Abstract
Minimally invasive, automated cot-side tools for monitoring early neurological development can be used to guide individual treatment and benchmark novel interventional studies. We develop an automated estimate of the EEG maturational age (EMA) for application to serial recordings in preterm infants. The EMA estimate was based on a combination of 23 computational features estimated from both the full EEG recording and a period of low EEG activity (46 features in total). The combination function (support vector regression) was trained using 101 serial EEG recordings from 39 preterm infants with a gestational age less than 28 weeks and normal neurodevelopmental outcome at 12 months of age. EEG recordings were performed from 24 to 38 weeks post-menstrual age (PMA). The correlation between the EMA and the clinically determined PMA at the time of EEG recording was 0.936 (95%CI: 0.932–0.976; n = 39). All infants had an increase in EMA between the first and last EEG recording and 57/62 (92%) of repeated measures within an infant had an increasing EMA with PMA of EEG recording. The EMA is a surrogate measure of age that can accurately determine brain maturation in preterm infants.
Introduction
Premature birth is a significant health problem that affects every tenth live birth and results in half of all admissions to the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU)1,2. Mortality and morbidity is increased in neonates born prematurely with neurological deficits persisting over the longer term3,4. These deficits result in a 10 to 25-fold increase in annual healthcare costs5. The premature brain is highly susceptible to disruption as it undergoes large scale, activity-dependent neuronal wiring during the last trimester6,7. Ensuring optimal brain development through dedicated neuro-critical care, therefore, requires effective monitoring of functional brain maturation8. The EEG is increasingly being used in this role in the NICU9. The major difficulty with the implementation of EEG monitoring is achieving interpretation by the human expert for long periods of time, on demand. This can be effectively overcome using automated analyses.
An important aspect in interpreting the EEG of preterm infants is the use of post-menstrual age (PMA) as a contextual benchmark10. An EEG derived measurement of PMA can be used as an objective summary measure of the EEG, used to aid diagnosis and prognosis or incorporated into alternate automated EEG analyses11–14. It can also provide support to maturational observations from the visual interpretation of the preterm EEG and be used as benchmark when translating the findings of animal models15.
The aim of this study was to apply a measure of EEG maturational age (EMA) to the clinical scenario of tracking functional brain maturation of early preterm infants (<28 weeks gestational age) over several weeks. The cohort of preterm infants used in this study had a normal neurodevelopmental outcome and a wide range of PMA at the time of EEG recording (24 to 38 weeks PMA). The EMA was compared to the clinically determined age (PMA) at the time of EEG recording and key practical aspects of its implementation were assessed in the context of wide scale brain monitoring and as an early outcome proxy for novel interventional studies.
Results
A total of 1080, 1 h epochs were initially extracted from 43 infants. After the application of artefact detection, 567, 1 h epochs from 101 recordings from 39 infants remained.
The EMA based on the full feature set (46 features) was strongly correlated with the PMA at the time of EEG recording. Linear mixed effects modelling resulted in a correlation of 0.936 (95%CI: 0.932–0.976, n = 39). Repeated EEG measures (longitudinal recordings) were available in 34 infants. We found an increasing EMA in all 34 infants (100%) between the first and last EEG recording. In 57/62 recordings (92%) there was an increasing EMA with PMA (Fig. 1A). Five infants had an instance of decreasing EMA with increasing PMA (deviant growth). All instances of deviant growth were due to deviations of the EMA in a single EEG recording by more than two weeks from the age at the time of EEG recording (Fig. 1B).
Several individual features showed a significant correlation with the PMA. The highest correlating features (n = 39) were the 95% percentile of the inter-SAT interval (Low SAT; r = 0.774), RMS of the inter-SAT interval (Low SAT; r = 0.762), 95% percentile of the rEEG (Full; r = 0.751), 95% percentile of the envelope (Full; r = 0.670), total spectral power (Full; r = 0.634), and the number of SATs per hour (Full; r = 0.599). Inter-SAT features were also highly correlated with PMA when estimated on the full recording and amplitude features were also highly correlated with PMA during the low SAT period. All summary measures of relative spectral power and SAT duration had low correlation with PMA (r < 0.5).
The performance of the EMA developed with the incorporation of feature selection is summarised in Table 1. EMA measures outperformed a worst-case estimate (the mean PMA of EEG recording in the dataset). Reducing the number of computational features did not significantly increase the MSE (no FS vs FS; p = 0.912, n = 101). After feature selection, the resultant EMA used a median number of 17 features (IQR: 15–19, n = 39). The five most commonly selected features were the median rEEG, the 5th percentile of the rEEG, 5th percentile of SAT duration, SATs per hour, and the activation synchrony index; these features did not have the highest individual correlations with PMA (see Fig. 2).
Table 1.
Mean PMA | SVR | SVR with FS | |
---|---|---|---|
MSE (days2) | 438.0 | 97.3* | 108.2* |
r | 0 | 0.936 | 0.940 |
SD (days) | 20.94 | 9.86 | 10.30 |
SE (%) | 9.72 | 4.51 | 4.75 |
Bias (days) | 0 | −0.45 | −1.33 |
% EMA (1 week) | 20.8 | 50.5 | 43.6 |
% EMA (2 weeks) | 39.6 | 82.2 | 85.2 |
Mean PMA is the worst case/best guess EMA estimate. *Is a significant reduction in the MSE compared to the mean PMA (p < 0.001), SVR is support vector regression, FS is feature selection, PMA is post-menstrual age, EMA is EEG maturational age, SD is the standard deviation and SE is the percentage error and %EMA denotes the percentage of EEG recordings where the EMA was within 1 week and 2 weeks of the PMA and MSE is the mean square error estimated on a per recording basis (n = 101). The adjusted correlation coefficient, r, is estimated from a linear mixed model: the slope of EMA over time was a fixed effect and the intercept was a random effect per infant (n = 39).
Reducing the number of electrodes used to form the bi-polar EEG montage did not significantly affect the accuracy of the EMA (Table 2). A 4-channel EEG montage with frontal polar, temporal and occipital derivations had a MSE that was not significantly different from an 8-channel EEG montage (p = 0.815, n = 101). Two-channel EEG montages containing fronto-temporal or temporo-occipital derivations also had a MSE that was not significantly different from the 8-channel montage (p = 0.427 and p = 0.575, respectively, n = 101).
Table 2.
4 channel montage | MSE | r | p-value | 2 channel montage | MSE | r | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fp-T, T-O | 104.2 | 0.934 | 0.815 | Fp-T | 111.5 | 0.928 | 0.427 |
Fp-C, C-O | 112.3 | 0.898 | 0.079 | T-O | 119.7 | 0.908 | 0.575 |
Fp-C | 125.6 | 0.896 | 0.025 | ||||
C-O | 162.6 | 0.829 | 0.016 | ||||
C-T | 172.8 | 0.789 | 0.013 |
The EMA used all features and applied to recordings that passed strict artefact detection. MSE is the means square error, r is the adjusted correlation coefficient (linear mixed model), electrode locations are Fp – frontal polar, C – central, T – temporal, O – occipital, and montages are symmetric across hemispheres. The p-value is estimated using a Wilcoxon signed rank test for paired data, n = 101; the null hypothesis tested is that the MSE was not different from that estimated using an 8-channel EEG montage.
Discussion
We developed an automated estimator of functional cortical maturation using cot-side EEG recordings from infants with normal neurodevelopmental outcome at 12 months of age. We showed that the EMA correlates with the clinical assessment of PMA of EEG recording over a wide range of ages, all infants had an increasing EMA between their first and last EEG recording, 92% of EEG recordings had an increasing EMA with increasing PMA and 82% of EMA measurements were within 2 weeks of the PMA. Streamlining the EMA measurement by reducing the number of computational features or the number of EEG channels did not significantly affect its performance.
The performance of the EMA is substantial given that there is an inherent error of at least plus or minus one week in the definition of PMA due to variability between conception and the last menstrual period16. The remaining variation between the EMA and PMA can be attributed to a combination of errors in training the support vector regression (SVR) and variation in natural biological growth.
Summary measures of EEG amplitude and the temporal characteristics of SATs had the highest individual correlation with PMA. These results align with previous findings that the rEEG and temporal information are correlated with maturation in the EEG17,18. Prior studies typically focus on individual features that maximise the correlation with PMA, however, we have shown that when combining features to improve maturational estimates the highest correlating features are not necessarily chosen by the feature selection algorithm. The most notable difference was in the relative beta power (RBP) which has been shown to correlate with PMA in infants with a gestational age less than 32 weeks but was never selected (see Fig. 2; feature 23). This may be due to the dominance of other features representing similar aspects of the EEG, overly age specific correlations that do not necessarily hold over a wider range of PMA or differences in inter-uterine versus extra-uterine maturation19. The most commonly selected features were the median and 5th percentile of the rEEG, not the 95th percentile, suggesting that the rEEG width defined by O’Reilly et al.18 may be improved by incorporating the 50th rather than the 90th percentile. It is interesting that amplitude was the most important maturational feature of the EEG; a measurement that may be considered as underappreciated as a maturational feature in the neonatal EEG literature, although recent work on the analysis of the amplitude integrated EEG (aEEG) supports the usefulness of amplitude for assessing maturation20–22. While two rEEG features provide a considerable amount of information to the EMA, they require support from an additional 15 features that measure other aspects of the EEG such as the temporal behaviour of SATs, spectral power and hemispheric synchrony within the EEG.
There are two factors which may challenge the wide scale implementation of EMA monitoring. The first factor is variation in EEG output across NICUs due to differences that may arise from the combination of EEG machine, electrode type, impedance and electrode positioning. Our work shows that an EMA is accurate with the 2-channel bipolar EEG montage that is routinely applied in aEEG monitoring the NICU (the raw EEG signal is readily accessible for automated analysis in aEEG monitors)23. The electrode placement must be, however, carefully selected to optimize the susceptibility to artefacts and the information value relating to maturation24,25. The second is potential clinical confounders such as neurological condition, non-pharmacological treatments and medication. There is evidence to suggest that medications, particularly sedatives, affect the EEG and should be considered during interpretation26. These considerations are also highly relevant when incorporating a variety of neonatal EEG analysis algorithms into neuro-critical care14,27,28.
Longitudinal monitoring is a powerful method for assessing human health. In infants, existing methods use measures such as weight, head circumference and clinical assessment of motor function29,30. In recent years, analyses have progressed to include measures based functional MRI and DNA methylation in cord blood31,32. While it is always difficult to compare studies, the correlation between EMA and the PMA was higher than these methods. Notably, implementation of the EMA in preterm infants has several comparative advantages for wider scale clinical practice. Firstly, it is minimally disruptive and relies on cot-side data that routinely accumulates in the NICU. Secondly, it is a direct measure of the functional development in the preterm brain, which is the target organ in most attempts to improve neonatal care. It also supplements the paradigm of assessing preterm EEG for acute and chronic abnormalities as an additional measurement over a longer time scale12,33.
Automated analysis of the EEG can generate a measure of maturation (EMA) that is highly correlated with PMA. The EMA can accurately, and continuously, track the maturation of cortical function in preterm infants over their entire stay in the NICU. The practical significance of this development will ultimately be measured in terms of clinical usefulness. This will be determined in prospective clinical trials which evaluate the added value of such a measure in the individualized neurological care of preterm infants. The EMA also offers an unprecedented opportunity to measure the effects of various treatments and therapies for preterm infants. The EMA as an early outcome measure holds promise for novel interventional studies by expediting their development cycle from several years of follow-up to near real-time assessment.
Methods
The overall organisation of this study is shown in Fig. 3. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Dataset
EEG recordings were collected as a part of a study that recruited infants in the NICU at the Medical University of Vienna, Austria. Multi-channel recordings were acquired at 256 Hz using a Brain Quick/ICU EEG (MicroMed, Treviso, Italy) with a referential (Cz) montage from 9 scalp electrodes located at Fp1, Fp2, C3, C4, T3, T4, O1, and O2. The analysis was performed on a standard bipolar montage: Fp1-C3, C3-O1, Fp1-T3, T3-O1, Fp2-C4, C4-O2, Fp2-T4, T4-O2. Each infant had a cranial ultrasound, once a week, until 34 weeks PMA. Data collection was approved by the local ethics committee (Medical University Vienna, Austria) and the protocol was registered (EK Nr 67/2008). Informed parental consent was obtained for all infants included in the study. The PMA of each infant was defined by the last menstrual period (LMP) of the mother and adjusted using ultrasound measurements in the first trimester if there was significant deviation between LMP and ultrasound analysis.
Study cohort
The initial study recruited all infants born at less than 28 weeks gestational age who were admitted to the NICU using the following inclusion criteria: the patient was clinically stable, did not receive any brain-acting drugs, and did not have a known neurological morbidity (congenital/chromosomal anomalies and severe perinatal asphyxia). For the inclusion into our present study on EMA validation, we also required that the patient had a normal neurodevelopmental outcome at 12 months of term-corrected age as measured by a Bailey’s II assessment (normal defined as a mental and physical developmental index greater than 85). Out of an initial cohort of 241 EEG recordings from 67 infants, 43 infants (152 EEG recordings) had normal neurodevelopmental outcome, and 16 infants were lost to follow up. Additional demographics of the cohort and the distribution of PMA at the time of EEG recordings are shown in Fig. 3B.
Calculation of EMA
The EEG signal was initially pre-processed with a band pass filter (low cut-off frequency of 0.5 Hz and a high cut-off frequency of 32 Hz) and then re-sampled to 64 Hz. Each EEG recording was segmented into 1 h long epochs with a 75% (45 minute) overlap. One hour epochs were chosen based on the reported duration of sleep states in preterm infants with the aim of capturing a significant proportion of a full sleep cycle in an epoch34. The EEG was then automatically annotated for the presence of spontaneous activity transients (SATs), periods of artefact, and periods of low SAT%35. Low SAT% is associated with quiet sleep36. The SAT% was defined as the accumulated duration of SAT detections within a 5-minute window (4-minute overlap) expressed as a percentage34. The low SAT% period was defined using a threshold (the lower quartile range of the SAT% over time). The estimate of the lower quartile range incorporated an intermediate stage that eliminated SAT% periods with a duration less than 5 minutes.
Several features of amplitude, spatial organization and temporal organization were calculated from the EEG using these annotations. The computational features are listed in Fig. 3A (see also O’Toole et al., 2016)37. Each feature was estimated per channel and the median value across channels was used. The feature was further summarised using the median across either the entire 1 h EEG epoch or the period of low SAT%, resulting in a total set of 46 features. These features were then combined using a trained SVR to calculate the EMA.
The EEG data was not preselected with any visual criteria, rather a simple automated artefact detection (AD) method was employed to exclude epochs with excessively low or high amplitude. Applying the EEG in a challenging environment such as the NICU results in recordings that are commonly contaminated by a large variety of artefacts. Here, we only consider two types of artefact that are associated with outlying amplitude behaviour10. The first type is excessive amplitude artefacts from movements or poor electrode contact. Artefacts were defined using the segmentation provided by a SAT detector35. A SAT segment was defined as artefactual if the EEG voltage was greater than 500μV at any time during the SAT. The second type of artefact is excessively low amplitude resulting from electrode shorting (electrodes placed too closely together). These detections affected the entire EEG recording and were performed on a channel by channel basis. Decisions were based on an estimate of the amplitude envelope (magnitude of the analytic associate of the EEG)37. Bipolar channels were excluded if the median envelope over time was less than 50% of the median envelope over time and channels. Analysis of referential channels was also used as a basis to exclude bipolar channels derived from the referential electrode. Channels were excluded if the median envelope over time was less than 50% of the median envelope over time and all referential channels and if the 95% percentile of the envelope over the 1 h epoch was less than 25μV. EEG recordings were excluded if any channel had excessively low amplitude or more than 20% of the epoch was contaminated by high amplitude artefact.
Training and Testing of the EMA
The EMA was trained and tested within a leave-one-infant-out cross-validation. The parameters of the SVR (regularization, ε-insensitive loss and sigma/gamma in the radial basis function) were optimised in an internal 3-fold cross-validation38. A feature selection procedure was also performed (backward selection; a complete selection was performed and the number of features selected corresponded to the minimum MSE)39. Features were selected within each training iteration using a secondary 10-fold cross-validation and SVR optimisation was incorporated into this training step as a tertiary loop. At each stage of training, the target for optimisation was the minimisation of the MSE between the EMA and the PMA at the time of the EEG recording. SVR was implemented using a MATLAB port (9.0, Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) of the LIBSVM toolbox38.
The ability of the EMA to track development using serial EEG recordings was evaluated using a linear mixed model for repeated measures. The rate of change of EMA over time was assumed to be a fixed effect and the intercept or offset was assumed to be a random effect due to inter-subject variability in maturation and date of conception. The correlation coefficient and the percentage of repeated measures with an increasing EMA with PMA were used as the summary measure of performance. The error between EMA and PMA were also estimated using a range of measures: bias, mean squared error, and standard deviation (expressed as days and percentage) between the EMA and PMA37. The association between individual EEG features and PMA was also evaluated using SVR and leave-one-out cross-validation. Values were estimated on the mean value of 1 h epochs extracted from the same EEG recording.
The effect of reducing the number of recording electrodes in the EEG montage was also assessed. This may be needed ad hoc as streamlined EEG recording with a lower (typically four) electrode number is part of current NICU practice23.
Differences between EMA estimators were tested using a Mann-Whitney Signed Rank test for paired data where a sample was an EEG recording. A p-value less than 0.05 was deemed significant and all tests were two-sided.
Data Availability
The raw EEG data analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions. All data generated from the raw EEG data (i.e. the features used) are included in the Supplementary Information files.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
NJS acknowledges the support of the EU Horizon 2020 programme (H2020-MCSA-IF-656131). NK was supported by Suomen Kulttuurirahasto. JMO was supported by the Irish Research Council (GOIPD/2014/396) and Science Foundation Ireland (15/SIRG/3580). SV was supported by the Academy of Finland (253130), and the Sigrid Juselius Foundation. KKS was supported by the Fonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung (FWF KLI 237).
Author Contributions
N.J.S., S.V., K.K.-S. devised and designed the study. K.K.-S., L.O., T.W. devised the data collection protocol, inclusion/exclusion criteria, prepared the dataset and performed neurodevelopmental follow-up. N.J.S., N.K. and J.O.’T. provided algorithms for EEG analysis and classification. N.J.S. and S.V. wrote the manuscript. All authors revised and edited the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-017-13537-3.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Blencowe H, et al. National, regional, and worldwide estimates of preterm birth rates in the year 2010 with time trends since 1990 for selected countries: a systematic analysis and implications. The Lancet. 2012;379:2162–72. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60820-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Harrison W, Goodman D. Epidemiologic trends in neonatal intensive care, 2007-2012. JAMA Pediatrics. 2015;169:855–62. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Marlow N, Wolke D, Bracewell MA, Samara M. Neurologic and developmental disability at six years of age after extremely preterm birth. New Eng J Med. 2005;352:9–19. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Johnson S, Marlow N. Early and long-term outcome of infants born extremely preterm. Arch Dis Child. 2017;102:97–102. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2015-309581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kancherla V, Amendah DD, Grosse SD, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Braun K. Medical expenditures attributable to cerebral palsy and intellectual disability among Medicaid-enrolled children. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33:832–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kostović I, Judas M. The development of the subplate and thalamocortical connections in the human foetal brain. Acta Paediatr. 2010;99:1119–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01811.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tau GZ, Peterson BS. Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:147–68. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bonifacio SL, Glass HC, Peloquin S, Ferriero DM. A new neurological focus in neonatal intensive care. Nature Rev Neurol. 2011;7:485–94. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Glass HC, Rowitch DH. The role of the neurointensive care nursery for neonatal encephalopathy. Clin. Perinatol. 2016;43:547–557. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2016.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hrachovy, R. A. & Mizrahi, E. M. Atlas of Neonatal Electroencephalography. 4th edition. Demos Medical Publishing, New York: USA. (2015).
- 11.Scher MS. Neurophysiological assessment of brain function and maturation II. A measure of brain dysmaturity in healthy preterm neonates. Pediatr. Neurol. 1997;16:287–95. doi: 10.1016/S0887-8994(96)00009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Watanabe K, Hayakawa F, Okumura A. Neonatal EEG: a powerful tool in the assessment of brain damage in preterm infants. Brain Dev. 1999;21:361–72. doi: 10.1016/S0387-7604(99)00034-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Holthausen K, Breidbach O, Scheidt B, Frenzel J. Brain dysmaturity index for automatic detection of high risk infants. Pediatr Neurol. 2000;22:187–91. doi: 10.1016/S0887-8994(99)00154-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Koolen N, et al. Automated classification of neonatal sleep states using EEG. Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;128:1100–1108. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Parmelee AH, Jr., et al. Maturation of EEG activity during sleep in premature infants. Electroen Clin Neurol. 1968;24:319–29. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(68)90193-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Geirsson RT. Ultrasound instead of last menstrual period as the basis of gestational age assignment. Ultrasound in Obst Gyn. 1991;1:212–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-0705.1991.01030212.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Aminoff, M. J. Aminoff’s Electrodiagnosis in Clinical Neurology. 6th ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier Saunders, (2012).
- 18.O’Reilly D, Navakatikyan MA, Filip M, Greene D, Van Marter LJ. Peak-to-peak amplitude in neonatal brain monitoring of premature infants. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123:2139–53. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.02.087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Nunes ML, Khan RL, Filho IG, Booij L, da Costa JC. Maturational changes of neonatal electroencephalogram: A comparison between intra uterine and extra uterine development. Clin Neurophysiol. 2014;125:1121–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.10.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.André M, et al. Electroencephalography in premature and full-term infants. Developmental features and glossary. Neurophysiol Clin. 2010;40:59–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2010.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Vesoulis ZA, et al. Normative amplitude integrated EEG (aEEG) measures in preterm infants. J Perinatol. 2015;35:428–433. doi: 10.1038/jp.2014.225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kato T, et al. Evaluation of brain maturation in pre-term infants using conventional and amplitude-integrated electroencephalograms. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122:1967–1972. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.12.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.van Rooij LG, de Vries LS, van Huffelen AC, Toet MC. Additional value of two-channel amplitude integrated EEG recording in full-term infants with unilateral brain injury. Arch Dis Child-Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2010;95:F160–8. doi: 10.1136/adc.2008.156711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Durston S, et al. Anatomical MRI of the developing human brain: what have we learned? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2001;40:1012–1020. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200109000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schumacher EM, Stiris TA, Larsson PG. Effective connectivity in long-term EEG monitoring in preterm infants. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126:2261–2268. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bell AH, Greisen G, Pryds O. Comparison of the effects of phenobarbitone and morphine administration on EEG activity in preterm babies. Acta Paediatrica. 1993;82:35–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1993.tb12511.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stevenson NJ, et al. An automated system for grading EEG abnormalities in term neonates with hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy. Ann Biomed Eng. 2013;41:775–785. doi: 10.1007/s10439-012-0710-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stevenson NJ, O’Toole JM, Rankine LJ, Boylan GB, Boashash B. A nonparametric feature for neonatal EEG seizure detection based on a representation of pseudo–periodicity. Med Eng Phy. 2013;34:437–446. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2011.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dubowitz LM, Dubowitz V, Goldberg C. Clinical assessment of gestational age in the newborn infant. J Pediatr. 1970;77:1–10. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(70)80038-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ehrenkranz RA, et al. Growth in the neonatal intensive care unit influences neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatrics. 2006;117:1253–61. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Knight, A. K. et al. An epigenetic clock for gestational age at birth based on blood methylation data. Genome Biology. 17, 206, 11 pages (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 32.Smyser CD, et al. Prediction of brain maturity in infants using machine-learning algorithms. NeuroImage. 2016;136:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hayashi-Kurahashi N, et al. EEG for predicting early neurodevelopment in preterm infants: an observational cohort study. Pediatrics. 2012;130:e891–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Stevenson NJ, Palmu K, Wikström S, Hellström-Westas L, Vanhatalo S. Measuring brain activity cycling (BAC) in long term EEG monitoring of preterm babies. Physiol Meas. 2014;35:1493–508. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/35/7/1493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Palmu K, et al. Optimization of an NLEO based algorithm for automated detection of spontaneous activity transients in early preterm EEG. Physiol Meas. 2010;31:N85–93. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/31/11/N02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Palmu K, Kirjavainen T, Stjerna S, Salokivi T, Vanhatalo S. Sleep wake cycling in early preterm infants: Comparison of polysomnographic recordings with a novel EEG-based index. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124:1807–14. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.O’Toole JM, Boylan GB, Vanhatalo S, Stevenson NJ. Estimating functional brain maturity in very and extremely preterm neonates using automated analysis of the electroencephalogram. Clin Neurophysiol. 2016;127:2910–2918. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Chang, C. C. & Lin, C. J. LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. ACM T Intel Syst Tec. 2, 1–27, Article 27 (2011).
- 39.Guyon I, Elisseeff A. An introduction to variable and feature selection. J Mach Learn Res. 2003;3:1157–82. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The raw EEG data analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions. All data generated from the raw EEG data (i.e. the features used) are included in the Supplementary Information files.