Skip to main content
Current Neuropharmacology logoLink to Current Neuropharmacology
. 2017 Oct;15(7):1043–1055. doi: 10.2174/1570159X15666170313122555

Cognitive Impacts of Estrogen Treatment in Androgen-Deprived Males: What Needs to be Resolved

Erik Wibowo 1,*
PMCID: PMC5652012  PMID: 28294068

Abstract

Background:

Many prostate cancer (PCa) patients are on androgen deprivation therapy (ADT) as part of their cancer treatments but ADT may lead to cognitive impairments. ADT depletes men of both androgen and estrogen. Whether estradiol supplementation can improve cognitive impairments in patients on ADT is understudied.

Objective:

To summarize data on the effects of estradiol treatment on cognitive function of androgen-deprived genetic male populations (PCa patients and male-to-female transsexuals) and castrated male animals.

Method:

Publications were identified by a literature search on PubMed and Google Scholar.

Results:

While some studies showed that estradiol improves cognitive function (most notably, spatial ability) for castrated rats, what remains uninvestigated are: 1) whether estradiol can improve cognition after long-term androgen deprivation, 2) how estradiol affects memory retention, and 3) how early vs. delayed estradiol treatment after castration influences cognition. For androgen-deprived genetic males, estradiol treatment may improve some cognitive functions (e.g., verbal and visual memory), but the findings are not consistent due to large variability in the study design between studies.

Conclusion:

Future studies are required to determine the best estradiol treatment protocol to maximize cognitive benefits for androgen-deprived genetic males. Tests that assess comparable cognitive domains in human and rodents are needed. What particularly under-investigated is how the effects of estradiol on cognitive ability intersect with other parameters; sleep, depression and physical fatigue. Such studies have clinical implications to improve the quality of life for both PCa patients on ADT as well as for male-to-female transsexuals.

Keywords: Estradiol, cognition, prostate cancer, androgen deprivation therapy, male-to-female transsexuals, verbal memory, visual memory, spatial memory

1. INTRODUCTION

Androgen deprivation therapy (ADT) is the primary treatment for systemic prostate cancer (PCa), with more than a half million men on ADT in North America [1]. ADT has many deleterious side effects, including potential deficits in cognitive function (reviewed in [2-4]). Furthermore, ADT can also result in suboptimal sleep, fatigue, and depression [5-12], all of which may contribute to cognitive decline.

The cognitive impairment after androgen loss is also observed in animal models. Castration in rodents influences both spatial [13-19] and non-spatial [15, 20-22] abilities. For example, castrated rats are slower to acquire spatial working memory [13, 14, 23, 24]. However, findings on how 
castrated rats learn spatial reference memory have been inconsistent [16, 17, 25, 26].

The cognitive impact of ADT may be associated with the loss of both testosterone and estradiol (E2), which in males is mainly derived from the aromatization of testosterone [27]. Decline in gonadal hormones decreases ligand bioavailability to bind to sex steroid receptors which are 
present in various brain areas, including those involved in cognition, such as the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex [28-31]. The activation of brain sex steroid receptors plays a role in normal cognitive functions. For example, without functional androgen receptors, male rodents have deficits in spatial reference memory [32, 33]. Furthermore, individuals with androgen insensitivity syndrome [34] and idiopathic hypogonadotropic hypogonadism [35] have reduced cognitive ability (in the spatial domain).

Replacing testosterone is not considered a safe option for PCa patients with androgen-sensitive PCa because testosterone can activate androgen receptor and stimulate PCa growth [36]. Estrogen replacement therapy is currently being studied, though, in clinical trials for patients who are on ADT [37, 38]. However, data on how E2 influences cognitive function on androgen-deprived males are sparse, as compared to data for hormone-deprived females (see the special 2015 issue on “Estradiol and Cognition” in Hormones and Behavior vol. 74, and recent reviews [39, 40]). This sex/gender disparity reflects the fact that E2 is the dominant sex steroid in females and is present more than ten times the concentrations in females than in males (for example, see [41] vs. [42] for rodents data; and [43] vs. [44] for human data).

Even in eugonadal males, there is no consensus on how endogenous E2 modulates cognition. A few studies have shown that men with higher E2 levels have better visual [45], visuospatial [46] and verbal [47, 48] memory performances. However, most studies found that higher plasma E2 concentrations in older men have either no or negative association with cognitive functions [49]. The varying data on the influence of endogenous E2 in eugonadal men could partially be because serum E2 assessment may not reflect the actual impact of E2 in the brain. This is because neurons in many brain areas—including those for cognitive functions—can directly aromatize testosterone into E2 [50, 51]. Determining the effects of endogenous E2 in eugonadal men is also confounded by the presence of endogenous androgens and sex hormone binding globulin, which may affect the bioavailability of gonadal steroids. Suffice to say, a few studies show that reduced E2 bioavailability in male rodents due to aromatase-knock out [52] or administration of aromatase inhibitor [53] impairs spatial task performances, though conflicting data have been reported [54, 55].

This paper reviews studies that have investigated the effects of E2 on cognitive function of androgen-deprived genetic males of our species—i.e., PCa patients on ADT and male-to-female transsexuals (MtFs) on hormone therapy (HT)—as well as castrated male rodents. These studies help isolate the effects of E2 on male cognition without the impacts of endogenous testosterone. Many MtFs are androgen-deprived due to surgical castration as part of sex reassignment surgery or HT with a dose high enough to suppress testosterone. Data from MtFs brings additional insight on how HT affects cognition of genetic male population. Data on castrated male rats are also reviewed because they served as a preclinical model for men who are on ADT. Admittedly there are limitations on animal research but they can provide insights on how E2 modulates cognitive function in androgen-deprived males. This paper points out areas that are understudied, but would be important to consider in the future when researchers investigate cognitive impact of ADT on PCa patients with or without E2 treatment. Studies in this area are clinically relevant to other genetic male populations who receive E2 treatment, such as MtFs [56, 57] and castrated men [58, 59].

2. METHODS

PubMed and Google Scholar were searched for terms including “estradiol”, “estrogen”, “cognition”, “male”, “prostate cancer”, “androgen deprivation therapy”, “male-to-female transsexuals”, and “cross-sex hormones”. From this search I identified publications that investigated the impact of E2 treatment on castrated male rats, PCa patients on ADT, and MtFs on HT. Additional publications were located in 
the references cited for papers found in the original search. Findings on how E2 affects specific cognitive domains are summarized on Tables 1 to 3. Experimental details, including E2 dose and duration, are summarized in Supplementary Tables 1 (187.5KB, pdf) and 2 (187.5KB, pdf) .

Table 1.

Summaries on the effects of E2 treatment on cognitive performance of castrated male rats. Data were compared to the Control group, which consists of castrated rats without E2 treatment.

Cognitive Domain Tested
(Testing Apparatus)
Studies Effects of E2 Treatment Parameters Measured
Spatial learning
  Barnes maze [71] Changes in path length, latency to find a goal box, and number of errors on
Day 1 testing.
  Barnes maze [25] Changes in path length, latency to find a goal box, and number of errors on
Day 1 testing.
  Cross-maze [23] Percentage of correct choices between the first and fourth day of testing.
  Operant conditioning chamber [15] 0 Number of sessions to reach 25 correct choices in a single session; i.e., to alternately press 2 levers for water reward.
  T-maze [61] Percentage of trials with a correct response; i.e., to visit an arm in which the rats previously received a food reward.
  T-maze [13] 0 Number of sessions to reach 7 correct choices in 9 tests; i.e., to visit alternating baited (with water reward) arm consecutively.
  Radial arm maze [60] 0 Number of trials to obtain 7 correct choices in the first 8 visits—to get food reward in 8 arms—in 5 consecutive trials.
Spatial working memory
  Barnes maze [71] ↑ on Day 1
0 on Day 2
Spatial working memory error was measured from the number of re-investigation of incorrect holes.
  Barnes maze [25] ↑ on Day 1
0 on Day 2
Spatial working memory error was measured from the number of re-investigation of incorrect holes.
  Open field with 2 identical objects [63] Time spent exploring moved and unmoved objects.
  Morris water maze [26] ↓ with EB
0 with ED
Changes in escape latency between the final and first day of testing.
  T-maze [61] 0 Percentage of trials with a correct response (to visit an arm in which the rats previously received a food reward) after the maze was rotated and inter-trial delay was increased.
  Radial arm maze [60] Data not shown The number of re-entries into previously visited arm was collected (indicative of working memory errors) but data were not shown.
Spatial reference memory
  Barnes maze [71] 0 Path length and latency to find a goal box on Day 2 (1 day delay) testing. Spatial reference memory error was measured from the number of first-time investigation of incorrect holes.
  Barnes maze [25] 0 on Day 2
↑ on Day 7
Path length and latency to find a goal box on Day 2 (1 day delay) and Day 7
(1 week delay) testing. Spatial reference memory error was measured from the number of first-time investigation of incorrect holes.
  Morris water maze [26] 0 Escape latency (i.e., time to find a submerged platform).
  Cross-maze [23] Averaged percentage of correct choices in 4 days of testing.
  T-maze [61] 0 Percentage of trials with a correct response; i.e., to visit an arm in which the rats previously received a food reward.
  Radial arm maze [60] 0 with no delay
↑ with delay
Number of correct choices (visits to baited arms) in the first 8 visits. Trials were done with and without 1-hour delay between the 4th and 5th arm visits.
Cognitive Domain Tested
(Testing Apparatus)
Studies Effects of E2 Treatment Parameters Measured
Object recognition memory
Open field with 2 objects [21] 0 Time spent exploring novel and old objects.
Learning of extradimensional shift
Operant conditioning chamber [15] 0 Number of sessions to reach 90% accuracy of a task; i.e., to press a lever with a cue light in order to receive a water reward; another lever with unlit lamp was present simultaneously.
Withholding response
Operant conditioning chamber [15] Percentage of impulsive lever press over total lever press.
Motivation for reward
Operant conditioning chamber [15] Number of water reward obtained.
Behavioural flexibility (learning of match or non-match-to-position)
Operant conditioning chamber [15] 0 Number of sessions to reach 90% accuracy of a task; i.e., in a pair of testing phases, rats needed to either press the same (match) or a different (non-match) lever in the second test as that in the first test in order to get a water reward.
Learning of configural association negative patterning
Operant conditioning chamber [61] 0 Changes in the number of responses (i.e., to enter a food cup for reward) and time to respond following a tone, light or both stimuli.

↑↓ = Significantly better or worse performance than castrated rats with no hormone treatment. 0 = No significant effect. E2 = Estradiol. EB = Estradiol benzoate. ED = Estradiol dipropionate.

Table 3.

Summaries on the effects of E2 treatment on cognitive performance of MtFs who are on HT. Some categories were from McGinty et al. [4]. Two studies [86, 92] were cross-sectional studies, and the rest were longitudinal studies. All cases were non-blinded studies. More details can be found in Supplementary Table 2.

Cognitive Domain Tested (Test used) Studies Effects of E2 Treatment
Attention and working memory
  Digit span [92]
[88]
0
↑ after HT
Language
  Controlled association test [92]
[88]
0
0
  Paced verb generation test [87] 0
  Paced categorical decision test [87] 0
  Verbal fluency test (word production) [89]
[91]

0
  Verbal fluency test (sentence production)
[89]
[91]
[88]

0
0
  Verbal reasoning test
[89]
[91]
[90]
0
0
0
  Vocabulary [92]
[88]
0
0
Verbal memory
  Logical memory task (WMS-R) [92]
[88]
0
↑ after long-term HT
  Verbal paired associates (WMS-R) [92]
[88]
> MtFs group
↑ after off HT
Visual memory
  Visual reproduction test [88] 0
  Visual paired associate learning [88] 0
↑ after off HT for ≥8 weeks
  Figural memory test [88] ↑ after long-term HT
  Visual memory span [88] 0
  Object memory test [88] 0
  Location memory test [88] 0
Visuomotor skill
  Fine motor movement test [91] 0
  Targeted throwing [90] 0
Visuospatial ability
  Line orientation test [90] 0
  Judgment of line angle and position [88] 0
  Mental rotation (2-dimensional) [91]
[90]
↓ at 3 months; but 0 post-SRS and 5 weeks off HT
↑ but other groups ↑ too*
  Mental rotation (3-dimensional)



[86]
[87]
[92]
[91]
[90]
[88]
= MtFs group
0
0
0
↑ but non-MtF groups ↑ too*
0
  Mental rotation (3D, same-different) [90] ↑ but non-MtF groups ↑ too*
  Hidden figure test [91] ↑ at 3 months; but 0 post-SRS and 5 weeks off HT
  Perceptual speed test [91] 0
  Card rotation test [89]

↑↓ Significantly improved or worse performance at follow-up visit. 0 no significant difference at follow-up. HT = hormone therapy. MtFs group = MtFs who do not receive HT. RCT = Randomized controlled trial. SRS = sex-reassignment surgery. WMS-R = Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised. * = the study included males and females without gender dysphoria.

3. STUDIES ON CASTRATED MALE RATS

3.1. Experimental Details

Ten studies have investigated how E2 influences the cognitive performances of castrated male rats (Supplementary Table 1 (187.5KB, pdf) ). In seven studies, the rats received E2 treatment immediately after castration whereas, in the other three studies [23, 26, 60], the rats started E2 treatment at 1-2 weeks after castration. Most studies administered E2 via a Silastic capsule or slow-release E2 pellets. The only exceptions were Hodosy et al. [26] and Lagunas et al. [23] who injected E2 intramuscularly and intraperitoneally respectively.

Plasma E2 levels were only measured in two studies [26, 61]. E2 levels were similar to female diestrus levels in both studies, but Hodosy et al. [26] also treated one more group of rats with a dose resulting in a plasma E2 levels almost four times higher than female proestrus levels [see Butcher et al. [41] for normal female E2 levels in estrus cycle]. Five studies used the same E2 dose as the one in the Kritzer’s study [62], which increased the plasma E2 levels to ~26.5 pg/mL (i.e., similar to diestrus levels in females). The remaining three studies used E2 doses that have been used by other researchers for raising plasma E2 levels of female rats to either diestrus [23] or proestrus [60, 63] levels. However, whether such doses in fact increased plasma E2 levels to those levels are not known. In my previous study [64], male rats required higher E2 dose than female rats to achieve similar E2 levels [see Deurveilher et al. [65] for comparison]. Therefore, there is a possibility that the plasma E2 levels in those studies [23, 60, 63] may be lower than expected. To date, a dose of E2 that only brings plasma E2 levels into the range of intact males (i.e., ~2 pg/mL [42, 61, 66]) has not been tested in cognitive studies on castrated rats. As such, it remains unknown as to how an E2 dose that raises the plasma E2 levels to the levels of intact male rats would influence cognitive function for the castrated male rats.

One factor, which is rarely investigated in animal studies, is how the interval between castration and the initiation of E2 treatment (Supplementary Table 1 (187.5KB, pdf) ) influences the effects of E2 treatment on cognitive function. Except in three studies [23, 26, 60], the rodents started receiving E2 treatment immediately after castration. In female rats, the effectiveness of E2 in improving cognitive performance is known to be sensitive to the time when the treatment begins after ovariectomy [67]. Similar findings have also been observed in women, who receive E2 treatment after menopause [68]. It remains unknown, though, how long-term hormone deprivation affects male cognitive function, and when the best time might be to start E2 treatment after castration to maximize any cognitive benefit.

One possible factor that likely contributes to the time-sensitive nature of E2’s effectiveness in restoring cognition is the autoregulation of estrogen receptor α (ERα) in brain areas for cognition. In female rats, early but not delayed E2 administration after ovariectomy increased ERα in the hippocampus, and the opposite results were found in the prefrontal cortex [69]. Changes in ERα autoregulation in both brain areas after prolonged hormone-deprivation have also been observed in male rodents. In male rats, hippocampal ERα decreases with early, but not delayed, E2 treatment after castration [30]. The opposite was found in the prefrontal cortex; i.e., ERα in the prefrontal cortex decreases with delayed E2 treatment [30]. How those results translate into cognitive performances for male rats has not been assessed (but see the discussion below on visual attention of androgen-deprived PCa patients with and without E2), and there may be other factors that may explain why the effect of E2 on cognition after hormone deprivation is time-sensitive. Interestingly, the patterns of brain ERα autoregulation in response to E2 treatment between sexes differ; i.e., downregulated in male [30] and upregulated in female [69] rats. Additionally, a recent study [70] showed that how E2 induces synaptic potentiation in the hippocampus is not the same in males and females. Specifically, E2 can increase pre-synaptic glutamate release probability in the hippocampus via ERα in males, but via ERβ in females. That same study also found that E2 can increase post-synaptic sensitivity to glutamate via ERβ in males, but via the G-protein coupled ER-1. These data suggest that there may be true sex differences in how E2 modulates cognition in males versus females.

3.2. Behavioural Findings

Among the reviewed studies, the cognitive domain that is most commonly assessed is spatial ability (Table 1; Supplementary Table 1 (187.5KB, pdf) ). These include spatial learning, spatial working memory, and spatial reference memory. Learning tasks involving spatial memory in castrated rats are improved by E2 treatment in some studies [23, 25, 61, 71] but not in others [13, 15, 60]. Obviously, the type of testing apparatus/mazes differs between studies, leading to variation in the difficulty of the tasks in the different tests. As such, this variation may contribute to discrepancy in findings on spatial learning.

Spatial working memory of castrated rats is improved by E2 treatment in some studies [23, 25, 63, 71], but not in other studies [60, 61] (Table 1). Again, the variance in results could be because of the different testing apparatus. Unlike the apparatus in other studies, the T-maze in the Gibbs’ study [61] was walled (5-inches high) so the rats may have less extra-maze cues. In addition, the radial maze in the Luine and Rodriguez study [60] has 8 arms so performance on this maze may have higher working memory demand. Negative impact of E2 on spatial working memory of castrated rats was also reported in one study [26], but that study used the Morris water maze, so their data could be confounded by stress due to water immersion [72].

Several studies have examined how E2 treatment affects spatial reference memory (Table 1)—a type of long-term memory that is constant from trial to trial [73, 74]. When there are minimal delays between testing, the acquisition of spatial reference memory is not significantly different between castrated rats with and without E2 [25, 26, 60, 71]. However, when a time delay is introduced between testing [25, 60], E2-treated castrated rats performed better on spatial reference memory task than do castrated rats with no hormone replacement. This suggests that a substantial delay between tests may be necessary to assess the beneficial effect of E2 on spatial reference memory. Future studies with such delay would more closely reflect real life situations for men, where the concern is on both previously learned and newly acquired spatial tasks.

The effect of castration with and without E2 replacement on non-spatial tasks has also been studied, but to a lesser extent (Table 1). For example, Aubele et al. [21] assessed object recognition memory, which requires visual memory, and found that E2 does not improve object recognition memory after castration. Kritzer et al. [15] also examined several other non-spatial tasks, but the only two tasks in which they found improvement with E2 treatment in castrated rats were the learning of withholding response (holding back from getting a reward) and motivational (motivation to get a reward) tasks. Therefore, though only reported in one study [15], E2 treatment in androgen-deprived males may also have some benefits on non-spatial cognitive functions.

4. STUDIES ON ANDROGEN-DEPRIVED GENETIC MALE HUMAN

4.1. Androgen-Deprived Men with Prostate Cancer

Three studies (total sample size = 41 patients; mean ages = ~70 years old) have investigated how E2 treatments affect cognitive functions for PCa patients on ADT (Supplementary Table 2 (187.5KB, pdf) ). Serum testosterone levels of the patients were in the castrate range in all studies. Patients received 0.6-1 mg E2 daily for 4 to 12 weeks. Serum E2 levels reached supraphysiological levels in one study [75], but not in the other two [76, 77].

Compared to the performance of androgen-deprived PCa patients without E2, scores on verbal memory tests of E2-treated patients were better in the Beer et al. study [75], but worse in the Matousek and Sherwin study [76] (Table 2). Verbal memory was also assessed by Taxel et al. [77], but by using a different test than the one used in the other two studies. However, Taxel et al. did not find a significant difference in verbal memory performance between the E2- and placebo-treated PCa patients. Considering that patients in the Beer et al. study [75] had supraphysiological E2 levels, but not in the other two studies, an E2 dose that yields supraphysiological E2 levels may be required to benefit verbal memory for androgen-deprived PCa patients.

Table 2.

Summaries on the effects of E2 treatment on cognitive performance of PCa patients who are on ADT. Categories were from McGinty et al. [4]. The three studies were longitudinal studies. Two studies [76, 77] were double blind RCTs, but one study [75] was not a blinded and treatment assignment was not randomized. More details can be found in Supplementary Table 2.

Cognitive Domain Tested
(Test used)
Studies Effects of E2 Treatment
Attention and working memory
  Trail making test A [77]
[75]

0
  Digit span [77] 0
  Digit symbol test (WAIS-III) [76] 0
  Letter-number sequencing task [76] 0
  Subject-ordered pointing [75] 0
Executive function
  Stroop test [77]
  Trail making test B [77]
[75]
0
0
Language
  Controlled oral word association test [77] 0
  Verbal fluency test [76] 0
Verbal memory
  Logical memory task (WMS-R) [76]
[75]
< ADT group
  Verbal paired associates (WMS-R) [76] 0
  Rey auditory verbal learning test [77] 0
Visual memory
  Benton visual retention test [77] 0
Visuomotor skill
  Digit symbol test [76] 0
  Block design [76] 0
  Paper folding [76] 0
Visuospatial ability
  Mental rotation test [76] 0

↑↓ = Significantly improved or worse performance at follow-up visit. ADT group = patients on ADT but do not receive E2. RCT = Randomized controlled trial. WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition. WMS-R = Wechsler Memory Scale - Revised.

Visual attention was tested with Trail tests in two 
of these studies [75, 77]. E2 treatment improved visual attention in the Taxel et al. study, but not in the Beer et al. study. One possible reason for this inconsistency could be because of the difference in the length of time between the initiation of ADT and the onset of E2 treatment. Patients in the Beer et al. study had been androgen-deprived for an 
average of 5.32 years when they began E2 treatment. In contrast, patients in the Taxel et al. study had been androgen-deprived for a shorter time; some had been on continuous ADT for an average of 2.6 years, whereas others were only on ADT for 3 weeks. Thus, E2 may not be able to improve visual attention in men, who have been androgen-deprived for a prolonged period of time. If this is true, such finding will be consistent with the data in females (discussed in the “Studies in Castrated Male Rats” section above), showing that the effect of E2 treatment is sensitive to when the treatment is started after hormone deprivation.

Visuospatial ability was tested in the Matousek and Sherwin study [76], but E2 treatment did not improve performance in visuospatial tests. This could be because the men in that study were only androgen-deprived for 12 weeks. Similar lack of impact of E2 on visuospatial ability was also found in healthy men [78] or PCa patients [79], who were on ADT for less than 6 months. Currently, data on visuospatial ability of PCa patients, who have been on ADT for 9-12 months, have been inconsistent (see [80-83] vs. [83-85]). How long-term ADT (i.e., >1 year) affects cognitive functions has not been studied, but warrants investigation because PCa patients with progressing systemic disease typically are on sustained ADT that can last for many years.

4.2. Male-to-Female Transsexuals

Seven studies have investigated the cognitive performances of MtFs, who are on HT (total sample size = 179 MtFs; mean ages vary between 22 and 40.3 years old). Admittedly, the participants of these studies are younger than PCa population (Supplementary Table 2 (187.5KB, pdf) ). HT regimens are heterogeneous among studies (Supplementary Table 2 (187.5KB, pdf) ); MtFs received estrogen as oral ethinyl E2, transdermal E2 or Premarin, and in some cases combined with progesteronic compounds, such as cyproterone acetate. Most studies did not measure plasma E2 levels, so the participants’ E2 concentrations are not known. The only exceptions are the studies by Schoning et al. [86] and Sommer et al. [87], but in both cases the plasma E2 levels did not reach supraphysiological levels. Whether higher E2 doses (i.e., as high as female E2 levels at the peak of their menstrual cycle) have different cognitive effects than lower dose treatments remains to be determined.

Generally, there are many inconsistencies on how HT affects cognitive performances for MtFs, which could be partially attributed to the varying tests, doses and hormone combinations used in the studies. For example, visual memory is improved after long-term HT [88], but that same study also found that performance on a different visual memory test is better after HT is stopped for ≥8 weeks. Unfortunately, no other studies on MtFs assessed visual memory so the findings from the Miles et al. study [88] cannot be compared.

Visuospatial ability of MtFs appears to be minimally affected by HT. Van Goozen et al. [89] found worsening performance on visuospatial ability after 3 months of HT. However, such visuospatial ability deficit is not found when assessed with a different test (i.e., mental rotation test) in other studies. In contrast, better mental rotation ability was observed after 14 weeks of HT [90], though the authors suggested that this could be a learning effect, because non-MtFs groups in the study also have similar improvements. In other studies, HT does not significantly affect mental rotation ability of MtFs [86-88, 90-92].

Data on verbal ability of MtFs on HT were not consistent across studies. In term of language skills, verbal reasoning was not affected by HT [89-91], whereas verbal fluency was better after HT in the Van Goozen et al. [89] study, but not in other studies [88, 91]. Verbal memory appears to improve for MtFs with HT [92] but, depending on the tests used, conflicting findings have also been reported [88, 89, 91, 92]. Potentially, the large heterogeneity between studies (e.g., type, dose, duration of HT, age) is the reason for the high variability on the effects of E2 on MtFs’ verbal ability.

5. SEXUAL COGNITION

One domain that has not been well explored is how E2 influences sexual cognition. Eugonadal males may first look at faces when viewing images of other humans, but their visual attention quickly shifts to the torso and pelvic area, when viewing sexually explicit images [93, 94]. Prolonged fixation of one’s gaze on sexual images may lead to sexual arousal [95-97], and men with sexual dysfunction may show reduced visual attention to sexual images [95]. A recent pilot study found that ADT may impair visual attention to sexual (swimsuit models) images [98]. The Skead et al. study provides an objective way to measure how ADT diminishes men’s attention to sexual stimuli. However, whether the decrease in attention to sexual images is due solely to androgen deprivation or also due in part to estrogen deprivation was not explored.

Whether E2 can improve visual attention to sexual stimuli in androgen-deprived men is not known, but warrants further investigation. Other studies have shown that E2 helps restore sexual interest in most castrated mammalian species [99]. High dose E2 treatment in eugonadal men reduces sexual desire but sexual interest appears to be higher in androgen-deprived men with E2 treatment than those without E2 treatment [99, 100]. Finkelstein et al. [101] recently found that testosterone improves sexual desire in androgen-deprived men, but the effect is dampened when testosterone is administered in combination with an aromatase inhibitor. That affirms a role for E2 in raising libido in androgen-deprived men. In addition, Handy et al. [58] provided independent evidence of estrogen’s preservative effect on libido for castrated men. That study showed that castrated men with no hormonal treatment are more likely to be sexually inactive than those on some form of estrogen treatment.

6. CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

A clinical trial [38, 100] is currently underway, exploring the use of high dose parenteral E2 as an alternative to the luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone (LHRH) agonists and antagonists for androgen suppression in PCa patients. If E2 treatment is shown to improve cognitive functions in these PCa patients, it may help justify the use of high dose E2 therapy for ADT or supplemental E2 therapy for patients who are on ADT achieved with other drugs, such as the LHRH drugs. However, one needs to also be aware of non-cognitive effects associated with E2 treatment such as breast and cardiovascular events as well as concerns on potential breast and prostate cancer risks [38, 102, 103]. Certainly, in the case of MtFs, breasts enlargement due to E2 treatment is a desired effect, but may be bothersome to PCa patients [104].

Future researchers investigating the effects of E2 on cognitive functions of PCa patients on ADT or MtFs on HT needs to consider several factors in study design, such as the type of cognitive tests used, E2 dose, treatment duration and follow-up time. Although short-term androgen deprivation impairs various cognitive ability of male rodents, it remains unclear why the cognitive deficits androgen-deprived PCa patients and MtFs are more subtle. One possibility is that, other than visual memory, most tests used in human studies may not measure comparable cognitive domains as those in the animal studies. As an example, spatial ability in rodents are measured using spatial navigation tests; i.e., locating object, rewards, or a goal hole (Table 1). However, visuospatial tests that have been used in human participants only assessed mental visualization ability (Tables 2 and 3). Other human studies have utilized different techniques to assess spatial memory, for example, by using virtual reality software [105-107] or a real-life wayfinding task [108]. These tests may more realistically reflect the spatial ability tests used in animal studies. However, such methods have not been used to examine how E2 treatment influences cognitive function for androgen-deprived PCa patients or MtFs who are on HT.

Two factors that need more investigations are E2 dosing and the timing of follow-up assessment. Animal studies have shown that an E2 dose higher than intact levels can have positive cognitive effects, but whether a lower dose, that raises E2 only to normal physiological levels, can still improve cognition has not been explored. However, based on the findings by Beer et al. [75], E2 may need to reach supraphysiological levels in order to bring cognitive benefits in androgen-deprived men. Furthermore, long-term studies of E2 treatment for androgen-deprived men are necessary to reflect the clinical reality that many PCa patients are on ADT for years at a time. This is particularly relevant since, for post-menopausal women (and in ovariectomized rodents [67]), supplemental estrogens may have different risks and benefits depending on how long they have been hormone-deprived before starting estrogen therapy [68]. It is thus possible that cognitive responses to supplemental E2 treatment will differ between androgen-deprived males with and without E2 treatment, if the men are on ADT for a longer time or the delay between E2 treatment and the onset of ADT is short. If early E2 treatment preserves cognition better, it may support the use of add-back E2 around the onset of ADT.

In addition, what needs to be explored in future studies is other factors known to be linked to cognition, such as sleep function. Based on self-reports from PCa patients, ADT results in sleep disturbance [5-8]. Using actigraphy, Hanisch et al. [109] found that, on average, PCa patients on ADT required more than 30 minutes to fall asleep, slept for only about 6 hours, and woke up 2.7 times per night. Not surprisingly, ADT patients often reported daytime sleepiness and took frequent naps [109]. However, whether the sleep problems correlate with cognitive impairment has not been explored.

Castration may also alter sleep-wake patterns in male rodents. Increased period of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep during the animals’ active period [110] and dampened REM sleep recovery after acute sleep deprivation [64] are found in castrated rats. However, other studies did not report such findings [64, 111, 112]. Among these studies, how partial sleep deprivation (i.e., not getting sufficient sleep on consecutive nights) impacts sleep/wake behaviour have not been studied in castrated animals, but will be important to investigate for several reasons. First, such study reflects what PCa patients on ADT experience in real life; i.e., having sleep loss for days in a row. Second, increased allostatic load (including metabolic, immune and neural changes) may occur with repeated sleep deprivation [113, 114], and may directly influence sleep-wake behaviour. Third, whether chronic sleep deprivation can exacerbate other side effects of androgen deprivation—such as cognitive impairment, depression and metabolic syndrome—have not been explored.

Lastly, future clinical research that investigates how E2 influence cognition of PCa patients on ADT should also take into account any effects of E2 on fatigue, depression and anxiety. ADT has been shown to cause fatigue, depression and anxiety [10, 11, 115, 116]. Preclinical research showed that E2 has beneficial effects on these parameters [64, 117, 118], but those findings have not been explored in PCa patients on ADT. There are, though, some evidence that HT may help reduce psychological symptoms (e.g., depression and anxiety) symptoms in MtFs [56]. Changes in these behaviours may interlink between each other as well as with cognitive function. Therefore, if E2 can alleviate one of these symptoms, other symptom(s) may also be potentially improved.

CONCLUSION

While some studies have suggested that E2 may have some protective role in cognitive function of androgen-deprived male rats and humans, there are still major knowledge gaps on the roles of E2 in male cognition. This is partly due to the fact that studies on genetic males treated with E2 are scarce, and there are large variations between studies (e.g., type of cognitive tests, E2 treatment regimen). Future studies need to explore which E2 treatment protocol can most effectively improve cognitive functions in androgen-deprived males. Such studies have important implications to improving the quality of life of not just androgen-deprived PCa patients, but also for MtFs.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I thank Drs. Elizabeth Hampson, Sheila Garland, and Richard Wassersug for providing critical feedback on the draft manuscript.

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

ADT

Androgen deprivation therapy

CA

Cyproterone acetate

DS

Digit span

E2

Estradiol

EB

Estradiol benzoate

ED

Estradiol dipropionate

ER

Estrogen receptor

fMRI

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

FtMs

Female-to-male transsexuals

HT

Hormone therapy

LHRH

Luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone

MtFs

Male-to-female transsexuals

NMDA

N-methyl-d-aspartate

PAL

Paired associate learning

PCa

Prostate cancer

REM

Rapid eye movement

SRS

Sex-reassignment surgery

WMS-R

Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

Supplementary material is available on the publisher’s web site along with the published article.

CN-15-1043_SD1.pdf (187.5KB, pdf)

CONSENT FOR PUBLICATION

Not applicable.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no conflict of interest, financial or otherwise.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Smith M.R. Androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer: new concepts and concerns. Curr. Opin. Endocrinol. Diabetes Obes. 2007;14(3):247–254. doi: 10.1097/MED.0b013e32814db88c. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/MED. 0b013e32814db88c]. [PMID: 17940447]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Donovan K.A., Walker L.M., Wassersug R.J., Thompson L.M., Robinson J.W. Psychological effects of androgen-deprivation therapy on men with prostate cancer and their partners. Cancer. 2015;121(24):4286–4299. doi: 10.1002/cncr.29672. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cncr.29672]. [PMID: 26372364]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Jamadar R.J., Winters M.J., Maki P.M. Cognitive changes associated with ADT: a review of the literature. Asian J. Androl. 2012;14(2):232–238. doi: 10.1038/aja.2011.107. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/aja.2011.107]. [PMID: 22343495]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McGinty H.L., Phillips K.M., Jim H.S., Cessna J.M., Asvat Y., Cases M.G., Small B.J., Jacobsen P.B. Cognitive functioning in men receiving androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Support. Care Cancer. 2014;22(8):2271–2280. doi: 10.1007/s00520-014-2285-1. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00520-014-2285-1]. [PMID: 24859915]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Savard J., Simard S., Hervouet S., Ivers H., Lacombe L., Fradet Y. Insomnia in men treated with radical prostatectomy for prostate cancer. Psychooncology. 2005;14(2):147–156. doi: 10.1002/pon.830. [http://dx.doi. org/10.1002/pon.830]. [PMID: 15386779]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Savard J., Hervouet S., Ivers H. Prostate cancer treatments and their side effects are associated with increased insomnia. Psychooncology. 2013;22(6):1381–1388. doi: 10.1002/pon.3150. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ pon.3150]. [PMID: 22888075]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Savard J., Ivers H., Savard M.H., Morin C.M. Cancer treatments and their side effects are associated with aggravation of insomnia: Results of a longitudinal study. Cancer. 2015;121(10):1703–1711. doi: 10.1002/cncr.29244. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cncr.29244]. [PMID: 25677509]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Grunfeld E.A., Halliday A., Martin P., Drudge-Coates L. Andropause syndrome in men treated for metastatic prostate cancer: a qualitative study of the impact of symptoms. Cancer Nurs. 2012;35(1):63–69. doi: 10.1097/NCC.0b013e318211fa92. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/NCC.0b013e318211fa92]. [PMID: 21558849]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hanisch L.J., Gehrman P.R. Circadian rhythm of hot flashes and activity levels among prostate cancer patients on androgen deprivation therapy. Aging Male. 2011;14(4):243–248. doi: 10.3109/13685538.2011.582528. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.3109/13685538.2011.582528]. [PMID: 21627561]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sharpley C.F., Christie D.R., Bitsika V., Miller B.J. Trajectories of total depression and depressive symptoms in prostate cancer patients receiving six months of hormone therapy. Psychooncology. 2017;26(1):60–66. doi: 10.1002/pon.4100. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon.4100]. [PMID: 26857160]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sharpley C.F., Bitsika V., Wootten A.C., Christie D.R. Differences in major depressive disorder and generalised anxiety disorder symptomatology between prostate cancer patients receiving hormone therapy and those who are not. Psychooncology. 2014;23(12):1350–1355. doi: 10.1002/pon.3566. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon.3566]. [PMID: 24789575]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sharpley C.F., Bitsika V., Christie D.R. Variability in depressive symptoms of cognitive deficit and cognitive bias during the first 2 years after diagnosis in Australian men with prostate cancer. Am. J. Men Health. 2016;10(1):6–13. doi: 10.1177/1557988314552669. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 1557988314552669]. [PMID: 25294866]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kritzer M.F., McLaughlin P.J., Smirlis T., Robinson J.K. Gonadectomy impairs T-maze acquisition in adult male rats. Horm. Behav. 2001;39(2):167–174. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1645. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/hbeh. 2001.1645]. [PMID: 11243744]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Daniel J.M., Winsauer P.J., Moerschbaecher J.M. Castration in rats impairs performance during acquisition of a working memory task and exacerbates deficits in working memory produced by scopolamine and mecamylamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2003;170(3):294–300. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1537-4. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-003-1537-4]. [PMID: 12898124]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kritzer M.F., Brewer A., Montalmant F., Davenport M., Robinson J.K. Effects of gonadectomy on performance in operant tasks measuring prefrontal cortical function in adult male rats. Horm. Behav. 2007;51(2):183–194. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.07.005. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.yhbeh.2006.07.005]. [PMID: 16942768]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Spritzer M.D., Gill M., Weinberg A., Galea L.A. Castration differentially affects spatial working and reference memory in male rats. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2008;37(1):19–29. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9264-2. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1007/s10508-007-9264-2]. [PMID: 18058217]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sandstrom N.J., Kim J.H., Wasserman M.A. Testosterone modulates performance on a spatial working memory task in male rats. Horm. Behav. 2006;50(1):18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.09.008. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. yhbeh.2005.09.008]. [PMID: 16263125]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Spritzer M.D., Daviau E.D., Coneeny M.K., Engelman S.M., Prince W.T., Rodriguez-Wisdom K.N. Effects of testosterone on spatial learning and memory in adult male rats. Horm. Behav. 2011;59(4):484–496. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.01.009. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.01. 009]. [PMID: 21295035]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hawley W.R., Grissom E.M., Martin R.C., Halmos M.B., Bart C.L., Dohanich G.P. Testosterone modulates spatial recognition memory in male rats. Horm. Behav. 2013;63(4):559–565. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.007. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.007]. [PMID: 23481590]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ceccarelli I., Scaramuzzino A., Aloisi A.M. Effects of gonadal hormones and persistent pain on non-spatial working memory in male and female rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2001;123(1):65–76. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00195-4. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00195-4]. [PMID: 11377730]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Aubele T., Kaufman R., Montalmant F., Kritzer M.F. Effects of gonadectomy and hormone replacement on a spontaneous novel object recognition task in adult male rats. Horm. Behav. 2008;54(2):244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.04.001. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.04.001]. [PMID: 18511051]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Edinger K.L., Lee B., Frye C.A. Mnemonic effects of testosterone and its 5alpha-reduced metabolites in the conditioned fear and inhibitory avoidance tasks. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2004;78(3):559–568. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.024. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.024]. [PMID: 15251265]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lagunas N., Calmarza-Font I., Grassi D., Garcia-Segura L.M. Estrogen receptor ligands counteract cognitive deficits caused by androgen deprivation in male rats. Horm. Behav. 2011;59(4):581–584. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.02.014. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.02.014]. [PMID: 21376723]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gibbs R.B., Johnson D.A. Sex-specific effects of gonadectomy and hormone treatment on acquisition of a 12-arm radial maze task by Sprague Dawley rats. Endocrinology. 2008;149(6):3176–3183. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-1645. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/en.2007-1645]. [PMID: 18292188]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Locklear M.N., Kritzer M.F. Assessment of the effects of sex and sex hormones on spatial cognition in adult rats using the Barnes maze. Horm. Behav. 2014;66(2):298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.06.006. [http://dx.doi.org/10. 1016/j.yhbeh.2014.06.006]. [PMID: 24937438]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hodosy J., Ostatnikova D., Riljak V., Myslivecek J., Celec P. The effect of rapid and depot testosterone and estradiol on spatial performance in water maze. Cent. Eur. J. Biol. 2012;7:596–602. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Naftolin F., Ryan K.J., Davies I.J., Reddy V.V., Flores F., Petro Z., Kuhn M., White R.J., Takaoka Y., Wolin L. The formation of estrogens by central neuroendocrine tissues. Recent Prog. Horm. Res. 1975;31:295–319. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-571131-9.50012-8. [PMID: 812160]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Beyenburg S., Watzka M., Clusmann H., Blümcke I., Bidlingmaier F., Elger C.E., Stoffel-Wagner B. Androgen receptor mRNA expression in the human hippocampus. Neurosci. Lett. 2000;294(1):25–28. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01542-1. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(00) 01542-1]. [PMID: 11044578]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Xiao L., Jordan C.L. Sex differences, laterality, and hormonal regulation of androgen receptor immunoreactivity in rat hippocampus. Horm. Behav. 2002;42(3):327–336. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2002.1822. [http://dx.doi.org/10. 1006/hbeh.2002.1822]. [PMID: 12460592]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wibowo E., Calich H.J., Currie R.W., Wassersug R.J. Prolonged androgen deprivation may influence the autoregulation of estrogen receptors in the brain and pelvic floor muscles of male rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;286:128–135. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.003. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr. 2015.03.003]. [PMID: 25746452]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Finley S.K., Kritzer M.F. Immunoreactivity for intracellular androgen receptors in identified subpopulations of neurons, astrocytes and oligodendrocytes in primate prefrontal cortex. J. Neurobiol. 1999;40(4):446–457. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695 (19990915)40:4<446:AID-NEU3>3.0.CO;2-J]. [PMID: 10453048]. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jones B.A., Watson N.V. Spatial memory performance in androgen insensitive male rats. Physiol. Behav. 2005;85(2):135–141. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.023. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.023]. [PMID: 15924910]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rizk A., Robertson J., Raber J. Behavioral performance of tfm mice supports the beneficial role of androgen receptors in spatial learning and memory. Brain Res. 2005;1034(1-2):132–138. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.12.002. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2004.12.002]. [PMID: 15713264]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Imperato-McGinley J., Pichardo M., Gautier T., Voyer D., Bryden M.P. Cognitive abilities in androgen-insensitive subjects: comparison with control males and females from the same kindred. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 1991;34(5):341–347. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.1991.tb00303.x. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1365-2265.1991.tb00303.x]. [PMID: 2060143]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hier D.B., Crowley W.F., Jr Spatial ability in androgen-deficient men. N. Engl. J. Med. 1982;306(20):1202–1205. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198205203062003. [http://dx.doi. org/10.1056/NEJM198205203062003]. [PMID: 7070432]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Saad F., Fizazi K. Androgen deprivation therapy and secondary hormone therapy in the management of hormone-sensitive and castration resistant prostate cancer. Urology. 2015;86(5):852–861. doi: 10.1016/j.urology.2015.07.034. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.urology.2015.07.034]. [PMID: 26282624]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Phillips I., Shah S.I., Duong T., Abel P., Langley R.E. Androgen deprivation therapy and the re-emergence of parenteral estrogen in prostate cancer. Oncol. Hematol. Rev. 2014;10(1):42–47. doi: 10.17925/ohr.2014.10.1.42. [http://dx.doi.org/10.17925/OHR.2014.10.1.42]. [PMID: 24932461]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Langley R.E., Cafferty F.H., Alhasso A.A., Rosen S.D., Sundaram S.K., Freeman S.C., Pollock P., Jinks R.C., Godsland I.F., Kockelbergh R., Clarke N.W., Kynaston H.G., Parmar M.K., Abel P.D. Cardiovascular outcomes in patients with locally advanced and metastatic prostate cancer treated with luteinising-hormone-releasing-hormone agonists or transdermal oestrogen: the randomised, phase 2 MRC PATCH trial (PR09). Lancet Oncol. 2013;14(4):306–316. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(13)70025-1. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1470-2045(13) 70025-1]. [PMID: 23465742]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Engler-Chiurazzi E.B., Brown C.M., Povroznik J.M., Simpkins J.W. Estrogens as neuroprotectants: Estrogenic actions in the context of cognitive aging and brain injury. Prog. Neurobiol. 2016;15 doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hamson D.K., Roes M.M., Galea L.A. Sex hormones and cognition: neuroendocrine influences on memory and learning. Compr. Physiol. 2016;6(3):1295–1337. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c150031. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cphy. c150031]. [PMID: 27347894]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Butcher R.L., Collins W.E., Fugo N.W. Plasma concentration of LH, FSH, prolactin, progesterone and estradiol-17beta throughout the 4-day estrous cycle of the rat. Endocrinology. 1974;94(6):1704–1708. doi: 10.1210/endo-94-6-1704. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/endo-94-6-1704]. [PMID: 4857496]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Jong F.H., Uilenbroek J., Molen H.J. Oestradiol-17beta, testosterone and gonadotrophins in oestradiol-17beta-treated intact adult male rats. J. Endocrinol. 1975;65(2):281–282. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.0650281. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1677/joe.0.0650281]. [PMID: 1151210]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ferrini R.L., Barrett-Connor E. Sex hormones and age: a cross-sectional study of testosterone and estradiol and their bioavailable fractions in community-dwelling men. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1998;147(8):750–754. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009519. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje. a009519]. [PMID: 9554416]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Sherman B.M., Korenman S.G. Hormonal characteristics of the human menstrual cycle throughout reproductive life. J. Clin. Invest. 1975;55(4):699–706. doi: 10.1172/JCI107979. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1172/JCI107979]. [PMID: 1120778]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kampen D.L., Sherwin B.B. Estradiol is related to visual memory in healthy young men. Behav. Neurosci. 1996;110(3):613–617. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.3.613. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.110.3.613]. [PMID: 8889006]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hogervorst E., De Jager C., Budge M., Smith A.D. Serum levels of estradiol and testosterone and performance in different cognitive domains in healthy elderly men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(3):405–421. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(03)00053-2. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00053-2]. [PMID: 14644069]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Yaffe K., Barnes D., Lindquist K., Cauley J., Simonsick E.M., Penninx B., Satterfield S., Harris T., Cummings S.R., Health A.B. Endogenous sex hormone levels and risk of cognitive decline in an older biracial cohort. Neurobiol. Aging. 2007;28(2):171–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.10.004. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.10.004]. [PMID: 17097195]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Zimmerman M.E., Lipton R.B., Santoro N., McConnell D.S., Derby C.A., Katz M.J., Baigi K., Saunders-Pullman R. Endogenous estradiol is associated with verbal memory in nondemented older men. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(1):158–165. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.01.011. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.01.011]. [PMID: 21354686]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hogervorst E. Effects of gonadal hormones on cognitive behaviour in elderly men and women. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2013;25(11):1182–1195. doi: 10.1111/jne.12080. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jne.12080]. [PMID: 23895362]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Stoffel-Wagner B., Watzka M., Schramm J., Bidlingmaier F., Klingmüller D. Expression of CYP19 (aromatase) mRNA in different areas of the human brain. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 1999;70(4-6):237–241. doi: 10.1016/s0960-0760(99)00114-4. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0960-0760 (99)00114-4]. [PMID: 10622413]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.MacLusky N.J., Walters M.J., Clark A.S., Toran-Allerand C.D. Aromatase in the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and mid-brain: ontogeny and developmental implications. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 1994;5(6):691–698. doi: 10.1006/mcne.1994.1083. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/mcne.1994.1083]. [PMID: 7704444]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Martin S., Jones M., Simpson E., van den Buuse M. Impaired spatial reference memory in aromatase-deficient (ArKO) mice. Neuroreport. 2003;14(15):1979–1982. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200310270-00020. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ 00001756-200310270-00020]. [PMID: 14561933]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hodosy J., Celec P., Ostatnikova D., Caganova M., Putz Z., Balaz P., Kudela M. The effect of anastrozol on spatial memory in adult rats. Cent. Eur. J. Biol. 2009;4:186–189. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Moradpour F., Naghdi N., Fathollahi Y. Anastrozole improved testosterone-induced impairment acquisition of spatial learning and memory in the hippocampal CA1 region in adult male rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2006;175(2):223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.037. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr. 2006.08.037]. [PMID: 17018237]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Alejandre-Gomez M., Garcia-Segura L.M., Gonzalez-Burgos I. Administration of an inhibitor of estrogen biosynthesis facilitates working memory acquisition in male rats. Neurosci. Res. 2007;58(3):272–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.03.011. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2007.03.011]. [PMID: 17467093]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Murad M.H., Elamin M.B., Garcia M.Z., Mullan R.J., Murad A., Erwin P.J., Montori V.M. Hormonal therapy and sex reassignment: a systematic review and meta-analysis of quality of life and psychosocial outcomes. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 2010;72(2):214–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2009.03625.x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2265.2009.03625. x]. [PMID: 19473181]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Klein C., Gorzalka B.B. Sexual functioning in transsexuals following hormone therapy and genital surgery: a review. J. Sex. Med. 2009;6(11):2922–2939. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01370.x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01370.x]. [PMID: 20092545]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Handy A.B., Jackowich R.A., Wibowo E., Johnson T.W., Wassersug R.J. Gender preference in the sexual attractions, fantasies, and relationships of voluntarily castrated men. Sex. Med. 2016;4(1):e51–e59. doi: 10.1016/j.esxm.2015.11.001. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.esxm.2015.11. 001]. [PMID: 26944778]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Brett M.A., Roberts L.F., Johnson T.W., Wassersug R.J. Eunuchs in contemporary society: expectations, consequences, and adjustments to castration (part II). J. Sex. Med. 2007;4(4 Pt 1):946–955. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2007.00522.x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2007.00522.x]. [PMID: 17627741]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Luine V., Rodriguez M. Effects of estradiol on radial arm maze performance of young and aged rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 1994;62(3):230–236. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(05)80021-4. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(05)80021-4]. [PMID: 7857245]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Gibbs R.B. Testosterone and estradiol produce different effects on cognitive performance in male rats. Horm. Behav. 2005;48(3):268–277. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.03.005. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.03.005]. [PMID: 15890350]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kritzer M.F. Effects of acute and chronic gonadectomy on the catecholamine innervation of the cerebral cortex in adult male rats: insensitivity of axons immunoreactive for dopamine-beta-hydroxylase to gonadal steroids, and differential sensitivity of axons immunoreactive for tyrosine hydroxylase to ovarian and testicular hormones. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;427(4):617–633. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1096-9861(20001127)427:4<617:AID-CNE9>3.0.CO;2-#]. [PMID: 11056468]. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.McConnell S.E., Alla J., Wheat E., Romeo R.D., McEwen B., Thornton J.E. The role of testicular hormones and luteinizing hormone in spatial memory in adult male rats. Horm. Behav. 2012;61(4):479–486. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.003. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.003]. [PMID: 22265851]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wibowo E., Deurveilher S., Wassersug R.J., Semba K. Estradiol treatment modulates spontaneous sleep and recovery after sleep deprivation in castrated male rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2012;226(2):456–464. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.045. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.045]. [PMID: 22004978]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Deurveilher S., Rusak B., Semba K. Estradiol and progesterone modulate spontaneous sleep patterns and recovery from sleep deprivation in ovariectomized rats. Sleep. 2009;32(7):865–877. [PMID: 19639749]. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.de Jong F.H., Hey A.H., van der Molen H.J. Effect of gonadotrophins on the secretion of oestradiol- and testosterone by the rat testis. J. Endocrinol. 1973;57(2):277–284. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.0570277. [http://dx.doi.org/10. 1677/joe.0.0570277]. [PMID: 4706601]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Daniel J.M. Estrogens, estrogen receptors, and female cognitive aging: the impact of timing. Horm. Behav. 2013;63(2):231–237. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.05.003. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.05.003]. [PMID: 22587940]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Sherwin B.B. Estrogen therapy: is time of initiation critical for neuroprotection? Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 2009;5(11):620–627. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2009.193. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrendo.2009.193]. [PMID: 19844249]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bohacek J., Daniel J.M. The ability of oestradiol administration to regulate protein levels of oestrogen receptor alpha in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of middle-aged rats is altered following long-term ovarian hormone deprivation. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21(7):640–647. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009.01882.x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009. 01882.x]. [PMID: 19453823]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Oberlander J.G., Woolley C.S. 17beta-estradiol acutely potentiates glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the hippocampus through distinct mechanisms in males and females. J. Neurosci. 2016;36(9):2677–2690. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4437-15.2016. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI. 4437-15.2016]. [PMID: 26937008]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Locklear M.N., Bhamidipaty S., Kritzer M.F. Local N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor antagonism in the prefrontal cortex attenuates spatial cognitive deficits induced by gonadectomy in adult male rats. Neuroscience. 2015;288:73–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.12.032. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroscience.2014.12.032]. [PMID: 25545712]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Paul C.M., Magda G., Abel S. Spatial memory: Theoretical basis and comparative review on experimental methods in rodents. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;203(2):151–164. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.05.022. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ j.bbr.2009.05.022]. [PMID: 19467271]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Nadel L., Hardt O. Update on memory systems and processes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):251–273. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.169. [http://dx.doi. org/10.1038/npp.2010.169]. [PMID: 20861829]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Olton D.S., Papas B.C. Spatial memory and hippocampal function. Neuropsychologia. 1979;17(6):669–682. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90042-3. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90042-3]. [PMID: 522981]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Beer T.M., Bland L.B., Bussiere J.R., Neiss M.B., Wersinger E.M., Garzotto M., Ryan C.W., Janowsky J.S. Testosterone loss and estradiol administration modify memory in men. J. Urol. 2006;175(1):130–135. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5347(05)00049-2. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5347(05) 00049-2]. [PMID: 16406889]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Matousek R.H., Sherwin B.B. A randomized controlled trial of add-back estrogen or placebo on cognition in men with prostate cancer receiving an antiandrogen and a gonadotropin-releasing hormone analog. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(2):215–225. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.012. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.012]. [PMID: 19615826]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Taxel P., Stevens M.C., Trahiotis M., Zimmerman J., Kaplan R.F. The effect of short-term estradiol therapy on cognitive function in older men receiving hormonal suppression therapy for prostate cancer. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2004;52(2):269–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52067.x. [http://dx. doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52067.x]. [PMID: 14728639]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Young L.A., Neiss M.B., Samuels M.H., Roselli C.E., Janowsky J.S. Cognition is not modified by large but temporary changes in sex hormones in men. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2010;95(1):280–288. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-1346. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/jc.2009-1346]. [PMID: 19880786]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Jenkins V.A., Bloomfield D.J., Shilling V.M., Edginton T.L. Does neoadjuvant hormone therapy for early prostate cancer affect cognition? Results from a pilot study. BJU Int. 2005;96(1):48–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2005.05565.x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-410X.2005.05565.x]. [PMID: 15963119]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Salminen E., Portin R., Korpela J., Backman H., Parvinen L.M., Helenius H., Nurmi M. Androgen deprivation and cognition in prostate cancer. Br. J. Cancer. 2003;89(6):971–976. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6601235. [http://dx.doi. org/10.1038/sj.bjc.6601235]. [PMID: 12966411]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Salminen E.K., Portin R.I., Koskinen A.I., Helenius H.Y., Nurmi M.J. Estradiol and cognition during androgen deprivation in men with prostate carcinoma. Cancer. 2005;103(7):1381–1387. doi: 10.1002/cncr.20962. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cncr.20962]. [PMID: 15717315]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Almeida O.P., Waterreus A., Spry N., Flicker L., Martins R.N. One year follow-up study of the association between chemical castration, sex hormones, beta-amyloid, memory and depression in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(8):1071–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.11.002. [http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.11.002]. [PMID: 15219659]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Alibhai S.M., Breunis H., Timilshina N., Marzouk S., Stewart D., Tannock I., Naglie G., Tomlinson G., Fleshner N., Krahn M., Warde P., Canning S.D. Impact of androgen-deprivation therapy on cognitive function in men with nonmetastatic prostate cancer. J. Clin. Oncol. 2010;28(34):5030–5037. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2010.30.8742. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1200/JCO.2010.30.8742]. [PMID: 21041708]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cherrier M.M., Aubin S., Higano C.S. Cognitive and mood changes in men undergoing intermittent combined androgen blockade for non-metastatic prostate cancer. Psychooncology. 2009;18(3):237–247. doi: 10.1002/pon.1401. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon.1401]. [PMID: 18636420]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Cherrier M.M., Rose A.L., Higano C. The effects of combined androgen blockade on cognitive function during the first cycle of intermittent androgen suppression in patients with prostate cancer. J. Urol. 2003;170(5):1808–1811. doi: 10.1097/01.ju.0000091640.59812.83. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ 01.ju.0000091640.59812.83]. [PMID: 14532781]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Schöning S., Engelien A., Bauer C., Kugel H., Kersting A., Roestel C., Zwitserlood P., Pyka M., Dannlowski U., Lehmann W., Heindel W., Arolt V., Konrad C. Neuroimaging differences in spatial cognition between men and male-to-female transsexuals before and during hormone therapy. J. Sex. Med. 2010;7(5):1858–1867. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01484.x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01484.x]. [PMID: 19751389]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Sommer I.E., Cohen-Kettenis P.T., van Raalten T., Vd Veer A.J., Ramsey L.E., Gooren L.J., Kahn R.S., Ramsey N.F. Effects of cross-sex hormones on cerebral activation during language and mental rotation: An fMRI study in transsexuals. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18(3):215–221. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.10.002. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. euroneuro.2007.10.002]. [PMID: 17997284]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Miles C., Green R., Hines M. Estrogen treatment effects on cognition, memory and mood in male-to-female transsexuals. Horm. Behav. 2006;50(5):708–717. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.008. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh. 2006.06.008]. [PMID: 16884726]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Van Goozen S.H., Cohen-Kettenis P.T., Gooren L.J., Frijda N.H., Van de Poll N.E. Gender differences in behaviour: activating effects of cross-sex hormones. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1995;20(4):343–363. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)00076-x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(94)00076-X]. [PMID: 8532819]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.van Goozen S.H., Slabbekoorn D., Gooren L.J., Sanders G., Cohen-Kettenis P.T. Organizing and activating effects of sex hormones in homosexual transsexuals. Behav. Neurosci. 2002;116(6):982–988. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.6.982. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.116.6.982]. [PMID: 12492297]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Slabbekoorn D., van Goozen S.H., Megens J., Gooren L.J., Cohen-Kettenis P.T. Activating effects of cross-sex hormones on cognitive functioning: a study of short-term and long-term hormone effects in transsexuals. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24(4):423–447. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00091-2. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00091-2]. [PMID: 10341369]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Miles C., Green R., Sanders G., Hines M. Estrogen and memory in a transsexual population. Horm. Behav. 1998;34(2):199–208. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1478. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1998.1478]. [PMID: 9799629]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Bolmont M., Cacioppo J.T., Cacioppo S. Love is in the gaze: an eye-tracking study of love and sexual desire. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25(9):1748–1756. doi: 10.1177/0956797614539706. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797614539706]. [PMID: 25031302]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Wenzlaff F., Briken P., Dekker A. Video-based eye tracking in sex research: a systematic literature review. J. Sex Res. 2015;53:1–12. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2015.1107524. [PMID: 26689496]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.de Jong D.C. The role of attention in sexual arousal: implications for treatment of sexual dysfunction. J. Sex Res. 2009;46(2-3):237–248. doi: 10.1080/00224490902747230. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490902747230]. [PMID: 19308846]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Barlow D.H. Causes of sexual dysfunction: the role of anxiety and cognitive interference. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1986;54(2):140–148. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.54.2.140. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.54.2.140]. [PMID: 3700800]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Wiegel M., Scepkowski L.A., Barlow D.H. Cognitiveaffective processes in sexual arousal and sexual dysfunction. Psychophysiol. Sex, 2007. pp. 143–166..
  • 98.Skead C., Palmer-Hague J.L., Tsang V., Wassersug R.J., Nasiopoulos E., Kingstone A. Using eye-tracking to quantify the impact of prostate cancer treatments on male libido: a pilot study. J. Clin. Oncol., 2016.
  • 99.Wibowo E., Wassersug R.J. The effect of estrogen on the sexual interest of castrated males: Implications to prostate cancer patients on androgen-deprivation therapy. Crit. Rev. Oncol. Hematol. 2013;87(3):224–238. doi: 10.1016/j.critrevonc.2013.01.006. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.critrevonc.2013.01. 006]. [PMID: 23484454]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Gilbert D.C., Duong T., Kynaston H.G., Alhasso A.A., Cafferty F.H., Rosen S.D., Kanaga-Sundaram S., Dixit S., Laniado M., Madaan S., Collins G., Pope A., Welland A., Nankivell M., Wassersug R., Parmar M.K., Langley R.E., Abel P.D. Quality-of-life outcomes from the Prostate Adenocarcinoma: TransCutaneous Hormones (PATCH) trial evaluating luteinising hormone-releasing hormone agonists versus transdermal oestradiol for androgen suppression in advanced prostate cancer. BJU Int. 2017;119(5):667–675. doi: 10.1111/bju.13687. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bju.13687]. [PMID: 27753182]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Finkelstein J.S., Lee H., Burnett-Bowie S.A., Pallais J.C., Yu E.W., Borges L.F., Jones B.F., Barry C.V., Wulczyn K.E., Thomas B.J., Leder B.Z. Gonadal steroids and body composition, strength, and sexual function in men. N. Engl. J. Med. 2013;369(11):1011–1022. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1206168. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa1206168]. [PMID: 24024838]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Wibowo E., Schellhammer P., Wassersug R.J. Role of estrogen in normal male function: clinical implications for patients with prostate cancer on androgen deprivation therapy. J. Urol. 2011;185(1):17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2010.08.094. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.juro.2010.08.094]. [PMID: 21074215]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Hedlund P.O., Damber J.E., Hagerman I., Haukaas S., Henriksson P., Iversen P., Johansson R., Klarskov P., Lundbeck F., Rasmussen F., Varenhorst E., Viitanen J. Parenteral estrogen versus combined androgen deprivation in the treatment of metastatic prostatic cancer: part 2. Final evaluation of the Scandinavian Prostatic Cancer Group (SPCG) Study No. 5. Scand. J. Urol. Nephrol. 2008;42(3):220–229. doi: 10.1080/00365590801943274. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 00365590801943274]. [PMID: 18432528]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Wassersug R.J., Gray R. The health and well-being of prostate cancer patients and male-to-female transsexuals on androgen deprivation therapy: a qualitative study with comments on expectations and estrogen. Psychol. Health Med. 2011;16(1):39–52. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2010.516364. [http://dx. doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2010.516364]. [PMID: 21218363]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Spiers M.V., Sakamoto M., Elliott R.J., Baumann S. Sex differences in spatial object-location memory in a virtual grocery store. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2008;11(4):471–473. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0058. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1089/cpb.2007.0058]. [PMID: 18721096]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Meneghetti C., Zancada-Menendez C., Sampedro-Piquero P., Lopez L., Martinelli M., Ronconi L., Rossi B. Mental representations derived from navigation: the role of visuo-spatial abilities and working memory. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2016;49:314–322. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.002]. [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Palermo L., Iaria G., Guariglia C. Mental imagery skills and topographical orientation in humans: a correlation study. Behav. Brain Res. 2008;192(2):248–253. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.04.014. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. bbr.2008.04.014]. [PMID: 18502521]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Postma A., Jager G., Kessels R.P., Koppeschaar H.P., van Honk J. Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain Cogn. 2004;54(1):24–34. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00238-0. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626 (03)00238-0]. [PMID: 14733897]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Hanisch L.J., Gooneratne N.S., Soin K., Gehrman P.R., Vaughn D.J., Coyne J.C. Sleep and daily functioning during androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer. Eur. J. Cancer Care (Engl.) 2011;20(4):549–554. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2354.2010.01226.x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2354.2010. 01226.x]. [PMID: 20825464]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Yamaoka S. Modification of circadian sleep rhythms by gonadal steroids and the neural mechanisms involved. Brain Res. 1980;185(2):385–398. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)91076-8. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(80)91076-8]. [PMID: 6101987]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Peder M. Rapid eye movement sleep deprivation affects sleep similarly in castrated and noncastrated rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 1987;47(2):186–196. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(87)90313-x. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(87) 90313-X]. [PMID: 3579837]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Paul K.N., Dugovic C., Turek F.W., Laposky A.D. Diurnal sex differences in the sleep-wake cycle of mice are dependent on gonadal function. Sleep. 2006;29(9):1211–1223. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1211. [http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1211]. [PMID: 17040009]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Kim Y., Chen L., McCarley R.W., Strecker R.E. Sleep allostasis in chronic sleep restriction: the role of the norepinephrine system. Brain Res. 2013;1531:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.048. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. brainres.2013.07.048]. [PMID: 23916734]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.McEwen B.S., Karatsoreos I.N. Sleep deprivation and circadian disruption: stress, allostasis, and allostatic load. Sleep Med. Clin. 2015;10(1):1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2014.11.007. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsmc.2014.11.007]. [PMID: 26055668]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Storey D.J., McLaren D.B., Atkinson M.A., Butcher I., Frew L.C., Smyth J.F., Sharpe M. Clinically relevant fatigue in men with hormone-sensitive prostate cancer on long-term androgen deprivation therapy. Ann. Oncol. 2012;23(6):1542–1549. doi: 10.1093/annonc/mdr447. [http://dx. doi.org/10.1093/annonc/mdr447]. [PMID: 22007078]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Nelson A.M., Gonzalez B.D., Jim H.S., Cessna J.M., Sutton S.K., Small B.J., Fishman M.N., Zachariah B., Jacobsen P.B. Characteristics and predictors of fatigue among men receiving androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer: a controlled comparison. Support. Care Cancer. 2016;24(10):4159–4166. doi: 10.1007/s00520-016-3241-z. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00520-016-3241-z]. [PMID: 27142516]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Carrier N., Saland S.K., Duclot F., He H., Mercer R., Kabbaj M. The anxiolytic and antidepressant-like effects of testosterone and estrogen in gonadectomized male rats. Biol. Psychiatry. 2015;78(4):259–269. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.024. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.024]. [PMID: 25683735]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Filova B., Malinova M., Babickova J., Tothova L., Ostatnikova D., Celec P., Hodosy J. Effects of testosterone and estradiol on anxiety and depressive-like behavior via a non-genomic pathway. Neurosci. Bull. 2015;31(3):288–296. doi: 10.1007/s12264-014-1510-8. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ s12264-014-1510-8]. [PMID: 25754146]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Luine V.N., Rhodes J.C. Gonadal hormone regulation of MAO and other enzymes in hypothalamic areas. Neuroendocrinology. 1983;36(3):235–241. doi: 10.1159/000123461. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000123461]. [PMID: 6835476]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material is available on the publisher’s web site along with the published article.

CN-15-1043_SD1.pdf (187.5KB, pdf)

Articles from Current Neuropharmacology are provided here courtesy of Bentham Science Publishers

RESOURCES