Abstract
The extent of cultural variation in emotion perception has long been assumed to be bounded by underlying universality. A growing body of research reveals, however, that evidence of universality in emotion perception is method-bound. Without the assumption of underlying universality, new lines of inquiry become relevant. Accumulating evidence suggests that cultures vary in what cues are relevant to perceptions of emotion. Further, cultural groups vary in their spontaneous inferences; mental state inference does not appear to be the only, or even most routine, mode of perception across cultures. Finally, setting universality assumptions aside requires innovation in the theory and measurement of culture. Recent studies reveal the promise of refinements in psychological approaches to culture. Together, the available evidence is consistent with a view of emotion perceptions as actively constructed by perceivers to fit the social and physical constraints of their cultural worlds.
Emotions are central to the human experience—emotions characterize the most profound moments in our lives, and sharing these experiences can bring us closer to other people, forming a foundation for intimacy. It is appealing, then, to assume that this capacity for sharing emotions transcends what sets humans apart-- the knowledge, values, beliefs, practices, institutions, roles and languages that vary substantially across individuals and groups as aspects of human culture. A large amount of empirical work has centered on whether emotion perceptions, the inferences people form about the emotional experiences of others, can occur across cultural divides. Evidence consistently reveals a “gap” in emotion perception across cultures: people are better at perceiving the emotions of others in their same cultural group (typically defined along national borders) than from another cultural group [1].
Variation in emotion perception is often interpreted within the constraints of universality: A set of innate, evolved emotional expressions exists—but the deployment and subsequent perception of these expressions is modulated by culture. Critical to this viewpoint are the ideas that certain content (i.e., facial actions that are mechanistically produced by emotions such as the wide-eyed expression for fear), and certain attributions (i.e., the ability to perceive another person as angry, sad and so on), are universal. The assumption of underlying universality has long been based on a small set of experiments [for reviews 2**, 3] conducted in the 1960s and 1970s by Ekman and colleagues [4–6] in which individuals from relatively isolated samples perceived the predicted emotions posed by Westerners at above-chance levels.
Does universality undergird cultural variation?
In the last decade [7–9], and at an accelerated pace in the last few years [10–16], several research groups have revisited the assumption that emotional expressions are universally perceived. These tests are a critical and timely endeavor in the face of many shifting sociodemographic factors across the globe (e.g., from rural to urban, from culturally isolated to interconnected, from subsistence agriculture to commerce, and so on), that are serving to produce profound cultural changes [17]. The pace of this cultural change is closing the window of opportunity to observe human diversity in its broadest scope, including in the remaining small-scale societies around the world.
These recent tests reveal that evidence for universality is not robust when less constrained (see Figure 1) methods are employed [11*,12–14,15*]. When free labeling [13–15*] and sorting [14] methods are used to study emotion perceptions, agreement with universality predictions is extremely low (and often at-chance). For example, facial actions that are labeled as expressions of sadness, disgust, fear, and so on, by Europeans or Americans are rarely labeled as translational equivalents by individuals from small-scale societies in Africa and Papua New Guinea [12,15]. Similarly, when perceivers are asked to free-sort expressions, they do not generate piles for the canonical expressions built into the set (i.e., poses for anger, fear, sadness, disgust, happiness and neutral) [14]. Further, when spontaneous expressions (taken from Ekman’s observational work in Papua New Guinea) were presented to perceivers in a small-scale society in Papua New Guinea, use of Ekman’s predicted labels was extremely low (0–16%), even in a forced-choice task (13–38%) [13]. These findings also replicate in other samples in the US and Middle-East [18,19].
Figure 1.
Examples of experimental tasks employed in the cross-cultural study of emotion perception. Tasks range from more conceptually constrained (choice-from-array) to less constrained (sorting, free labeling) varieties. More constrained tasks are confirmatory-oriented; words and stories provide content for the perceiver that narrows the meaning that can be constructed. Less constrained tasks are discovery-oriented; without conceptual content provided to the perceiver, the researcher can observe how meaning is spontaneously constructed by the perceiver.
In contrast, a handful of recent studies have used more constrained methods in research aimed to expand the scope of universality. These experiments were conducted to test whether an expanded cadre of facial expressions [7,16] and vocalizations [9,10] can also be considered universal cues to emotional states such as pride and awe. These studies have used choice-from-array tasks (Figure 1), akin to those used by Ekman and colleagues in their classic work, and have found evidence consistent with universality, although not as robustly as in Ekman’s classic work. That is, perceivers from small-scale societies can match facial actions (e.g., a wide-eyed pose) and vocalizations (e.g., a scream) to a word (e.g., “fear”) or story in a manner consistent with universalist predictions (above-chance). Yet these research findings should be contextualized by lab-based research revealing that methods involving forced-choice can produce artefactual agreement on an emotion label, even a nonsense one [20]. Further, some research has not revealed robust universalist performance, even when the constrained choice-from-array method (Figure 1) is used, but with modifications (e.g., minimizing repeated measures) [12]. This evidence indicates that support for universality is highly method-bound and calls into question the widely-held assumption that cultural variation in emotion perception is only superficial, due to undergirding universal emotion mechanisms.
These recent tests of the universality of emotion perception also underscore the value of replication, direct and conceptual, and mirror a critical discussion occurring in the field of psychological science more broadly [21]. Careful consideration of experimental methods as a context is critical to our ability to integrate findings and arrive at meaningful conclusions [22]. Perhaps more importantly, this literature points to future directions for the study of emotion perception across cultures: 1) certain cues (e.g., facial actions) are not privileged, such that future work must consider how culture impacts what types of cues inform emotion perception; 2) certain attributions (e.g., perceptions of “fear” “sadness” and so on) are not privileged, such that future work must consider what types of attributions are spontaneously made by perceivers across cultures; 3) to understand variation in emotion perception, research needs to take a deeper, more theoretical approach to culture. In the remaining sections, emerging work that speaks to each of these important directions is reviewed.
Culture modulates which cues inform emotion perception
Universalist accounts of emotion perception assume that a uniform set of facial expressions (and now vocalizations and body postures) serve as cues to emotion perception. For example, if someone widens their eyes and gasps, a perceiver will infer that behavior is an expression of fear. Without the assumption of universal perception of a uniform set of expressions, there is the possibility of profound variation in how human actions (e.g., facial movements, vocalizations, bodily movements) are used as cues to infer emotion. To address this possibility, it is critical that researchers evaluate how a larger variety of human behavior may variably inform perceptions of emotion across cultures. In the history of emotion perception research, embracing this question has typically involved broadening the scope of research from a focus on the face to other types of information (e.g., vocalizations, body posture) [23], or by “contextualizing” the face by embedding it in other information (e.g., vocalizations, body posture, other people, visual scenes) [24]. The integration of these approaches within cultural and cross-cultural investigations is resulting in intriguing findings. For instance, vocalizations appear to be consistently privileged over facial actions (in neural response and in contribution to perceptions) by East Asians compared to Europeans or European Americans [25–27], an effect that tracks with acculturation [28]. East Asians also use facial actions of the group (i.e., surrounding individuals) to inform their perceptions of the facial actions of target individual to a greater extent than European Americans [29,30]. Critically, this effect is not context-general. European Americans are just as likely to integrate context (e.g., a visual scene) when it doesn’t violate the agency/independence of the target expressing emotion [31*]. This finding is consistent with European Americans’ relatively independent cultural framework, in which emotions are conceptualized of as internal states driven by external factors in the situation, but separable from the emotions of other people [32].
Of course, perceivers use more than non-verbal behavior to infer the emotional states of others. Language is a critical way that people communicate about, and therefore perceive, emotions. In European and European American cultural contexts, emotion terms are frequently used to communicate about emotions as mental states [33]. Yet the same communicative functions that are typically assumed for mental state labels—allowing people to bridge subjectivity and elicit sympathy, may be served by non-mental language. For instance, when Koreans describe negative emotional events, they more frequently use words referring to body states (e.g., headache, dizziness) than Americans. Not only are these words perceived to be more effective at communicating distress by Koreans, they actually elicit more sympathy in Korean perceivers compared to American perceivers [34]. This type of variation may be more rampant than this single study suggests. For instance, it was recently documented that “emotion” terms in two languages in West Africa actually reference bodily states, when literal translations were elicited [35].
These relatively few findings suggest that cultural variation in the content that informs emotion perception is present, particularly when experimental tasks provide context beyond single cues like facial expressions. More research is necessary to capture emotion perception as it occurs in vivo. Studying interactions that better approximate those that occur in the real-world will help researchers model how perceivers from different cultural contexts spontaneously use non-verbal and verbal cues, in conjunction with environmental information, to infer emotion.
Meaning is variously constructed across cultures
Cultural variability is present in how people attribute meaning to emotional cues (e.g., facial actions) when their spontaneous inferences are assessed. A growing body of evidence suggests that perceptions of emotional cues (e.g., facial actions) often emphasize situated behaviors rather than mental states. For instance, the African pastoralists (the Himba of Namibia) frequently perceived facial actions and vocalizations as instances of situated behaviors (“crying at a death”) rather than emotional feelings (“feeling sad”), despite instructions that emphasized feelings [14,15]. This style of perception is likely the product of cultural learning [36**], including caregiver shaping, as is the typical European and European American pattern of mental state inference [37, Atzil, this issue]. Evidence from China reveals this type of cultural shaping: Chinese mothers more frequently emphasize situated behaviors, rather than mental states, when discussing emotional events with their children [38,39]. Additional empirical investigation is needed to determine whether these finding extend to other cultural contexts. For example, it may be that individualist-collectivist dimension accounts for the emphasis on emotions as internal mental states or situated behaviors. The emphasis on external, observable actions rather than internal feelings is consistent with the cultural patterns of external attribution of behavior (as opposed to internal traits) in collectivist societies (i.e., valuing tight-knit relationships fostering dependence and supportiveness) compared to more individualistic societies (i.e., valuing loosely-knit social relationships fostering individual agency and achievement) [32]. Consistent with this interpretation, a recent cross-national comparison revealed that individuals from collectivist societies are less adept than individuals from individualistic societies at applying mentalized emotion labels (as stipulated by researchers) to non-verbal expressions [40*].
Perceptions of emotional cues can also reflect social-motives as opposed to emotional states. A social-motives framework suggests that perceivers infer an expressor’s intent, rather than an internal emotional state, because facial actions are tools for social influence [41]. Perceptions of social motives are similar to perceptions of situated behavior in that an internal emotional state is not the implied cause of emotional cues. But these modes are perception are also distinct in that the target’s goal is made more explicit in the case of social motives and the goal is specific to influencing another social agent. For example, a perceiver might describe the smile of another person as “wanting to get along” (a social motive), rather than simply “smiling at me” (a situated behavior). People within a cultural group can consistently associate social motives with emotional cues like facial actions, indicating this is one way that shared social meaning about facial expressions can be constructed [42,43]. Critically, new research indicates there is variability in this particular social meaning of facial actions, depending on the cultural context [11]. The wide-eyed gasping expression (consistently associated with “fear” in European and European American samples) was associated the social motive to “threaten another” by Trobriand Islanders, but not Europeans. Recent evidence on the social motives inferred from smiling behavior [Niedenthal, this issue] similarly reveals variability across cultures.
Universalist approaches to studying emotion perception did not leave room for these findings, because emotion perception was typically tested in a confirmatory manner. Take the forced-choice paradigm, which provides perceivers with a set of mental state labels (anger, fear, disgust etc.) and asks them to select which best describes the emotion conveyed on a face. This method not only limits perceivers’ choices to certain categories of emotion, which are not universally present as lexicalized concepts [44], but it tacitly assumes that all cultural groups will spontaneously infer a causal mental state (as opposed to a behavior or social motive) from observable action [45,46]. This approach does not capture the range of spontaneous perceptions even in a Western cultural context, where individual differences exist in tendency to use mental states or behaviors to explain the actions of others [47]. The use of open-ended methods can reveal multiple modes of inference and allows for systematic variation across cultural groups to be mapped.
Refining our approach to Culture
Cross-cultural comparisons in the emotion perception literature are conducted in an atheoretical manner; “culture” is typically operationalized along national boundaries or broad geographic areas. As definitional issues continue to be hashed out in cultural psychology [48,49], these debates have brought with them advances in the operationalization of culture. For example, approaching culture as within-the-head, as set of mental representations [49,50],i has led to the “cultural priming” paradigm, in which the accessibility of these mental representations is manipulated. This culture priming approach was recently employed to demonstrate that the impact of “group” facial actions on emotion perception in a target individual, previously documented in East Asians, can be produced by priming a collectivistic cultural value orientation (compared to an individualistic orientation) in Greek participants [51]. Similarly, research that reveals the internal perceptual representations of emotions via psychophysical methods [52] also underscores how culture, in-the-head, shapes emotion perception [Jack, this issue].
Approaching culture as in-the-world, a set of “practices, institutions, products and artifacts” [48], has led to an emphasis on discovering the distal causes of these cultural features. For instance, the well documented “gap” in the accuracy of emotion perception between in-group and out-group members [1] was recently illuminated by analyzing broad-scale, historical, sociodemographic patterns. Cultures with a history of individuals migrating from a high number of source countries, termed historical heterogeneity (HH), tend to portray emotions that are consistently perceived by other-cultural groups (i.e., these expressions produce less of a cultural gap in perception) [40]. This is consistent with the earlier finding [53] that HH is associated with prioritizing non-verbal displays of emotionii as communicative tools.
Work on “culture” in emotion perception is also being refined by incorporating additional “groups” that meet definitions of a culture. For instance, socioeconomic status, as a form of culture [57], impacts the perception of emotion such that individuals of lower social class are more accurate about the emotions of interaction partners [58]. This finding underscores the active role that culture plays in the construction of emotion perception. Emotion perception may be a more critical interpersonal process for lower class individuals who must frequently tune their behavior to meet the demands of higher status individuals.
Conclusions
Advances in the methods used to assess emotion perception and in the operationalization of culture are revealing diversity in how emotional cues are perceived. The cues that perceivers anchor on, and the types of inferences that perceivers make appear to depend on cultural context. These new findings challenge the conclusions of some of the most widely accepted emotion experiments, including the conclusion that there is strong cross-cultural consistency in emotion perception. The evidence is broadly consistent with a constructionist framework for understanding the nature of emotions, a viewpoint that emphasizes that the human brain evolved [59] to wire itself to its physical and cultural context over the course of development [60]. This cultural learning affords diversity in experiences and behaviors, allowing for multiple models of successful social functioning that are tuned to a given context [61**,62**].
Highlights.
Supportive results for universality of emotion perception are method-bound.
Cultural variation is observed in the specific cues that are prioritized by perceivers and attributions (as mental states, actions, social motives) made about those cues.
Innovations in operationalizing culture promise to provide deeper insight into the sources of variation in emotion perception.
Acknowledgments
Preparation of this paper was supported by a NIMH F32 Fellowship (MH105052) awarded to M. G.
Footnotes
This approach has grown out of both situated-cognition [49] and dynamic social constructivist [50] approaches to culture, which vary in the assumption of whether cultural models reflect universal but situationally-constrained, versus culturally generated, meanings.
When universality is assumed, this modulation is proposed to occur via culturally set rules for what (and when) inborn expressions are appropriate to display, i.e., display rules [54], or via culturally acquired accents that modulate the form of innate facial actions [55] and vocalizations [56].
Declaration of interest
The author declares no conflict of interest.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Elfenbein HA, Ambady N. On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:203–235. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- **2.Crivelli C, Gendron M. Facial expressions and emotions in indigenous societies. In: Russell JA, Fernandez-Dols J-M, editors. The Science of Facial Expression. Oxford University Press; 2017. This chapter reviews the studies assessing cross-cultural emotion perception in small-scale societies, which provide the most stringent tests of the universality of emotion. This chapter focuses on the details of the empirical studies and how well the results stand up against the various standards that have been proposed for evidence of universality. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Nelson NL, Russell JA. Universality revisited. Emotion Review. 2013;5:8–15. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ekman P, Friesen WV. Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1971;17:124–129. doi: 10.1037/h0030377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ekman P, Sorenson ER, Friesen WV. Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science. 1969;164:86–88. doi: 10.1126/science.164.3875.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ekman P. Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In: Cole J, editor. Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971. Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press; 1972. pp. 207–282. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tracy JL, Robins RW. The nonverbal expression of pride: evidence for cross-cultural recognition. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2008;94:516. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bryant GA, Barrett HC. Vocal emotion recognition across disparate cultures. Journal of Cognition and Culture. 2008;8:135–148. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sauter DA, Eisner F, Ekman P, Scott SK. Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107:2408–2412. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908239106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cordaro DT, Keltner D, Tshering S, Wangchuk D, Flynn LM. The voice conveys emotion in ten globalized cultures and one remote village in Bhutan. Emotion. 2016;16:117–128. doi: 10.1037/emo0000100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *11.Crivelli C, Russell JA, Jarillo S, Fernández-Dols J-M. The fear gasping face as a threat display in a Melanesian society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016;113:12403–12407. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611622113. This empirical report provides evidence that the assumed universal association between a wide-eyed gasping expression and ‘fear’ is not universal. In Trobriand Islanders, the wide-eyed gasping face appears to be linked to the social motive to “threaten” someone and the emotion category of “anger”. These findings underscore cultural variation in the social motives inferred from facial actions. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Crivelli C, Jarillo S, Russell JA, Fernández-Dols J-M. Reading Emotions From Faces in Two Indigenous Societies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2016;145:830–843. doi: 10.1037/xge0000172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Crivelli C, Russell JA, Jarillo S, Fernández-Dols J-M. Recognizing Spontaneous Facial Expressions of Emotion in a Small-Scale Society of Papua New Guinea. Emotion. 2017;17:337–347. doi: 10.1037/emo0000236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gendron M, Roberson D, van der Vyver JM, Barrett LF. Cultural relativity in perceiving emotion from vocalizations. Psychological Science. 2014;25:911–920. doi: 10.1177/0956797613517239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *15.Gendron M, Roberson D, van der Vyver JM, Barrett LF. Perceptions of emotion from facial expressions are not culturally universal: evidence from a remote culture. Emotion. 2014;14:251–262. doi: 10.1037/a0036052. This empirical report provides evidence that Western-style poses of facial emotion are not universally perceived as the emotions sadness, fear, and disgust by a sampled of African pastoralists. Instead, these poses were grouped together based on the situated behaviors that the faces appeared to be engaged in (e.g., smelling something). These findings underscore cultural variation in the tendency to spontaneously infer mental states from facial actions. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Keltner D, Cordaro DT. Understanding Multimodal Emotional Expressions: Recent Advances in Basic Emotion Theory. In: Fernandez-Dols JM, Russell JA, editors. The science of facial expression. Oxford University Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Greenfield PM. Social change, cultural evolution, and human development. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2016;8:84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kayyal MH, Russell JA. Americans and Palestinians judge spontaneous facial expressions of emotion. Emotion. 2013;13:891. doi: 10.1037/a0033244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Naab PJ, Russell JA. Judgments of emotion from spontaneous facial expressions of New Guineans. Emotion. 2007;7:736–744. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.DiGirolamo MA, Russell JA. The Emotion Seen in a Face Can Be a Methodological Artifact: The Process of Elimination Hypothesis. Emotion. 2016 doi: 10.1037/emo0000247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Collaboration OS. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 2015;349:aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Barrett LF, Gendron M. The importance of context: Three corrections to Cordaro, Keltner, Tshering, Wangchuk, and Flynn (2016) 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gendron M, Barrett LF. Facing the Past: A history of the face in psychological research on emotion perception. In: Russell JA, Fernandez-Dols J-M, editors. The Science of Facial Expression. Oxford University Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Gendron M. Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2011;20:286–290. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Liu P, Rigoulot S, Pell MD. Culture modulates the brain response to human expressions of emotion: Electrophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia. 2015;67:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tanaka A, Koizumi A, Imai H, Hiramatsu S, Hiramoto E, de Gelder B. I feel your voice cultural differences in the multisensory perception of emotion. Psychological science. 2010;21:1259–1262. doi: 10.1177/0956797610380698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Liu P, Rigoulot S, Pell MD. Cultural differences in on-line sensitivity to emotional voices: comparing East and West. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2015;9:311. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Liu P, Rigoulot S, Pell MD. Cultural immersion alters emotion perception: Neurophysiological evidence from Chinese immigrants to Canada. Social Neuroscience. 2016:1–16. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1231713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Masuda T, Wang H, Ishii K, Ito K. Do surrounding figures’ emotions affect judgment of the target figure’s emotion? Comparing the eye-movement patterns of European Canadians, Asian Canadians, Asian international students, and Japanese. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2012;6:72. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Masuda T, Ellsworth PC, Mesquita B, Leu J, Tanida S, Van de Veerdonk E. Placing the face in context: cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2008;94:365. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *31.Ito K, Masuda T, Li LMW. Agency and facial emotion judgment in context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2013;39:763–776. doi: 10.1177/0146167213481387. This empirical report provides evidence that the European American tendency to ignore the facial expressions of a group (surrounding a target individual) reflects the prioritization of the agency of the target as an individual whose emotions are distinct from those around them. Critically, the context was taken into account by European Americans when it was an affectively laden scene, suggesting this does not reflect a general bias to ignore context. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological review. 1991;98:224–253. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wierzbicka A. Semantics, culture, and cognition: Universal human concepts in culture-specific configurations. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Choi E, Chentsova-Dutton Y, Parrott W. The Effectiveness of Somatization in Communicating Distress in Korean and American Cultural Contexts. Frontiers in psychology. 2016;7:383. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dzokoto V, Senft N, Kpobi L, Washington-Nortey P-M. Their Hands Have Lost Their Bones: Exploring Cultural Scripts in Two West African Affect Lexica. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2016;45:1473–1497. doi: 10.1007/s10936-016-9415-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- **36.Heyes CM, Frith CD. The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science. 2014;344:1243091. doi: 10.1126/science.1243091. In this review, the authors distinguish between explicit mental state inference and goal-directed action perception, suggesting that the former is a culturally learned skill, whereas the latter is an automatic, universal capacity that emerges in ontogeny. This review highlights relevant findings from across a range of disciplines to support their arguments. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Atzil S, Gendron M. Bio-behavioral synchrony promotes the development of conceptualized emotions. Current opinion in psychology. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.009. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wang Q, Fivush R. Mother–child conversations of emotionally salient events: exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European-American and Chinese families. Social Development. 2005;14:473–495. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Doan SN, Wang Q. Maternal discussions of mental states and behaviors: Relations to emotion situation knowledge in European American and immigrant Chinese children. Child development. 2010;81:1490–1503. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01487.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *40.Wood A, Rychlowska M, Niedenthal PM. Heterogeneity of long-history migration predicts emotion recognition accuracy. 2016 doi: 10.1037/emo0000137. This empirical report links cultural variation in historical heterogeneity and individualism-collectivism to the well-documented cross-cultural gap in emotion perception. Whereas historical heterogeneity accounts for whether posers close the cultural gap, the degree of individualism accounts for whether perceivers close the cultural gap. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Fridlund AJ. Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego: Academic Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Yik MSM, Russell JA. Interpretation of faces: A Cross-cultural study of a prediction from Fridlund’s theory. Cognition & Emotion. 1999;13:93–104. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Shuman V, Clark-Polner E, Meuleman B, Sander D, Scherer KR. Emotion perception from a componential perspective. Cognition and Emotion. 2015;9931:1–10. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1075964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Russell JA. Culture and the Categorization of Emotions. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110:426–450. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Duranti A. The anthropology of intentions. Cambridge University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hirschfeld LA. The myth of mentalizing and the primacy of folk sociology. Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us. 2013:101–106. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kozak MN, Marsh Aa, Wegner DM. What do I think you’re doing? Action identification and mind attribution. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2006;90:543–555. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Markus HR, Kitayama S. Cultures and Selves. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2010;5:420–430. doi: 10.1177/1745691610375557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Oyserman D. Culture Three Ways: Culture and Subcultures Within Countries. Annual Review of Psychology. 2017;68 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033617. annurev-psych-122414-033617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hong Y-y, Morris MW, Chiu C-y, Benet-Martinez V. Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American psychologist. 2000;55:709. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.7.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kafetsios K, Hess U. Are you looking at me? The influence of facial orientation and cultural focus salience on the perception of emotion expressions. Cogent Psychology. 2015;2:1005493. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Jack RE, Schyns PG. Toward a Social Psychophysics of Face Communication. Annual Review of Psychology. 2016:68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Rychlowska M, Miyamoto Y, Matsumoto D, Hess U, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Kamble S, Muluk H, Masuda T, Niedenthal PM. Heterogeneity of long-history migration explains cultural differences in reports of emotional expressivity and the functions of smiles. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112:E2429–E2436. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1413661112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Matsumoto D, Yoo SH, Fontaine J. Mapping expressive differences around the world the relationship between emotional display rules and individualism versus collectivism. Journal of cross-cultural psychology. 2008;39:55–74. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Elfenbein HA. Nonverbal dialects and accents in facial expressions of emotion. Emotion Review. 2013;5:90–96. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Laukka P, Neiberg D, Elfenbein HA. Evidence for cultural dialects in vocal emotion expression: Acoustic classification within and across five nations. Emotion. 2014;14:445. doi: 10.1037/a0036048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Kraus MW, Piff PK, Keltner D. Social Class as Culture: The convergence of resources and rank in social life. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2011;20:246–250. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kraus MW, Côté S, Keltner D. Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science. 2010 doi: 10.1177/0956797610387613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Finlay BL, Uchiyama R. The timing of brain and body maturation, early experience and the human social niche. In: Kaas JH, editor. Evolution of Nervous Systems. 2. Elsevier: Oxford University Press; in press. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Boyd R, Richerson PJ, Henrich J. The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108:10918–10925. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100290108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- **61.Mesquita B, Boiger M, De Leersnyder J. The cultural construction of emotions. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2016;8:31–36. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.015. This review provides a complementary discussion of how emotional experiences are constructed by culture. Specifically, evidence that emotional experiences are cultivated to “fit” within a culture, and the evidence that this promotes well-being, is reviewed. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- **62.Barrett LF. How emotions are made. New York, NY: Houghton-Mifflin-Harcourt; 2017. This book provides the most comprehensive and broadly accessible account of a constructionist approach to emotions to-date. A multi-level theoretical approach is articulated by integrating neuroscience, developmental, cultural, and comparative perspectives. The book articulates the larger insight that human experiences, and emotions in particular, come in many varieties because the brain evolved to wire itself to its context. [Google Scholar]