Skip to main content
Dementia & Neuropsychologia logoLink to Dementia & Neuropsychologia
. 2013 Jan-Mar;7(1):55–59. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642013DN70100009

Analysis of a case series of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: emphasis on diagnostic delay

Análise de uma série de casos de variante comportamental da demência frontotemporal: ênfase no atraso diagnóstico

Henrique Cerqueira Guimarães 1, Thiago Cardoso Vale 1, Victor Pimentel 2, Nayara Carvalho de Sá 2, Rogério Gomes Beato 2, Paulo Caramelli 3,
PMCID: PMC5619545  PMID: 29213820

Abstract

INTRODUCTION

Despite many advances in the characterization of the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), the diagnosis of this syndrome poses a significant challenge, while delays or diagnostic mistakes may impact the proper clinical management of these patients.

OBJECTIVE

To describe the clinical profile at first evaluation of a sample of patients with bvFTD from a specialized outpatient neurological unit, with emphasis on the analysis of the delay between the onset of symptoms and diagnosis.

METHODS

We selected 31 patients that fulfilled international consensus criteria for possible or probable bvFTD. Patients' medical admission sheets were thoroughly reviewed.

RESULTS

Patients' mean age was 67.9±8.2 years; 16 (51.6%) were men. Mean number of years of formal education was 7.7±4.0 years. Mean age at onset was 62.2±7.7 years, indicating a mean of 5.8 years of diagnostic delay. Thirteen patients (41.9%) presented with initial behavioral complaints only, eleven patients (35.5%) had mixed behavioral and memory complaints, five patients (16.1%) presented with memory complaints only, and two patient (6.4%) had behavioral and speech problems. Nine patients (29%) were admitted with alternative diagnoses. Mean and standard deviation scores for the mini-mental state examination, animal category fluency and memory test for drawings (five-minute delayed recall) were 19.3±6.3, 8.3±4.1and 3.7±2.7, respectively.

CONCLUSION

Most patients from this sample were evaluated almost six years after the onset of symptoms and performed poorly on both cognitive screening tests and functional evaluation measures.

Keywords: frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer's disease, dementia, diagnosis

INTRODUCTION

The behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) is the most frequent presenting phenotype from a wide spectrum of neurodegenerative diseases collectively named Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration (FTLD). This miscellaneous entity also includes the language-impaired subtypes, Progressive Non-fluent Aphasia (PNFA) and Semantic Dementia (SD); extrapyramidal-predominant clinical phenotypes such as Progressive Supranuclear Palsy (PSP) and Corticobasal Syndrome (CBS); and finally, the bvFTD associated with motor neuron disease.1 It remains intriguing as to how a handful of implicated genes can determine two major groups of pathological landmarks i.e. tau pathology and ubiquitin inclusions, albeit so many heterogeneous and overlapping phenotypes.2 Indeed, even among subjects with the same genetic mutation, the clinical picture can differ dramatically.3

The bvFTD seems to equally affect both sexes, although some series have reported a male predominance.4 The mean age of onset is in the sixth decade and a positive family history is found in up to 40% of cases. The degenerative process targets preferentially cortical regions that involve the anterior cingulate, insular and orbitofrontal cortex, and anterior temporal pole, which are essential to value-guided decision-making, motivation, social cognition, emotion identification, emphatic concern, and appropriate personal conduct, with relative preservation of episodic memory.5 At disease onset, patients may manifest psychiatric symptoms such as maniform states, compulsive behavior, apathy and even psychotic features6 that might be misconstrued as depression or a primary psychiatric disorder, delaying the proper evaluation and clinical management. Moreover, there is a growing body of evidence7 that a significant proportion of bvFTD patients can present with severe amnestic symptoms, exhibiting a performance on classical neuropsychological tests that is indistinguishable from that observed in Alzheimer's disease (AD).8,9 In contrast, pathologically-confirmed AD patients can be misdiagnosed as bvFTD if behavioral presentation is more salient at onset.10

Despite major advances in the characterization of bvFTD,1 the diagnosis of this syndrome remains a challenge, and delays or diagnostic mistakes may occur in many patients. These difficulties may be even greater in developing countries where several resources, such as functional neuroimaging, e.g., positron emission tomography (PET) or single photon emission tomography (SPECT) scans, genetic testing, and trained professionals are still scarce.

To our knowledge, scant studies have discussed the issue of diagnostic delay in bvFTD.11-13 Nonetheless, it was the Brazilian study13 that reported the longest time delay between disease onset and proper diagnosis. However, all of these reports included PNFA and SD patients, in whom the salient language impairment might have facilitated early referral for specialized assessment. In this regard, the primary aim of this study was to provide a summarized clinical picture from a sample of bvFTD patients upon first arrival for evaluation at a cognitive and behavioral neurology outpatient unit, and also to investigate the possible variables associated with delay in specialized assessment.

METHODS

Thirty-one patients were consecutively selected after medical chart review. Subjects that, on follow-up, fulfilled international consensus criteria for probable or possible bvFTD had their medical admission sheets thoroughly reviewed. Only patients that had their bvFTD diagnosis first ascertained at our unit were included. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Minas Gerais, in Belo Horizonte, Brazil.

We collected data on major demographic and clinical features, along with basic neuroimaging data. Salient clinical symptoms at disease onset were obtained through caregiver reports. The first behavioral core diagnostic disorder was used to define the age at onset. All patients were evaluated with a brief cognitive screening battery consisting of the mini-mental state examination (MMSE), semantic category fluency for animals, and a picture drawing episodic memory test.14 They were also evaluated by means of the Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ)15 and Katz's Index of independence in activities of daily living.16

According to consensus diagnostic criteria17, delayed evaluation was defined as those participants in whom it took more than three years from symptoms onset to diagnosis. Statistics consisted of a descriptive analysis (means±standard deviation), emphasizing the demographic and clinical profile of these patients at first evaluation. We also estimated potential variables that might have been associated with delayed admission, by performing a stepwise logistic regression analysis. Medcalc version 11.6 was used for statistical analysis.

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the main demographic and clinical data extracted from the admission sheets of the participants. The sample consisted of 31 patients, with a mean age of 67.9±8.2 years at the time of evaluation in our specialized unit. Subjects had 7.7±4.0mean years of formal education, and 16 (51.6%) were men. Patients' clinical history suggested that the onset of disease was on average at 62.2±7.7mean years of age. This data revealed a mean delay of 5.8±3.8 years until evaluation at our unit.

Table 1.

Main demographic and clinical features along with evaluation performance of the sample.

Variables Age at onset (mean±SD ; range) 62.2±8.2 ; 43-78  
Age at first evaluation (mean±SD ; range) 67.9±7.7 ; 47-81  
Delay since onset, years (mean±SD ; range) 5.8±3.8 ; 1-16  
Years of formal education (mean±SD ; range) 7.7±4.0 ; 0-18  
Gender (male/female) 16/15  
Diagnosis after first evaluation (%):    
    bvFTD 70.9%  
    Other 29.1%  
Diagnostic category after follow-up (n):    
    probable bvFTD 22  
    possible bvFTD*: 9  
      absent progression** 2  
      unspecified neuroimaging features*** 7  
Clinical features at onset (%)    
    behavioral disorder 41.9%  
    memory complaints and behavioral disorder 35.5%  
    memory complaints 16.1%  
    behavioral and speech disorder 6.4%  
    Observed Expected
Evaluations Brief Cognitive Screening Battery (mean±SD)    
MMSE (0-30), 19.3±6.3 >24
Category fluency (animals/min.) 8.3±4.1 >12
Memory test for drawings    
    Learning (0-30) 11.1±7.2 >20
    5-minute delayed recall (0-10) 3.7±2.7 >5
Functional measures (mean±SD)    
    Katz Index of independence in BADL (0-6) 5.1±1.8 6
    Functional Activities Questionnaire (30-0) 16.5±8.8 <5
*

Six patients had only structural neuroimaging data.

**

both patients also have unspecified neuroimaging features.

***

including abnormal SPECT findings in three patients. BADL: Basic Activities of Daily Living; Numbers between brackets mean range of possible scores from the worst to best performance.

Weighted average according to 5 levels of schooling ( 0, 1-3, 4-7, 8-11, >11 years).

Almost 30% of the participants had a diagnosis other than bvFTD at the first round of evaluations in the unit, meeting consensus diagnostic criteria only on follow-up visits: four had unspecified psychiatric diagnosis and five supposedly had AD. Regarding clinical presentation at onset, 13 patients (41.9%) displayed only behavioral disorders, 11 (35.5%) had mixed behavioral disorders and memory complaints, five (16.1%) had only memory complaints, and two (6.4%) participants presented with behavioral disorder and motor speech problems without aphasia (both developed features consistent with PSP-spectrum on follow-up).

There were nine participants that did not fulfill probable bvFTD consensus criteria: two because of absent progression, one of whom had a strong family history of bvFTD whereas the other, a conspicuous disinhibited non-amnestic patient, had abnormal SPECT findings with diffuse and heterogeneous perfusion deficits, especially in posterior parietal regions. The other seven participants with possible bvFTD were progressors and did not meet consensus criteria because of unspecific neuroimaging data. All of these cases had mostly diffuse brain atrophy without a typical frontotemporal pattern. Unfortunately, six of these patients had just structural neuroimaging findings, precluding additional conclusions that might have been achieved with PET or SPECT scans.

Data given in Table 1 show that, at first evaluation in the unit, average performance of the participants was unequivocally impaired on all the brief cognitive screening tests, including the episodic memory test. The lower right-hand side column shows the expected scores according to control-reference performance,18 weight-adjusted for five levels of formal education. Functional impairment was also salient, with moderate impairment in instrumental activities of daily living (FAQ: 16.5±8.8 points), and mild dependence in self-care (Katz Index: 5.1±1.8points).

Using a stepwise logistic regression model, we tested which clinical variables might have contributed to the delay (>3 years) from onset of symptoms to first evaluation at our specialized unit. The best model (AUC=0.734; 95% CI: 0.545-0.876), considering gender, schooling and age at onset, showed only memory complaints at presentation as a significant variable associated (OR: 0.12; 95% CI: 0.020-0.714) with delayed evaluation.

DISCUSSION

Most patients in this case series were evaluated in a specialized unit after almost six years from symptom onset and performed poorly on both cognitive screening tests and functional evaluation measures. Even in this specialized unit, almost 30% of the reported patients did not receive accurate diagnosis during the first round of evaluations. In these cases, bvFTD was only ascertained at follow-up visits. Regarding the characteristics of bvFTD, memory complaints were present in almost half of the sample, isolated or associated with behavioral disorders. The presence of memory issues, according to caregiver reports, was a strong predictor of an earlier assessment at the unit.

Previous studies from developed countries that sought to evaluate early manifestations of bvFTD found, on average, around two years of delay from onset to proper diagnosis.11,12 In a Spanish case series of 42 mixed FTLD patients, the reported delay was 3.5 years on average. According to Bahia et al.,13 in a cohort of FTLD patients from Brazil, almost 75% of the sample received an AD or psychiatric diagnosis prior to a specialized evaluation, and the delay from onset to proper diagnosis was around four years.

It seems quite reasonable that the bvFTD heterogeneous genetic and pathological background potentially gives rise to very complex and variable clinical presentations. In turn, these issues inevitably increase the effort for an accurate and early diagnosis of this syndrome, especially when technological resources are not available. It was only recently, after the new proposed diagnostic criteria,17 that most of the research groups became more comfortable to define a bvFTD diagnosis in patients with an amnestic presentation. The finding that memory complaints might have contributed to an early assessment at our unit suggests that the appropriate characterization of psychiatric symptoms, which dominate the clinical picture in the early stages of the disease, may still be a major obstacle to correct identification and treatment of these patients.19

Regarding limitations, we should emphasize that the small sample size and the study's retrospective design preclude any generalizations of our findings, although the results were in accordance with a previous report in Brazil. We chose to include nine patients that only met possible bvFTD consensus criteria. These subjects had no better explanation for their clinical presentation. Additionally, except for one subject who had a strong family history of FTLD, these patients did not match the current definition of FTD phenocopy, since they had either progressive deterioration or abnormal neuroimaging findings, even if non-specific. A common limitation of research based on clinical settings is the lack of autopsy-confirmed diagnosis. This caveat could have been reduced in our series by using pathophysiological biomarkers either in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) or position emission tomography.20 It has been previously demonstrated that CSF markers help discriminate bvFTD from AD with high sensitivity and specificity.21 The selection of patients according to their CSF biomarkers profile would increase the specificity of our clinical diagnosis,10 avoiding the inclusion of AD patients with frontal presentation.

Footnotes

Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Van Lagenhove T, van der Zee J, Gijselinck I, et al. Distinct Clinical Characteristics of C9orf72 Expansion Carriers Compared With GRN, MAPT, and Nonmutation Carriers in a Flanders-Belgian FTLD Cohort. JAMA Neurol. 2013;1:1–9. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamaneurol.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Galimberti D, Scarpini E. Genetics of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Front Neur. 2012;3:52. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Benussi L, Guidoni R, Pegoiani E, Moretti DV, Zanetti O, Binetti G. Progranulin Leu271LeufsX10 is one of the most commom FTLD and CBS associated mutations worldwide. Neurobiol Dis. 2009;33:379–385. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2008.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Seelaar H, Rohrer JD, Pijnenburg YA, Fox NC, van Swieten JC. Clinical, genetic and pathological heterogeneity of frontotemporal dementia: a review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2011;82:476–486. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2010.212225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Neary D, Snowden J, Mann D. Frontotemporal dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:771–780. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70223-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Snowden JS, Rollinson S, Thompson JC, et al. Distinct clinical and pathological characteristics of frontotemporal dementia associated with C9orf72 mutations. Brain. 2012;135:693–708. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hornberger M, Piguet O. Episodic memory in frontotemporal dementia: a critical review. Brain. 2012;135:678–692. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hornberger M, Piguet O, Grahan AJ, Nestor PJ, Hodges JR. How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia? Neurology. 2010;74:472–479. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181cef85d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Liscic RM, Storandt M, Cairns NJ, Morris JC. Clinical and psychometrical distinction of frontotemporal and Alzheimer frontotemporal dementias. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:535–540. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.4.535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mendez MF, Joshi A, Tassniyom K, et al. Clinicopathologic differencess among patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. 2013;80:561–568. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182815547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hallam BJ, Silverberg ND, LaMarre AK, Mackenzie IR, Feldman HH. Clinical presentation of prodromal frontotemporal dementia. Am J Alzheimer Dis Other Demen. 2008;22:456–467. doi: 10.1177/1533317507308781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pijnenburg AL, Gillissen F, Jonker C, Scheltens P. Initial complaints in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;17:302–306. doi: 10.1159/000077159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bahia VS. Underdiagnosis of frontotemporal lobar degeneration in Brazil. Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1:361–365. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642008DN10400006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nitrini R, Lefèvre BH, Mathias SC, et al. Brief and easy-to-administer neuropsychological tests in the diagnosis of dementia. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 1994;52:457–465. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x1994000400001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pfeffer RI, Kurosaki TT, Harrah CH, et al. Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol. 1982;37:323–329. doi: 10.1093/geronj/37.3.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Katz S, Downs TD, Cash HR, Grotz RC. Progress in the development of the index of ADL. Gerontologist. 1970;10:20–30. doi: 10.1093/geront/10.1_part_1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, et al. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2011;134:2456–2477. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Machado TH, Guimarães HC, Barbosa MT, et al. Cognitive descriptive analysis of control subjects in an epidemiologic study: The Pietà Study. Dement & Neuropsychol. 2009;3(Suppl 1):17. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Manes F. Psychiatric conditions that can mimic early behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: the importance of the new diagnostic criteria. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2012;14:450–452. doi: 10.1007/s11920-012-0294-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rabinovici GD, Rosen HJ, Alkalay A, Kornak J, Furst AJ, et al. Amyloid vs FDG-PET in the differential diagnosis of AD and FTLD. Neurology. 2011;77:2034–2042. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823b9c5e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bian H, Van Swieten JC, Leight S, Massimo L, Wood E, et al. CSF biomarkers in frontotemporal lobar degeneration with known pathology. Neurology. 2008;70:1827–1835. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000311445.21321.fc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dementia & Neuropsychologia are provided here courtesy of Academia Brasileira de Neurologia

RESOURCES