Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addiction. 2017 Jun 29;112(10):1765–1772. doi: 10.1111/add.13879

The Impact of Normative Perceptions on Alcohol Consumption in Military Veterans

Heather Krieger 1, Eric R Pedersen 2, Clayton Neighbors 1
PMCID: PMC5653214  NIHMSID: NIHMS879758  PMID: 28548347

Abstract

Background and aims

Perceptions of both descriptive norms (prevalence of drinking) and injunctive norms (others’ approval of drinking) relate to alcohol consumption but mechanisms for these associations have received little attention, especially in military samples. This study tested the direct and indirect associations between perceived descriptive and injunctive norms on drinking through personal attitudes (i.e., personal approval) in a veteran sample.

Design

Data were collected as part of a longitudinal randomized controlled alcohol intervention study. The study involved two time points: baseline/intervention (time 1) and one-month follow-up (time 2).

Setting

A national sample of veterans was recruited from Facebook to participate in an online study between June and October 2015.

Participants

Data included responses of 621 adult military veterans (age 18–34; 17% female).

Measures

Respondents reported on their weekly alcohol consumption (primary outcome), perceptions of typical drinking, and approval by other same-gender veterans. Covariates included gender, intervention condition, and combat experience.

Findings

Regression results found no significant effects of perceived descriptive or injunctive norms on time 2 drinking when accounting for the effects of personal attitudes, time 1 drinking, and covariates. However, mediation analyses found support for personal attitudes as a mediator of the relationship between perceived descriptive norms and time 2 drinking (indirect effect = 0.003, SE = .001, p = .001) and between perceived injunctive norms and time 2 drinking (indirect effect = 0.004, SE = .001, p < .001).

Conclusions

Attitudes to drinking appear to mediate the association between descriptive and injunctions norms about alcohol and subsequent level of alcohol consumption in US military personnel.


Alcohol misuse in the military has increased over the past two decades [1,2] and resulted in significant personal and organizational costs [3,4]. In 2001, rates of heavy drinking in military personnel (27%) were more than 10% higher than civilian populations and 5% higher than college student populations [5] with rates among veterans (i.e., separated military personnel) even higher (ranging from 30–40%) [68]. Thus, it is important to understand factors that can help reduce heavy drinking among veterans. One low cost, efficacious intervention technique used in other high risk drinking populations such as college students, is personalized normative feedback (PNF). PNF targets perceptions of social norms [911]. Studies examining the impact of these interventions on alcohol consumption in multiple populations have found lower rates of drinking at follow-up [1214]. However, the mechanisms associated with how PNF interventions change drinking behaviors have not been closely examined.

Two types of social norms most often associated with behavior are descriptive and injunctive norms [15]. Descriptive norms refer to typical behavior in a given group or population (e.g., average number of drinks consumed per week by male/female veterans). Injunctive norms refer to the degree of approval or acceptability other people feel regarding a behavior (e.g., how many drinks a typical male/female veteran would consider acceptable to consume in a week) [15,16]. A critical distinction related to social norms is the difference between perceived norms and actual norms. For example, perceived descriptive and injunctive drinking norms refer to how much someone thinks their peers are drinking and how many drinks they think their peers would approve of drinking, respectively. As individuals are often influenced by their perceptions of norms rather than actual norms, there is frequently a large discrepancy between an individual’s perceptions and the actual behaviors or attitudes of others [17].

Perceptions of descriptive drinking norms are associated with individual drinking behaviors in veterans and active duty military personnel. Specifically, perceived descriptive norms are positively associated with meeting criteria for an alcohol use disorder [18,19], higher blood alcohol levels and binge drinking behaviors [20], and greater quantity and frequency of alcohol consumption [21]. Less work has examined the direct relationship between perceptions of injunctive norms and drinking behaviors. In one study, Ames and colleagues found relationships for perceived injunctive norms and heavy drinking frequency to be similar to relationships between perceived descriptive norms and heavy drinking frequency [18]. Conversely, Pedersen and colleagues generally found no relationship between perceptions of injunctive norms and weekly drinking, binge drinking, and alcohol problems [22]. Military personnel have also been found to have exaggerated perceptions of the drinking behaviors of their fellow service members relative to actual drinking norms of their peers [23]. Preliminary evidence suggests that normative messages used in military drinking interventions reduce risky alcohol consumption over time [24]. However, few alcohol interventions tested in military populations have examined the impact of perceived social norms alone on drinking behaviors or assessed potential mechanism of change in drinking behaviors.

While there is some evidence for the relationships between descriptive and injunctive normative perceptions and individual drinking behaviors, less is understood about the potential underlying mechanisms of change or mediators of effective personalized normative feedback (PNF) interventions. In a recent review examining 22 plausible mediators (including protective behavioral strategies, outcome expectancies, coping motives, etc.) of a wide range of interventions (including single session, in-person, online, and multicomponent), perceived norms were identified as the only consistently supported mechanism for explaining the impact of interventions on drinking [25]. However, this finding does not speak to why changes in perceived descriptive and/or injunctive norms translate to corresponding changes in drinking behaviors. One unexplored mechanism, suggested by Borsari and Carey [17], is that changes in perceived norms are effective because they adjust personal attitude levels.

High approval ratings of drinking were found to be associated with greater weekly drinking, binge drinking, and alcohol problems in a sample of veterans [22]. In college samples, more accepting personal drinking attitudes and approval of heavier drinking are positively associated with weekly drinking, drinking frequency, and quantity consumed on a drinking occasion after accounting for the impact of perceived descriptive norms [16] and injunctive norms [26,27]. In addition, the association between perceived injunctive norms for driving after drinking and engagement in drinking after drinking has been found to be mediated by personal attitudes regarding driving after drinking [27]. In sum, there is evidence suggesting that personal attitudes about drinking may mediate the association between perceived norms and drinking. However, previous work has primarily focused on college student samples and has not included comparable assessments (e.g. analogous measurement between the constructs) of injunctive and descriptive norms [16]. This research attempts to address these two limitations.

The overarching goal of this research is to better understand mechanisms associated with reduced drinking resulting from changes in perceptions of descriptive and injunctive norms and personal attitudes among veterans. Specifically, this research was designed to test the hypothesis that perceived norms impact subsequent drinking indirectly through personal attitudes about drinking. In this longitudinal study we assess whether there are significant indirect relationships between perceived descriptive and injunctive norms and alcohol consumption through personal attitudes towards drinking. Specifically, we expected that: 1) personal attitudes, perceptions of descriptive norms, and perceptions of injunctive norms would be positively associated with drinking among veterans; 2) personal attitudes would mediate associations between perceived descriptive norms and drinking; 3) personal attitudes would mediate associations between perceived injunctive norms and drinking. Thus, findings from this study may provide a solid foundation for the development and implementation of brief norms-based interventions in military and veteran populations, which have been supported in other high-risk drinking populations.

Method

Participants and Procedure

Design and Procedure

Baseline and follow-up data were collected as part of larger longitudinal randomized controlled trial designed to test the efficacy of an online normative alcohol intervention to young adult veterans [28]. While the term young adult typically refers to individuals between the ages of 18–25, only 9.6% of U.S veterans are under the age of 35 [29]. Study recruitment was conducted using paid advertisements on Facebook that were promoted to individuals who listed military interests (e.g., “liked” veteran service organizations like Iraq and Afghanistan Veterans of America). Inclusion criteria were 1) being between 18 and 34 years of age, 2) having been separated from the Air Force, Army, Marine Corps, or Navy, and 3) scores of 3 or more (for women) or 4 or more (for men) on the 10-item AUDIT [3032]. Active duty military, individuals affiliated with the reserve components of the U.S. military, and those active in the Coast Guard were excluded. AUDIT scores of 3 or 4 were used so that the sample included both moderate and heavy drinkers who were at-risk for hazardous alcohol consumption rather than only heavy drinkers. Participants were randomized into intervention or control conditions. Intervention participants received PNF on their drinking behaviors, while attention control participants received feedback on video game playing behaviors.

A total of 2,312 individuals clicked on the study’s Facebook advertisements, of which 1,177 consented to and completed the screening questionnaire to determine eligibility. After screening out those ineligible the final sample consisted of 784. Ineligible respondents included those reporting inconsistent responses to survey items, which did not allow us to verify these participants were actual veterans, and those who dropped out of the baseline survey prior to completing the items necessary for the analyses in this study. Of the 784 baseline completers, 622 completed the one-month follow-up survey with 621 providing information on their drinking behaviors and were included in the analysis. Veterans who agreed to participate in the longitudinal study completed two online surveys about their drinking behaviors, attitudes, and consequences. They received $20 for completing the baseline survey (time 1) and $25 for completing the follow-up survey (time 2). Greater detail regarding the design of the study can be found in Pedersen, et al. [33] and Pedersen, et al. [34]. Demographics of the sample included mean age 28.88 (SD = 3.39) and 83.3% male. The average length of service was 5.60 years (SD = 2.79) and the average number of deployments was 1.52 (SD = 1.24). Table 1 displays the demographic characteristics of the sample including gender, race, ethnicity, and service branch percentages.

Table 1.

Demographic and Descriptive Characteristics of the Sample

Count %
Male 513 82.61%
White 526 84.70%
Black/African American 25 4.03%
Asian 8 1.29%
Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander 3 0.48%
American Indian/Alaska Native 13 2.09%
Multiracial 38 6.12%
Other 8 1.29%
Air Force 67 10.79%
Army 363 58.45%
Marines 133 21.42%
Navy 58 9.34%
Mean SD

Descriptive Norms 31.67 23.88
Injunctive Norms 31.34 23.51
Personal Attitudes 19.19 17.74
T1 Weekly Drinking 18.61 18.09
T2 Weekly Drinking 11.49 13.53
Combat Experiences (0–11) 4.85 2.73

Note. Counts are from time 2 (n = 621), means and SD are from time 1 unless otherwise specified.

Measures

Alcohol use

Weekly drinking was measured using the Daily Drinking Questionnaire (DDQ) [35]. Participants were asked to report how many standard drinks they consumed on average as well as the time period of consumption for each day of the week over the previous month (e.g., “How much alcohol, on average, do you drink on each day of a typical week?”). Weekly drinking was measured by summing the number of reported drinks on each day of the week to create a total amount consumed during a typical week in the past month. The DDQ also measures drinking frequency and the average number of drinks per occasion, which were not used as outcomes for this study. This measure has been widely used and demonstrates good test-retest reliability [3638]. Responses ranged from 0 to 21 drinks a week. Alcohol use was measures at time 1 and time 2 and used as the primary outcome measure.

Descriptive norms

Perceived descriptive norms were measured using the Drinking Norms Rating Form (DNRF) [39]. This measure mirrors the DDQ and asks participants to estimate the drinking practices of same gender military veterans, such as the quantity of alcohol consumed on each day of the week. Total perceived descriptive norms were calculated by summing the reported estimates of drinking for each day of the week. The DRNF demonstrates good convergent validity with measures of alcohol consumption [40]. Responses ranged from 0 to 30. Perceptions of descriptive norms were assessed at time 1 and time 2.

Personal attitudes and perceived injunctive norms

The measure of personal attitudes and perceived injunctive norms was assessed using a drink-based injunctive norms measure [16] that also mirrors the DDQ. Individuals reported the average number of drinks they would consider acceptable to consume on each day of the week (personal attitudes) and the average number of drinks a gender-matched young adult military veteran would consider acceptable (perceived injunctive norms). Personal attitudes and perceived injunctive norms scores were calculated by summing the reports for each day of the week. Responses for personal attitudes and perceived injunctive norms ranged from 0 to 30 and 0 to 15 for injunctive norms. Personal attitudes and perceptions of injunctive norms were measured at time 1.

Analysis Strategy

Structural equation modeling using MPlus 7.3 software was used to test hypotheses. Direct and indirect associations examined in the mediation analysis were tested. Time 2 drinking was a negative binomially distributed count variable and parameters were estimated using Monte-Carlo integration and full information maximum likelihood [41,42]. Indirect effects were calculated using the ab product approach, where ‘a’ represents the association between norms and attitudes and ‘b’ represents the association between attitudes and the log rate of time 2 drinking [41]. Standard errors and confidence intervals for parameter estimates and indirect effects were estimated using bias corrected bootstrapping with 1000 samples. Bias corrected bootstrapping addresses the non-normality of the product terms used to evaluate indirect effects.

The model (displayed in Figure 1) included one endogenous variables: Time 2 drinking. Time 2 drinks per week was the terminal outcome and was examined as a function of covariates (intervention condition, previous alcohol consumption, combat experience, and gender; based on previous associations with alcohol consumption [22,4345]), perceived descriptive norms, perceived injunctive norms, and personal attitudes. Attitudes were modeled as a function of perceived descriptive norms, and perceived injunctive norms. Covariance paths were included for associations of time 1 drinking with perceived descriptive norms, perceived injunctive norms, and attitudes. The model also included the covariance path between perceived descriptive norms, and perceived injunctive norms. Mediation tests were conducting with model constraints defined to calculate indirect paths from perceived descriptive norms and time 2 drinking through attitudes and between perceived injunctive norms and time 2 drinking through attitudes [41].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mediation Model Tested

Results

Means, and standard deviations of study variables are displayed in Table 1 along with counts and percentages from the National Center for Veterans Analysis and Statistics [29]. Hypothesis 1 was that personal attitudes, perceptions of descriptive norms, and perceptions of injunctive norms would be positively and directly associated with time 2 drinking. Personal attitudes significantly predicted time 2 weekly drinking (b = 0.015, 95% CI[0.009, 0.021]). Conversely, perceptions of descriptive and perception of injunctive norms did not predict time 2 weekly drinking when accounting for the covariates. Detailed results are presented in the top of Table 2.

Table 2.

Direct and Indirect Effects Results

Direct Effects on T2
Drinking
b SE b Z Boot LCI Boot HCI

Condition   0.24051 0.06883   3.49***   0.101 0.365
Combat Experience −0.01032 0.01162 −0.89 −0.032 0.013
Gender (Male)   0.32400 0.08955   3.62***   0.161 0.523
T1 Weekly Drinking   0.02736 0.00259 10.58***   0.022 0.032
Perc. Descriptive Norms −0.00004 0.00267 −0.02 −0.004 0.005
Perc. Injunctive Norm −0.00129 0.00273 −0.47 −0.006 0.004
Personal Attitudes   0.01456 0.00324   4.50***   0.009 0.021
Direct Effects on Personal Attitudes

Perc. Descriptive Norms   0.19344 0.04927 3.93***   0.108 0.304
Perc. Injunctive Norm   0.30040 0.05173 5.81***   0.187 0.389
Indirect Effects

Dnorm →  Attitudes →  T2
Weekly Drinking   0.00282 0.00087 3.22**   0.001 0.005
Inorm →   Attitudes →   T2
Weekly Drinking   0.00437 0.00129 3.38***   0.002 0.007
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001

Note. Dnorm represents perceived descriptive norms and Inorm represents perceived injunctive norms. T1 and T2 represents time 1 and time 2, respectively.

For hypotheses 2, we expected that personal attitudes would mediate the associations between perceived descriptive norms and time 2 drinking. Results revealed that personal attitudes significantly mediated the relationships between perceived descriptive norms and drinking (b = 0.003, 95% CI[0.001, 0.005]). Finally, for hypotheses 3, personal attitudes were expected to mediate the associations between perceived injunctive norms and time 2 drinking. Analysis indicated that personal attitudes significantly mediated the relationships between perceived injunctive norms and drinking (b = 0.004, 95% CI[0.002, 0.007]). The direct and indirect effect estimates, standard errors, test statistics, and confidence intervals of the mediation analysis are displayed in Table 2.

Discussion

Though much work has been conducted with college students, little work has looked at the relationships between perceived norms and personal drinking behavior among veterans [18,46] with even less work examining the association between injunctive norms, personal attitudes toward drinking, and actual drinking [21]. This study attempts to expand on previous research by using a large study of veteran drinkers to assess personal attitudes as a potential mediator of the relationships between both perceived descriptive and injunctive norms and weekly drinking. Results indicate that personal attitudes mediate the relationship between perceived descriptive norms and time 2 weekly drinking. Similarly, personal attitudes mediate the relationship between perceived injunctive norms and time 2 weekly drinking.

The results of this study attempt to enhance the understanding of the relationships between normative perceptions and drinking behavior, which is needed within military samples to improve current and inform future interventions. The provision of normative messages, focused on highlighting the difference between an individual’s perceptions of their peers’ behaviors (perceived descriptive norms) and actual peer behavior, have resulted in reductions in drinking behaviors in veteran [24] and active duty samples [47], but the specific mechanisms through which this occurs are less clear. It is not well understood what impact, if any, perceptions of peers’ attitudes (perceived injunctive norms) have on personal behavior and if correcting these perceived norms in intervention studies would add any additional impact beyond that seen in the studies that include descriptive norms only.

The findings of this study suggest that personal attitudes predict future drinking behaviors and may be a key variable in the relationship between normative perceptions and drinking behaviors. Several potential clinical implications can be interpreted from this finding. First, at least among young adult veterans, personal attitudes about drinking appear to be more proximal predictors of drinking that perceived norms. This suggests strategies which effectively reduce positive personal attitudes about heavy drinking or increase negative personal attitudes about drinking are likely to be successful in reducing heavy drinking. While changing perceived norms has been found to be a successful strategy for changing personal attitudes, other strategies may be equally or more effective. The broader attitude change literature is extensive and attitudes have been a central construct in social psychology since at least 1935 [48]. A review of the literature on attitudes and persuasion [49] outlines factors that influence attitudes including 1) factors that contribute to the persuasiveness of communications (source credibility; message content); 2) factors associated with self-perception, cognitive dissonance, and reactance, all of which have been centrally incorporated within motivational interviewing [50]. For example, people are more likely to change their attitudes when they hear themselves arguing for reasons to change than when others tell them reasons to change. Similarly, coercive and controlling manipulations tend to backfire.

Limitations & Future Directions

The findings of this study provide valuable knowledge but limitations should be considered when interpreting the results. First, this study did not examine the impact of drinking motives and negative emotions on weekly drinking and the mediation model. Heavy drinking is associated with drinking to cope, combat experiences, and posttraumatic stress disorder [51,52]. Replication studies should test and extend this work by assessing whether mental health symptoms and combat experiences moderate the relationship between normative perceptions, attitudes, and drinking. Belief in permissive drinking norms may exacerbate drinking for individuals who are using alcohol to cope with negative emotions. Finally, it is important to note that this sample was recruited using a social media platform and it may not be complete representative of all military veteran populations, particularly those that do not use social media. In the absence of population data comparing veteran drinkers between the ages of 18 and 34 such as those in our sample, it is unknown how representative our sample is.

Conclusions

The finding of this study support the importance of personal attitudes, as well as, perceptions of descriptive and injunctive norms in understanding weekly drinking in a veteran sample. Results indicate that personal attitudes toward drinking are a possible mechanism of how much individuals choose drink and how changes in drinking may result from interventions that leverage perceived social norms. Furthermore, this paper lends support to the implementation and testing of interventions targeting normative perceptions and personal attitude levels for alcohol consumption in veteran populations.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by a grant from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (R34 AA022400, “Brief Online Intervention to Reduce Heavy Alcohol Use among Young Adult Veterans”) awarded to Eric R. Pedersen. Manuscript preparation was also supported by the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grant #R01AA01476 and #F31AA024377.

Footnotes

The authors have no competing interest to report.

References

  • 1.Bray RM, Pemberton MR, Hourani LL, Witt M, Olmsted KL, Brown JM, et al. Department of Defense Survey of Health Related Behaviors Among Active Duty Military Personnel. RESEARCH TRIANGLE INST (RTI); RESEARCH TRIANGLE PARK NC: 2009. Report No.: ADA527178. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mattiko MJ, Olmsted KLR, Brown JM, Bray RM. Alcohol use and negative consequences among active duty military personnel. Addict Behav. 2011;36:608–14. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bray RM, Brown JM, Williams J. Trends in Binge and Heavy Drinking, Alcohol-Related Problems, and Combat Exposure in the U.S. Military. Subst Use Misuse. 2013;48:799–810. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2013.796990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Tanielian T. Assessing Combat Exposure and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder in Troops and Estimating the Costs to Society: Implications from the RAND Invisible Wounds of War Study [Internet] Santa Monica, CA: RAND CORP; 2016. Report No.: RAND-CT-321. Available from: http://www.va.gov/VETDATA/Veteran_Population.asp. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ames G, Cunradi C. Alcohol use and preventing alcohol-related problems among young adults in the military. Alcohol Res Health. 2004;28:252–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Calhoun PS, Elter JR, Jones ER, Kudler H, Straits-Tröster K. Hazardous Alcohol Use and Receipt of Risk-Reduction Counseling Among U.S. Veterans of the Wars in Iraq and Afghanistan. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69:1686–93. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n1103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Erbes C, Westermeyer J, Engdahl B, Johnsen E. Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder and Service Utilization in a Sample of Service Members from Iraq and Afghanistan. Mil Med. 2007;172:359–63. doi: 10.7205/milmed.172.4.359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Seal KH, Cohen G, Waldrop A, Cohen BE, Maguen S, Ren L. Substance use disorders in Iraq and Afghanistan veterans in VA healthcare, 2001–2010: Implications for screening, diagnosis and treatment. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;116:93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.11.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Carey KB, Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Carey MP, DeMartini KS. Individual-level interventions to reduce college student drinking: a meta-analytic review. Addict Behav. 2007;32:2469–94. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cronce JM, Larimer ME. Individual-focused approaches to the prevention of college student drinking. Alcohol Res Health J Natl Inst Alcohol Abuse Alcohol. 2011;34:210–21. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Miller MB, Leffingwell T, Claborn K, Meier E, Walters S, Neighbors C. Personalized feedback interventions for college alcohol misuse: An update of Walters & Neighbors (2005) Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27:909–20. doi: 10.1037/a0031174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Lewis MA. Targeting Misperceptions of Descriptive Drinking Norms: Efficacy of a Computer-Delivered Personalized Normative Feedback Intervention. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72:434–47. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Martens MP, Smith AE, Murphy JG. The efficacy of single-component brief motivational interventions among at-risk college drinkers. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2013;81:691–701. doi: 10.1037/a0032235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Dotson KB, Dunn ME, Bowers CA. Stand-Alone Personalized Normative Feedback for College Student Drinkers: A Meta-Analytic Review, 2004 to 2014. Zeeb H, editor. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0139518. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cialdini RB, Reno RR, Kallgren CA. A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58:1015–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Krieger H, Neighbors C, Lewis MA, LaBrie JW, Foster DW, Larimer ME. Injunctive Norms and Alcohol Consumption: A Revised Conceptualization. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2016;40:1083–92. doi: 10.1111/acer.13037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Borsari B, Carey KB. Descriptive and Injunctive Norms in College Drinking: A Meta-Analytic Integration. J Stud Alcohol. 2003;64:331–41. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ames GM, Cunradi CB, Moore RS, Stern P. Military Culture and Drinking Behavior Among U.S. Navy Careerists. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68:336–44. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ames GM, Duke MR, Moore RS, Cunradi CB. The Impact of Occupational Culture on Drinking Behavior of Young Adults in the U.S. Navy. J Mix Methods Res. 2009;3:129–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Williams J, Herman-Stahl M, Calvin SL, Pemberton M, Bradshaw M. Mediating mechanisms of a military web-based alcohol intervention. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;100:248–57. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Aldridge-Gerry A, Cucciare MA, Ghaus S, Ketroser N. Do normative perceptions of drinking relate to alcohol use in U.S. Military Veterans presenting to primary care. Addict Behav. 2012;37:776–82. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pedersen ER, Marshall GN, Schell TL, Neighbors C. Young adult veteran perceptions of peers’ drinking behavior and attitudes. Psychol Addict Behav. 2016;30:39–51. doi: 10.1037/adb0000120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Neighbors C, Walker D, Rodriguez L, Walton T, Mbilinyi L, Kaysen D, et al. Normative Misperceptions of Alcohol Use Among Substance Abusing Army Personnel. Mil Behav Health. 2014;2:203–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.McDevitt-Murphy ME, Murphy JG, Williams JL, Monahan CJ, Bracken-Minor KL, Fields JA. Randomized controlled trial of two brief alcohol interventions for OEF/OIF veterans. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2014;82:562–8. doi: 10.1037/a0036714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Reid AE, Carey KB. Interventions to reduce college student drinking: State of the evidence for mechanisms of behavior change. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;40:213–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Neighbors C, Lindgren KP, Knee CR, Fossos N, DiBello A. The influence of confidence on associations among personal attitudes, perceived injunctive norms, and alcohol consumption. Psychol Addict Behav. 2011;25:714–20. doi: 10.1037/a0025572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kenney SR, LaBrie JW, Lac A. Injunctive Peer Misperceptions and the Mediation of Self-Approval on Risk for Driving After Drinking Among College Students. J Health Commun. 2013;18:459–77. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2012.727963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Pedersen ER, Parast L, Marshall GN, Schell TL, Neighbors C. A randomized controlled trial of a web-based personalized normative feedback alcohol intervention for young adult veterans. J Consult Clin Psychol. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000187. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.U.S Department of Veteran Affairs. National Center for Veterans Analysis and Statistic: Veteran Population [Internet] 2016 [cited 2017 Apr 4]. Available from: https://www.va.gov/vetdata/Veteran_Population.asp.
  • 30.Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, De La Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption-II. Addiction. 1993;88:791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bradley KA, Bush KR, Epler AJ, Dobie DJ, Davis TM, Sporleder JL, et al. Two Brief Alcohol-Screening Tests From the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): Validation in a Female Veterans Affairs Patient Population. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:821. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.7.821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bradley KA, Bush KR, McDonell MB, Malone T, Fihn SD. Screening for problem drinking: Comparison of CAGE and AUDIT. J Gen Intern Med. 1998;13:379–88. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.1998.00118.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pedersen ER, Naranjo D, Marshall GN. Recruitment and retention of young adult veteran drinkers using Facebook. Mendelson JE, editor. PLOS ONE. 2017;12:e0172972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Pedersen ER, Helmuth ED, Marshall GN, Schell TL, PunKay M, Kurz J. Using Facebook to Recruit Young Adult Veterans: Online Mental Health Research. JMIR Res Protoc. 2015;4:e63. doi: 10.2196/resprot.3996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Collins RL, Parks GA, Marlatt GA. Social determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of social interaction and model status on the self-administration of alcohol. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1985;53:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.53.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Marlatt GA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Dimeff LA, Larimer ME, Quigley LA, et al. Screening and brief intervention for high-risk college student drinkers: Results from a 2-year follow-up assessment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1998;66:604–15. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.4.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Martens MP, Smith AE, Murphy JG. The efficacy of single-component brief motivational interventions among at-risk college drinkers. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2013;81:691–701. doi: 10.1037/a0032235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Neighbors C, Dillard AJ, Lewis MA, Bergstrom RL, Neil TA. Normative misperceptions and temporal precedence of perceived norms and drinking. J Stud Alcohol. 2006;67:290–9. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Baer JS, Stacy A, Larimer M. Biases in the perception of drinking norms among college students. J Stud Alcohol. 1991;52:580–6. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1991.52.580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, Fossos N, Larimer ME. Are social norms the best predictor of outcomes among heavy-drinking college students. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68:556–65. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Muthen BO, Muthen LK, Asparouhov T. Regres Mediat Anal Using Mplus. Los Angeles, CA: 2016. Chapter 8: Mediation non-continuous variables; pp. 307–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Muthén B. Applications of causally defined direct and indirect effects in mediation analysis using SEM in Mplus. Manuscr Submitt Publ [Internet] 2011 [cited 2017 Apr 1 ]; Available from: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/776d/2df784e67ab691bbffc280d3f4a480740300.pdf.
  • 43.Jacobson IG, Ryan MAK, Hooper TI, Smith TC, Amoroso PJ, Boyko EJ, et al. ALcohol use and alcohol-related problems before and after military combat deployment. JAMA. 2008;300:663–75. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.6.663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ramchand R, Miles J, Schell T, Jaycox L, Marshall GN, Tanielian T. Prevalence and correlates of drinking behaviors among previously deployed military and matched civilian populations. Mil Psychol. 2011;23:6–21. doi: 10.1080/08995605.2011.534407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Stahre MA, Brewer RD, Fonseca VP, Naimi TS. Binge Drinking Among U.S. Active-Duty Military Personnel. Am J Prev Med. 2009;36:208–17. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Cucciare MA, Blonigen DM, Sox-Harris A. Associations between alcohol-related concerns, normative perceptions of peer alcohol use, and veterans’ drinking behavior over six months. J Subst Use. 2015;20:128–35. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Pemberton MR, Williams J, Herman-Stahl M, Calvin SL, Bradshaw MR, Bray RM, et al. Evaluation of Two Web-Based Alcohol Interventions in the U.S. Military*. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72:480–9. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Allport GW. Handb Soc Psychol. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press; 1935. Attitudes; pp. 798–844. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Albarracín D, Vargas P. Attitudes and persuasion: From biology to social responses to persuasive intent. In: Fiske ST, Gilbert DT, Lindzey G, editors. Handb Soc Psychol. 5th. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 2010. pp. 394–427. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing: Helping people change. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Whiteman SD, Barry AE. A Comparative Analysis of Student Service Member/Veteran and Civilian Student Drinking Motives. J Stud Aff Res Pract. 2011;48:297–313. doi: 10.2202/1949-6605.6322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Barry AE, Whiteman S, Wadswroth SM, Hitt S. The alcohol use and associated mental health problems of student service members/veterans in higher education. Drugs Educ Prev Policy. 2012;19:415–25. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES