Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2017 Mar 25;38:6–18. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.03.004

Behavioral comparisons in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Coordination Disorder: A systematic literature review

Priscila Caçola 1, Haylie L Miller 2, Peace Ossom Williamson 3
PMCID: PMC5646683  NIHMSID: NIHMS863174  PMID: 29057009

Abstract

Background

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) are developmental disorders that, since the DSM-5, can be diagnosed as co-occurring conditions. While some recent studies suggest that ASD and DCD have similar traits, others show clear behavioral distinctions between the two conditions. By gathering all studies that included (1) an ASD group and a DCD group, (2) an ASD+DCD group and a DCD group, or (3) ASD, ASD+DCD, and DCD groups, we aimed to identify similarities and differences in behaviors between the two disorders.

Method

We used a systematic search of PubMed (1946 –), Scopus (1970 –), PsycINFO (via EBSCO, 1600 –), CINAHL (via EBSCO, 1937 –), SportDiscus (via EBSCO, 1985 –), and WorldCat (via FirstSearch) in addition to reference list and author name searching PubMed, Scopus, PsycINFO, CINAHL, SportDiscus, and WorldCat to identify original studies that met the following criteria: (1) an ASD group and a DCD group, (2) an ASD+DCD group and a DCD group, or (3) ASD, ASD+DCD, and DCD groups.

Results

From the 1,598 articles screened, 11 were included in the qualitative analysis. The articles included reported more differences than similarities in individuals with ASD and DCD, with clear distinctions for working memory ability, gestural performance, grip selection, and cortical thickness. Only two studies reported similarities in face processing abilities and perceived competence, and the interventional studies showed group similarities in behavior improvement, such as intelligence and attention.

Conclusions

Based on the articles reviewed, we conclude that while DCD and ASD share some behavioral symptoms, the symptom profiles of each disorder are unique and separable. We recommend that the evaluation of potential DCD in individuals with ASD be performed systematically and thoroughly, so as to distinguish this co-occurring condition from sensorimotor symptoms associated with ASD.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder, developmental coordination disorder, behaviors, co-morbidities, cooccurrence, motor skills, systematic literature review


Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) are developmental disorders with distinct definitions and diagnostic criteria. ASD is currently diagnosed based on symptoms in two core domains: difficulty with social interaction/communication, and restricted interests and repetitive behaviors. DCD, on the other hand, is characterized by significant difficulty performing motor skills at an age-appropriate level. Individuals with DCD experience substantial impact of these difficulties on everyday tasks as well as in social relationships (Leonard & Hill, 2014). Due to recent changes in the Diagnostic and Static Manual for Mental Disorders–Fifth Edition (DSM-5) in 2013, these two conditions can be diagnosed as co-occurring in certain individuals (e.g., children with ASD with significant difficulties in motor skills can also be diagnosed with DCD). However, questions remain on behavioral similarities and differences in profiles of individuals that have ASD, individuals that have DCD, and individuals diagnosed with both conditions, in motor behavior as well as in other domains. Therefore, this review aimed to investigate similarities and differences of behaviors in children diagnosed with DCD, ASD, or both at the same time. This examination has important clinical implications, given the increasing attention paid to sensorimotor features of ASD (e.g., Whyatt & Craig, 2013; Donnellan, Hill, & Leary, 2013; Fournier et al., 2010). It is crucial to determine which behavioral and motor challenges are part of the core symptom profile of ASD, and whether they are fundamentally different in a sub-set of individuals with ASD and co-occurring DCD (hereafter abbreviated as ASD+DCD).

DCD is characterized by difficulties in performing accurate and fast gross- and fine-motor skills, including problems with coordination and balance. The movements of children with DCD frequently lead to performance difficulties in activities of daily living and physical games that typically-developing (TD) children perform easily. Many researchers use the terms “dyspraxia” and DCD interchangeably. However, some have argued that dyspraxia is a condition specifically impacting the ability to plan unfamiliar motor tasks (Bundy, Lane, & Murray, 2002), or to perform skilled gestures (Dziuk et al., 2007). For the purpose of this discussion, we consider these two conditions similar in their clinical characteristics, as indicated by the European Academy of Childhood Disability (EACD) guidelines (Blank, Smits-Engelslman, Polatajko, & Wilson, 2012).

DCD has been described as a “hidden problem” (Gibbs, Appleton, & Appleton, 2007), with an estimated prevalence as high as 10% in school-aged children. In general, estimates of 2% to 7% are more likely (APA, 2013), implying that most school classes have at least one affected child. The diagnostic process involves assessing motor skills, evaluating whether these skills affect daily living, determining whether there was an early onset of motor delays, and ensuring that the disturbance was not due to a general medical and/or neurological condition. Prior to the changes in the DSM-5, the diagnostic criteria for DCD included ruling out the presence of a co-occurring Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD): a category that, at the time, included Autistic Disorder (AD), Asperger's Syndrome (AS), Rett's Syndrome, and PDD-Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS). The most recent revision of the DSM eliminated this rule, making it possible for an individual to be diagnosed with co-occurring ASD+DCD.

In DSM-5, a single diagnostic code–Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)–replaces the earlier diagnoses of AD, AS, and PDD-NOS. ASD is a broad term used for a clinical population characterized by complex and often heterogeneous patterns of biological and behavioral symptoms, with shared features in the domains of communication and social interaction, and restricted interests and repetitive behaviors (APA, 2013). ASD affects a lower proportion of children than DCD, with prevalence estimates at 1 in 68 children (Christensen et al., 2016). In addition to the behaviors described above, motor impairments are a common symptom of ASD, and have been observed from the earliest descriptions of the disorder (Kanner, 1943). Ozonoff and colleagues (2008) and others have suggested that motor disturbances appear to be among the first manifestations of developmental abnormalities in ASD, and could serve as biomarkers of this condition in the first years of life before other core symptoms (i.e., social communication, restricted interests) can be reliably measured. Fournier et al. (2010) concluded after a systematic literature review and meta-analysis that motor coordination deficits are pervasive across the range of ASD-related diagnoses, and can be considered a cardinal feature of ASD. In general, several researchers suggest that when compared to typically-developing (TD) individuals, individuals with ASD also have significant motor praxis and coordination impairments, although they may not carry a formal diagnosis of dyspraxia or DCD (Dziuk et al., 2007; Downey & Rapport, 2012).

Neither gross- nor fine-motor impairments are an essential or defining characteristic of ASD in the current diagnostic framework (Green et al., 2002), despite the fact that they are recognized as a common feature. As a result, previous diagnostic criteria did not provide a means of differentiating between individuals with ASD who had motor problems and those who did not (Founder-Hughes & Prior, 2014). When evaluating potential DCD, Missiuna and Magalhães (2015) caution that clinicians must carefully evaluate criterion D of the DSM-5. That is, they must determine whether the motor deficits observed could be better explained by other disabilities or neurological condition, such as ASD. The authors explain that while this is open to interpretation, physical and occupational therapists will often be in the best position to determine whether (1) the motor challenges of a child with ASD are better explained by ASD, or (2) the motor difficulties reflect the co-occurrence of DCD (p. 94). However, little is known about the differences and similarities of behaviors that individuals with ASD and DCD demonstrate, and physical and occupational therapists receive limited training in differentiating between these two conditions.

Therefore, the aim of this systematic literature review (SLR) was to identify: (a) what types of behaviors (variables) have been investigated in ASD and DCD to determine similarities or differences between the conditions, and (b) whether any of the studies that met our criteria had a group of ASD+DCD or explored the possibility of co-occurrence between ASD and DCD.

Method

In order to identify separable and overlapping features of these two disorders, we conducted a SLR gathering all studies that investigated behavioral profiles of individuals diagnosed with ASD, DCD, and ASD+DCD (if included) and their derivatives. Because of the high rate of co-occurrence among neurodevelopmental conditions (Kaplan et al., 2001; Jongmans et al., 2003; Simonoff et al., 2008), we included papers that used groups with other conditions as well as TD individuals. The SLR process involves gathering all existing knowledge using a thorough and methodical approach and summarizing the best available research on a specific topic or question, in order to provide a repeatable method that reduces reporting bias.

This review follows the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) standards described by Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, and Altman (2009). A systematic search of all full-length studies was performed by an information specialist (POW) on November 9, 2015 (the unregistered protocol is available by request). The databases searched were PubMed (1946 –), Scopus (1970 –), PsycINFO (via EBSCO, 1600 –), CINAHL (via EBSCO, 1937 –), SportDiscus (via EBSCO, 1985 –), and WorldCat (via FirstSearch). The search terms and relevant subject headings for DCD were grouped together using the Boolean operator OR; search terms and subject headings for ASD were also grouped using OR. Both groups were contained within parentheses and combined with the AND Boolean operator to create the final search. In PubMed and Scopus, to exclude research on animals, the following string was added to the search with the NOT Boolean operator: (animal* OR rat OR rats OR mouse OR mice). Table 1 shows the search keywords by group.

Table 1. Search keywords by group.

DCD ASD
Subject Headingsa Keywordsb Subject Headingsa Keywordsb
“Motor Skills Disorders” “Developmental coordination disorder” “Autistic Disorder” Autistic
“Neurogenerative Diseases” DCD “Autism” Autism
“Agraphia” “Developmental co-ordination disorder” “Autistic Spectrum Disorders” Asperger's
“Dyspraxia” Dyspraxia Autistic Thinking Aspergers
“Psychomotor Disorders” Dyspraxic ASD
“Apraxia” Maladroitness “Pervasive developmental disorder”
“Movement Disorders” “Clumsy child” “Childhood disintegrative disorder”
Dyscoordination
Dys-coordination
Dysgraphia
“Minimal brain dysfunction”
“Sensorimotor difficulties”
“Sensorimotor dysfunction”
“Sensory integrative dysfunction”
“Psychomotor disorder”
“Perceptual motor dysfunction”
“Minimal cerebral dysfunction”
“Developmental right hemisphere syndrome”
“Minimal brain dysfunction”
“Minor neurological dysfunction”
a

Subject headings include MeSH in PubMed and CINAHL, SPORTDiscus, and PsycINFO headings.

b

Truncation (e.g., autis*) was used for keywords in Scopus, PyscINFO, CINAHL, and SPORTDiscus.

There were no publication date restrictions. Filters were used to limit our search to English-language studies of humans. To update the systematic review, the searches were repeated in each database on July 9, 2016 and November 3, 2016 and results were added to the aforementioned RefWorks account for the systematic review. Figure 1 illustrates the search process from identification and inclusion of articles. Authors' names and reference lists from the 120 studies remaining after primary screening were also used to find additional studies. The authors' names of these 120 studies were searched, including a review of faculty profile pages and author pages in WorldCat to identify any additional studies. One study was sent to the authors. A total of 1,598 articles were retrieved from database, author, and reference list searching. The metadata of the studies retrieved were imported and encoded in the RefWorks 2.0 web-based bibliographic program.

Figure 1. PRISMA Flowchart.

Figure 1

Criteria for Inclusion and Exclusion

We limited our review of the literature to studies that included at least two groups of individuals: (1) an ASD group and a DCD group, (2) an ASD+DCD group and a DCD group, or (3) ASD, ASD+DCD, and DCD groups. Some studies included additional groups, but provided that they met the previous criteria, they were included. Our goal was to compare the profile of these individuals as determined by each study. We included all relevant DSM-IV diagnostic categories (AS, AD, PDD-NOS). All outcome measures typically used in the assessment of behavioral symptom severity in DCD and ASD (e.g., fine-motor, gross-motor, and psychosocial variables) were included. Studies that did not include participants with ASD and participants with DCD were excluded, and review articles were excluded.

Study Selection and Quality Assessment

Through primary screening of the titles and abstracts of the retrieved studies, 120 studies remained. During secondary screening, two independent reviewers (PC and HM) evaluated the abstracts and full text of these 120 studies according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria. At the conclusion of secondary screening, 12 studies met criteria for inclusion. During this analysis, the reviewers agreed that one article should be removed from the final inclusion (Van Waelvelde et al., 2010). While the study aimed to investigate the stability of motor problems in a sample of children with or at risk for ASD, ADHD, and/or DCD, none of the participants were given a DCD diagnosis (a DCD diagnosis is not typically given before 6 years of age. Therefore, for this article, it was not possible to determine the stability of motor problems in the ASD and DCD groups.

Because we were not able to find a gold-standard tool that would give us a good quality assessment of the articles selected for this SLR, we adopted criteria based on the PRISMA standards that reflected the quality of the studies meeting the search criteria. These criteria included appropriate selection and description of participants, use of appropriate outcome measures, appropriate statistical analysis, consideration of confounding factors, and a discussion of implications and limitations of the findings. This resulted in a list of seven questions to be answered with a “yes” or “no” that were used for each one of the articles selected, showed in Table 2. Articles that scored a “no” in more in four or more of the questions were classified as ‘poor quality’, articles with a “no” for 3 questions were classified as ‘average quality’, and articles with two or less “no” answers were classified as ‘high quality’.

Table 2. Quality assessment.

Appropriate selection and description of participants
  1. Was there a clear description of participants?

  2. Was a diagnosis checked by further assessments?

  3. Were controls involved in the study?

Appropriate outcome measures
  • 4. Was there a clear description of the outcomes measured in the study?

Appropriate statistical analysis
  • 5. Were all the statistical measures described clearly?

Consideration of confounding factors
  • 6. Were confounding factors taken into account?

Appropriate discussion of implications and limitations
  • 7. Was a clear discussion considering implications and limitations of the study presented in the article?

Results

Upon final screening, a total of eleven articles reporting data spanning cognitive, motor, social, and multiple domains were acceptable for the SLR (full description of studies provided in Table 3), all of which were published in the last 10 years. These studies reported data from a total of 1,053 individuals between 3 and 20 years of age. Among these individuals, 295 were identified as DCD, 182 were diagnosed with ASD, and 12 were diagnosed with ASD+DCD. Six studies compared only ASD and DCD, four of which also included a TD group. As expected, only two recent studies involved a group of individuals diagnosed with ASD+DCD (Foulder-Hughes & Prior, 2014; Caeyenberghs et al., 2016), and one study discussed the possibility that DCD may be part of the autism spectrum (Wisdom et al., 2006). Five studies explored other groups in addition to DCD and ASD (Specific Language Impairment [SLI], Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder [ADHD], Down Syndrome [DS], Intellectual Disability [ID], Cerebral Palsy [CP], Receptive-Expressive Language Disorder [RELD], and typical development [TD]).

Table 3. Description of studies included.

Study Design Variable(s) Disorders (n) Group matching Age (years) Summary of findings Observations Quality
Alloway, Rajendran, and Archibald (2009) Cross-sectional Working memory (Automated Working Memory Assessment) SLI (15)
DCD (55)
ADHD (83)
AS (10)
Similar age
Children in SLI group did not show motor difficulties
8-11 DCD: noticeable visuospatial deficits
AS: only showed poor performance on verbal short-term memory
*Autism group had Aspergers High
Cosper et al. (2009) Interventional Attention (Vigilance task, Model III-R)
Motor skills (Bruininks– Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency-Short Form)
ADHD + PDD (2)
ADHD + DCD (10)
Similar age 6-13 All children made significant improvements in complex visual choice reaction time and visuomotor control after the training *Both groups had ADHD combined with another disorder, Autism was considered “PDD”, no comparison between groups Low
Dewey, Cantell, and Crawford (2007) Cross-sectional control group Motor skills (Bruininks– Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency-Short Form)
Gestural skills (Gestures Testa)
ASD (49)
DCD + ADHD (38)
ADHD (27)
DCD (46)
TD (78)
Similar age
All groups obtained IQ > 70 (except for 5 children in ASD group)
All groups were tested for ADHD
Most children with ASD scored low on motor functioning, all DCD did, and all TD obtained high scores
5-18 Children with ASD, DCD, and DCD+ADHD were significantly impaired on motor coordination skills
Only ASD showed a generalized impairment in gestural performance
*ASD group had PDD and AS High
Foulder-Hughes, and Prior (2014) Qualitative Perceptions about transition (Interviews) DCD + ASD (4)
DCD (1)
ASD (1)
Similar age 10-11 Children with DCD and/or ASD have anxiety related to Physical Education when transitioning schools *Qualitative study Average
Kozulin et al. (2010) Interventional Cognitive functioning (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Revised, Raven Coloured Matrices) DS (51)
Genetic Intellectual disorders (12)
Non-genetic Intellectual disorders (39)bASD (11)
CP (13)
DCD (45)
ADHD (2)
Other (15)
Similar age 5-7 (mental age) IE-basic is a valid program to enhance general cognitive functioning in children with learning disabilities and a mild to moderate intellectual impairment *Children recruited in different countries Low
Kinnealey et al. (2012) Interventional Attending behaviors (Videotape)
Sensory behaviors (The Sensory Profile)
Perceptions (Journaling)
ASD (3)
Dyspraxia (1)
No matching 13-20 Reduction in classroom sound as measured by a decibel meter after the installation of the Owens Corning Basement Finishing System *Findings are not clear Low
van Swieten et al. (2010) Cross sectional control group Motor and executive planning (Grip selection task with stimuli on computer screen) DCD (27)
ASD (20)
TD (70)
Similar age
DCD was ensured to have typical executive level planning, since ASD has problems with such tasks
5-14 DCD: Showed the predicted bias towards Minimal Rotation, and the proportion of children with DCD who showed this bias was higher than even the youngest group of TD
children
ASD: Performed identically to age-matched controls
*Clear difference between DCD and ASD groups High
Wisdom et al. (2007) Cross sectional Intelligence (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children)
Language (Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals)
Motor coordination (McCarron Assessment of Neuromuscular Development)
Social ability (Theory of mind tasks, Emotion Recognition Scales)
Executive functioning (Go/No-Go Task)
Working memory (Trailmaking/Me mory Updating and the Goal Neglect Task)
ASD (30)
RELD (30)
DCD (22)
Similar age 3-13 AD and DCD groups have poorer fine and gross motor coordination and better response inhibition than the RELD group
AD and DCD groups differ in fine and gross motor coordination, emotion understanding, and theory of mind scores (AD always lower)
*Discusses DCD as part of the autism diagnosis High
Sumner et al. (2016) Cross sectional control group Motor ability (Early motor abilities questionnaire, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales– gross and fine motor scales)
Social functioning (Benton Test of Facial Recognition, Face processing battery, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales – socialization domain)
ASD (30)
DCD (30)
TD (20)
Similar age
All groups had IQ > 70
TD group had did not meet criteria for DCD and ASD on assessments
7-10 Children with DCD and ASD were delayed in reaching early motor milestones (DCD more than ASD)
Children with ASD and DCD had similar fine- and gross motor skills
Children with ASD and DCD were worse on face processing measures compared to TD children
*Discusses the notion that DCD may have problems with social functioning High
Green et al. (2015) Cross sectional control group Self-perception of movement and social difficulties (Harter's Self- Perception Profile for Children)
Teacher's perception of movement and social difficulties (The Teacher Rating Scale)
Motor skills (Movement Assessment Battery for Children)
Social communication (Autistic Diagnostic Inventory-Revised)
AS (11)
DCD (9)
TD (20)
Similar age 6-10 Children with AS and DCD rated themselves as less competent than the TD group (especially in athletic and social domains, with a significant difference between AS and TD for the athletic domain)
Teachers significantly perceived both the groups with AS and DCD as less competent than the TD group
*Discuss the lack of formal diagnosis in the DCD group and the unusual good “ball skills” in this group as possible explanation for lower perception in AS group High
Caeyenberghs et al. (2016) Cross sectional control group Cortical thickness (MRI)
Motor ability (Movement Assessment Battery for Children)
Visuomotor integration (Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration)
ASD + DCD (8)
ASD (15)
DCD (11)
TD (19)
Similar age
All groups had IQ > 75
ADHD was excluded from all groups
8-12 ASD children showed increased normalized path length and higher values of clustering coefficient compared with TD and DCD
Children with ASD and DCD exhibited changes that were more widespread than those seen in children with only DCD
*Concludes that DCD and ASD are neurodevelopment al disorders with a low degree of overlap in abnormalities for cortical thickness, and the cooccurrence of DCD + ASD was associated with a distinct topological pattern High
a

Previously used by authors

Note: Acronyms stand for Developmental Coordination Disorder [DCD], Autism Spectrum Disorders [ASD], Specific Language Impairment [SLI], Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder [ADHD], Down Syndrome [DS], Cerebral Palsy [CP], Receptive-Expressive Language Disorder [RELD], and typical development [TD]).

The results of the quality assessment are presented in Table 3. Seven studies were classified as high quality, one was classified as average quality, and three were classified as poor quality. Most of the poor quality studies reflected a selection bias, lacking clear description of participants, assessment of the diagnosis, and a control group. Two studies were identified as interventional, and nine were cross-sectional comparisons among two or more groups. Two studies were qualitative. A wide range of variables were explored across the studies: cognitive ability including intelligence quotient (IQ), executive functioning, attention, and working memory; motor skills including grip selection; perception of motor and social competence; social skills including perceptions of school transitions and social challenges; and “general” abilities, including language, perceived competence, face processing, and cortical connectivity. We grouped the studies into categories (cognitive domain only, motor domain only, social domain only, and multiple domains) for a more cohesive presentation of the results.

Cognitive domain only

Two studies focused specifically on the cognitive domain, testing working memory performance (Alloway et al., 2009) and improvements in IQ following a cognitive intervention (Kozulin et al., 2010). Alloway and colleagues (2009) compared working memory performance across students with different developmental disorders (SLI, DCD, ADHD, and AS). Working memory was defined as the ability to simultaneously store and process information for a brief period. Children in the DCD, ADHD, and AS groups completed a working memory assessment for verbal and visuospatial aspects of short-term memory and working memory. Memory performance was distinct for each group: children with DCD had depressed performance in all areas, with particularly low scores in visuospatial memory tasks; and children with AS had a selective verbal short-term memory deficit. The authors concluded that memory is a secondary deficit shared across several different developmental disorders, which may be driven by overlap in core deficits.

Kozulin and colleagues (2010) tested the effectiveness of a cognitive intervention using a multi-center design involving several countries and a broad range of developmental disorders. The intervention was a cognitive enrichment program focusing on perceptual-motor development, decoding emotional expressions, and abstractive/integrative thinking. Scores in three areas of IQ and in nonverbal abilities improved from pre- to post-intervention. However, the impact of the intervention did not vary systematically by disorder.

Motor domain only

Three studies reported data related to the motor domain, including grip selection (van Swieten et al., 2010), fine- and gross-motor ability and production of gestures (Dewey et al., 2006), and the relationship between motor performance and cortical connectivity (Caeyenberghs et al., 2016).

van Swieten et al. (2010) used a goal-directed motor planning task to differentiate between motor planning and executive functioning ability in participants with DCD, ASD, and TD. They observed an age-related difference in motor plans motivated by end-state comfort (e.g., making a less comfortable initial grasp to turn an object, so as to end in a comfortable posture) for children with and without ASD. These results display a typical developmental trajectory in motor planning. While the ASD group performed identically to the TD group, children with DCD were biased towards selecting the simplest initial movement, which led to uncomfortable end-states after rotation. In addition, the proportion of children with DCD who showed a bias toward this simple initial movement was higher than even the youngest group of TD children. van Swieten and colleagues concluded that motor planning deficits in the DCD group were specifically related to their motor difficulties, rather than to deficits in executive functioning.

Dewey et al. (2006) evaluated motor and gestural performance with a motor proficiency assessment and the production of meaningful gestures to verbal command and through imitation. Their sample included children with ASD, DCD, ADHD, DCD+ADHD, and TD. Results indicated that the ASD, DCD, and DCD+ADHD groups had significantly impaired motor coordination; however, only children with ASD showed impairments in gestural performance. In addition, children with ASD made significantly more action and orientation errors during gestural performance in response to verbal command, and significantly more orientation and distortion errors during gestural imitation than children in the DCD, DCD+ADHD, ADHD, and TD groups. The authors concluded that the generalized impairment in gestural performance seen in children with ASD may be influenced by factors other than motor skills.

Caeyenberghs et al. (2016) recently used magnetic resonance imaging to link brain structure to behavioral motor performance and compare cortical thickness in children with DCD, ASD, ASD+DCD, and TD. Both the DCD and ASD+DCD groups performed worse on tests of motor ability and visuomotor integration. Structural architecture of the brain was significantly altered in children with ASD compared to DCD and TD. Children with DCD displayed global network organization that was notably similar to that of TD children, while children with ASD had key differences in their network parameters, namely increased normalized path length and higher values of clustering coefficient. Caeyenberghs and colleagues concluded that the patterns observed in the ASD group reflected an atypical–and as a result, potentially unbalanced and inefficient–network organization. Specifically, high clustering coefficients suggest overconnectivity, which may not be functional. Instead, this pattern of overconnectivity may result in diffuse processing and recruitment of nonspecialized neural mechanisms (Glazebrook & Wallace, 2015). The increased energetic cost required to transmit signals across a diffuse processing system may in turn decrease efficiency of processing (Bullmore & Sporns, 2009). With respect to specific network nodes, the DCD group showed increases in clustering coefficient in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex–a part of the expanded limbic system which is especially engaged during response inhibition (Elliot, Dolan, & Frith, 2000). On the other hand, children with ASD+DCD had more widespread deviations from typical patterns of cortical thickness than those seen in children with only DCD and only ASD, for example, alterations of clustering coefficient in (para)limbic regions, primary areas, and association areas (resulting in more pronounced behavioral and motor issues in this group).

Social domain only

While several studies involved variables linked to social communication (e.g., gesturing, language, face processing), only one study specifically examined children's perceptions of transition from primary to secondary school. Foulder-Hughes & Prior (2014) assessed these perceptions of the school environment in six students with ASD, DCD, and ASD+DCD using interviews and qualitative analysis. Four students had ASD+DCD, one student had DCD, and one student had ASD. Most notably, all students consistently reported worries related to performance in physical education class.

Multiple domains

Sumner et al. (2016) compared motor and face-processing difficulties in children with DCD, ASD, and TD, using a combination of lab assessment and parent reports. Parent reports indicated that children with DCD and ASD could be distinguished from TD on the basis of their early motor development, with a greater distinction between DCD and TD groups than between ASD and TD groups. The DCD and ASD groups were similar in fine- and gross-motor performance during assessment, and notably, half of the group with ASD met the cut-off for motor difficulties on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children – 2nd edition (MABC-2; Henderson, Sugden, & Barnett, 2007), a criterion-standard test for DCD. In addition, Sumner and colleagues assessed face processing ability, and found that the ASD and DCD groups had similar performance impairments, perhaps driven by visuospatial processing challenges seen in both groups.

Wisdom et al. (2006) created an “ability profile” composed of language, intelligence, social cognition, motor coordination, and executive functioning. They used this profile to assess differences between AD, DCD, and RELD. They observed overlap in deficits across groups, with some distinctions: namely, the AD and DCD groups differed in fine and gross motor skills, emotion understanding, and theory of mind.

Kinnealey et al. (2012) used a quantitative and qualitative mixed-method analysis to explore whether alterations to the sensory environment reduced “non-attending behaviors” in one student with dyspraxia/DCD and three students with ASD. Sound-absorbing walls and halogen lightning were installed in a classroom, and the authors analyzed the frequency of non-attending behaviors captured in videos of class segments. All students demonstrated a decline in non-attending behaviors, and all students self-reported in journals that they noticed the classroom improvements.

Cosper et al. (2009) compared the benefits of an interactive metronome treatment in children with ADHD only, ADHD+PDD, and ADHD+DCD in both sustained attention and motor proficiency. During treatment sessions, children were instructed to practice various combinations of hand and foot movements in time with a PC-based metronome. Children made improvements on complex visual choice reaction time and several aspects of motor proficiency. No between-group comparison analyses were performed, so it is unclear whether one group improved more than the others.

Green and colleagues (2015) evaluated self- and teacher's perception of competence in children with AS, DCD, and typically developing and later correlated with motor and social abilities. Results showed that children with AS and DCD perceived themselves as less competent than the TD group, a view also shared by the teachers.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to conduct a SLR gathering all studies that investigated behavioral similarities and differences between groups of individuals diagnosed with ASD, DCD, and ASD+DCD (if included). More specifically, we aimed to identify: (a) what types of behaviors (variables) were compared in ASD and DCD in order to determine similarities or differences between the conditions, and (b) whether any of the studies explored the possibility of co-occurrence between ASD and DCD. Eleven studies met the criteria defined by this study, with seven classified as high quality, one classified as average quality, and three classified as low quality. Based on the results of all studies, particularly the ones of high quality, we explore the fact that while DCD and ASD share some behavioral characteristics, the symptom profiles of each disorder are unique and separable.

ASD and DCD: Distinct but potentially co-occurring disorders

The eleven studies reviewed reported important differences between individuals with ASD and DCD, suggesting that they are indeed separable, but potentially co-occurring, diagnoses. Most of the high quality studies suggested that individuals with ASD, DCD, and ASD+DCD have symptoms spanning several domains. Notable distinctions between ASD and DCD were found in working memory ability, gestural performance, severity of motor challenges, grip selection, and cortical thickness. For example, a clear distinction was found between the two disorders for working memory ability (Alloway et al., 2006), such that children with DCD had noticeable visuospatial working memory deficits, performing worse than those with AS, while children with AS only had poor performance in verbal short-term memory. However, as Alloway and colleagues (2009) suggest, known visual and motor system differences in ASD and DCD may mediate or moderate the relationship between disorder and working memory task performance. Since evidence suggests that AS is indistinct from the rest of the autism spectrum (Miller & Ozonoff, 2000), new studies are needed to determine whether the same working memory profile is present a broader range of symptom severity.

When comparing gestural performance, a clear distinction also emerged: while the ASD, DCD, and DCD+ADHD groups were all significantly impaired on motor coordination skills, only children with ASD showed an impairment in gestural performance (Dewey et al., 2007). These findings suggest that gestural impairments in ASD are not solely attributable to deficits in motor coordination skills. In grip selection, children with DCD showed the predicted bias towards minimal rotation, while children with ASD performed identically to age-matched controls (van Swieten et al., 2010).

The most recent study, and the only one that directly assessed brain structure, found clear differences in neural architecture between ASD, DCD, and ASD+DCD (Caeyenberghs et al., 2016). This result underscores the idea that ASD and DCD are separable, but can be co-occurring. However, behavioral distinctions may not be easily identified, given that children with ASD and DCD may have similar motor and face-processing challenges (Sumner et al., 2016). Motor impairments are common in ASD, but the body of available evidence suggests that for some, they may be severe enough to warrant a diagnosis of cooccurring DCD, while for others, they may not. Some evidence also suggests that children with DCD have social challenges, but these are thought to stem from difficulty interacting with and performing skills at the same level as TD children because of their motor symptoms, rather than reflecting a fundamental difference in social communication ability (Chen & Cohn, 2009; Dewey et al., 2002). One limitation of some studies with DCD is the fact that children with DCD are typically not tested for ASD, or ASD might not be fully excluded in groups of DCD. The issue of potential co-occurrence is not limited to ASD and DCD, but rather, may extend to other developmental disorders as suggested by the DAMP hypothesis (Gillberg & Kadejo, 2003). This theoretical perspective posits that co-occurrence between DCD, ADHD, and ASD can be described in terms of symptom overlap, namely, deficits in attention, motor control, and perception. The findings of this SLR add to the body of literature that supports the DSM-5 method of describing DCD and ASD as unique and distinct diagnostic conditions with the possibility of co-occurrence.

As Wisdom and colleagues (2006) and others have noted, processes underlying motor skill deficits may play a key role in ASD. Indeed, both behavioral and imaging data support this conclusion (Ming et al., 2007; Ozonoff et al., 2008; Vernazza-Martin et al., 2005), and so it is essential that studies involving ASD and DCD include thorough assessment of motor skills, especially when these are likely to affect the outcome of the experiment. However, it is important to note that children with ASD may require adjustments to traditional motor assessments, in order to overcome communication barriers and facilitate optimal performance. For example, Breslin and Rudisill (2011) elicited more accurate gross motor scores for children with ASD on the Test of Gross Motor Development by using a picture task card protocol, which eliminated receptive and expressive language demands often associated with motor assessments.

Potential similarities in ASD and DCD

Interestingly, Wisdom et al. (2006) found both similarities and differences for DCD and AD, such that the DCD group had higher theory of mind, emotion recognition, and fine and gross motor coordination than the AD group, but similar response inhibition abilities. Wisdom and colleagues noted that when stratified by symptom severity, children with AD who were classified as “more able” did not differ on any measures from children with DCD, unlike children who were classified as “less able”. Given this result, the authors questioned whether AD and DCD differ more in the possible range of symptom severity than in any specific behavioral domain.

The recent study by Sumner et al. (2016) also demonstrated several overlapping characteristics in face processing, expression, speech sound, and gaze for groups of ASD and DCD, suggesting that children with DCD may have problems processing social information. However, the DCD group scored at an intermediate level (between TD and ASD) in two other measures of socialization, which led the authors to conclude that socialization in the DCD group may not be as marked as those seen in the ASD group.

The clearest similarity between DCD and ASD in the studies we reviewed was a lack of significant response to cognitive intervention. With respect to intervention studies, no differences in improvement were observed between ASD and DCD groups in attention (Kinnealey et al., 2012; Cosper et al., 2009) or IQ (Kozulin et al., 2010). It is possible that the capacity to train and improve in several domains, especially cognition, is similar for these conditions. In addition, no differences were found in a qualitative study investigating the transition from primary to secondary school, possibly because the main variables of interest related to their motor difficulties.

Challenges to accurate diagnostic classification

There is still a striking lack of consensus in the field about whether DCD and ASD are distinct and mutually-exclusive disorders with some shared motor features, or whether some individuals with ASD have DCD while others do not. As new evidence emerges, researchers and clinicians will be better-equipped to make recommendations regarding diagnosis and intervention. Additionally, clinicians recommending a DCD diagnosis must carefully evaluate DSM-5 criterion D–whether motor skill deficits are better explained by intellectual disability, visual impairment, or a neurological condition–to determine whether there is a potential for co-occurrence with ASD.

Several key issues may arise when attempting to diagnose both ASD and DCD in the same child. In a child with ASD, a diagnosis of ASD will likely occur first, given that it can be reliably diagnosed as early as 24 months (Lord et al., 2006). Therefore, motor symptoms 1) may not be comprehensively assessed, 2) thought to be more than secondary consequences of ASD or 3) may not be prioritized in assessment given that motor skills change as a function of development and/or maturation. Conversely, DCD is typically only diagnosed after age 6 (van Waelvelde et al., 2006), although an exception may be made if a child between 3 and 5 years shows a marked motor impairment (Blank et al., 2012). Therefore, for children with ASD diagnosed before school age, the most likely question will be whether to add DCD to an existing ASD diagnosis. However, the average age of ASD diagnosis remains around 5 years (CDC, 2016), with many children only receiving a full assessment and diagnosis when they reach the school system. In these instances, it is less straightforward which diagnosis a child should carry, and how to address comorbidities.

The question of whether children can be reliably diagnosed (or perhaps screened for) DCD earlier than age 6 is also important. Fundamental motor skills (e.g., running, jumping, hopping, throwing, etc.) are building blocks for more complex motor behaviors (e.g., gesturing) and long-term engagement in physical activity (Cairney & King-Dowling, 2016). Given the demonstrated efficacy of early interventions focused on motor development it is critically important to identify at-risk and affected children as early as possible (Blauw-Hospers & Hadders-Algra, 2005). At present, the most common diagnostic tools for DCD are the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCD-Q; Wilson et al., 2009) and MABC-2 (Henderson et al., 2007). However, given the individual variability in the slope of motor developmental trajectories, it remains difficult to reliably identify DCD in very young children.

Another important aspect to be considered is the distinction between dyspraxia and DCD. DCD is a fairly recent term, introduced in DSM-III (APA, 2013), though the phenomenology was noted as early as 1925 and established in the literature in the 1960s (for review, see Kirby & Sugden, 2001). In 1994, the international research community determined that the term DCD should replace dyspraxia in both research and practice (Polatajko & Cantin, 2006). Nonetheless, some studies continue to use the term dyspraxia to describe a range of motor impairments in ASD (e.g., Abelenda et al., 2015). It is important to clarify the definition of dyspraxia in future assessments. While dyspraxia and DCD should be used interchangeably as a specific diagnostic classification, some researchers and clinicians refer to imitation deficits seen in ASD (often measured by observation of gestures) and other motor symptoms as dyspraxia, without reference to a specific co-occurrence. A DCD diagnosis involves much more than observation of gestures or specific motor behaviors in a research study, and should include a thorough evaluation of motor skills and the impact that motor difficulties have on activities of daily living.

Limitations

The most significant limitation of the present review is the sample size. Since so few studies have directly compared ASD and DCD, our conclusions are preliminary and warrant further exploration as the available body of literature increases. In addition, differences in study designs (comparative, interventional) compromised our ability to make significant cross-study comparisons. The number of co-occurrences present in the studies also limited our ability to generalize results to all individuals diagnosed with DCD, ASD, or both. In general, numerous complex confounding factors create unique challenges for studies investigating neurodevelopmental disorders and potential co-occurrences. Apart from controlling for cognitive ability, broadly defined as IQ ≥ 70, few studies directly address specific domains in which individuals may differ (see Table 3), leaving questions about potential confounds that limit the ability to compare findings between and within groups. Finally, though our conclusions are based on the studies that met ‘high quality’ criteria, it is notable that three studies included in this review were classified as ‘poor quality’. Despite these limitations, our findings offer the benefit of exposing gaps in the literature and drawing attention to important questions about the specific nature of overlap between ASD and DCD.

Conclusions

Overall, the high quality studies selected for this review reported more differences than similarities between the ASD and DCD in several behavioral domains, with notable overlap in symptoms for some individuals with ASD+DCD. Symptom overlap between the two conditions was particularly notable in the domain of motor skills. These results suggest that ASD and DCD are, as currently classified in DSM-5, separable but potentially co-occurring disorders. Perhaps the greatest challenge to understanding the relationship between DCD and ASD is a dearth of studies with large, well-defined samples. Future studies should include consideration of co-occurrence between disorders, age and developmental trajectory, task demands, and the sensitivity of diagnostic and assessment tools used. Urgent attention is needed to determine the breadth and depth of shared symptoms between ASD and DCD, and how best to classify persons for the purpose of intervention. The extant literature suggests that these are dissociable disorders. However, it is still unknown whether the motor features observed in both ASD and DCD vary among individuals along a spectrum of severity and functional impact, or whether their severity and functional impact can be used diagnostically to distinguish between ASD, DCD, and ASD+DCD. As the field awaits further studies that contribute to our understanding of the nuanced differences and similarities between these disorders, we recommend systematic and thorough evaluation of motor skills in individuals with ASD. This will aid clinicians in making important choices about intervention, informed by the distinction between individuals with co-occurring ASD+DCD and individuals with ASD who display some motor differences but do not meet criteria for DCD.

Highlights.

  • Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) are diagnosed as co-occurring conditions.

  • Few studies have compared behaviors of individuals diagnosed with each disorder.

  • Through a systematic literature review (SLR), we found more differences than similarities in behavioral profiles of individuals with ASD and DCD.

  • The evaluation of potential DCD in individuals with ASD must be performed systematically.

Acknowledgments

Funding: Research reported in this publication was supported in part by the National Center for Advancing Translational Science of the National Institutes of Health under the Center for Translational Medicine [KL2TR001103]. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors, and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Abelenda J, Mailloux Z, Roley SS. Dyspraxia in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Evidence and implications. Sensory Integration Special Interest Section Quarterly. 2015;38(3):1–4. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alloway TP, Rajendran G, Archibald LMD. Working memory in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2009;42(4):372–382. doi: 10.1177/0022219409335214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th. Washington, DC: Author; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  4. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  5. Blank R, Smits-Engelsman B, Polatajko H, Wilson P. European Academy for Childhood Disability (EACD): Recommendations on the definition, diagnosis and intervention of developmental coordination disorder (long version) Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2012;54:54–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04171.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blauw-Hospers CH, Hadders-Algra M. A systematic review of the effects of early intervention on motor development. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2005;47(6):421–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01165.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Breslin CM, Rudisill ME. The effect of visual supports on performance of the TGMD-2 for children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly. 2011;28:342–353. doi: 10.1123/apaq.28.4.342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: A graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bundy AC, Lane SJ, Murray EA. Sensory integration: Theory and practice. 2nd. Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  10. Caeyenberghs K, Taymans T, Wilson PH, Vanderstraeten G, Hosseini H, van Waelvelde H. Neural signature of developmental coordination disorder in the structural connectome independent of comorbid autism. Developmental Science. 2016;19(4):599–612. doi: 10.1111/desc.12424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cairney J, King-Dowling S. Developmental Coordination Disorder. In: Matson JL, editor. Comorbid conditions among children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York, NY: Springer; 2016. pp. 303–322. [Google Scholar]
  12. Christensen DL, Baio J, Braun KV, Bilder D, Charles J, Constantino JN, et al. Yeargin-Allsopp M. Prevalence and characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder among children aged 8 years – Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. MMWR Surveillance Summaries. 2016;65(SS-3):1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6503a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cosper SM, Lee GP, Peters SB, Bishop E. Interactive Metronome training in children with attention deficit and developmental coordination disorders. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research. 2009;32(4):331–336. doi: 10.1097/mrr.0b013e328325a8cf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dewey D, Cantell M, Crawford SG. Motor and gestural performance in children with autism spectrum disorders, developmental coordination disorder, and or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2007;13:246–256. doi: 10.1017/s1355617707070270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dewey D, Kaplan BJ, Crawford SG, Wilson BN. Developmental coordination disorder: associated problems in attention, learning, and psychosocial adjustment. Human Movement Science. 2002;21(5-6):905–918. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00163-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Donnellan AM, Hill DA, Leary MR. Rethinking autism: implications of sensory and movement differences for understanding and support. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2013;6:124. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00124.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Downey R, Rapport MK. Motor activity in children with autism: A review of current literature. Pediatric Physical Therapy. 2012;24:2–10. doi: 10.1097/pep.0b013e31823db95f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dziuk MA, Gidley Larson JC, Apostu A, Mahone EM, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH. Dyspraxia in autism: Association with motor, social, and communicative deficits. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2007;49(10):734–739. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00734.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Elliott R, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2000;10(3):308–317. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Foulder-Hughes L, Prior C. Supporting pupils with DCD and ASD with the transition to secondary school. Research in Education. 2014;92:79–92. doi: 10.7227/rie.0011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fournier KA, Hass CJ, Naik SK, Lodha N, Cauraugh JH. Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40:1227–1240. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Glazebrook JF, Wallace R. Pathologies in functional connectivity, feedback control and robustness: a global workspace perspective on autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Processing. 2015;16(1):1–16. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0636-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gibbs J, Appleton J, Appleton R. Dyspraxia or developmental coordination disorder? Unravelling the enigma. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2007;92:534–39. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.088054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Green D, Arscott C, Barnett AL, Henderson L, Henderson SE, Huber J, May A, Baird G. Impairment of movement and social difficulties in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (Asperger Syndrome) and Developmental Coordination Disorder - how are they perceived by the children and their teachers? In: Barnett AL, Sugden DA, editors. Moving, Developing and Learning A Festschrift in celebration of the career of Sheila E Henderson. Oxford Brookes University; 2015. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  25. Green D, Baird G, Barnett AL, Henderson L, Huber J, Henderson SE. The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger's syndrome: A comparison with specific developmental disorder of motor function. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43(5):655–668. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Henderson SE, Sugden DA, Barnett AL. Movement assessment battery for children. 2nd. San Francisco, CA: Pearson; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hsiu-Fen C, Cohn ES. Social participation for children with developmental coordination disorder. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics. 2003;23(4):61–78. doi: 10.1080/J006v23n04_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jongmans MJ, Smits-Engelsman BCM, Schoemaker MM. Consequences of comorbidity of developmental coordination disorders and learning disabilities for severity and pattern of perceptual—motor dysfunction. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2003;36(6):528–537. doi: 10.1177/00222194030360060401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child. 1943;2:217–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kaplan BJ, Dewey DB, Crawford SG, Wilson BN. The term comorbidity is of questionable value in reference to developmental disorders: data and theory. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2001;34(6):555–565. doi: 10.1177/002221940103400608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kirby A, Sugden D. Children with developmental coordination disorders. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine. 2007;100(4):182–186. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.100.4.182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kinnealey M, Pfeiffer B, Miller J, Roan C, Schoener R, Ellner ML. Effect of classroom modification on attention and engagement of students with autism or dyspraxia. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2012;66(5):511–519. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2012.004010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kozulin A, Lebeer J, Madella-Noja A, Gonzalez F, Jeffrey I, et al. Cognitive modifiability of children with developmental disabilities: A multicentre study using Feuerstein's Instrumental Enrichment—Basic program. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2010;31:551–559. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Leonard HC, Hill EL. Review: The impact of motor development only typical and atypical social cognition and language: A systematic review. Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2014;19(3):163–170. doi: 10.1111/camh.12055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore PS, Shulman C, Thurm A, Pickles A. Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(6):694–701. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Miller J, Ozonoff S. The external validity of Asperger Disorder: Lack of evidence from the domain of neuropsychology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109(2):227–238. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ming X, Brimacombe M, Wagner GC. Prevalence of motor impairment in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Development. 2007;29(9):565–570. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2007.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Missiuna M, Magalhães L. Prospecting in the field of Developmental Coordination Disorder. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics. 2015;35(2):93–96. doi: 10.3109/01942638.2015.1013758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. The PRISMA Group Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine. 2009;6(7) doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Goldring S, Greiss-Hess L, Herrera AM, Steele J, et al. Rogers SJ. Gross motor development, movement abnormalities, and early identification of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38:644–656. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0430-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Polatajko HJ, Cantin N. Developmental Coordination Disorder (Dyspraxia): An overview of the state of the art. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology. 2006;12:250–258. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2005.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, Baird G. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47(8):921–929. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Sumner E, Leonard HC, Hill EL. Overlapping phenotypes in autism spectrum disorder and developmental coordination disorder: a cross-syndrome comparison of motor and social skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2016;46:2609–20. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2794-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Van Swieten LM, Williams JHG, Plumb MS, van Bergen E, Wilson AD, Kent SW, et al. A test of motor (not executive) planning in Developmental Coordination Disorder and Autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2010;36(2):493–499. doi: 10.1037/a0017177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Van Vaelvelde H, Oostra A, Dewitte G, Van den Broeck C, Jongmans MJ. Stability of motor problems in young children with or at risk of autism spectrum disorders, ADHD, and or developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2010;52:e174–e178. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03606.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Tissot C, Evans R. Visual teaching strategies for children with autism. Early Child Development and Care. 2003;173:425–433. doi: 10.1080/0300443032000079104. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Vernazza-Martin S, Martin N, Lepellec-Muller A, Rufo M, Massion J, Assaiante C. Goal directed locomotion and balance control in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2005;35(1):91–102. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1037-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Watemberg N, Waiserberg N, Zuk L, Lerman-Sagie T. Developmental Coordination Disorder in children with Attention-Deficit–Hyperactivity Disorder and physical therapy intervention. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2007;49(12):920–925. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00920.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Whyatt C, Craig C. Sensory-motor problems in autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2013;7:51. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wilson BN, Crawford SG, Green D, Roberts G, Aylott A, Kaplan BJ. Psychometric properties of the revised Developmental Coordination Disorder questionnaire. Journal of Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics. 2009;29(2):182–202. doi: 10.1080/01942630902784761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wisdom SN, Dyck MJ, Piek JP, Hay D, Hallmayer J. Can autism, language and coordination disorders be differentiated based on ability profiles? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;16:178–186. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0586-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES