Abstract
Predation is the main cause of mortality during early life stages. The ability to avoid and evade potential threats is, therefore, favoured to evolve during the early stages of life. It is also during these early stages that the process of familiarization occurs. It has long been recognized that associating with familiar individuals confers antipredator benefits. Yet gaps in our knowledge remain about how predator evasion is affected by social experience during early stages. In this study, we test the hypothesis that familiarization acquired during early life stages improves escape responses. Using the guppy Poecilia reticulata, we examine the effect of different recent social conditions in the three main components of predator evasion. Using high-speed motion analysis, we compared the number of individuals in each test group that responded to a visual stimulus, their reactive distance and magnitude of their response (maximum speed, maximum acceleration and distance) in groups composed either of familiar or non-familiar individuals. Contrary to the prediction, groups composed of familiar individuals were less responsive than groups of unfamiliar individuals. Reactive distance and magnitude of response were more dependent on individual size rather than on familiarity. Larger individuals reached higher maximum speeds and total distances in their escape response. Our result indicates that familiarity is likely to affect behaviour earlier in a predator-prey interaction, which then affects the behavioural component of the response. Taken together, our study contributes to previous ones by distinguishing which components of an escape response are modulated by familiarity.
Keywords: Predator evasion, Escape response, Antipredator benefits, Familiarity, Early social conditions
Introduction
Predation is a powerful agent of mortality, particularly during early life stages when organisms are at heightened risk due to their smaller size (Cushing, 1974). Natural selection is therefore expected to favour the development of antipredator behaviours early in life (Braithwaite & Salvanes, 2005; Vilhunen & Hirvonen, 2003). Antipredator behaviours are generally divided into two major types: (1) avoidance and (2) evasion (Fuiman & Magurran, 1994; Weihs & Webb, 1984). Avoidance includes any pre-emptive behaviours in which the individual reduces the likelihood of encountering a predator and consequently of its attack (Fuiman & Magurran, 1994). Evasion, on the other hand, occurs once the predator initiates the attack. As predator avoidance is not always possible, successful predator evasion tactics are essential for survival. The behaviour and frequency at which each evasion tactic is employed is context-dependent; individuals adopt behaviours that improve their evasive response and, thus, enhance survival (Domenici, 2010).
One way in which organisms may reduce the risk of predation is by associating with others, either by schooling or just by joining a group (Ruxton & Johnsen, 2016; Ward & Webster, 2016). Though groups might be more conspicuous to a predator, each individual within the group has a smaller probability of being predated than if alone. Among the group antipredator benefits of enhanced vigilance, dilution of risk, predator confusion and coordinated antipredator maneuverers (Krause & Ruxton, 2002; Ward & Webster, 2016), there is strong evidence showing that familiarity within the group enhances antipredator behaviours (Griffiths et al., 2004). Familiarity between conspecifics can be broadly defined as the ability to discriminate between individuals based on previous interactions (Griffiths, 2003). The process of familiarization is based on visual, and auditory and olfactory cues (Coffin, Watters & Mateo, 2011; Reby et al., 2001; Zajitschek & Brooks, 2008). Repeated exposure to a stimulus can lead to familiarisation, in a social context that may be conspecifics with whom an individual interacts, such as during foraging. Fitness benefits of joining a group composed of familiar conspecifics over unfamiliar individuals has been demonstrated in various taxa (Figueroa et al., 2013; Grabowska-Zhang, Sheldon & Hinde, 2012; Grabowska-Zhang, Wilkin & Sheldon, 2011; Strodl & Schausberger, 2012; Strodl & Schausberger, 2013), particularly in shoaling fish (Barber & Wright, 2001; Griffiths & Magurran, 1997b).
The benefits in associating with familiar individuals for the social learning and for the development and acquisition of successful antipredator responses in shoaling fish are acknowledged (Swaney et al., 2001; Ward & Hart, 2003). Groups composed by familiar individuals may be more cohesive and have reduced neighbour distance (Chivers, Brown & Smith, 1995; Höjesjö et al., 1998), characteristics which enhance predator confusion and dilute individual risk. Further, familiar groups generally experience reduced within-group aggression and evolve more stable social hierarchies (Griffiths et al., 2004; Höjesjö et al., 1998; Johnsson, 1997; Tanner & Keller, 2012). Reduced aggression within familiar groups allows more time for predator vigilance, which may improve escape latency (Griffiths et al., 2004; Strodl & Schausberger, 2012). Additionally, individuals are more likely to perform cooperative antipredator behaviours when in familiar groups, as they may remember whether the others have behaved cooperatively in the past (Dugatkin & Alfieri, 1991). For example, individuals in familiar groups may be more likely to perform more risky antipredator manoeuvres (Chivers, Brown & Smith, 1995), join predator mobbing (Grabowska-Zhang, Sheldon & Hinde, 2012), or perform predator inspection (Dugatkin & Godin, 1992). Such antipredator behaviours put individuals at higher risk, but improve group antipredator response.
While the effect and importance of familiarity on predator avoidance is well recognised, how familiarity shapes predator evasion, particularly the escape response, remains largely unexplored For example, studies to date have focused exclusively on the effect of familiarity on the latency of the response (Griffiths et al., 2004; Strodl & Schausberger, 2012) and have not considered other aspects of the escape performance. Successful escape responses depend on various components, such as latency, velocity and distance travelled in the response (Domenici & Blake, 1997). For instance, latency, considered as the time between the onset of the predator attack and the start if the response, is crucial for the outcome of the interaction (Fuiman et al., 2006). Also, an effective response requires moving away from the attack trajectory fast enough so the predator cannot adjust it (Fuiman & Cowan, 2003). Studies on escape behavior have focused on the aspects of the escape response which are modulated by the relative cost of escaping and perceived risk, such as latency, reactive distance (the distance between the predator and prey when the prey initiates a response) and responsiveness (whether or not a prey responds to an attack) (Domenici, 2010). Kinematic aspects of escape responses, on the other hand, are less often considered, as they have been considered to be constrained by the sensory-motor system of the individual (Domenici & Blake, 1997). A review by Domenici (2010) emphasizes that performance in escape responses is not always maximized to the physical capabilities of the individual, which suggests that other factors may cause variability in escape responses. Given the importance of social behaviour in reactive distance and responsiveness (Dial, Reznick & Brainerd, 2016), it is plausible that the kinematic aspects of an escape response may be modulated in a similar way by familiarity. In order to fully assess the escape performance of fish, we need to employ an approach that takes into account the multiple behavioural aspects on an escape response. The aim of this study was to address the role of familiarity acquired during early life stages in affecting the different components of the antipredator escape responses in the Trinidadian guppy (Poecilia reticulata).
Guppies shoal immediately after birth (Magurran et al., 1994). These early stages are important for the establishment and reinforcement of individual discrimination and familiarity in guppies (Barbosa, Camacho-Cervantes & Ojanguren, 2016; Barbosa, Ojanguren & Magurran, 2013; Chapman et al., 2008; Chapman, Ward & Krause, 2008; Laland, Brown & Krause, 2003). Within group familiarity is likely to affect how a group of individuals respond to a potential predator. Guppies respond to a predator attack by performing a “fast-start” escape response, characteristic to most fish species (Dial, Reznick & Brainerd, 2016). This evasion tactic consists of an unambiguous quick and sudden burst of swimming activity usually of only tenths of a second that propels the fish away from an oncoming predator (Domenici & Blake, 1997; Fuiman, Meekan & McCormick, 2010; Webb, 1978; Weihs, 1973). Fast-start escape responses integrate a combination of behavioural and kinematic components (Marras et al., 2011), both of which were examined in this study.
In view of the antipredator benefits of familiarity, we predicted that juvenile guppies are also more responsive and perform more successful escape responses when in groups of familiar conspecifics. To test this prediction, we exposed familiar and unfamiliar groups of juvenile guppies to a digital display of a looming object and quantified the difference in responsiveness (number of fish responding), reactive distance (based on the size of the stimulus when the response started) and magnitude of the escape response (maximum speed and acceleration achieved during the response, and distance covered by the escaping fish). This approach allows us to identify the role of familiarity in a behaviour closely related to survival during early life stages and to pinpoint which components of an escape response are more likely to be affected by social experience.
Methods
All guppies used were 8th generation descendants of individuals collected from the Lower sections of the Tacarigua River in Trinidad. Several species of fish predators have been reported in this locality including the pike cichlid (Crenicichla alta), the blue acara (Aequidens pulcher) and the wolf fish (Hoplias malabaricus), which also prey intensively on juvenile guppies (Magurran et al., 1994). Experimental fish were housed, and all observations recorded, at the aquarium facility at the Sir Harold Mitchell Building, University of St Andrews, UK. The aquarium has an air temperature control system, which kept the tank temperatures at a mean (±SD) temperature of 24.5 °C (± 0.3 °C). All stock tanks contained similar numbers of males, females and juveniles. Lighting conditions followed a 12-hour light/dark cycle. All fish were fed daily with TetraMin® flake food. Our experimental design was examined by the Biology School Ethics Committee from the University of St Andrews and declared our study to be exempted of Animal Ethics approval.
Test fish collection and rearing
Prior to the experiment, we collected three juveniles from three different stock tanks (60 × 40 × 40 cm) that contained a mix of males, females and juveniles using a dip net (there are 15 Lower Tacarigua stock tanks in at the University of St Andrews aquarium facility). This ensured that the test groups were composed neither of familiar conspecifics nor of close kin. Further, in all stock tanks there are large and smaller boulders and java moss, which allows a more natural environment for guppies. Immature juvenile guppies (i.e., age between five and six weeks) were allocated to a holding tank (20 × 22 × 30 cm) a to create a test group. Each test group was composed of three individuals. A total of 42 holding tanks were used. Black plastic sheets were placed between each tank to ensure each test group was visually isolated from adjacent groups. Fish were of similar size and randomly distributed between holding tanks (mean (±SD) 10.8 (± 1.7) mm). Nevertheless, in order to be able to identify each individual during tracking, test groups were carefully constituted of different sized individuals. Each test group remained in its holding tank for two weeks to ensure the establishment of familiarity between tank mates (Griffiths & Magurran, 1997a).
Escape response trials
We split the juveniles into two treatments: a familiar and an unfamiliar. Each group was composed of three juveniles (a total 42 groups, 21 familiar and 21 unfamiliar). Each day we tested six groups, three groups with familiar individuals and three of unfamiliar individuals. In familiar groups, individuals were tested with those fish that they shared the holding tank with for two weeks prior to testing. For unfamiliar groups, we took three fish, each from a different holding tank so they had not seen each other before, and put them together in the observation chamber for testing (Fig. 1). Unfamiliar groups were treated as a control. Each group was only tested once.
All tests occurred between 9:00 and 11:00 am and at least an hour after being fed. These measures were taken to avoid differences in satiation rate and time of day that might affect the behaviour of the individuals. The experimental setup used to assess escape response was based on an established protocol (Fuiman, Meekan & McCormick, 2010), but modified for this experiment (Fig. 2). Each trial involved presenting a digital display of a looming object to a test group. The digital display consists 1.8-second sequence showing black oval in the middle of a white background that increases its size to simulate an approaching object (Supplemental Information). The same stimulus has been shown to elicit a startle response in larval fish of similar size (Fuiman et al., 2006; Ojanguren & Fuiman, 2010). The video was presented using a LCD screen (Braun 1210) located 0.23 cm from a 10 × 10 × 10 cm glass test chamber. Water depth within the observation chamber was kept at 225 ml to minimise vertical movement in escape responses. For each trial, a test group was transported to the observation chamber one individual at a time and given at least 10 min of acclimatisation to their new surroundings before testing began. Each individual fish was only tested once. After the terminus of the trials the individuals were returned to a stock tank and were not reused in the experiment.
Individual response to the visual stimulus was recorded at 240 frames s−1 using a high-speed video camera (Casio EX-FH25 EXILM) through a 45°-angled mirror to obtain an overhead view of the observation chamber. The observation chamber sat on top of a black surface and was illuminated by lamps positioned left and right of the chamber so that the response could be clearly observed. All individuals tested were transferred to a small petri dish with a small amount of water and photographed from above. Individual standard length was measured to nearest millimetre using ImageJ analysis software (Abràmofff, Magalhães & Ram, 2004). All tested individuals resumed normal routine swimming activity immediately after the escape responses. No fish died during the tests, or after the picture was taken. After the terminus of the study, all individuals were returned to stock tanks.
Data analysis
Video recordings were analysed frame by frame to determine responsiveness (the number of fish that responded to the stimulus in each test group) and the reactive distance (the virtual distance between the looming object and the first individual that responded, calculated from the size of the oval on the screen at the moment of the start of the response and the distance of the fish from the screen) (see Fuiman, Meekan & McCormick, 2010 for details). The position of the fish in 2-dimensional coordinates for the overhead view was obtained using the manual tracking plugin in ImageJ (Cordelières, 2005), this allowed us to calculate maximum speed, maximum acceleration and total distance covered in the response (see Fuiman, Meekan & McCormick, 2010; Fuiman et al., 2006).
Statistical analysis
Differences in responsiveness between familiar and unfamiliar groups were tested with a Wilcoxon rank sum test to account for the fact that responsiveness was a discrete variable. The responsiveness of each test group was ranked according to the number of individuals within the group that responded (either 0, 1, 2 or 3). We considered that the response was over when the distance travelled between three consecutive frames (12.5 ms) was 1 mm or less.
In order to investigate the effect of familiarity on reactive distance and in the magnitude of the response (maximum speed, maximum acceleration and distance covered in a response) we used Generalised Linear Fixed Effect Models (GLM). Reactive distance, maximum speed, maximum acceleration and distance travelled during a response were only measured on the first fish that responded. On the only trial that two fish responded in the same frame, the fish that had the larger reactive distance was considered the first responder. Each group was only tested once. Each full model included familiarity as main effect treatment and standard length as a covariate (i.e., of the individual that first responded), as well as their interaction. The linear predictor and expected values scales were linked using a log function. Diagnostic plots revealed significant departures from normality of the residuals for both response variables reactive distance and total distance. Normality and homogeneity assumptions about the distributions of residual values on the dependent variable were improved by log-transforming the response variables. To account for the effect of size in escape responses, all models included individual standard length as covariate. All analyses were performed in using R (Team, 2016).
Results
Individual standard length between familiar and unfamiliar treatments did not differ (mean (± SD), Familiar = 117.3 (19.7); Unfamiliar = 122.2 (27.9), p = 0.089).
Responsiveness
A total of 42 groups composed of three different sized individuals were tested. Of the 30 groups in which one or more individuals responded, 19 groups were familiar and 16 groups were unfamiliar. There was a significant effect of familiarity on responsiveness (Wilcoxon rank sum: W = 451.5, p < 0.001) (Fig. 3), where responsiveness was higher in unfamiliar groups. In the majority of familiar groups only one individual in the group responded, whereas the unfamiliar groups showed more instances where two or more individuals reacted to the stimulus.
Reactive distance
We failed to detect an effect of familiarity and individual standard length on reactive distance (Table 1, Fig. 4A).
Table 1. Generalised linear models for testing the effect of familiarity on different qualitative measures of response.
Response variable | Explanatory variable | Estimate | Std Error Sq | T-value | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reactive distance | Intercept | 2.526 | 1.819 | 1.389 | 0.175 |
Unfamiliar | 1.985 | 2.879 | 0.690 | 0.496 | |
Length | 0.069 | 0.142 | 0.488 | 0.629 | |
Unfamiliar: length | −0.178 | 0.243 | −0.737 | 0.467 | |
Maximum speed | Intercept | 175.7 | 269.9 | 0.651 | 0.519 |
Familiar | −487.8 | 427.2 | −1.140 | 0.263 | |
Length | 44.14 | 21.16 | 2.086 | 0.045 | |
Familiar: length | 37.91 | 36.06 | 1.051 | 0.301 | |
Maximum acceleration | Intercept | −147.6 | 4,846 | −0.003 | 0.997 |
Familiar | 2,989 | 7,671 | 0.390 | 0.699 | |
Length | 6,909 | 3,799 | 1.819 | 0.078 | |
Familiar: length | −3032 | 6,473 | −0.468 | 0.642 | |
Total distance | Intercept | 0.699 | 0.908 | 0.771 | 0.447 |
Familiar | 0.562 | 1.437 | 0.391 | 0.698 | |
Length | 0.194 | 0.071 | 2.733 | 0.010 | |
Familiar: length | −0.055 | 0.121 | −0.454 | 0.653 |
Magnitude of the response
We failed to detect an effect of social treatment on maximum speed (p = 0.263), maximum acceleration (p = 0.699) and total distance (p = 0.698) (Table 1, Fig. 4). For maximum acceleration the effect of individual standard length was similar between treatments (p = 0.078). There was an increased in both maximum speed and total distance travelled as a function on increased standard length (Figs. 4C, 4D, Table 1). Notably, we observed an almost twice-greater gradient in the familiar treatment than in the unfamiliar treatment in terms of maximum speed. There was an increase of 82.05 ms in maximum speed per millimetre of standard size in the familiar treatment, while in the unfamiliar treatment the gradient was of 44.14 ms per millimetre of standard size (Table 1, Fig. 4B).
Discussion
A novel contribution of this study is that it examines the consequences of familiarity during early stages in the performance of escape responses separating the multiple aspects of the response to determine which parts depend on the social environment. Through high-speed analysis of the escape responses in familiar and unfamiliar groups of guppies, we demonstrate that early social experience plays a role in shaping how groups of fish respond to a stimulus. Namely, we showed that unfamiliar groups had more individuals perform an escape response than those in familiar groups. Unexpectedly, other components of the escape response, namely latency and magnitude, were not affected by familiarity. Furthermore, the maximum speed and distance covered in the response were correlated with individual size rather than with level of familiarity within the group. In combination, our study suggests, that familiarity plays a less meaningful role in determining some behavioural components of the escape response.
Our results show that familiarity affects group responsiveness. There were a greater number of individuals responding within each group among unfamiliar groups than among familiar groups (Supplemental Information). While most fish species rely on the escape response to avoid a potential predator (Domenici, 2010; Fuiman & Magurran, 1994), escape responses may however vary within and among individuals (Lima & Dill, 1990; Ward & Webster, 2016; Ydenberg & Dill, 1986). If there is enough information to accurately predict the level of threat in a given environment, then it is advantageous for a prey to only flee when it is necessary for survival avoiding false alarms that could in turn attract the attention of nearby predators (Ward et al., 2011). For example, minnows performed antipredator behaviours in response to a realistic pike model, whereas an unrealistic stimulus elicited no response (Magurran & Girling, 1986). The lower responsiveness in familiar groups may be a result of improved vigilance. According to the theory of limited attention, performance is reduced when attention must be divided among different tasks (Dukas, 2002). Therefore, if individuals are not spending time inspecting or acting aggressively toward group mates, as is often found among unfamiliar individuals (Griffiths et al., 2004; Johnsson, 1997; Tanner & Keller, 2012), then they are likely to have more time to dedicate to other tasks, such as predator vigilance (Strodl & Schausberger, 2012; Strodl & Schausberger, 2013; Zach et al., 2012). Guppies from familiar groups may have been able to accurately assess the non-threatening nature of the stimulus. An alternative explanation is that fish in familiar groups feel safer as they are with individuals they have seen before and this may be why familiar individuals are more likely to perceive the oval shape stimulus as non-threatening. On the other hand, unfamiliar groups may have been more skittish and thus more likely to be startled by the stimulus. Interacting with unfamiliar individuals can be stressful (Choleris et al., 1998), particularly if such interactions are associated with increased aggression (Galef, Kennett & Wigmore, 1984). Individuals may perceive higher risk when shoaling with unfamiliar conspecifics, as was found in fathead minnows who had a higher production of epidermal alarm substance cells when in unfamiliar shoals than familiar shoals (Wisenden & Smith, 1998). Furthermore, escape responses from the digital display may be misinterpreted as an attack by the other group mates. Aggression is common among guppies, in both natural as well as laboratory conditions (Magurran, 2005; Thibault, 1974). Therefore, it is plausible that an individual guppy would flee from an unfamiliar group mate that is performing a fast-start response, as this could be misinterpreted as an attack.
We failed to detect an effect of familiarity on the reactive distance of an escape response. Comparable studies have found that familiarity reduces the latency of an escape response. Similarly, familiar juvenile brown trout responded 14% faster than unfamiliar ones when exposed to a simulated predator attack (Griffiths et al., 2004). In both their and our study, reduction in reaction time has been attributed to the associated benefits of the theory of limited attention. Our results, therefore, suggest that familiarity is more important in antipredator behaviours earlier in a predator sequence. A predator must successfully encounter, attack and capture a prey, where a prey’s strategy is to interrupt this sequence. It has been suggested that avoiding the encounter and attack, are prey’s best strategy (Fuiman & Magurran, 1994). Previous experiments included an entire predator interaction, such as a model heron swinging forward and plunging its beak into the water (Griffiths et al., 2004) or a live predator (Strodl & Schausberger, 2012), and could, therefore elicit such behaviours. In contrast, our experiment only elicited behaviours seen in the last few milliseconds of the attack.
Familiarity has been found to enhance avoidance tactics. For example, predator confusion was enhanced in shoals of familiar fathead minnows that had reduced neighbour distance and more shoal cohesion in response to predator stimuli compared to unfamiliar shoals (Chivers, Brown & Smith, 1995). Tighter shoal cohesion reduces the probability of being captured by a predator (Mathis & Smith, 1993). In addition, familiar shoals exhibited a greater number of predator inspections with more inspectors per inspection when faced with a model pike (Chivers, Brown & Smith, 1995). Predator inspection, where an individual or small group of individuals approach a predator, pause and swim away (Pitcher, 1991), enables prey to gain valuable information on the threat of a predator. This behaviour, though risky to inspectors, is associated with improved avoidance of a predator attack (Godin & Davis, 1995; Magurran, 1990; Magurran & Pitcher, 1987). Therefore, it is likely that familiarity is more crucial in antipredator behaviour associated with predator avoidance than predator evasion.
The effect of familiarity on the magnitude of the response was not significant. The kinematic aspects of escape responses are often assumed to be constrained by the sensory-motor system of the individual (Domenici & Blake, 1997). However, juvenile guppies reared in an environment with intense social aggression travelled a greater distance in the first five frames after a simulated avian attack than those reared in absence of social aggression (Chapman et al., 2008). It is then recognized the need to implement an integrative approach that accounts for all aspects of an escape response in order to obtain a clear understanding of the mechanisms of response to a predator (Domenici, 2010). While other behavioural variables may affect the magnitude of an escape response, our study provides evidence that familiarity is not one of them. Our results showed that size, rather than familiarity, influenced the magnitude of the response than familiarity. This result is consistent with previous studies that have shown that the magnitude of the fast-start response in young fish increases with body length (Dial, Reznick & Brainerd, 2016). While behavioural effects on the locomotive performance cannot be ruled out (Domenici, 2010), our study and others (Gibb et al., 2006; Ojanguren & Braña, 2003) lend strong support that the magnitude of a fast-start response is largely determined by morphology, rather than by social conditions.
In this study, we provided a test for the relative effect of familiarity in modulating predator avoidance behaviour by measuring several components of the escape responses using high speed video analysis. Our results suggest that familiar groups may have improved antipredator performance, as individuals conserve energy and are less conspicuous by not fleeing in a non-threatening situation. Nevertheless, further studies are necessary to elucidate this. Future studies may try to tease the contribution of group size and familiarity in modulating the predator escape response, by testing familiar and non-familiar groups composed of different number of individuals. Our results also suggest that the effects of familiarity on the response are perhaps unlikely to play a role on escape performance in the last few milliseconds of a predator attack. Instead, we believe that familiarity is more likely to affect behaviour earlier in a predator–prey interaction, which then affects the quality of the response.
Supplemental Information
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Maria Dornelas, Anne Magurran and Mike Webster for providing helpful comments on early drafts. We are also thankful to Maria Joao Janeiro for helping with the statistics and to the Biodiversity and Behaviour Group for the rewarding discussions and comments throughout this research.
Funding Statement
This study was funded by a Postdoctoral fellowship to Miguel Barbosa (SFRH/BPD/82259/2011). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare there are no competing interests.
Author Contributions
Hayley L. Wolcott conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables.
Alfredo F. Ojanguren and Miguel Barbosa conceived and designed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Animal Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
Approval was provided by the University of St. Andrews Animal Welfare and Ethics Committee (2015). The review panel declared no need to obtain Animal Ethics approval.
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data supporting this publication can be accessed at
http://dx.doi.org/10.17630/92831d81-38f0-4573-b2e5-e1d11adf9322.
References
- Abràmofff, Magalhães & Ram (2004).Abràmofff MD, Magalhães PJ, Ram SJ. Image processing with ImageJ. Biophotonics International. 2004;11:36–43. [Google Scholar]
- Barber & Wright (2001).Barber I, Wright HA. How strong are familiarity preferences in shoaling fish? Animal Behaviour. 2001;61:975–979. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1665. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barbosa, Camacho-Cervantes & Ojanguren (2016).Barbosa M, Camacho-Cervantes M, Ojanguren AF. Phenotype matching and early social conditions affect shoaling and exploration decisions. Ethology. 2016;122:171–179. doi: 10.1111/eth.12455. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barbosa, Ojanguren & Magurran (2013).Barbosa M, Ojanguren AF, Magurran AE. Courtship display persists despite early social deprivation. Ethology. 2013;16:496–502. doi: 10.1111/eth.12087. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Braithwaite & Salvanes (2005).Braithwaite VA, Salvanes AGV. Environmental variability in the early rearing environment generates behaviourally flexible cod: implications for rehabilitating wild populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2005;272:1107–1113. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman et al. (2008).Chapman BB, Morrell LJ, Benton TG, Krause J. Early interactions with adults mediate the development of predator defenses in guppies. Behavioral Ecology. 2008;19:87–93. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arm111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman, Ward & Krause (2008).Chapman BB, Ward AJW, Krause J. Schooling and learning: early social environment predicts social learning ability in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Animal Behaviour. 2008;76:923–929. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.03.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chivers, Brown & Smith (1995).Chivers DP, Brown GE, Smith RJF. Familiarity and shoal cohesion in fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas): implications for antipredator behaviour. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1995;73:955–960. doi: 10.1139/z95-111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Choleris et al. (1998).Choleris E, Valsecchi P, Wang Y, Ferrari P, Kavaliers M, Mainardi M. Social learning of a food preference in male and female mongolian gerbils is facilitated by the anxiolytic, chlordiazepoxide. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1998;60:575–584. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00005-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coffin, Watters & Mateo (2011).Coffin HR, Watters JV, Mateo JM. Odor-based recognition of familiar and related conspecifics: a first test conducted on captive humboldt penguins (Spheniscus humboldti) PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e25002. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordelières (2005).Cordelières FP. Manual tracking. 2005. https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/plugins/track/track.html https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/plugins/track/track.html
- Cushing (1974).Cushing DH. The possible density-dependence of larval mortality and adult mortality in fishes. In: Blaxter J, editor. The early life history of fish. Springer-Verlag; New York: 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Dial, Reznick & Brainerd (2016).Dial TR, Reznick DN, Brainerd EL. Effects of neonatal size on maturity and escape performance in the Trinidadian guppy. Functional Ecology. 2016;30:943–952. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12565. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Domenici (2010).Domenici P. Context-dependent variability in the components of fish escape response: integrating locomotor performance and behavior. Journal of Experimental Zoology Part A: Ecological Genetics and Physiology. 2010;313A:59–79. doi: 10.1002/jez.580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domenici & Blake (1997).Domenici P, Blake R. The kinematics and performance of fish fast-start swimming. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1997;200:1165–1178. doi: 10.1242/jeb.200.8.1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dugatkin & Alfieri (1991).Dugatkin LA, Alfieri M. Guppies and the TIT FOR TAT strategy: preference based on past interaction. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1991;28:243–246. doi: 10.1007/bf00175096. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dugatkin & Godin (1992).Dugatkin LA, Godin J-GJ. Prey approaching predators: a cost-benefit perspective. Annales Zoologici Fennici. 1992;29:233–252. [Google Scholar]
- Dukas (2002).Dukas R. Behavioural and ecological consequences of limited attention. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2002;357:1539–1547. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Figueroa et al. (2013).Figueroa J, Solà-Oriol D, Manteca X, Pérez JF. Social learning of feeding behaviour in pigs: effects of neophobia and familiarity with the demonstrator conspecific. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2013;148:120–127. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fuiman & Cowan (2003).Fuiman LA, Cowan JH. Behavior and recruitment success in fish larvae: repeatability and covariation of survival skills. Ecology. 2003;84:53–67. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2003)084[0053:BARSIF]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fuiman & Magurran (1994).Fuiman LA, Magurran AE. Development of predator defences in fishes. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries. 1994;4:145–183. doi: 10.1007/bf00044127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fuiman, Meekan & McCormick (2010).Fuiman LA, Meekan MG, McCormick MI. Maladaptive behavior reinforces a recruitment bottleneck in newly settled fishes. Oecologia. 2010;164:99–108. doi: 10.1007/s00442-010-1712-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuiman et al. (2006).Fuiman LA, Rose KA, Cowan JH, Smith EP. Survival skills required for predator evasion by fish larvae and their relation to laboratory measures of performance. Animal Behaviour. 2006;71:1389–1399. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.11.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Galef, Kennett & Wigmore (1984).Galef BG, Kennett DJ, Wigmore SW. Transfer of information concerning distant foods in rats: a robust phenomenon. Animal Learning & Behavior. 1984;12:292–296. doi: 10.3758/bf03199970. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gibb et al. (2006).Gibb AC, Swanson BO, Wesp H, Landels C, Liu C. Development of the escape response in teleost fishes: do ontogenetic changes enable improved performance? Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 2006;79:7–19. doi: 10.1086/498192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Godin & Davis (1995).Godin J-GJ, Davis SA. Who dares, benefits: predator approach behaviour in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) deters predator pursuit. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 1995;259:193–200. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0028. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grabowska-Zhang, Sheldon & Hinde (2012).Grabowska-Zhang AM, Sheldon BC, Hinde CA. Long-term familiarity promotes joining in neighbor nest defence. Biology Letters. 2012;8:544–546. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabowska-Zhang, Wilkin & Sheldon (2011).Grabowska-Zhang AM, Wilkin TA, Sheldon BC. Effects of neighbor familiarity on reproductive success in the great tit (Parus major) Behavioral Ecology. 2011;23:322–333. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0183. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths (2003).Griffiths SW. Learned recognition of conspecifics by fishes. Fish and Fisheries. 2003;4:256–268. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-2979.2003.00129.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths et al. (2004).Griffiths SW, Brockmark S, Höjesjö J, Johnsson JI. Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2004;271:695–699. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths & Magurran (1997a).Griffiths SW, Magurran AE. Familiarity in schooling fish: how long does it take to acquire? Animal Behaviour. 1997a;53:945–949. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0315. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths & Magurran (1997b).Griffiths SW, Magurran AE. Schooling preferences for familiar fish vary with group size in a wild guppy population. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 1997b;264:547–551. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0078. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Höjesjö et al. (1998).Höjesjö J, Johnsson JI, Petersson E, Järvi T. The importance of being familiar: individual recognition and social behavior in sea trout (Salmo trutta) Behavioral Ecology. 1998;9:445–451. doi: 10.1093/beheco/9.5.445. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Johnsson (1997).Johnsson JI. Individual recognition affects aggression and dominance relations in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Ethology. 1997;103:267–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1997.tb00017.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krause & Ruxton (2002).Krause J, Ruxton GD. Evolution OSiEa. Oxford University Press; New York: 2002. Living in groups; p. 228. [Google Scholar]
- Laland, Brown & Krause (2003).Laland KN, Brown C, Krause J. Learning in fishes: from three-second memory to culture. Fish and Fisheries. 2003;4:199–202. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-2979.2003.00124.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lima & Dill (1990).Lima SL, Dill LM. Behavioural decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1990;68:619–640. doi: 10.1139/z90-092. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Magurran (1990).Magurran AE. The adaptive significance of schooling as an anti-predator defence in fish. Annales Zoologici Fennici. 1990;27:51–66. [Google Scholar]
- Magurran (2005).Magurran AE. Evolutionary ecology: the Trinidadian guppy. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Magurran & Girling (1986).Magurran AE, Girling SL. Predator model recognition and response habituation in shoaling minnows. Animal Behaviour. 1986;34:510–518. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80119-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Magurran & Pitcher (1987).Magurran AE, Pitcher TJ. Provenance, shoal size and the sociobiology of predator-evasion behaviour in minnow shoals. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences. 1987;229:439–465. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1987.0004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Magurran et al. (1994).Magurran AE, Seghers BH, Shaw PW, Carvalho GR. Schooling preferences for familiar fish in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Journal of Fish Biology. 1994;45:401–406. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.1994.tb01322.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Marras et al. (2011).Marras S, Killen SS, Claireaux G, Domenici P, McKenzie DJ. Behavioural and kinematic components of the fast-start escape response in fish: individual variation and temporal repeatability. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2011;214:3102–3110. doi: 10.1242/jeb.056648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathis & Smith (1993).Mathis A, Smith RJF. Chemical alarm signals increase the survival time of fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) during encounters with northern pike (Esox Lucius) Behavioral Ecology. 1993;4:260–265. doi: 10.1093/beheco/4.3.260. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ojanguren & Braña (2003).Ojanguren AF, Braña F. Effects of size and morphology on swimming performance in juvenile brown trout (Salmo trutta L.) Ecology of Freshwater Fish. 2003;12:241–246. doi: 10.1046/j.1600-0633.2003.00016.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ojanguren & Fuiman (2010).Ojanguren AF, Fuiman LA. Seasonal variability in antipredator performance of red drum larvae. Marine Ecology Progress Series. 2010;413:117–123. doi: 10.3354/meps08697. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pitcher (1991).Pitcher T. Who dares, wins: the function and evolution of predator inspection behaviour in shoaling fish. Netherlands Journal of Zoology. 1991;42:371–391. doi: 10.1163/156854291X00397. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reby et al. (2001).Reby D, Hewison M, Izquierdo M, Dominique P. Red deer (Cervus elaphus) hinds discriminate between the roars of their current harem-holder stag and those of neighbouring stags. Ethology. 2001;107:951–959. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00732.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ruxton & Johnsen (2016).Ruxton GD, Johnsen S. The effect of aggregation on visibility in open water. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2016;283 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.1463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strodl & Schausberger (2012).Strodl MA, Schausberger P. Social familiarity reduces reaction times and enhances survival of group-living predatory mites under the risk of predation. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e43590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strodl & Schausberger (2013).Strodl MA, Schausberger P. Social familiarity relaxes the constraints of limited attention and enhances reproduction of group-living predatory mites. Oikos. 2013;122:1217–1226. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2012.20833.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swaney et al. (2001).Swaney W, Kendal J, Capon H, Brown C, Laland KN. Familiarity facilitates social learning of foraging behaviour in the guppy. Animal Behaviour. 2001;62:591–598. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1788. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tanner & Keller (2012).Tanner CJ, Keller L. Nest distribution varies with dispersal method and familiarity-mediated aggression for two sympatric ants. Animal Behaviour. 2012;84:1151–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Team (2016).Team RDC. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Thibault (1974).Thibault RE. Genetics of cannibalism in a viviparous fish and its relationship to population density. Nature. 1974;251:138–140. doi: 10.1038/251138a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vilhunen & Hirvonen (2003).Vilhunen S, Hirvonen H. Innate antipredator responses of Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) depend on predator species and their diet. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2003;55:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00265-003-0670-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ward & Hart (2003).Ward AJW, Hart PJB. The effects of kin and familiarity on interactions between fish. Fish and Fisheries. 2003;4:348–358. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-2979.2003.00135.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ward et al. (2011).Ward AJW, Herbert-Read JE, Sumpter DJT, Krause J. Fast and accurate decisions through collective vigilance in fish shoals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108:2312–2315. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007102108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward & Webster (2016).Ward AJW, Webster M. Sociality: the behaviour of group-living animals. Springer International Publishing; Cham: 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Webb (1978).Webb PW. Fast-start performance and body form in seven species of teleost fish. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1978;74:211–226. [Google Scholar]
- Weihs (1973).Weihs D. The mechanism of rapid starting of slender fish. Biorheology. 1973;10:343–350. doi: 10.3233/bir-1973-10308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weihs & Webb (1984).Weihs D, Webb PW. Optimal avoidance and evasion tactics in predator–prey interactions. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1984;106:189–206. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(84)90019-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wisenden & Smith (1998).Wisenden BD, Smith RJF. A re-evaluation of the effect of shoalmate familiarity on the proliferation of alarm substance cells in ostariophysan fishes. Journal of Fish Biology. 1998;53:841–846. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.1998.tb01837.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ydenberg & Dill (1986).Ydenberg RC, Dill LM. The economics of fleeing from predators. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 1986;16:229–249. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60192-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zach et al. (2012).Zach GJ, Peneder S, Strodl MA, Schausberger P. Social familiarity governs prey patch-exploitation, - leaving and inter-patch distribution of the group-living predatory mite Phytoseiulus persimilis. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e42889. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zajitschek & Brooks (2008).Zajitschek SRK, Brooks RC. Distinguishing the effects of familiarity, relatedness, and color pattern rarity on attractiveness and measuring their effects on sexual selection in guppies (Poecilia reticulata) The American Naturalist. 2008;172:843–854. doi: 10.1086/593001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data supporting this publication can be accessed at
http://dx.doi.org/10.17630/92831d81-38f0-4573-b2e5-e1d11adf9322.