchat-Fichman et al.52 concluded that 94.3% of the sample had significant EF impairment. The researchers hypothesized that this result was a reflection of the low socioeconomic status of the sample, noting that perhaps the level of education is not the only factor involved in EF decline among elderly. Socioeconomic variables may contribute to the low performance of elderly on EF tests.52 These data raises the question of whether the tests commonly used to assess EF in aging are suitable for the Brazilian population.

This study identified the tests and specific domains most frequently used in the last five years to assess EF in older adults. To our knowledge, there are no other review studies in the literature on EF testing during aging. This study raises the issue of the adequacy of the most frequently used tests for use in the Brazilian older population, considering the inherent variability in educational and socio-demographic levels, which result in heterogeneity of cognitive test outcomes. Knowing which EF tests and domains are chosen by the main research groups can direct future research and help build appropriate EF assessment protocols for different educational levels and socio-demographic profiles of older adults in Brazil.

Footnotes

This study was conducted at the Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro.

Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Author contributions. Camila de Assis Faria realizou a revisão sistemática, escreveu a introdução, métodos, resultados e discussão. Helenice Charchat Fichman delineou o artigo, orientou todas as etapas de confecção do artigo e revisou a introdução, métodos, resultados e discussão. Heloisa Veiga Dias Alves revisou o conteúdo do artigo e o texto em inglês.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Espinosa A, Alegret M, Boada M, Vinyes G, et al. Ecological assessment of executive functions in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:751–757. doi: 10.1017/S135561770999035X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Diamond A. Executive Functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kluwe-Schiavon B, Viola TW, Grassi-Oliveira R. Theoretical models about single or multiple processes of executive functions. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam. 2012;4:29–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Uehara E, Charchat-Fichman H, Landeira-Fernandez J. Executive functions? An integrative portrait of the main models and theories of this concept. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam. 2013;5:25–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lehto JE, Juujaarvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Brit J Develop Psychol. 2003;21:59–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brocki KC, Bohlin G. Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Develop Neuropsychol. 2004;26:571–593. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mograbi DC, Faria CA, Charchat-Fichman H, Paradela EMP, Lourenço RA. Relationship between activities of daily living and cognitive ability in a sample of older adults with heterogeneous educational level. Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2014;17:71–76. doi: 10.4103/0972-2327.128558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Amieva H, Phillips L, Sala SD. Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain Cogn. 2003;53:129–132. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00094-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med. 2014;275:214–228. doi: 10.1111/joim.12190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman D, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:1447–1455. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mitchell MB, Shaughnessy LW, Shirk SD, Yang FM, Atri A. Neuropsychological Test Performance and Cognitive Reserve in Healthy Aging and the Alzheimer's Disease Spectrum: A Theoretically-Driven Factor Analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:1071–1080. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Thorvaldsson V, Macdonald SW, Fratiglioni L, et al. Onset and rate of cognitive change before dementia diagnosis: findings from two Swedish population-based longitudinal studies. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:154–162. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Caramelli P, Carthery MT, Porto SP, Charchat-Fichman H, Nitrini R. Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007;21:65–67. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31802f244f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19:203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hamdan AC, Hamdan EMLR. Effects of age and education level on the Trail Making Test in A healthy Brazilian sample. Psychol Neurosci. 2009;2:199–203. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative Data Stratified by Age and Education for Two Measures of Verbal Fluency: FAS and Animal Naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14:167–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Machado TH, Charchat-Fichman H, Santos EL, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol. 2009;3:55–60. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientifc Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2005;63:720–727. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2005000400034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2009;3:49–54. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Agrell B, Dehljn O. The clock-drawing test. Age Ageing. 1998;27:399–403. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Leyhe T, Saur R, Eschweiler GW, Milian M. Clock test deficits are associated with semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2009;22:235–245. doi: 10.1177/0891988709335798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.de Paula JJ, Bertola L, Ávila RT, et al. Clinical applicability and cutoff values for an unstructured neuropsychological assessment protocol for older adults with low formal education. PLoS One. 2013;8:e73167. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Forti P, Olivelli V, Rietti E, Maltoni B, Ravaglia G. Diagnostic performance of an Executive Clock Drawing Task (CLOX) as a screening test for mild cognitive impairment in elderly persons with cognitive complaints. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:20–27. doi: 10.1159/000315515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fabricio AT, Aprahamian I, Yassuda MS. Qualitative analysis of the Clock Drawing Test by educational level and cognitive profile. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2014;72(4):289–295. doi: 10.1590/0004-282x20140004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Choi HJ, Lee DY, Seo EH, et al. A Normative Study of the Digit Span in an Educationally Diverse Elderly Population. Psychiatry Investig. 2014;11:39–43. doi: 10.4306/pi.2014.11.1.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bayard S, Erkes J, Moroni C. Victoria Stroop Test: Normative Data in a Sample Group of Older People and the Study of Their Clinical Applications in the Assessment of Inhibition in Alzheimer's Disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2011;26:653–661. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Klein M, Adda CC, Miotto EC, Lucia MCS, Scaff M. O Paradigma Stroop Em Uma Amostra De Idosos Brasileiros. Psicol Hosp. 2010;8:93–112. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Abe M, Suzuki K, Okada K, et al. Normative data on tests for frontal lobe functions: Trail Making Test, Verbal fluency, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Keio version) No To Shinkei. 2004;56:567–574. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jacus JP, Gély-Nargeot MC. Impact of dysexecutive syndrome on autonomy in early Alzheimer's disease and Mild cognitive impairment. Geriatr Psychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil. 2014;12:101–112. doi: 10.1684/pnv.2014.0449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gaudreau G, Monetta L, Macoir J, Laforce R Jr, Poulin S, Hudon C. Verbal irony comprehension in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology. 2013;27:702–712. doi: 10.1037/a0034655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bastug G, Ozel-Kizil ET, Sakarya A, Altintas O, Kirici S, Altunoz U. Oral trail making task as a discriminative tool for different levels of cognitive impairment and normal aging. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;28:411–417. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Parra MA, Ascencio LL, Urquina HF, Manes F, Ibáñez AM. P300 and neuropsychological assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer dementia. Front Neurol. 2012;3:172–172. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cercy SP. The Verbal Clock Test: preliminary validation of a brief, vision- and motor-free measure of executive function in a clinical sample. Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;26:1312–1341. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2012.725100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Berna F, Schönknecht P, Seidl U, Toro P, Schröder J. Episodic autobiographical memory in normal aging and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:807–812. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Summers MJ, Saunders NL. Neuropsychological measures predict decline to Alzheimer's dementia from mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:498–508. doi: 10.1037/a0028576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Malek-Ahmadi M, Raj A, Small BJ. Semantic clustering as a neuropsychological predictor for amnestic-MCI. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2011;18:280–292. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2010.540642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Chang YL, Bondi MW, McEvoy LK, et al. Global clinical dementia rating of 0.5 in MCI masks variability related to level of function. Neurology. 2011;76:652–659. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820ce6a5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Yubero R, Gil P, Paul N, Maestú F. Influence of memory strategies on memory test performance: a study in healthy and pathological aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2011;18:497–515. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2011.597840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Nordlund A, Rolstad S, Göthlin M, Edman A, Hansen S, Wallin A. Cognitive profiles of incipient dementia in the Goteborg MCI study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:403–410. doi: 10.1159/000321352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pereira FS, Yassuda MS, Oliveira AM, et al. Profiles of functional deficits in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: benefits from objective measurement. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:297–305. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709991330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bangen KJ, Jak AJ, Schiehser DM, et al. Complex activities of daily living vary by mild cognitive impairment subtype. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:630–639. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Karantzoulis S, Troyer AK, Rich JB. Prospective memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:407–415. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Paula JJ, Miranda DM, Moraes EN, Malloy-Diniz LF. Mapping the clockworks: what does the Clock Drawing Test assess in normal and pathological aging? Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2013;71:763–768. doi: 10.1590/0004-282X20130118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Schroeter ML, Vogt B, Frisch S, et al. Executive deficits are related to the inferior frontal junction in early dementia. Brain. 2012;135:201–215. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bombin I, Santiago-Ramajo S, Garolera M, et al. Functional impairment as a defining feature of: amnestic MCI cognitive, emotional, and demographic correlates. Int Psychogeriatr. 2012;24:1494–1504. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212000622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Vicini-Chilovi B, Riva M, Conti M, et al. Does age at observation time affect the clinical presentation of mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:212–218. doi: 10.1159/000318839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Costa Armentano CG, Porto CS, Nitrini R, Dozzi Brucki SM. Ecological evaluation of executive functions in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013;27:95–101. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31826540b4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.de Paula JJ, Moreira L, Nicolato R, de Marco LA. The Tower of London Test: different scoring criteria for diagnosing Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Psychol Rep. 2012;110:477–488. doi: 10.2466/03.10.13.PR0.110.2.477-488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rainville C, Lepage E, Gauthier S, Kergoat MJ, Belleville S. Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: a study with a Tower of London task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2012;34:306–324. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.639298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Oliveira-Souza R, Moll J, Passman LJ, et al. Trail making and cognitive set-shifting. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2000;58:826–829. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2000000500006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Fernandes-Lopes R, Lima-Agrimon I. Idosos com diabetes mellitus tipo 2 e o desempenho cognitivo no teste Wisconsin de classificação de cartas (WCST) Universitas Psychol. 2010;9:697–713. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Charchat-Fichman H, Fernandes CS, Oliveira RM, Caramelli P, Aguiar D, Novaes R. Disexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment predominance in elderly assisted at geriatric clinic into public hospital in Rio de Janeiro. Rev Neuropsicol Latinoam. 2013;5:31–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Porto CS, et al. Brief cognitive battery in the diagnosis of mild Alzheimer's disease in subjects with medium and high levels of education. Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1:32–36. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642008DN10100006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Foss MP, Carvalho VA, Machado TH, et al. Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) Normative data for the Brazilian middle-age and elderly populations. Dement Neuropsychol. 2013;7:374–379. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642013DN74000004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dementia & Neuropsychologia are provided here courtesy of Academia Brasileira de Neurologia

RESOURCES