Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Hypertension. 2017 Sep 11;70(5):964–971. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.117.09807

Left ventricular Mass, Brain MRI and Cognitive Performance: Results from the Strong Heart Study

Bernhard Haring a,*, Adam Omidpanah b, Astrid M Suchy-Dicey b, Lyle G Best c, Steven P Verney d, Dean K Shibata e, Shelley A Cole f, Tauqeer Ali g, Barbara V Howard h, Dedra Buchwald i, Richard B Devereux j
PMCID: PMC5638703  NIHMSID: NIHMS900906  PMID: 28893898

Abstract

Left ventricular mass (LVM) has been shown to to serve as measure of target organ damage resulting from chronic exposure to several risk factors. Data on the association of mid-life LVM with later cognitive performance are sparse. We studied 721 adults (mean age 56 years at baseline) enrolled in the Strong Heart Study (SHS, 1993–1995) and the ancillary Cerebrovascular Disease and its Consequences in American Indians Study (CDCAI, 2010–13), a study population with high prevalence of cardiovascular disease (CVD). LVM was assessed with transthoracic echocardiography at baseline in 1993 to 1995. Cranial magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and cognitive testing were undertaken between 2010 and 2013. Generalized estimating equations were used to model associations between LVM and later imaging and cognition outcomes. The mean follow-up period was 17 years. A difference of 25 gm in higher LVM was associated with marginally lower hippocampal volume (0.01 %; 95% CI 0.02, 0.00; p-value 0.001) and higher white matter grade (0.10; 95% CI 0.02, 0.18; p-value 0.014). Functionally, participants with higher LVM tended to have slightly lower scores on the modified mini-mental state examination (3MSE) (0.58; 95% CI 1.08, 0.08; p-value 0.024). The main results persisted after adjusting for blood pressure levels or vascular disease. The small overall effect sizes are partly explained by survival bias due to the high prevalence of cardiovascular disease in our population. Our findings emphasize the role of cardiovascular health in mid-life as a target for the prevention of deleterious cognitive and functional outcomes in later life.

Keywords: left ventricular mass, echocardiography, MRI, brain morphology, cognitive function

Introduction

Hypertension, diabetes and obesity have been associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment, with greater risk among those with longer duration and greater severity.1 Unfortunately, infrequent screening, delayed diagnoses, and poor management of these disease risk factors are common problems particularly among populations without adequate access to health care which points to cognitive impairment as a unique burden especially in medically underserved areas. This disparity underscores the need for clinical markers that capture cumulative risk burden, in order to better identify individuals with excess risk for later cognitive decline.

Left ventricular mass (LVM) has been shown to be able to serve as an easily acquired clinical marker of target organ damage that provides a time-integrated summation of exposure to various risk factors.24 Beyond its association with incident cardiovascular disease (CVD)4,5, previous results indicate a possible relationship between LVM and later cognitive performance.6 Specifically, in the Framingham Study, higher LVM was associated with lower cognitive performance over approximately 3.5 years follow-up.6 However, this association was attenuated with adjustment for cardiovascular risk factors or cardiovascular disease, suggesting vascular pathology as a mediating factor. Moreover, a relatively short duration of follow-up may have been inadequate to assess the entirety of purported cumulative effects of LVM on cognition. Other studies, including the Helsinki Aging Study, also reported left ventricular hypertrophy to be associated with cognitive decline as assessed by the mini-mental-state examination.7 Additionally, data stemming from a small cross-sectional European analysis found LVM to be associated with cognitive decline in elderly subjects but cognitive and covariate assessment was limited.8 All together, current evidence suggests a relationship between LVM and cognitive decline, but mediating factors such as vascular brain injury or atrophy are yet unclear as all prior analyses lacked morphological brain data. Previous studies only included populations of European ancestry, so generalizability to other populations has not been established. Because genetic components, such as ApoE ε4 allele status, may play an important role in hippocampal atrophy and cognitive decline, risk for non-white populations may be different.9,10

The objective of this study was to assess the association of mid-life LVM with later vascular brain injury and atrophy, and with cognitive performance in elderly American Indians. We hypothesized that higher LVM is associated with abnormal cranial MRI findings and with reduced cognitive performance.

Methods

Study population

The ‘Cerebrovascular Disease and its Consequences in American Indians Study (CDCAI)’ recruited surviving members of a 25-year, population-based cohort of American Indians focused on cardiovascular disease, its risk factors, and its consequences (‘Strong Heart Study’).12 The goals of the CDCAI aim were to characterize the burden, risk factors, and manifestations of vascular brain injury identified via cranial magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).11,12 Between 2010 to 2013, the CDCAI enrolled 1,033 participants aged 64 and older from American Indian communities in the Northern Plains, Southern Plains, and Southwestern United States. All participants underwent cranial MRI and cognitive testing according to standardized protocols.11 For this analysis, 215 participants were removed because one community withdrew consent. An additional 74 individuals were excluded from analysis because they were missing LVM data collected at a previous Strong Heart Study visit. Of the remaining participants (N= 744), 23 with a self-reported history of stroke or TIA were excluded from analyses because these conditions are known independent causes and contributors of cognitive dysfunction.1 The resulting final analytic sample consisted of 721 American Indians. The authorized body of each participating tribe approved the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants at enrollment. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants at enrollment.

Left Ventricular Mass

During the second examination of the Strong Heart Study (SHS) in 1993 to 1995 (SHS phase 2), transthoracic echocardiograms were performed using previously described methods.13,14 Echocardiographic measures of cardiac geometry and function were collected in all participants by expert sonographers and reviewed offline by a highly-experienced investigator. Left ventricular internal diastolic diameter, left ventricular posterior wall thickness, and interventricular septal thickness were measured in diastole on 2-dimensional echocardiograms according to American Society of Echocardiography criteria.15 LVM was calculated by a necropsy-validated formula.16

MRI measurements

Between 2010 to 2013 cranial MRI examinations of study participants were undertaken with methods as described previously in detail.11,17 In short, MRI scans consisted of six MRI sequences undertaken with routine 1.5 T scanners nearby the reservations of American Indians: 1) sagittal T1-weighted localizer, 2) 5mm axial-T1, 3) 5 mm axial-T2, and 4) 5mm axial-T2* susceptibility-weighted images, 5) 3mm axial fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images, and 6) 1.5mm sagittal T1-weighted volumetric gradient echo images. Neuroradiologists trained in the study protocols and blinded to participant information read and graded all scans. A primary reader scored all MRI features, and a secondary reader indpendently scored for the infarcs, hemorrhages, or other focal lesions. Both neuroradiologists reviewed any scans with discrepant readings until a consensus was reached. A quality control committee oversaw the conduct and evaluation of study procedures.11,17

Quantitative volumetric brain data were estimated using automated software, including FLEX for WMH volume18; FIRST in FSL 5.0 and the ENIGMA1 protocol for the hippocampal volumes1921; and FreeSurfer for total brain volume.22 Brain and hippocampal, and white matter hyperintensity volumes were normalized to the total intracranial volume. Brain infarcts were defined as lesions 3mm or larger (including both lacunes and larger cortical infarcts) with characteristic shape, absence of mass effect, and hyperintensity to gray matter on both T2-weighted images and FLAIR to contrast with perivascular spaces, which have characteristic location and shape and demonstrate cerebrospinal fluid intensity on all sequences.17,23 Lesions within white matter were required to be hypo-intense on T1-weighted images to distinguish them from focal white matter hyperintensities (WMH).17,2325 Severity of WMH was graded using a semi-quantitative 10-point scale based on previously validated image standards for FLAIR images for WMH.17,2629 Performing visual scoring based on a semi-quantitative grading system was done because elderly patients often have a difficult time holding their head still in the scanner and because 3D volumetric scans and subsequent image processing with segmentation are relatively vulnerable to motion artifacts.2932 As this was a study of volunteers without acute symptoms, diffusion imaging was not performed for the detection of acute infarcts. Infarcts were scored for acuity based on the presence of edema or mass effect on the FLAIR and T2 images. Brain hemorrhages were defined as lesions hypointense on gradient echo images (sequence 4), which are sensitive to even small amount of old blood in the brain tissue.17,33 Both microhemorrhages and larger hemorrhages were recorded.

Neuropsychological test performance

Cognitive testing was completed at a clinic visit within 30 days of the MRI scan, typically the same day or the following day. Trained examiners evaluated general cognitive function using the 100-point Modified Mini-Mental State (3MSE); processing speed using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) Coding test34; phonemic fluency and executive functioning using the Controlled Oral Word Association test (COWA)35,36; and verbal learning, immediate, and both immediate and delayed memory using the California Verbal Learning Test- Second Edition Short Form (CVLT-II SF).37,38 For the purpose of this analysis, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) was defined as 3MSE < 78 points.39

Covariates

All study participants underwent clinic examination at both the LVM visit and the cognitive examination visit, which included personal interview, physical examination, and medication review.12 Blood pressure status was assessed by the average of two seated blood pressure readings at clinic examination. Hypertension was defined as a self-report of current antihypertensive therapy or clinic measurement of SBP ≥140mmHg or DBP ≥90mmHg.40 Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as body weight divided by height squared (kg/m2). Diabetes mellitus was evaluated based on American Diabetes association criteria of fasting glucose ≥140 mg/dL, 2-hour post-challenge glucose ≥200 mg/dL, or use of oral hypoglycemic medication or insulin.41,42 History of CVD (coronary heart disease, heart failure, TIA, or stroke) was determined based on self-report, and by adjudication procedures using systematic review of medical records.43 Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) was used to describe the level of general stress.44 ApoE ε4 allele status was assayed by immunoblot.45,46,47

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics of demographic characteristics and covariates are presented as mean (standard deviation) or frequency (percent) (Table 1; Supplementary Table S1). Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) were used to assess associations between LVM, cranial MRI findings, and cognitive performance (Table 2). An initial model adjusted for age, sex, education (continuously as number of years), income (less than 10K, 10 to less than 25K, 25K or more annually per household), BMI (normal, overweight, obese), presence of at least one ApoE ε4 allele and study site. A second model additionally adjusted for BMI, alcohol use (never, ever, current), smoking status (never, ever, current), PSS, CVD, atrial fibrillation, diabetes status and hypertension at time of echocardiogram. Sensitivity analyses for the main models were undertaken by additionally adjusting for SBP and DBP at follow-up (Table 3) as well as by adjusting for self reported vascular disease at time of neurocognitive examination (Table 4). Multiple testing was addressed using the false discovery rate (FDR), set at the 0.10 alpha level using the Benjamani Hochberg procedure for each set of neuroimaging and neurocognitive analyses.48 Multiple imputation by chained equations was used to handle missing covariate data, and results were pooled using Rubin’s rules for model based statistics and directly pooling resampling.49 Additional analyses were undertaken to test for effect modification by sex (Table S2) and stratified by ApoE ε4 carrier status (Table S3). Finally, to assess whether associations between LVM and cognitive function were attributable to MRI findings, we applied mediation analyses using Sobel’s statistic, which is interpreted as the average causally mediated effect, along with a bias-corrected percentile bootstrap to obtain 95% CIs and p-values (Table S4).50 Statistical analyses were conducted using R version 3.3.3 and packages mice and sandwich.49,51,52

Table 1.

Baseline Characteristics of Strong Heart Study participants, 1993 to 1995 (n=721)

Variable

Left ventricular mass (gm); mean (sd) 150.1 (32.1)
Age (years); mean (sd) 55.7 (5.6)
Female; n (%) 495 (68.7)
ApoE ε4 allele presence; n (%) 170 (23.6)
Income; n (%)
Less than $10,000 195 (33.6)
$10,000 to less than $25,000 253 (43.5)
$25,000 or greater 133 (22.9)
Education (number of years); mean (sd) 12.4 (2.9)
Study Site; n (%)
Arizona 88 (12.2)
Oklahoma 305 (42.3)
Dakotas 328 (45.5)
Smoking status; n (%)
Never 202 (28.8)
Ever 263 (37.5)
Current 236 (33.7)
Alcohol use status; n (%)
Never 137 (19.3)
Ever 339 (47.7)
Current 234 (33.0)
Perceived stress scale (PSS); mean (sd) 10.8 (4.4)
BMI (kg/m2); mean (sd) 31.5 (5.8)
BMI (CDC); n (%)
Normal 83 (11.5)
Overweight 234 (32.5)
Obese 402 (55.9)
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg); mean (sd) 123.3 (16.4)
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg); mean (sd) 74.8 (9.3)
Heart disease; n (%) 86 (11.9)
Atrial fibrillation; n (%) 1 (0.1)
Hypertension; n (%) 225 (31.2)
Treatment for Hypertension; n (%) 154 (21.4)
Diabetes; n (%) 211 (29.3)
Treatment for diabetes; n (%)
None 589 (92.8)
Oral 0 (0.0)
Insulin 44 (6.9)
Oral/Insulin 2 (0.3)

Table 2.

Associations of LVM with neuroimaging and neurocognitive findings (per 25gm difference of LVM)

Basic adjustment Full adjustment

Neuroimaging findings
Coefficient 95% CI p Coefficient 95% CI p
Brain volume (%) −0.287 (−0.618, 0.043) 0.088 −0.321 (−0.656, 0.013) 0.060
Hippocampal volume (%) −0.009 (−0.014, −0.003) 0.003 * −0.010 (−0.015, −0.004) 0.001 *
White matter grade 0.102 (0.026, 0.178) 0.008 * 0.098 (0.020, 0.176) 0.014 *
White matter hyperintensity volume (%) 0.028 (−0.005, 0.062) 0.095 0.026 (−0.009, 0.061) 0.152
Odds ratio 95% CI p Odds ratio 95% CI p
Infarcts 1.134 (0.971, 1.324) 0.112 1.116 (0.951, 1.308) 0.178
Hemorrhage 0.761 (0.493, 1.173) 0.216 0.748 (0.474, 1.179) 0.211
Neurocognitive findings
Coefficient 95% CI p Coefficient 95% CI p
3MSE −0.632 (−1.124, −0.140) 0.012 * −0.576 (−1.075, −0.077) 0.024
WAIS-IV Coding −0.356 (−1.159, 0.446) 0.384 −0.225 (−1.053, 0.602) 0.593
COWA −0.779 (−1.482, −0.075) 0.030 −0.693 (−1.410, 0.024) 0.058
CVLT-II Learning −0.038 (−0.344, 0.269) 0.810 −0.043 (−0.361, 0.274) 0.788
CVLT-II Short recall −0.064 (−0.201, 0.074) 0.364 −0.077 (−0.218, 0.065) 0.289
CVLT-II Long recall 0.001 (−0.145, 0.147) 0.987 0.000 (−0.150, 0.151) 0.997

Basic models adjust for age, sex, education, income, site, ApoE ε4 allele carrier status and BMI.

Full models adjust additionally for smoking, alcohol use, PSS, prior atrial fibrillation, diabetes and hypertension.

95% CI, 95% confidence interval; p, p-value (unadjusted for multiple comparisons)

Statistical significant results are indicated by * and are corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini Hochberg procedure to obtain a false discovery rate of 0.10.

3MSE, Modified Mini-Mental State Examination

WAIS-IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition

COWA, Controlled Oral Word Association Test

CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test-II Short Form

Table 3.

Blood pressure adjusted associations of LVM with neuroimaging and neurocognitive findings (per 25gm difference of LVM)

Basic adjustment Full adjustment

Neuroimaging findings
Coefficient 95% CI p Coefficient 95% CI p
Brain volume (%) −0.291 (−0.628, 0.046) 0.091 −0.318 (−0.657, 0.021) 0.066
Hippocampal volume (%) −0.009 (−0.015, −0.003) 0.003 * −0.010 (−0.016, −0.004) 0.001 *
White matter grade 0.078 (0.002, 0.155) 0.045 0.077 (−0.001, 0.155) 0.052
White matter hyperintensity volume (%) 0.017 (−0.017, 0.051) 0.334 0.019 (−0.017, 0.054) 0.301
Odds ratio 95% CI p Odds ratio 95% CI p
Infarcts 1.093 (0.930, 1.286) 0.281 1.098 (0.933, 1.293) 0.261
Hemorrhage 0.755 (0.481, 1.186) 0.223 0.735 (0.457, 1.181) 0.203
Neurocognitive findings
Coefficient 95% CI p Coefficient 95% CI p
3MSE −0.586 (−1.084, −0.087) 0.021 −0.569 (−1.076, −0.061) 0.028
WAIS-IV Coding −0.333 (−1.161, 0.495) 0.431 −0.229 (−1.063, 0.604) 0.590
COWA −0.753 (−1.473, −0.033) 0.041 −0.700 (−1.421, 0.021) 0.057
CVLT-II Learning −0.032 (−0.347, 0.283) 0.843 −0.035 (−0.355, 0.286) 0.832
CVLT-II Short recall −0.054 (−0.195, 0.087) 0.452 −0.057 (−0.198, 0.085) 0.433
CVLT-II Long recall 0.017 (−0.133, 0.166) 0.828 0.020 (−0.133, 0.172) 0.802

Basic models adjust for age, sex, education, income, site, BMI, ApoE ε4 allele carrier status, duration of follow-up, systolic and diastolic blood pressure at LVM and neurocognitive assessment.

Full models adjust additionally for smoking status, alcohol use, PSS, prior atrial fibrillation, diabetes, and hypertension.

95% CI, 95% confidence interval; p, p-value (unadjusted for multiple comparisons)

Statistical significant results are indicated by * and are corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini Hochberg procedure to obtain a false discovery rate of 0.10.

3MSE, Modified Mini-Mental State Examination

WAIS-IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition

COWA, Controlled Oral Word Association Test

CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test-II Short Form

Table 4.

Vascular disease adjusted associations of LVM with neuroimaging and neurocognitive findings (per 25gm difference of LVM)

Basic adjustment Full adjustment

Neuroimaging findings
Coefficient 95% CI p Coefficient 95% CI p
Brain volume (%) −0.288 (−0.620, 0.044) 0.089 −0.324 (−0.660, 0.012) 0.059
Hippocampal volume (%) −0.009 (−0.015, −0.003) 0.002 * −0.010 (−0.016, −0.004) 0.001 *
White matter grade 0.102 (0.026, 0.178) 0.008 * 0.098 (0.020, 0.177) 0.013 *
White matter hyperintensity volume (%) 0.028 (−0.005, 0.062) 0.097 0.026 (−0.009, 0.061) 0.152
Odds ratio 95% CI p Odds ratio 95% CI p
Infarcts 1.131 (0.969, 1.321) 0.119 1.114 (0.950, 1.306) 0.185
Hemorrhage 0.767 (0.498, 1.179) 0.227 0.754 (0.481, 1.184) 0.221
Neurocognitive findings
Coefficient 95% CI p Coefficient 95% CI p
3MSE −0.620 (−1.110, −0.131) 0.013 * −0.565 (−1.063, −0.067) 0.026
WAIS-IV Coding −0.328 (−1.130, 0.475) 0.424 −0.208 (−1.037, 0.621) 0.622
COWA −0.760 (−1.465, −0.055) 0.035 −0.685 (−1.403, 0.033) 0.061
CVLT-II Learning −0.034 (−0.341, 0.272) 0.827 −0.038 (−0.355, 0.279) 0.815
CVLT-II Short recall −0.063 (−0.200, 0.075) 0.371 −0.075 (−0.216, 0.066) 0.300
CVLT-II Long recall 0.001 (−0.146, 0.147) 0.991 0.000 (−0.151, 0.151) 0.998

Basic models adjust for age, sex, education, income, site, BMI, ApoE ε4 allele carrier status, duration of follow-up and vascular disease at follow-up.

Full models adjust additionally for smoking, alcohol use, PSS, prior atrial fibrillation, diabetes, and hypertension.

95% CI, 95% confidence interval; p, p-value (unadjusted for multiple comparisons)

Statistical significant results are indicated by * and are corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini Hochberg procedure to obtain a false discovery rate of 0.10.

3MSE, Modified Mini-Mental State Examination

WAIS-IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition

COWA, Controlled Oral Word Association Test

CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test-II Short Form

Results

Baseline characteristics of study participants at the second exam of SHS (1993–1995) are presented in Table 1. At time of LVM assessment, the mean age of the study population was approximately 56 years, predominantly female with a mean of 12 years of education. Risk factors for vascular disease were markedly elevated: mean BMI was 31.5 kg/m2, prevalence of hypertension was 31% and prevalence of diabetes was 29%. Prevalence of self-reported current alcohol use was 33% and current smoking status was 34%. Responses to the perceived stress scale indicated an average value of 10.8 and a very small fraction (4.0%) had stress greater than 20, which is considered high stress. Mean LVM was 150 gm (142gm in women and 169 gm in men).

Characteristics of study participants at time of neurocognitive examination (2010–2013), an average of 17.3 years following the second exam of SHS, are presented in Table S1. The mean age of the study population was 73 years. The burden of cardiovascular risk factors deteriorated. Most notably, diabetes prevalence escalated to 52%, hypertension prevalence to 80%, whereas mean BMI was still 31.5 kg/m2. Incident vascular events (i.e. stroke, congestive heart failure, or myocardial infarcation) were self-reported by 13% of participants. MCI was present in 10% of study participants.

Regression coefficients and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) expressing associations for LVM, per 25 gm difference, with cranial MRI findings and cognitive performance are presented in Table 2. Among MRI findings, a 25 gm difference in LVM was statistically significantly associated with a 0.009% lower hippocampal volume adjusting for age, sex, education, site, income, BMI and ApoE ε4 allele carrier status (95% CI, −0.014%, −0.003%, p-value 0.003) and a slightly higher grade of white matter hyperintensities (0.10, 95% CI, 0.026, 0.178, p-value 0.008). These associations were also statistically significant after further control of smoking, alcohol use, PSS, atrial fibrillation, diabetes and hypertension (−0.01%, 95% CI, −0.015%, −0.004%, p-value 0.001 and 0.10, 95% CI, 0.02, 0.176, p-value 0.014, respectively). Associations with WMH volume, brain volume, hemorrhage and infarcts were not statistically significant. Among measures of cognitive performance, a 25gm LVM difference was statistically significantly associated with a slightly lower 3MSE score adjusting for age, sex, education, income, site, BMI and presence of at least one ApoE ε4 allele (0.63, 95% CI, −1.124, −0.14, p-value 0.012), whereas no statistically significant associations with COWA, WAIS-IV coding, CVLT-II SF total learning scoring and CVLT-II SF immediate and delayed free recall scoring were observed. Further control of comorbidities and risk factors rendered the 3MSE association non-significant. When we conducted sensitivity analyses with adjustment for systolic and diastolic blood pressure (Table 3), only associations with hippocampal volume remained statistically significant in our fully adjusted models. Further sensitivity analyses with adjustment for self-reported vascular events including stroke, myocardial infarction, or congestive heart failure are presented in Table 4. In models adjusting for demographic factors as well as comorbidities and risk factors, associations with hippocampal volume and white matter grade were statistically significant.

No models showed effect modification by sex (Table S2). In models stratified by ApoE ε4 allele carrier status (Table S3), no associations were statistically significant, however, this may owe to a smaller effective sample size, particularly among ApoE ε4 positive participants (n = 170). Mediation analyses found a statistically significant average causal mediated effect (ACME) relating LVM to 3MSE scores via hippocampal volume after adjusting for age, sex, education, income, site, duration of follow-up, BMI and presence of at least one ApoE ε4 allele (Table S4). Hippocampal volume had a −0.137 ACME relating LVM to 3MSE (95% boostrap CI, −0.275, −0.034, p-value 0.03). White matter grade had a −0.100 ACME relating LVM to 3MSE (95% bootstrap CI, −0.236, −0.011, p-value 0.07).

Discussion

These findings, from a population-based cohort study of middle-aged and elderly American Indians, suggest that higher LVM in midlife is associated with a slightly lower general cognitive performance in later life. These functional changes are accompanied by lower hippocampal volume and greater severity of white matter hyperintensities which may play an explanatory role. In fact, mediation analyses suggest that LVM may lead to hippocampal atrophy which in turn could lead to impaired cognition. However, overall effect sizes are very small. This lack of clinical significance can on the one hand be explained by survival bias due to the high prevalence of cardiovascular disease and concomitant risk factors in our study population, on the other hand by the multifactorial nature of cognitive decline.

Vascular risk factors such as hypertension are known to play a significant role in accelerating structural brain aging and cognitive decline.1,53 Landmark studies such as the Honolulu Asia Aging Study have shown a long-term relationship of midlife blood pressure levels to late-life cognitive function.54 In the same study, β-Amyloid (Aβ) plasma levels started decreasing ≥15 years before Alzheimer Disease was diagnosed, and the association of Aβ to Alzheimer disease was mediated by midlife blood pressure.55 Similarly, an active blood pressure lowering regime has been shown to alter the progression of white matter hyperintensities, a key finding in the present study.56 Our observed associations of LVM with cognitive performance can hence be partially explained by adverse effects of cardiovascular risk factors (in particular blood pressure) on the microcirculatory system of the hippocampal region leading to atrophy and its consequent functional deficits. We therefore performed additional sensitivity analyses adjusting for blood pressure levels. Our main results did not change. These findings seem to suggest that CVD – predicted by higher LVM – may indeed explain in part some morphological brain changes which supports a role in increased brain aging.57 Nonetheless, whether the relationship of LVM with lower cognitive performance is due to residual factors or unmeasured confounding leading to functional deficits, some other process, or whether the occurrence of LVM and cognitive decline are independent but convergent disease processes remains uncertain. However, while on the one hand the concomitant occurrence of LVM and dementia may synergize and increase the risk of poor cognition in later life58, it may on the other hand open a window of opportunity for future prevention through early detection and intervention.59,60

These analyses are the first to assess the association of mid-life LVM with later vascular brain injury and atrophy, and with cognitive performance in American Indians. Other strengths of this work include a large, well-characterized cohort with standardized, rigorous assessment of echocardiographic measures of LVM, MRI findings, and cognitive performance. However, there are also noteworthy limitations. Survival bias may have significantly influenced our findings as more than half of participants from the baseline SHS phase I exam died prior to the CDCAI study. This bias may have led to an underestimation of our findings. Furthermore, cognitive ability and brain morphology were not evaluated at baseline, preventing analyses of longitudinal changes in such measures. This limitation is shared with previous investigations that partly relied on cross-sectional data.6,8 Additionally, cognitive dysfunction has not been validated in American Indians, so the degree of clinical significance for loss in these performance measures is yet unclear.61 Finally, as this was a study of volunteers without acute symptoms nearby the reservations of American Indians, more advanced imaging techniques such as diffusion tensor imaging or perfusion imaging which we might have done in a university medical center setting could not be performed. Thus, we may have underestimated the burden of small-vessel disease in our population.

Perspectives

Higher LVM in middle-aged American Indians is associated with slight morphological brain changes and decreased cognitive performance in later years. As elderly American Indians are one of the fastest growing segments of the US population, and since CVD patients are surviving longer, our findings warrant further research into clinical outcomes of CVD and related conditions as possible targets for prevention and intervention to ameliorate deleterious cognitive and functional outcomes.62

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Tables

Novelty and Significance.

What is New ?

Data on the association of mid-life left ventricular mass with later cognitive performance are sparse and all prior analyses lacked morphological brain data.

What is Relevant ?

In brain MRI assessment, a 25 gm increase in left ventricular mass in mid-life was associated with marginally lower hippocampal volume and higher white matter grade at later age. Functionally, individuals with higher left ventricular mass tended to have lower scores in the modified mini-mental state examination.

Summary.

Higher left ventricular mass in middle-aged American Indians was associated with slightly decreased cognitive performance in later years. The small overall effect sizes are partly explained by survival bias due to the high prevalence of cardiovascular disease in our population. These findings emphasize the role of cardiovascular health in mid-life as a target for the prevention of deleterious cognitive and functional outcomes in later life.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Will Longstreth (University of Washington) and Dr. Paul Jensen (Washington State University) for their comments in the preparation of this manuscript. Furthermore, the authors thank the Indian Health Service, all SHS and CDCAI participants, staff, and investigators. The opinions expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Indian Health Service.

Sources of funding

This study was supported by cooperative agreement grants U01-HL41642, U01-HL41652, U01-HL41654, U01-HL65520, and U01-HL65521 and research grants R01-HL109315, R01-HL109301, R01-HL109284, R01-HL109282, R01-HL109319 and R01-HL093086 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, Bethesda, MD.

Footnotes

Disclosures

None

References

  • 1.Gorelick PB, Scuteri A, Black SE, et al. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the american heart association/american stroke association. Stroke. 2011;42:2672–2713. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Devereux RB, de Simone G, Ganau A, Koren MJ, Mensah GA, Roman MJ. Left ventricular hypertrophy and hypertension. Clin Exp Hypertens. 1993;15:1025–1032. doi: 10.3109/10641969309037090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Devereux RB, Roman MJ. Left ventricular hypertrophy in hypertension: stimuli, patterns, and consequences. Hypertens Res. 1999;22:1–9. doi: 10.1291/hypres.22.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Levy D, Garrison RJ, Savage DD, Kannel WB, Castelli WP. Prognostic implications of echocardiographically determined left ventricular mass in the Framingham Heart Study. N Engl J Med. 1990;322:1561–1566. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199005313222203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bikkina M, Levy D, Evans JC, et al. Left ventricular mass and risk of stroke in an elderly cohort. The Framingham Heart Study. JAMA. 1994;272:33–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Elias MF, Sullivan LM, Elias PK, et al. Left ventricular mass, blood pressure, and lowered cognitive performance in the Framingham offspring. Hypertension. 2007;49:439–445. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000256361.68158.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kahonen-Vare M, Brunni-Hakala S, Lindroos M, Pitkala K, Strandberg T, Tilvis R. Left ventricular hypertrophy and blood pressure as predictors of cognitive decline in old age. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2004;16:147–152. doi: 10.1007/BF03324544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Scuteri A, Coluccia R, Castello L, Nevola E, Brancati AM, Volpe M. Left ventricular mass increase is associated with cognitive decline and dementia in the elderly independently of blood pressure. Eur Heart J. 2009;30:1525–1529. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehp133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jak AJ, Houston WS, Nagel BJ, Corey-Bloom J, Bondi MW. Differential cross-sectional and longitudinal impact of APOE genotype on hippocampal volumes in nondemented older adults. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;23:382–389. doi: 10.1159/000101340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.den Heijer T, Oudkerk M, Launer LJ, van Duijn CM, Hofman A, Breteler MM. Hippocampal, amygdalar, and global brain atrophy in different apolipoprotein E genotypes. Neurology. 2002;59:746–748. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.5.746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Suchy-Dicey AM, Shibata D, Best LG, et al. Cranial Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Elderly American Indians: Design, Methods, and Implementation of the Cerebrovascular Disease and Its Consequences in American Indians Study. Neuroepidemiology. 2016;47:67–75. doi: 10.1159/000443277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lee ET, Welty TK, Fabsitz R, et al. The Strong Heart Study. A study of cardiovascular disease in American Indians: design and methods. Am J Epidemiol. 1990;132:1141–1155. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Devereux RB, Roman MJ, de Simone G, et al. Relations of left ventricular mass to demographic and hemodynamic variables in American Indians: the Strong Heart Study. Circulation. 1997;96:1416–1423. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.96.5.1416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Devereux RB, Roman MJ, Paranicas M, et al. Relations of Doppler stroke volume and its components to left ventricular stroke volume in normotensive and hypertensive American Indians: the Strong Heart Study. Am J Hypertens. 1997;10:619–628. doi: 10.1016/s0895-7061(97)00059-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schiller NB, Shah PM, Crawford M, et al. Recommendations for quantitation of the left ventricle by two-dimensional echocardiography. American Society of Echocardiography Committee on Standards, Subcommittee on Quantitation of Two-Dimensional Echocardiograms. J Am Soc Echocardiogr. 1989;2:358–367. doi: 10.1016/s0894-7317(89)80014-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Devereux RB, Alonso DR, Lutas EM, et al. Echocardiographic assessment of left ventricular hypertrophy: comparison to necropsy findings. Am J Cardiol. 1986;57:450–458. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(86)90771-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Suchy-Dicey AM, Shibata DK, Madhyastha TM, Grabowski TJ, Longstreth WT, Jr, Buchwald DS. Findings of Vascular Brain Injury and Structural Loss from Cranial Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Elderly American Indians: The Strong Heart Study. Neuroepidemiology. 2017;48:39–47. doi: 10.1159/000459624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gibson E, Gao F, Black SE, Lobaugh NJ. Automatic segmentation of white matter hyperintensities in the elderly using FLAIR images at 3T. Journal of magnetic resonance imaging. 2010;31:1311–1322. doi: 10.1002/jmri.22004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Stein JL, Medland SE, Vasquez AA, et al. Identification of common variants associated with human hippocampal and intracranial volumes. Nature genetics. 2012;44:552–561. doi: 10.1038/ng.2250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE transactions on medical imaging. 2001;20:45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Patenaude B, Smith SM, Kennedy DN, Jenkinson M. A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. NeuroImage. 2011;56:907–922. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. NeuroImage. 2012;61:1402–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bryan RN, Cai J, Burke G, et al. Prevalence and anatomic characteristics of infarct-like lesions on MR images of middle-aged adults: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Neuroradiol. 1999;20:1273–1280. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Windham BG, Griswold ME, Shibata D, Penman A, Catellier DJ, Mosley TH., Jr Covert neurological symptoms associated with silent infarcts from midlife to older age: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities study. Stroke. 2012;43:1218–1223. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.643379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wardlaw JM, Smith EE, Biessels GJ, et al. Neuroimaging standards for research into small vessel disease and its contribution to ageing and neurodegeneration. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:822–838. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70124-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Liao D, Cooper L, Cai J, et al. Presence and severity of cerebral white matter lesions and hypertension, its treatment, and its control. The ARIC Study. Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Stroke. 1996;27:2262–2270. doi: 10.1161/01.str.27.12.2262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Price TR, Manolio TA, Kronmal RA, et al. Silent brain infarction on magnetic resonance imaging and neurological abnormalities in community-dwelling older adults. The Cardiovascular Health Study. CHS Collaborative Research Group. Stroke. 1997;28:1158–1164. doi: 10.1161/01.str.28.6.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Knopman DS, Penman AD, Catellier DJ, et al. Vascular risk factors and longitudinal changes on brain MRI: the ARIC study. Neurology. 2011;76:1879–1885. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821d753f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Longstreth WT, Jr, Manolio TA, Arnold A, et al. Clinical correlates of white matter findings on cranial magnetic resonance imaging of 3301 elderly people. The Cardiovascular Health Study. Stroke. 1996;27:1274–1282. doi: 10.1161/01.str.27.8.1274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Liao D, Cooper L, Cai J, et al. The prevalence and severity of white matter lesions, their relationship with age, ethnicity, gender, and cardiovascular disease risk factors: the ARIC Study. Neuroepidemiology. 1997;16:149–162. doi: 10.1159/000368814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Manolio TA, Kronmal RA, Burke GL, et al. Magnetic resonance abnormalities and cardiovascular disease in older adults. The Cardiovascular Health Study. Stroke. 1994;25:318–327. doi: 10.1161/01.str.25.2.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Power MC, Deal JA, Sharrett AR, et al. Smoking and white matter hyperintensity progression: the ARIC-MRI Study. Neurology. 2015;84:841–848. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Tsushima Y, Aoki J, Endo K. Brain microhemorrhages detected on T2*-weighted gradient-echo MR images. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2003;24:88–96. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 4th. San Antonio, TX: Pearson; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ruff RM, Light RH, Parker SB, Levin HS. Benton Controlled Oral Word Association Test: reliability and updated norms. Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 1996;11:329–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Benton AL, editor. Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lacritz LH, Cullum CM. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test and CVLT: a preliminary comparison. Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 1998;13:623–628. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B, editors. California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) 2nd. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bland RC, Newman SC. Mild dementia or cognitive impairment: the Modified Mini-Mental State examination (3MS) as a screen for dementia. Can J Psychiatry. 2001;46:506–510. doi: 10.1177/070674370104600604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chobanian AV, Bakris GL, Black HR, et al. The Seventh Report of the Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure: the JNC 7 report. JAMA. 2003;289:2560–2572. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.19.2560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.WHO. WHO Expert Committee on Diabetes Mellitus: second report. World Health Organ Tech Rep Ser. 1980;646:1–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.ADA. Report of the expert committee on the diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care. 2003;26(Suppl 1):S5–20. doi: 10.2337/diacare.26.2007.s5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Howard BV, Lee ET, Cowan LD, et al. Rising tide of cardiovascular disease in American Indians. The Strong Heart Study. Circulation. 1999;99:2389–2395. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.99.18.2389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kataoka S, Paidi M, Howard BV. Simplified isoelectric focusing/immunoblotting determination of apoprotein E phenotype. Clin Chem. 1994;40:11–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kataoka S, Robbins DC, Cowan LD, et al. Apolipoprotein E polymorphism in American Indians and its relation to plasma lipoproteins and diabetes. The Strong Heart Study. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 1996;16:918–925. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.16.8.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kamboh MI, Ferrell RE, Kottke B. Genetic studies of human apolipoproteins. V. A novel rapid procedure to screen apolipoprotein E polymorphism. J Lipid Res. 1988;29:1535–1543. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological) 1995:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software. 2011;45:1–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Mackinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J. Confidence Limits for the Indirect Effect: Distribution of the Product and Resampling Methods. Multivariate Behav Res. 2004;39:99. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.R Core Team, editor. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Zeileis A. Object-Oriented Computation of Sandwich Estimators. Journal of Statistical Software. 2006;16:1–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2011;77:461–468. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227b227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, Foley D, Havlik RJ. The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function. The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. JAMA. 1995;274:1846–1851. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Shah NS, Vidal JS, Masaki K, et al. Midlife blood pressure, plasma beta-amyloid, and the risk for Alzheimer disease: the Honolulu Asia Aging Study. Hypertension. 2012;59:780–786. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.111.178962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dufouil C, Chalmers J, Coskun O, et al. Effects of blood pressure lowering on cerebral white matter hyperintensities in patients with stroke: the PROGRESS (Perindopril Protection Against Recurrent Stroke Study) Magnetic Resonance Imaging Substudy. Circulation. 2005;112:1644–1650. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.104.501163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Srinivasa RN, Rossetti HC, Gupta MK, et al. Cardiovascular Risk Factors Associated with Smaller Brain Volumes in Regions Identified as Early Predictors of Cognitive Decline. Radiology. 2016;278:198–204. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2015142488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Farooq MU, Gorelick PB. Vascular cognitive impairment. Curr Atheroscler Rep. 2013;15:330. doi: 10.1007/s11883-013-0330-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Devereux RB, Wachtell K, Gerdts E, et al. Prognostic significance of left ventricular mass change during treatment of hypertension. JAMA. 2004;292:2350–2356. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.19.2350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Launer LJ, Hughes T, Yu B, et al. Lowering midlife levels of systolic blood pressure as a public health strategy to reduce late-life dementia: perspective from the Honolulu Heart Program/Honolulu Asia Aging Study. Hypertension. 2010;55:1352–1359. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.147389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Verney SP, Bennett J, Hamilton JM. Cultural Considerations in the Neuropsychological Assessment of American Indians/Alaska Natives. In: Ferraro FR, editor. Minority and Cross-Cultural Aspects of Neuropsychological Assessment. 2nd. New York: Psychology Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Pearson-Stuttard J, Guzman-Castillo M, Penalvo JL, et al. Modeling Future Cardiovascular Disease Mortality in the United States: National Trends and Racial and Ethnic Disparities. Circulation. 2016;133:967–978. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.115.019904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Tables

RESOURCES