Abstract
When we hear a new voice we automatically form a "first impression" of the voice owner's personality; a single word is sufficient to yield ratings highly consistent across listeners. Past studies have shown correlations between personality ratings and acoustical parameters of voice, suggesting a potential acoustical basis for voice personality impressions, but its nature and extent remain unclear. Here we used data-driven voice computational modelling to investigate the link between acoustics and perceived trustworthiness in the single word "hello". Two prototypical voice stimuli were generated based on the acoustical features of voices rated low or high in perceived trustworthiness, respectively, as well as a continuum of stimuli inter- and extrapolated between these two prototypes. Five hundred listeners provided trustworthiness ratings on the stimuli via an online interface. We observed an extremely tight relationship between trustworthiness ratings and position along the trustworthiness continuum (r = 0.99). Not only were trustworthiness ratings higher for the high- than the low-prototypes, but the difference could be modulated quasi-linearly by reducing or exaggerating the acoustical difference between the prototypes, resulting in a strong caricaturing effect. The f0 trajectory, or intonation, appeared a parameter of particular relevance: hellos rated high in trustworthiness were characterized by a high starting f0 then a marked decrease at mid-utterance to finish on a strong rise. These results demonstrate a strong acoustical basis for voice personality impressions, opening the door to multiple potential applications.
Introduction
When we see a new face or hear a new voice, we automatically form a first impression of the person’s personality, inferring traits such as trustworthiness, competence, attractiveness, etc. [1–5]. These personality impressions may not be accurate but they are robust, showing high inter-rater agreement [1, 3, 4, 6]; they also form very rapidly as sub-second exposure to face or voice is sufficient to yield consistent judgments [4, 7].
For both faces and voices, first impressions are well summarized by a 2-dimensional « social space » with axes mapping onto perceived trustworthiness and dominance, respectively [4, 6]. These two dimensions are thought to reflect evolutionary ancient mechanisms, developed under selective pressures of living in large social groups, to rapidly assess the dispositions (trustworthiness) and ability to act on these dispositions (dominance) of an unfamiliar conspecific individual [8, 9].
For faces, data-driven computational modelling has demonstrated a very tight relationship between perceived personality impressions and specific facial features [1, 10]. The generation of model faces at different positions of the “social face space” shows that the main dimensions of trustworthiness and dominance map onto very different sets of facial features, such as jaw shape vs. eyebrow shape.
For voices, significant correlations between personality ratings and acoustical parameters such as average fundamental frequency (f0) have been observed [2, 4–6, 11, 12] suggesting an acoustical basis for voice personality judgments by which principled changes in voice acoustics could potentially map onto desired changes in perceived personality. But until now the correlational approach has fallen short of precisely characterizing this acoustical basis. How exactly should one say “hello” to be perceived as e.g., trustworthy by new listeners?
Here we used voice computational modelling to investigate more closely the acoustical basis for perceived voice personality. We focused on perceived trustworthiness because due to its evolutionary link to cooperation and survival, trust is a central aspect of human communication, the foundation for functioning relationships, interpersonal interaction, effective collaboration [13] as well as the cooperation and mutual adaptation needed to surmount problems [14, 15]. Ratings along that trait were found in a previous study to correlate most strongly with the first principal component explaining more than half the variance in overall voice personality ratings [4], indicating the foremost role of perceived trustworthiness in overall voice personality impressions.
We first asked whether voices modeled as the acoustical average of several low- and high-trustworthy voices would also be rated with low and high perceived trustworthiness—potential evidence of a mapping between acoustical and social voice spaces. We further explored the possible parametric nature of the acoustics-personality link by collecting trustworthiness ratings for voice stimuli generated in voice space along the direction defined by the low- and high-trustworthiness prototypes, including caricatures of one relative to the other.
Thirty-two male voices saying the word “hello” were ranked by their average trustworthiness ratings collected as part of a previous study [4] (Fig 1A). A low- and a high-trustworthiness prototypical model voice stimuli were generated via high-quality analysis/resynthesis [16] based on the average acoustical characteristics of the eight voices (25%) rated lowest and highest on perceived trustworthiness, respectively (Fig 1B; S1 Fig; cf. Methods). This resulted in two natural-sounding “hellos” (cf. audio files in Supporting Information) with perceptually different voice qualities and which we hypothesized would yield different trustworthiness ratings.
Next we generated via morphing a 9-stimulus “Trustworthiness continuum” in acoustically equal steps that contained the low- and high-trustworthiness prototypes at intermediate positions (stimuli S3 and S7) and that inter- and extrapolated their acoustical difference resulting at the extreme in low- and high-trustworthiness caricatures (stimuli S1 and S9; Fig 1C). To provide a set of control stimuli generated similarly but unrelated to trustworthiness we generated a second 9-stimulus “Control continuum” based on a random selection of a set of 32 female voices (S2 Fig; audio files in Supporting Information).
Five hundred listeners (146 males, 354 females; aged 19–65 years old, median age = 24) each provided 18 trustworthiness ratings using a visual analogue scale via an online interface, 9 for the morphed voice stimuli of the Trustworthiness continuum and 9 for those of the control continuum (stimuli presented in random orders in counterbalanced blocks; cf. Methods). Ratings were z-scored within participant prior to analysis to account for inter-individual variation in rating scale use and because we were primarily interested in rating variations within the morphed continua rather than in their absolute values.
Results and discussion
We observed a compelling relationship between average trustworthiness rating z-scores and position along the Trustworthiness, but not the Control, continuum (Fig 2). Ratings for the low- and high- trustworthiness prototypes (stimuli S3 and S7) differed highly significantly with lower average ratings for S3 than S7 (S3: z = -0.39; S7: z = 0.40; paired t-test: t(499) = 14.2, p = 1.38e-38) while no such difference was apparent for control stimuli (S3: z = 0.03; S7:z = -0.09; t(499) = 2.08, p = 0.038 n.s. after Bonferroni correction).
We also observed a strong caricaturing effect, as exaggerating acoustical differences between S3 and S7 yielded higher average trustworthiness ratings for S9 than S7 (S7: z = 0.40; S9; z = 0.80; t(499) = 7.40, p = 5.93e-13), and lower average trustworthiness ratings for S1 than for S3 (S1: z = -0.67; S3: z = -0.38; t(499) = 6.06; p = 2.62e-9). In fact, the average ratings followed an extremely tight relation with acoustical differences over the whole range of the continuum generated resulting in a correlation of r = 0.99 (p = 5.03e-8) between position along the acoustical continuum and average ratings. Again no such effect was observed for control stimuli on either end of the continuum (S1: z = 0; S3: z = 0.03; t(499) = -0.49, p = 0.62 n.s.; S7: z = -0.09; S9: z = -0.19; t(499) = 1.63, p = 0.10 n.s.). Importantly these effects were highly similar for male and female listeners (S3 Fig) suggesting the involvement of gender-independent mechanisms.
These results demonstrate a strong acoustical basis for perceived voice personality—an extremely tight mapping between acoustical and social voice spaces. Not only does averaging voices from different individuals with low or high ratings trustworthiness ratings results in the predicted rating differences, but these personality impressions can be parametrically modulated, with unsuspected accuracy, by acoustical changes and considerably increased (or decreased) by caricaturing.
Furthermore the results allow listening to the prototypes and caricatures, permitting direct access to the listeners’ underlying representations of voice trustworthiness in a way that correlations between traits and acoustical measures could not allow. The comparison of the prototypes and caricatures’ acoustical measures not only gives insight on which acoustical parameters are relevant, it directly shows how to modulate these parameters in order to achieve the desired increase in perceived trustworthiness.
Thus, past studies have hinted at a relation between f0 variation in voice and perceived personality traits such as attractiveness [5, 11, 17] or trustworthiness [4] but they could not determine the exact f0 contour that would drive this percept. What intonation should one use when saying “hello” to be perceived as trustworthy? Fig 3 provides important elements of answer to that question by illustrating f0 contour for the 9 stimuli of the Trustworthiness continuum. F0 at mid-duration is roughly similar for all stimuli but a clear difference is apparent for starting and ending f0: while the low-trustworthiness stimuli started with a relatively low f0 remaining flat or slightly increasing over the course of the syllables, the high-trustworthiness stimuli started with a much higher f0 at first syllable then decreased markedly to reach the average f0 at mid utterance and finish on a strongly rising intonation (cf. High trustworthiness caricature, Fig 3, audio file in Supporting Information). While f0 contour is but one of the many only acoustical parameters varying between stimuli (S1 Table) this particular intonation likely plays a central role on yielding the observed change in perceived personality impressions. Further work should weigh the respective importance of the different parameters by selectively including them or not in the prototypical models, for instance by generating an acoustic continuum that only manipulates f0 between the different stimuli, all other parameters being kept constant.
The results could potentially generalize to other personality traits, providing unprecedented insight into the voice acoustics that drive the perception of different traits and opening numerous possibilities for research. They also open the door to practical applications where desired personality impressions need to be achieved. Indeed, there are many situations in life where voice is the only cue available to form a new person’s first impression and where that first impression strongly influences future interactions. For example, telephone interviewers face the task of establishing trust a creating a positive impression with only vocal cues. Since the great majority of refusals occur within the opening phrases [18], a positive attitude towards both the subject and interviewer could be induced early on via acoustical modulation, resulting in increased response rates. Furthermore, speech synthesis and computer-generated voices foe e.g. social robots could be addressed as a promising area for applications in the customer service sector.
Some limitations of the study should be mentioned: They were only obtained on the word “hello”, which is justified as this word is often associated with first encounters, yet it is unclear how they would generalize other words or longer utterances. Moreover, the 8-voices averages are probably but a crude approximation of the listeners’ internal representations of trustworthy and untrustworthy voices; results could likely be further enhanced by using high-level reverse correlation for a more precise estimation of the listeners’ internal templates [19, 20]. Our choice of female voices for the control condition and male voice for the experimental condition was entirely arbitrary; it will be important in future studies to examine the potential role of factors such as gender, but also age, ethnicity, accent, verbal content, etc. on the acoustical basis for voice personality impressions.
Methods
Participants
500 participants (146 males, 354 females; aged 19–65 years old, median age = 24) took part in the voice rating experiment. Participants were recruited via email and social networking sites and directed to the web address of the experiment. Experimental procedures were approved by the University of Glasgow ethics committee and the study was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants gave informed consent prior to the experiment, via reading a series of statements regarding freedom to withdraw, anonymity of responses and secured storage of data. As the experiment was carried out online, they confirmed that they had read and agreed to these statements by registering on the website. Participants were not permitted to take part without providing consent.
Stimuli
Voice stimuli were modelled and generated using STRAIGHT [16] in Matlab (Mathworks, Inc., Natick, USA). STRAIGHT performs an instantaneous pitch-adaptive spectral smoothing in each stimulus for separation of contributions to the voice signal arising from the glottal source vs. supra-laryngeal filtering. A natural voice stimulus is decomposed by STRAIGHT into five parameters: f0, frequency, time, spectro-temporal density and aperiodicity that can be manipulated and combined across stimuli independently of one another, then synthesized into a novel voice stimulus. Time-frequency landmarks to be put in correspondence across voices during morphing were manually identified in each stimulus and corresponded to the frequencies of the first 3 formants at temporal landmarks corresponding to onset and offset of phonation or formant transitions (S1 Fig).
The low- and high-trustworthiness prototypes were generated by averaging each voice parameter across the eight voices having obtained lowest or highest average trustworthiness ratings, respectively, in a previous study [4]. These two prototypes were then used to generate a 9-stimulus acoustic continuum in eight steps of equal acoustical distance (25% of the difference between the two prototypes) by inter- and extrapolation between the low-trustworthiness prototype in position S3 and the high-trustworthiness prototype in position S7. Thus the respective weights of S3 and S7 for the 9 stimuli of the continuum were: S1: 150%/-50%; S2: 125%:-25%;S3: 100%/0%; S4: 75%/25%; S5: 50%/50%; S6:25%/75%; S7: 0%/100%; S8: -25%125%; S9: -50%/150%. A second continuum (Control Continuum) was generated using the exact similar approach using the female voice recordings and a random selection of the 16 underlying voices (Audio files in Supporting Information). Acoustical measures of the stimuli were obtained using VoiceSauce [21] and are shown in S1 Table. Spectrograms of the 9 stimuli of the Trustworthiness continuum are shown in Fig 1C, and their f0 contours in Fig 3. Spectrograms of stimuli from the Control continuum are shown in S2 Fig.
Procedures
The experiment was carried out online on Glasgow University’ Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology experiments website (http://experiments.psy.gla.ac.uk/). Participants performed two blocks of ratings (order counterbalanced across subjects): they were either presented with male voice block first, followed by control voices (female) or vice versa, and asked to rate each voice on a continuum of trustworthiness represented by a visual analogue scale ranging from “very untrustworthy” to “very trustworthy”. At the start of each block, participants were asked, “For each voice, please rate how trustworthy it sounds, that is, how much you would be ready to trust that person.” No contextual information for the experiment was given, akin to a “no acquaintance” scenario. All voices were heard once per block with presentation order randomised in both blocks to avoid possible order effects. Participants were given the opportunity to take an untimed break between the two blocks.
Rating data are available as supporting information S1 Data.
Analysis
Raw trustworthiness ratings (range 0–500) were converted to individual z-scores based on the mean and standard deviation of the 18 ratings provided by each participant. For both the Trustworthiness and Control continua, the distributions of individual z-scores were compared between the S3 and S7 stimuli via paired t-tests. A possible caricaturing effect was also examined via paired t-tests comparing S1 to S3 and S9 to S7 in each continuum. Bonferronni correction was applied to account for the 3 comparisons per continuum.
Supporting information
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
Supported by grants RES-060-25-0010 by ESRC/MRC (UK) and grant AJE201214 by the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (France) to P.B. We are grateful for the help provided by the Labex BLRI (ANR-11-LABX-0036 to P.B.), supported by the French National Agency for Research (ANR), under the program "Investissements d'Avenir" (ANR-11-IDEX-0001-02). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Oosterhof NN, Todorov A. The functional basis of face evaluation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(32):11087–92. Epub 2008/08/08. 10.1073/pnas.0805664105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Scherer KR. Judging personality from voice: a cross-cultural approach to an old issue in interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality. 1972;40(2):191–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Aronovitch CD. Voice of Personality—Stereotyped Judgments and Their Relation to Voice Quality and Sex of Speaker. Journal of Social Psychology. 1976;99:207–20. 10.1080/00224545.1976.9924774 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.McAleer P, Todorov A, Belin P. How do you say 'hello'? Personality impressions from brief novel voices. PLoS One. 2014;9(3):e90779 Epub 2014/03/14. 10.1371/journal.pone.0090779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Feinberg DR, DeBruine LM, Jones BC, Perrett DI. The role of femininity and averageness of voice pitch in aesthetic judgments of women's voices. Perception. 2008;37:615–23. 10.1068/p5514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Scherer KR. Personality Inference from Voice Quality—Loud Voice of Extroversion. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1978;8(4):467–87. 10.1002/ejsp.2420080405 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Willis J, Todorov A. First impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science 2006;17:592–8. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zebrowitz LA, Montepare JM. Social Psychological Face Perception: Why Appearance Matters. Social Personality and Psychology Compass. 2008;2(3):1497 Epub 2008/05/01. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00109.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Zebrowitz LA, Collins MA. Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: the affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personality and social psychology review: an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc. 1997;1(3):204–23. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0103_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Todorov A, Olivola CY, Dotsch R, Mende-Siedlecki P. Social attributions from faces: determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66:519–45. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Feinberg DR, Jones BC, Little AC, Burt DM, Perrett DI. Manipulations of fundamental and formant frequencies influence the attractiveness of human male voices. Animal Behaviour. 2005;69:561–8. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mileva M, Tompkinson J, Watt D, Burton AM. Audiovisual Integration in Social Evaluation. Journal of Exprimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2017. 10.1037/xhp0000439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tsankova E, Aubrey AJ, Krumhumer E, Möllering G, Kappas A, Marshall D, et al. Facial and Vocal Cues in Perception of Trustworthiness. Computer Vision—ACCV Workshops Lecture Notes in Computer Science. 2013;7729:308–91. [Google Scholar]
- 14.McAllister DJ. Affect- and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Journal. 1995;38:24–59. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Thompson JD. Organizations in action: social science bases of administrative theory New-York: McGraw-Hill; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kawahara H, Matsui H,. In Proc. 2003 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics S, and Signal Processing, Vol. I, pp. 256–259. Auditory morphing based on an elastic perceptual distance metric in an interference-free time-frequency representation.
- 17.Feinberg DR, Jones BC, DeBruine LM, O'Connor JJM, Tigue CC, Borak DJ. Integrating fundamental and formant frequencies in women's preferences for men's voices. Behavioral Ecology. 2011;22:1320–5. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Oksenberg L, Coleman L, Cannell CF. Interviewer's voices and refusal rates in telephone surveys The Public Opinion Quarterly. 1986;50:97–111. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Varnet L, Knoblauch K, Meunier F, Hoen M. Using auditory classification images for the identification of fine acoustic cues used in speech perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:865 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00865 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Varnet L, Knoblauch K, Serniclaes W, Meunier F, Hoen M. A Psychophysical Imaging Method Evidencing Auditory Cue Extraction during Speech Perception: A Group Analysis of Auditory Classification Images. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0118009 10.1371/journal.pone.0118009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shue Y-L, Keating P, Vicenik C, Yu K, editors. VoiceSauce: A program for voice analysis. ICPhS XVII; 2011.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.