Skip to main content
Journal of Obesity logoLink to Journal of Obesity
. 2017 Oct 8;2017:5923862. doi: 10.1155/2017/5923862

Relationship between Obesity and Cognitive Function in Young Women: The Food, Mood and Mind Study

Rebecca L Cook 1, Nicholas J O'Dwyer 1,2, Cheyne E Donges 2, Helen M Parker 1,3, Hoi Lun Cheng 3,4, Katharine S Steinbeck 4, Eka P Cox 1, Janet L Franklin 5, Manohar L Garg 6, Kieron B Rooney 1,3, Helen T O'Connor 1,3,
PMCID: PMC5651104  PMID: 29291133

Abstract

Limited research addresses links between obesity and cognitive function in young adults. Objective. To investigate the relationship between obesity and cognitive function in young women. Methods. This cross-sectional study recruited healthy, young (18–35 y) women of normal (NW: BMI = 18.5–24.9 kg·m−2) or obese (OB: BMI ≥ 30.0 kg·m−2) weight. Participants completed a validated, computer-based cognitive testing battery evaluating impulsivity, attention, information processing, memory, and executive function. Questionnaires on depression and physical activity and a fasting blood sample for C-reactive protein and the Omega-3 Index were also collected. Cognition data are presented as z-scores (mean ± SD), and group comparisons were assessed via ANOVA. Potential confounding from questionnaire and blood variables were evaluated using ANCOVA. Results. 299 women (NW: n = 157; OB: n = 142) aged 25.8 ± 5.1 y were enrolled. Cognition scores were within normal range (±1 z-score), but OB had lower attention (NW: 0.31 ± 1.38; OB: −0.25 ± 1.39; ES: 0.41, CI: 0.17–0.64; p < 0.001) and higher impulsivity (NW: 0.36 ± 1.14; OB: −0.07 ± 1.07; ES: 0.39, CI: 0.15–0.62; p=0.033). Confounder adjustment had minimal impact on results. Conclusion. The OB group had normal but significantly lower performance on attention and were more impulsive compared to NW participants. This may indicate early cognitive decline, but longitudinal research confirming these findings is warranted.

1. Introduction

Young women are at increased risk of weight gain as they transition into adulthood. Contributing factors include lifestyle changes such as moving away from the family home, cohabitation with peers or partners, increased takeaway food and alcohol consumption, and decreased physical activity [13]. For some young women, pregnancy is also a risk factor for weight gain [4]. In Australia, close to one-third of young (18–23 y) women are overweight or obese, and as a group, they are gaining weight faster than older women and young men [3, 5, 6]. By 2025, modeling predicts that one in six Australian women compared to one in 10 men will have severe obesity [7]. Aside from the known metabolic and psychological health risks [8], obesity also affects reproductive health via increased risks for infertility and obstetric complications [911]. Growing evidence also indicates a negative impact of maternal obesity on the health of subsequent generations through epigenetic effects on offspring [9, 10, 12, 13]. These risks and the greater rate of weight gain in young Australian women compared to young men indicate a need for targeted research and intervention in this population.

The health consequences of obesity extend to cognitive function, with evidence for reduced memory [14, 15] and executive function [1619] as well as increased impulsivity [2022]. Several recent systematic reviews have found that, because of numerous methodological limitations, it is unclear whether the association between obesity and cognitive impairment is independent of obesity-related comorbidities [1619, 23]. Equivocal findings are likely attributable to poor study design, small sample size, lack of consideration for confounders, and the heterogeneity of psychometric tests used [24].

Proposed mechanisms underpinning reduced cognitive function in obesity include oxidative stress, hypertension, metabolic dysfunction, cardiovascular disease, and systemic inflammation, which have been reported to alter brain structure and volume [2427]. However, young adults with obesity often have no apparent metabolic and cardiovascular abnormalities although systemic inflammation is usually present which may act directly to impair cognition [28, 29]. Alternatively, reduced physical activity may be important. Physical activity is reported to have beneficial effects on cognitive function, with adequate activity levels shown to be associated with an improvement in cognitive function and attenuation of cognitive decline [30, 31]. In recent years, 57% of Australian adults failed to meet physical activity guidelines [32], and moreover, women are less likely than men to meet recommended levels [33], a trend that begins during adolescence and continues into adulthood [34]. Finally, substantial evidence exists in the literature for an association between blood fatty acids and cognitive impairment, particularly in Alzheimer's disease [35, 36]. An increasing number of studies suggest that the omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) and eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) are protective against cognitive impairment and dementia [3741]; however, young Australian women have been shown to have inadequate intake of omega-3 fatty acids [42, 43], and low blood omega-3 levels have been commonly observed in young women with depression [44] and during pregnancy [45].

The majority of current research is focused on middle-aged cohorts who have other potential age-related factors influencing cognition. As such, the association between obesity and cognition in younger, healthier adults is equivocal [1618, 24], particularly with respect to the specific cognitive domains most affected [17, 19]. In line with previous limitations identified in cognition research [17, 23], inconsistencies related to poor study design, small sample size, limited consideration of confounders, and the array of psychometric tests used make it difficult to compare studies, and additionally, there are only a limited number of studies specifically examining young adults [15, 17]. Hence, the current study investigated healthy, young women and hypothesized that young women with obesity would have significantly lower cognitive function, across the memory, executive function, and impulsivity (more impulsive) domains. We further hypothesized that decreased cognitive performance would be ameliorated after adjustment for systemic inflammation. A detrimental impact of obesity on cognition in young adulthood might influence not only the capacity to function optimally at this important age stage but also the risk for earlier cognitive decline [19]. Better evidence of reduced cognition in young adults with obesity would also support advocacy for targeted public health programs aimed at addressing weight gain during this age stage, particularly in women who are gaining weight more rapidly than men [3, 5], and have specific obesity-related reproductive health risks [9, 10, 12, 13].

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design and Participants

The primary aim of this cross-sectional study, the Food, Mood and Mind study, was to compare cognitive function in young (18–35 y), healthy women of either normal weight (NW) or obese weight (OB). A secondary aim was to investigate the influence of potential confounders including systemic inflammation, omega-3 index (O3I), depression symptoms, and physical activity on cognitive function. In light of the methodological limitations of existing studies highlighted in recent reviews [1216], Prickett et al. [17] identified five key design considerations for studies of the relationship between obesity and cognition: control of confounding variables (e.g., age, education, and depression); appropriate study design (e.g., relevant participant exclusions and adequate sample size); appropriate control groups; assessment of cognitive domains relevant to the specific research question; and appropriate, valid, and reliable cognitive assessment tools. All of these design features have been incorporated in the current study.

Participants were recruited from both urban/metropolitan (Sydney) and rural/regional (Bathurst) areas in Australia. To ensure adequate representation from regional participants, one-third of the sample was recruited from the rural/regional site. A multipronged approach identified as effective for recruiting young adults to research was employed [46], including flyers, websites, newspaper advertisements, e-newsletters, social media, radio, and letterbox drops. The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees of the local health district (HREC/10/RPAH/455), the University of Sydney (protocol number: 2014/050), and Charles Sturt University (protocol number: X10-0259). Written informed consent was obtained from all volunteers prior to study participation. Participants who completed the study received a gift card ($100 AUD) to cover time and travel costs.

2.2. Eligibility Criteria

Volunteers were initially screened for eligibility via telephone using a standardized medical questionnaire, with eligible individuals reporting no medical conditions or medication use on a regular basis (oral contraceptive pill and asthma medications were allowed), and a BMI in the normal weight (18.5–24.9 kg·m−2) or obese weight (≥30.0 kg·m−2) category according to the World Health Organization guidelines [47]. No volunteers in the overweight category (BMI = 25.0–29.9 kg·m−2) were recruited so as to establish a substantial BMI gap for detection of significant cognitive differences. An upper age limit of 35 y was also applied to reduce confounding from age-related cognitive decline [48]. The standardized medical screening questionnaire sought to exclude those with significant medical conditions (e.g., cardiovascular or metabolic diseases including type 2 diabetes) and conditions which may compromise the assessment of cognitive function or the assessment of BMI: (1) neurological or psychiatric conditions and use of medications/substances known to alter mood, reaction time, or cognitive capacity including smoking, alcohol consumption (≥50 g per week), and recreational drug use; (2) vision, hearing, or motor coordination problems and poor English literacy, which may impair the ability to complete the touchscreen cognition testing; and (3) pregnancy, breastfeeding, elite athletes (due to increased muscular development), and regular blood donations (≥3 per year or having donated blood within the previous three months) as iron deficiency/anemia has also been associated with impaired cognitive function [49]. A specialist endocrinologist (Katharine S. Steinbeck) oversaw the recruitment process and provided a medical opinion on participant eligibility prior to their entry to the study.

2.3. Data Collection

Participants attended two study visits (at a university laboratory or an obesity clinic within a major teaching hospital) approximately one week apart. The first study visit included anthropometric assessment, followed by assessment of physical activity (International Physical Activity Questionnaire Short Form, IPAQ-SF) [50, 51] and then by assessment of depression (Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale, DASS) [52] and cognitive function (IntegNeuroTM) [53]. All cognitive assessments were conducted after breakfast and prior to 13:00 hrs. Participants were asked to refrain from heavy exercise, alcohol, and caffeine (and other stimulants) on the morning of the cognition test and to consume their usual breakfast. The second visit involved collection of a fasting blood sample. Blood analysis was used to help exclude or adjust for parameters known to influence cognitive function. Fasting blood glucose was analyzed to assist in the exclusion of women with diabetes. Measurement of C-reactive protein (CRP) and omega-3 (n-3) polyunsaturated fatty acid status (via the Omega-3 Index) enabled cognitive function to be adjusted for these potential confounders. See Blood Collection and Biochemical Analysis for further details.

2.4. Anthropometry

Anthropometric measurements were taken in light clothing (no shoes). Height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm in duplicate with a stadiometer (Seca 213; Seca, Hamburg, Germany). Weight was recorded on a digital platform scale accurate to 0.1 kg (PW-200KGL; A&D Weighing, Thebarton, Australia). Waist circumference was measured at the midpoint between the lowest rib and the iliac crest to the nearest 0.1 cm in duplicate (mean reported) with a retractable metal tape (Lufkin W606PM; Cooper Industries, Sparks, USA), according to the International Diabetes Federation guidelines [54].

2.5. Cognitive Assessment

Cognitive function was assessed using a computer-based testing platform (IntegNeuro; Brain Resource Company, Woolloomooloo, Australia), using a touchscreen and headset. This platform has well-established validity, reliability, cross-cultural consistency, and norms [55, 56]. The five cognitive domains impulsivity, attention, information processing, memory, and executive function were assessed. The tests for each domain and the specific performance measures for each test are summarized in Table 1 [53]. The measures in each domain were expressed as norm-based z-scores, with values between ±1 classified as within normal range, and then averaged to obtain a single z-score for each domain. Positive and negative z-scores reflect above- and below-average performance, respectively (for impulsivity, positive scores reflect less impulsive behavior, whereas negative scores reflect more impulsive behavior), and are adjusted for age and education using internal regression methods based on Brain Resource Company's extensive normative databank [53]. Participants completed the cognitive assessment using their dominant hand seated in a quiet location.

Table 1.

Summary of cognitive domains, tests, and measures.

Cognitive domain Tests Measures
Impulsivity Go/no-go Variability of reaction time (Go)
Go/no-go Total errors
Attention Continuous performance task Reaction time
Continuous performance task False alarm errors
Continuous performance task False miss errors
Information processing Switching of attention Completion time (digits and letters)
Switching of attention Errors (digits and letters)
Choice reaction time Reaction time
Memory Memory recall and recognition Total immediate recall
Memory recall and recognition Long-delay (distractor) recall
Digit span forwards Number recalled correctly
Digit span backwards Number recalled correctly
Executive function Maze test Completion time
Maze test Overrun errors

The switching of attention task was the trail making test, part B.

2.6. Depression and Physical Activity Assessment

Symptoms of depression were measured using the DASS [52], administered electronically as part of the IntegNeuro testing platform. Results are expressed as z-scores, with positive scores indicating greater depressive symptoms. Physical activity level was assessed via the IPAQ-SF, which estimates duration (in minutes) and frequency (days) of walking, moderate and vigorous activity, and sitting hours per weekday [50, 51]. Physical activity (PA) data are expressed as metabolic equivalent of task (MET) minutes per week.

2.7. Blood Collection and Biochemical Analysis

The second study visit involved a morning fasted (12 h) venous blood draw. Blood analyses were undertaken by a NATA accredited laboratory. Inflammation was assessed via CRP, with levels > 5.0 mg/l indicative of elevated inflammation [57, 58]. The O3I was used as a reliable indicator of overall omega-3 (n-3) polyunsaturated fatty acid status, calculated as the sum of the percentage of eicosapentaenoic acid (20:5n-3) and docosahexaenoic acid (22:6n-3) in erythrocyte membranes, as reported elsewhere [59, 60]. Proposed cutoffs for cardiovascular health were used: O3I of <4% is classified as low, 4–8% as safe, and >8% as optimal [60]. Fasting blood glucose was also measured to ensure exclusion of women with diabetes (≥7.0 mmol/l) [61].

2.8. Statistical Analysis

Analyses were carried out using Statistica Version 12 (StatSoft Inc., Tulsa, USA). All data were checked for normality and outliers, with cognition scores ±4 standard deviations (SDs) excluded (n = 11). Unpaired t-tests and chi-square tests were used to compare participant characteristics between the NW and OB groups. The relationship between BMI and cognitive function was initially investigated via a 2 × 5 analysis of variance (ANOVA) model, using the BMI group (NW or OB) as the independent factor and the five cognition z-scores (i.e., impulsivity, attention, information processing, memory, and executive function) as repeated measures. This model was rerun as separate analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) models using the covariates DASS score, PA, CRP, and O3I. Finally, the relative contribution of these confounders to the overall BMI-cognition relationship was assessed using a single ANCOVA model which included all the four covariates combined. Tukey's post hoc tests were used in all cases to determine the precise locus of any significant difference observed. Since the cognition domains in the IntegNeuro test battery have been shown via principal component analysis to represent independent factors [62], univariate analyses were further conducted to examine group differences on the cognition z-scores. Significance was set at p < 0.05, with results reported as mean ± SD unless indicated otherwise and effect size (ES) ± 95% confidence interval (CI).

3. Results

3.1. Participant Characteristics

A total of 300 women were recruited, with 299 completing both study visits (NW: n = 157; OB: n = 142); one participant failed to attend the second study visit (Figure 1()). Major reasons for exclusion included BMI outside the study limits (32%), depression or anxiety medications or significant medical condition (15%), and time constraints or geographical location (11%).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow diagram for recruitment, eligibility screening, and participation in the Food, Mood and Mind study.

Participant characteristics are summarized in Table 2. Average age of the participants was 25.8 ± 5.1 y although the OB group was slightly but significantly older by ∼2 y (p < 0.001). A majority of participants (57%) held tertiary qualifications, with the mean years of education being 16.2 ± 2.2 y. Average time spent in education (∼0.6 years less; p=0.022) and the proportion of tertiary educated women (11% less; p=0.041) were both lower in the OB group. The majority of participants in the OB group had class I obesity (55%), with smaller proportions having class II (29%) and III (16%) obesity. The majority of the OB group (97%) had a waist circumference above the current recommendation (≥80 cm) to reduce risk of metabolic disorders and other comorbidities [54]. A substantial proportion (85%) had a waist circumference classified as very high risk of chronic disease (≥88 cm) [54]. The high waist circumference of the participants in the OB group confirms that their BMI was due to excess adiposity rather than increased muscular development.

Table 2.

A summary of participant characteristics.

Total group (n = 299) NW (n = 157) OB (n = 142) p value
Age (y) 25.8 ± 5.1 24.9 ± 4.6 26.9 ± 5.4 <0.001
Education (y) 16.2 ± 2.2 16.5 ± 2.2 15.9 ± 2.2 0.022
Highest qualification (n, %)
 Secondary school 83 (28%) 44 (28%) 39 (27%) 0.028
 Certificate/diploma 46 (15%) 15 (10%) 31 (22%)
 Tertiary degree (bachelor/diploma) 120 (40%) 69 (44%) 51 (36%)
 Higher degree 49 (17%) 28 (18%) 21 (15%)
Weight (kg) 78.1 ± 23.5 59.7 ± 7.0 98.5 ± 17.9 <0.001
Height (cm) 165.4 ± 6.9 165.5 ± 7.3 165.3 ± 6.6 0.805
BMI (kg/m2) 28.6 ± 8.6 21.8 ± 1.7 36.1 ± 6.8 <0.001
 Obese class I 79 (26%) N/A 79 (56%)
 Obese class II 41 (14%) N/A 41 (29%)
 Obese class III 22 (7%) N/A 22 (15%)
Waist circumference (cm) 84.5 ± 18.7 69.7 ± 4.2 101.2 ± 14.1 <0.001
 Below 80 cm (n, %) 156 (52%) 154 (98%) 2 (1%) <0.001
 80–88 cm inclusive (n, %) 20 (7%) 3 (2%) 17 (12%)
 Above 88 cm (n, %) 120 (40%) 0 (0%) 120 (85%)
Physical activity (MET-min/wk)
 Total MET-min/wk 2603 ± 2141 3076 ± 2302 2080 ± 1815 <0.001
 Low MET-min/wk 1137 ± 1190 1246 ± 1234 1017 ± 1131 0.096
 Moderate MET-min/wk 499 ± 755 563 ± 809 428 ± 685 0.124
 High MET-min/wk 967 ± 1263 1267 ± 1435 636 ± 939 <0.001
CRP (normal range < 5 mg/l) 3.4 ± 4.3 1.4 ± 2.1 5.5 ± 5.0 <0.001
 CRP < 5 mg/l (n, %) 233 (78%) 149 (96%) 84 (60%) <0.001
 CRP ≥ 5 mg/l (n, %) 63 (21%) 6 (4%) 57 (40%)
Omega-3 index (%) 6.3 ± 1.7 6.8 ± 1.7 5.8 ± 1.6 <0.001
 Low: <4% (n, %) 13 (4%) 1 (1%) 12 (8%) 0.002
 Safe: 4–8% (n, %) 239 (83%) 125 (83%) 114 (85%)
 Optimal: >8% (n, %) 36 (12%) 25 (16%) 11 (8%)
Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (z-score) −0.12 ± 0.88 −0.33 ± 0.71 0.11 ± 0.98 <0.0001

p value for NW versus OB. Missing data: waist circumference (OB: n = 3); O3I (NW: n = 6; OB: n = 5). Obese class I = 30.0–34.9 kg/m2; obese class II = 35.0–39.9 kg/m2; obese class III ≥ 40.0 kg/m2. BMI, body mass index; N/A, not applicable; PA, physical activity; MET, metabolic equivalent of task; min, minute; wk, week; CRP, C-reactive protein.

3.2. Inflammation, Physical Activity, and Omega-3 Index

The OB group showed significantly higher CRP levels (NW: 1.4 ± 2.1; OB: 5.5 ± 5.0 mg/l; p < 0.001) and significantly lower PA levels (NW: 3076 ± 2302; OB: 2080 ± 1816 MET-min/wk; p < 0.0001) than the NW group (Table 2). Most participants (64%) met the American College of Sports Medicine guidelines for moderate to vigorous PA (500 MET-min/wk) [63], although fewer participants in the OB group met these guidelines (NW: 74%; OB: 54%; p < 0.001). Mean O3I was lower in the OB group (p < 0.001), with a greater proportion of participants showing low O3I status (p=0.001) and a smaller proportion having optimal O3I (p=0.029).

3.3. Obesity and Cognition

In both the NW and OB groups, mean z-scores for all the five cognitive domains (impulsivity, attention, information processing, memory, and executive function) were within the normal range (i.e., ±1 z-score) (Figure 2()). The group mean values (±95% confidence intervals) are presented in Table 3. Analysis of cognitive performance showed a significant overall between-group difference (p < 0.001) and a significant interaction between groups and domains (p=0.028). Post hoc analyses identified lower scores in the OB group for impulsivity (NW: 0.36 ± 1.14; OB: −0.07 ± 1.07; p=0.033; ES: 0.39, CI: 0.15–0.62; lower scores indicate greater impulsivity) and attention (NW: 0.31 ± 1.38; OB: −0.25 ± 1.39; ES: 0.41, CI: 0.17–0.64; p < 0.001). Univariate analyses showed significantly poorer performance for impulsivity (p=0.001), attention (p=0.0007), and memory (p=0.022) in OB relative to NW, as well as a nonsignificant trend for lower information processing (p=0.055).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Comparison of cognition z-scores assessed across five domains between young women categorized into NW and OB groups. Domain scores adjusted for age and education, with data presented as mean ± standard error. Normal range is between ±1. p < 0.05 between NW and OB in post hoc analyses; #p < 0.05 between NW and OB in univariate analysis. Lower scores on the impulsivity domain indicate increased impulsive responses. BMI, body mass index; NW, normal weight (BMI = 18.5–24.9 kg/m2); OB, obese weight (BMI ≥ 30.0 kg/m2).

Table 3.

Mean z-scores (±95% confidence intervals) for normal weight and obese groups.

Cognition domain Normal weight Obese p value
Mean ±95% CI Mean ±95% CI
Impulsivity 0.36 0.18 to 0.54 −0.07 −0.25 to 0.11 0.001
Attention 0.31 0.09 to 0.53 −0.25 −0.49 to −0.02 0.001
Information processing 0.45 0.31 to 0.60 0.24 0.07 to 0.41 0.055
Memory 0.01 −0.14 to 0.16 −0.25 −0.43 to −0.08 0.022
Executive function 0.49 0.33 to 0.64 0.41 0.22 to 0.60 0.540

Univariate tests.

3.4. Influence of Known Confounders on Cognition

Adjustment for DASS score and PA in separate ANCOVA models did not alter the significant group effect (DASS: p < 0.001; PA: p < 0.001) or interaction effect (DASS: p < 0.01; PA: p < 0.019) observed between BMI and cognitive domains. Adjustment for CRP and O3I weakened the interaction effect (CRP: p=0.060; O3I: p=0.067), but a significant overall difference between BMI groups remained (CRP: p=0.003; O3I: p < 0.001). Adjusted post hoc analyses yielded results similar to the unadjusted ANOVA, with significantly greater impulsivity (i.e., a poorer/lower score in the impulsivity domain) in the OB group after adjustment for DASS score (p=0.033), PA (p=0.034), and CRP (p=0.043), but not for O3I (p=0.12), and significantly lower attention following adjustment for the four covariates (all p < 0.001). After adjusting for each of the four covariates, univariate analyses showed that performance remained significantly lower in the OB group than in the NW group on impulsivity (DASS: p=0.002; PA: p=0.001; CRP: p=0.004; O3I: p=0.004) and attention (DASS: p < 0.001; PA: p < 0.001; CRP: p=0.005; O3I: p=0.002), with memory remaining significant for all except CRP (DASS: p=0.013; PA: p=0.007; CRP: p=0.19; O3I: p=0.027). These effects remained largely unchanged when a final ANCOVA model combining the four covariates was carried out. After adjustment for all potential confounders, the reduction of cognitive performance in the OB group in the impulsivity and attention domains was 0.38 and 0.59 SD, respectively.

4. Discussion

This cross-sectional study found that while cognition was within normal range, healthy young women with obesity achieved significantly lower performance in the attention and impulsivity domains compared to their NW peers, with evidence of lower performance also in the memory domain. Adjustment for known confounders had minimal impact on these findings although there was some attenuation of BMI-related differences for impulsivity and memory. Diminished cognitive performance in young women with obesity may indicate the beginning of a persistent and early cognitive decline. However, longitudinal research is required to confirm our findings, and studies including a more socioeconomically diverse sample are warranted. Despite this, our rigorous exclusion criteria suggest that a true cognitive deficit exists in OB individuals, especially as our sample included highly educated women who would be expected to be at their peak age for cognitive function.

Lower performance in the OB group for the attention, impulsivity, and memory domains differs from reports in the literature. Contrary to existing studies that link obesity with poorer executive function [1619, 24], our scores obtained for this domain were found to be the most similar between the OB and NW groups (Figure 2()). This discrepancy may be attributed to the way that cognitive tests are classified. Attention and impulsivity, which were tested separately and where we found clear BMI group differences, are sometimes categorized under executive function [16, 17, 64]. The trail making test, for example, which is classified under information processing in the IntegNeuro platform used here, has usually been classified under executive function, with mostly nonsignificant findings reported for BMI groups in recent reviews [1619, 24]. Executive function was assessed in the current study using the maze test, and the only other comparable study using this same test employed it in a cross-sectional study examining cognition across the adult lifespan (n = 408; 20–82 y) [64]. These researchers found poorer performance in both younger and older overweight/obese adults (BMI > 25) compared to NW adults [64], but it should be noted that the age range of their “younger” group was 20–50 years and hence, unlike the present study, the influence of age-related comorbidities could not be ruled out.

Attention was consistently and significantly lower in OB participants. This finding is in agreement with a study by Cserjési et al. reporting lower attention in middle-aged women with obesity compared to nonobese controls [65]. Both our study and that of Cserjési et al. utilized the continuous performance test, which measures sustained attention [53, 65]. Gunstad et al. reported no significant differences between NW and overweight/obese adults on four tests classified under the attention domain, including switching of attention (similar to the trail making test [53]), choice reaction time, digit span forward, and span of visual memory [64]. Switching of attention and choice reaction time tests are classified under the information processing domain in the IntegNeuro platform. Hence, our finding of no significant differences between BMI groups on these tests is in agreement with the results of Gunstad et al. [64].

There is increasing evidence of an association between obesity and attention disorders such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [66]. In fact, a reward deficiency syndrome has been identified in obesity and ADHD, which results in insufficient dopamine-mediated “natural” rewards, leading to the seeking of “unnatural rewards” such as risk taking, gambling, and uncontrolled eating [67, 68]. Dysfunctional attention and impulsivity in ADHD may lead to obesity via abnormal eating behaviors [66]. Obesity was also found to be significantly associated with impulsivity in the current study, using the go/no-go task to assess cognitive inhibition (this test assesses the capacity to suppress well-learned, automatic responses). Other studies which have employed the Iowa Gambling Task or delay discounting tasks provide further evidence of increased impulsivity in individuals with obesity [22, 6971].

Literature linking impulsivity and obesity is consistent with studies reporting that individuals with obesity may have difficulty inhibiting automatic or dominant behavior (response inhibition) and delaying gratification [69, 71, 72]. They may have a higher sensitivity to reward compared to normal weight controls [65, 6972]. Studies have also reported that high impulsivity is related to diminished weight loss during treatment and may predict attrition in those seeking treatment [72, 73]. Addressing deficits in attention and impulsivity may therefore be relevant for weight management interventions [73]. Moreover, reduced inhibition may underlie overeating in obesity [72, 73], and possible shared mechanisms/pathways between decreased attention and impulsivity (as discussed previously) may also explain the lower scores observed in the young women with obesity in this study.

Memory was also identified to be lower in the OB group compared to the NW group, although the association was less strong than for the attention and impulsivity domains (Figure 2()). Post hoc tests were not significant, but the univariate tests for memory remained significant after adjustment for three of the four confounders. The memory tests used in our study were memory recognition (immediate and delayed) and digit span (forward and reverse). Using the memory recognition tests, detrimental effects of obesity on memory were reported previously by Gunstad et al. in their cross-sectional study across the adult lifespan (n = 486, 21–82 y) [14]; however, when using the digit span forward test, there were no significant effects in the same population [64]. Leptin and ghrelin dysregulation has been implicated in both memory and obesity, which may account for observed differences in NW versus OB individuals [14, 7476]. Additionally, while requiring elucidation in humans, elevated BMI has been associated with increased concentrations of brain metabolites such as myoinositol/creatine (MI/Cr) [77]. Such metabolites accumulate in grey matter regions associated with memory and are suggested to indirectly influence memory performance [77]. Overall, the evidence in the literature regarding the effects of obesity on memory function is equivocal. Further research in this area is particularly important as obesity has been linked to an increased risk of dementia [78, 79].

4.1. Limitations and Conclusions

Overall, cognitive performance in OB participants was within normal range and was minimally influenced by physical activity, depression symptoms, inflammation, or omega-3 status. Adjusting for these potential confounders did not alter the significant BMI group differences for attention or impulsivity and only partially attenuated OB-related deficits for memory. Such findings indicate that obesity per se may influence cognition independently of these confounders [29, 80]. It is also important to consider that the relationship between obesity and cognition may be in the reverse direction such that specific cognitive attributes increase the risk for obesity [72, 73, 81]. This has certainly been previously proposed and associated in particular with impulsivity and attention as discussed earlier. More research is needed in this area and specifically how different cognitive limitations challenge effective weight management. As the cognitive function was within the normal range, it is difficult to speculate on the functional implications for everyday living or on the clinical significance of these cognitive differences.

This study was designed to examine cognition in young women with obesity and compare their performance with women within the normal weight range. We did not measure women in the overweight range, so we are unable to speculate on the cognitive performance of this “middle group” and this is an important limitation of the study. However, as one of the first studies investigating the association between uncomplicated obesity and cognition in young women, the finding of significant differences in cognitive function supports the need for additional research across the BMI spectrum. An important question for future research is whether a negative relationship (which could be linear or nonlinear) between cognitive performance and BMI applies across the BMI spectrum or, instead, whether there is a BMI threshold above which cognitive performance deteriorates. Future studies may also be able to incorporate brain imaging techniques to elucidate this further and determine if pathological changes are evident above a BMI threshold. Furthermore, given the relationships that have been reported between individual omega-6 as well as omega-3 fatty acids and cognitive decline in older adults and animal models [35, 36, 82, 83], in-depth lipid analysis will benefit future studies in this area. A further limitation of our study is that our focus on young women means that our results are not generalizable to young men. Our decision to focus on young women with obesity was primarily due to the greater rate of weight gain in young women when compared to young men and the prediction that more Australian women than men will be severely obese by 2025 [7].

This study is one of the first to exclude and/or adjust for a range of confounding variables when examining the influence of obesity on cognitive function in young women. The cohort was healthy, well educated, and free of known comorbidities. Exclusion or adjustment was also undertaken for potential confounders. Although the cross-sectional design of the study precludes causal relationships from being determined, our finding of reduced cognitive performance in the attention and impulsivity domains for young women with obesity, and weaker evidence for lower memory function when compared to normal weight women, deserves further examination via longitudinal research. Large epidemiological studies across the BMI spectrum and clinical trials examining the effect of acute and longer-term weight loss on the cognitive function of young women (or young adults) with obesity are warranted.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a grant from Meat and Livestock Australia. Hoi Lun Cheng holds the Marie Bashir Fellowship for Clinical Research in Adolescent Health, funded by New South Wales Health.

Disclosure

Preliminary results, which in this manuscript are presented in full with complete analyses and discussion, were presented as a poster at the European Obesity Summit (EOS)—Joint Congress of EASO and IFSO-EC, Gothenburg, Sweden, June 1–4, 2016, details of which can be found in the conference proceedings [84]. Meat and Livestock Australia had no influence on any part of the review process, including the conclusions drawn.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

Authors' Contributions

Rebecca L. Cook, Helen T. O'Connor, Nicholas J. O'Dwyer, Katharine S. Steinbeck, and Kieron B. Rooney designed the study. Rebecca L. Cook, Cheyne E. Donges, Hoi Lun Cheng, Helen M. Parker, Eka P. Cox, and Janet L. Franklin conducted the research. Rebecca L. Cook, Cheyne E. Donges, Nicholas J. O'Dwyer, Helen T. O'Connor, Helen M. Parker, and Manohar L. Garg analyzed and interpreted data. Rebecca L. Cook wrote the first draft, with Helen T. O'Connor and Nicholas J. O'Dwyer having primary responsibility for final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

References

  • 1.Ball K., Brown W., Crawford D. Who does not gain weight?. Prevalence and predictors of weight maintenance in young women. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders. 2002;26(12):1570–1578. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Brown W. J., Trost S. G. Life transitions and changing physical activity patterns in young women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2003;25(2):140–143. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(03)00119-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mishra G., Loxton D., Anderson A., et al. August 2015. http://www.alswh.org.au/images/content/pdf/major_reports/2014Major_Report I_FINAL.pdf.
  • 4.Brown W. J., Hockey R., Dobson A. J. Effects of having a baby on weight gain. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2010;38(2):163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.09.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Australian Bureau of Statistics. Version ABS cat. no 4364.0.55.003. October 2015, http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/abs@.nsf/DetailsPage/4364.0.55.0032011-2012?OpenDocument.
  • 6.Ball K., Crawford D., Ireland P., Hodge A. Patterns and demographic predictors of 5-year weight change in a multi-ethnic cohort of men and women in Australia. Public Health Nutrition. 2007;6(3):269–280. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hayes A. J., Lung T. W., Bauman A., Howard K. Modelling obesity trends in Australia: unravelling the past and predicting the future. International Journal of Obesity. 2017;41(1):178–185. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2016.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Haslam D. W., James W. P. Obesity. Lancet. 2005;366(9492):1197–1209. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67483-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Leddy M. A., Power M. L., Schulkin J. The impact of maternal obesity on maternal and fetal health. Reviews in Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2008;1(4):170–178. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lutsiv O., Mah J., Beyene J., McDonald S. D. The effects of morbid obesity on maternal and neonatal health outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analyses. Obesity Reviews. 2015;16(7):531–546. doi: 10.1111/obr.12283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Metwally M., Li T. C., Ledger W. L. The impact of obesity on female reproductive function. Obesity Reviews. 2007;8(6):515–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2007.00406.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gaillard R. Maternal obesity during pregnancy and cardiovascular development and disease in the offspring. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2015;30(11):1141–1152. doi: 10.1007/s10654-015-0085-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Santangeli L., Sattar N., Huda S. S. Impact of maternal obesity on perinatal and childhood outcomes. Best Practice & Research Clinical Obstetrics & Gynaecology. 2015;29(3):438–448. doi: 10.1016/j.bpobgyn.2014.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gunstad J., Paul R. H., Cohen R. A., Tate D. F., Gordon E. Obesity is associated with memory deficits in young and middle-aged adults. Eating and Weight Disorders. 2006;11(1):e15–e19. doi: 10.1007/bf03327747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cheke L. G., Simons J. S., Clayton N. S. Higher body mass index is associated with episodic memory deficits in young adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2016;69(11):2305–2316. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1099163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fitzpatrick S., Gilbert S., Serpell L. Systematic review: are overweight and obese individuals impaired on behavioural tasks of executive functioning? Neuropsychology Review. 2013;23(2):138–156. doi: 10.1007/s11065-013-9224-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Prickett C., Brennan L., Stolwyk R. Examining the relationship between obesity and cognitive function: a systematic literature review. Obesity Research & Clinical Practice. 2015;9(2):93–113. doi: 10.1016/j.orcp.2014.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Siervo M., Arnold R., Wells J. C., et al. Intentional weight loss in overweight and obese individuals and cognitive function: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Reviews. 2011;12(11):968–983. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00903.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Smith E., Hay P., Campbell L., Trollor J. N. A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obesity Reviews. 2011;12(9):740–755. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00920.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Danner U. N., Ouwehand C., van Haastert N. L., Hornsveld H., de Ridder D. T. D. Decision-making impairments in women with binge eating disorder in comparison with obese and normal weight women. European Eating Disorders Review. 2012;20(1):e56–e62. doi: 10.1002/erv.1098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Davis C., Patte K., Curtis C., Reid C. Immediate pleasures and future consequences. A neuropsychological study of binge eating and obesity. Appetite. 2010;54(1):208–213. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pignatti R., Bertella L., Albani G., Mauro A., Molinari E., Semenza C. Decision-making in obesity: a study using the Gambling Task. Eating and Weight Disorders. 2006;11(3):126–132. doi: 10.1007/bf03327557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Crichton G. E., Elias M. F., Buckley J. D., Murphy K. J., Bryan J., Frisardi V. Metabolic syndrome, cognitive performance, and dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2012;30(2):S77–S87. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.van den Berg E., Kloppenborg R. P., Kessels R. P., Kappelle L. J., Biessels G. J. Type 2 diabetes mellitus, hypertension, dyslipidemia and obesity: a systematic comparison of their impact on cognition. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta. 2009;1792(5):470–481. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2008.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Elias M. F., Elias P. K., Sullivan L. M., Wolf P. A., D’Agostino R. B. Lower cognitive function in the presence of obesity and hypertension: the Framingham Heart Study. International Journal of Obesity. 2003;27(2):260–268. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.802225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.McAfoose J., Baune B. T. Evidence for a cytokine model of cognitive function. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33(3):355–366. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Saxby B. K., Harrington F., McKeith I. G., Wesnes K., Ford G. A. Effects of hypertension on attention, memory, and executive function in older adults. Health Psychology. 2003;22(6):587–591. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gimeno D., Marmot M. G., Singh-Manoux A. Inflammatory markers and cognitive function in middle-aged adults: the Whitehall II study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(10):1322–1334. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sweat V., Starr V., Bruehl H., et al. C-reactive protein is linked to lower cognitive performance in overweight and obese women. Inflammation. 2008;31(3):198–207. doi: 10.1007/s10753-008-9065-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cox E. P., O’Dwyer N., Cook R., et al. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: a systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2016;19(8):612–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2015.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hamer M., Chida Y. Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychological Medicine. 2009;39(1):3–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Australian Bureau of Statistics. October 2014. http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/Lookup/4364.0.55.004Chapter4002011-12.
  • 33.World Health Organisation. October 2014. http://www.who.int/dietphysicalactivity/factsheet_inactivity/en/
  • 34.Moreno J. P., Johnston C. A. Barriers to physical activity in women. American Journal of Lifestyle Medicine. 2014;8(3):164–166. doi: 10.1177/1559827614521954. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Conquer J. A., Tierney M. C., Zecevic J., Bettger W. J., Fisher R. H. Fatty acid analysis of blood plasma of patients with Alzheimer’s disease, other types of dementia, and cognitive impairment. Lipids. 2000;35(12):1305–1312. doi: 10.1007/s11745-000-0646-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cunnane S. C., Schneider J. A., Tangney C., et al. Plasma and brain fatty acid profiles in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2012;29(3):691–697. doi: 10.3233/jad-2012-110629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kalmijn S., van Boxtel M. P. J., Ocke M., Verschuren W. M. M., Kromhout D., Launer L. J. Dietary intake of fatty acids and fish in relation to cognitive performance at middle age. Neurology. 2004;62(2):275–280. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000103860.75218.a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Morris M. C., Evans D. A., Bienias J. L., et al. Consumption of fish and n-3 fatty acids and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology. 2003;60(7):940–946. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.7.940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Samieri C., Feart C., Letenneur L., et al. Low plasma eicosapentaenoic acid and depressive symptomatology are independent predictors of dementia risk. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2008;88(3):714–721. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2015.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schaefer E. J., Bongard V., Beiser A. S., et al. Plasma phosphatidylcholine docosahexaenoic acid content and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: the Framingham Heart Study. Archives of Neurology. 2006;63(11):1545–1550. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.11.1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.van Gelder B. M., Tijhuis M., Kalmijn S., Kromhout D. Fish consumption, n-3 fatty acids, and subsequent 5-y cognitive decline in elderly men: the Zutphen Elderly Study. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2007;85(4):1142–1147. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.4.1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Papanikolaou Y., Brooks J., Reider C., Fulgoni V. L. U.S. adults are not meeting recommended levels for fish and omega-3 fatty acid intake: results of an analysis using observational data from NHANES 2003-2008. Nutrition Journal. 2014;13:p. 31. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-13-31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Meyer B. J. Australians are not meeting the recommended intakes for omega-3 long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: results of an analysis from the 2011-2012 National Nutrition and Physical Activity Survey. Nutrients. 2016;8(3):p. 111. doi: 10.3390/nu8030111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Markhus M. W., Skotheim S., Graff I. E., et al. Low omega-3 index in pregnancy is a possible biological risk factor for postpartum depression. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):p. e67617. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Min Y., Djahanbakhch O., Hutchinson J., et al. Efficacy of docosahexaenoic acid-enriched formula to enhance maternal and fetal blood docosahexaenoic acid levels: randomized double-blinded placebo-controlled trial of pregnant women with gestational diabetes mellitus. Clinical Nutrition. 2016;35(3):608–614. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2015.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lam E., Partridge S. R., Allman-Farinelli M. Strategies for successful recruitment of young adults to healthy lifestyle programmes for the prevention of weight gain: a systematic review. Obesity Reviews. 2016;17(2):178–200. doi: 10.1111/obr.12350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.World Health Organisation. October 2015. http://apps.who.int/bmi/index.jsp?introPage=intro_3.html.
  • 48.Salthouse T. A. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging. 2009;30(4):507–514. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Halterman J. S., Kaczorowski J. M., Aligne C. A., Auinger P., Szilagyi P. G. Iron deficiency and cognitive achievement among school-aged children and adolescents in the United States. Pediatrics. 2001;107(6):1381–1386. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.6.1381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Craig C. L., Marshall A. L., Sjostrom M., et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2003;35(8):1381–1395. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000078924.61453.fb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ekelund U., Sepp H., Brage S., et al. Criterion-related validity of the last 7-day, short form of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire in Swedish adults. Public Health Nutrition. 2006;9(2):258–265. doi: 10.1079/PHN2005840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Henry J. D., Crawford J. R. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2005;44(2):227–239. doi: 10.1348/014466505x29657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sugarman R. IntegNeuroTM User Manual Version 3. Australia: Brain Resource Company, New South Wales; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Alberti K. G. M. M., Zimmet P., Shaw J. Metabolic syndrome—a new world-wide definition. A consensus statement from the International Diabetes Federation. Diabetic Medicine. 2006;23(5):469–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2006.01858.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Paul R. H., Lawrence J., Williams L. M., Clark R. C., Cooper N., Gordon E. Preliminary validity of “IntegNeuroTM”: a new computerized battery of neurocognitive tests. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;115(11):1549–1567. doi: 10.1080/00207450590957890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Williams L. M., Simms E., Clark C. R., Paul R. H., Rowe D., Gordon E. The test-retest reliability of a standardized neurocognitive and neurophysiological test battery: “neuromarker”. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;115(12):1605–1630. doi: 10.1080/00207450590958475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Northrop-Clewes C. A., Thurnham D. I. Biomarkers for the differentiation of anemia and their clinical usefulness. Journal of Blood Medicine. 2013;4:11–22. doi: 10.2147/JBM.S29212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Thurnham D. I., McCabe L. D., Haldar S., Wieringa F. T., Northrop-Clewes C. A., McCabe G. P. Adjusting plasma ferritin concentrations to remove the effects of subclinical inflammation in the assessment of iron deficiency: a meta-analysis. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2010;92(3):546–555. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2010.29284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ferguson J. J., Veysey M., Lucock M., et al. Association between omega-3 index and blood lipids in older Australians. The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry. 2016;27:233–240. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2015.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Harris W. S., Von Schacky C. The omega-3 index: a new risk factor for death from coronary heart disease? Preventive Medicine. 2004;39(1):212–220. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.02.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.World Health Organisation. Definition and diagnosis of diabetes mellitus and intermediate hyperglycemia: report of a WHO/IDF consultation. Geneva: International Diabetes Federation; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Rowe D. L., Cooper N. J., Liddell B. J., Clark C. R., Gordon E., Williams L. M. Brain structure and function correlates of general and social cognition. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience. 2007;6(1):35–74. doi: 10.1142/s021963520700143x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Pollock M. L., Gaesser G. A., Butcher J. D., et al. ACSM position stand: the recommended quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory and muscular fitness, and flexibility in healthy adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 1998;30(6):975–991. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199806000-00032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gunstad J., Paul R. H., Cohen R. A., Tate D. F., Spitznagel M. B., Gordon E. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2007;48(1):57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Cserjési R., Luminet O., Poncelet A.-S., Lénárd L. Altered executive function in obesity. Exploration of the role of affective states on cognitive abilities. Appetite. 2009;52(2):535–539. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Cortese S., Moreira-Maia C. R., St Fleur D., Morcillo-Peñalver C., Rohde L. A., Faraone S. V. Association between ADHD and obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2016;173(1):34–43. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15020266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Altfas J. R. Prevalence of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adults in obesity treatment. BMC Psychiatry. 2002;2:p. 9. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-2-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Comings D. E., Blum K. Reward deficiency syndrome: genetic aspects of behavioral disorders; Proceedings from the 21st International Summer School of Brain Research; Amsterdam, Netherlands. 1999. pp. 325–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Brogan A., Hevey D., Pignatti R. Anorexia, bulimia, and obesity: shared decision making deficits on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2010;16(4):711–715. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fagundo A. B., de la Torre R., Jimenez-Murcia S., et al. Executive functions profile in extreme eating/weight conditions: from anorexia nervosa to obesity. PLoS One. 2012;7(8):p. e43382. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Weller R. E., Cook E. W., 3rd, Avsar K. B., Cox J. E. Obese women show greater delay discounting than healthy-weight women. Appetite. 2008;51(3):563–569. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Nederkoorn C., Smulders F. T. Y., Havermans R. C., Roefs A., Jansen A. Impulsivity in obese women. Appetite. 2006;47(2):253–256. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Cortese S., Castellanos F. X. The relationship between ADHD and obesity: implications for therapy. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics. 2014;14(5):473–479. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2014.904748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Carlini V. P., Varas M. M., Cragnolini A. B., Schioth H. B., Scimonelli T. N., de Barioglio S. R. Differential role of the hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal raphe nucleus in regulating feeding, memory, and anxiety-like behavioral responses to ghrelin. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications. 2004;313(3):635–641. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2003.11.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Diano S., Farr S. A., Benoit S. C., et al. Ghrelin controls hippocampal spine synapse density and memory performance. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9(3):381–388. doi: 10.1038/nn1656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Farr S. A., Banks W. A., Morley J. E. Effects of leptin on memory processing. Peptides. 2006;27(6):1420–1425. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2005.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Gonzales M. M., Tarumi T., Eagan D. E., Tanaka H., Vaghasia M., Haley A. P. Indirect effects of elevated body mass index on memory performance through altered cerebral metabolite concentrations. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2012;74(7):691–698. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31825ff1de. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Beydoun M. A., Beason-Held L. L. Does hypertension interact with body weight to impact cognitive function in the elderly?: emerging evidence. American Journal of Hypertension. 2008;21(6):p. 603. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2008.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Kullmann S., Callaghan M. F., Heni M., et al. Specific white matter tissue microstructure changes associated with obesity. Neuroimage. 2016;125:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Luchtman D. W., Song C. Cognitive enhancement by omega-3 fatty acids from child-hood to old age: findings from animal and clinical studies. Neuropharmacology. 2013;64:550–565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Elias M. F., Goodell A. L., Waldstein S. R. Obesity, cognitive functioning and dementia: back to the future. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2012;30(2):S113–S125. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Amtul Z., Uhrig M., Wang L., Rozmahel R. F., Beyreuther K. Detrimental effects of arachidonic acid and its metabolites in cellular and mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease: structural insight. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33(4):e21–e31. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Mapstone M., Cheema A. K., Fiandaca M. S., et al. Plasma phospholipids identify antecedent memory impairment in older adults. Nature Medicine. 2014;20(4):415–418. doi: 10.1038/nm.3466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.O’Dwyer N., Cook R., Donges C., et al. Relationship between obesity and cognitive function in young normal weight and obese women. Obesity Facts. 2016;9(1):p. 189. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Obesity are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES